FFA: a flexible fusiform area for subordinate-level visual processing automatized by expertise

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "FFA: a flexible fusiform area for subordinate-level visual processing automatized by expertise"

Transcription

1 commentary FFA: a flexible fusiform area for subordinate-level visual processing automatized by expertise Michael J. Tarr and Isabel Gauthier Much evidence suggests that the fusiform face area is involved in face processing. In contrast to the accompanying article by Kanwisher, we conclude that the apparent face selectivity of this area reflects a more generalized form of processing not intrinsically specific to faces. Michael Tarr is at the Department of Cognitive and Linguistic Sciences, Brown University, Box 1978, Providence, Rhode Island 02912, USA. Isabel Gauthier is at the Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, 301 Wilson Hall, Nashville, Tennessee 37240, USA. isabel.gauthier@vanderbilt.edu or Michael_Tarr@brown.edu Factors affecting FFA processing Given that face selectivity may not be intrinsically related to the specific geometry of faces, what else might account for the strong activation for faces in the FFA? Behavioral work suggests two critical factors associated with face recognition because of our particular experience with faces. First, the level of categorization at which objects are recognized is different faces generally being recognized at the individual level, but objects such as chairs or birds being recognized at a more categorical level. Second, we have far greater expertise with faces as compared to almost any other visual category. Expertise with faces may automatize face processing at the individual level, thereby rendering task manipulations less effective for faces, for example, the rapid presentation rates used in some studety of conditions, including line drawings, two-tone, gray-scale or colored images stimulus manipulations that affect facerecognition performance but have little impact on activation in FFA 1,4,5. For example, face inversion seems to only minimally influence activity in the FFA 1,6,7. This result appears at odds with the strong effect of inversion on face recognition performance 8. However, inversion only impairs, but does not abolish, the perception of identity. Inverted faces are typically recognized well above chance level, which is consistent with the small but significant fmri face inversion effects reported in some studies 1,9. Moreover, this same small inversion effect has been replicated with non-face objects in expert subjects 9. The absence or relatively small inversion effect in the FFA may also mean that activity as measured by fmri simply is not sufficiently fine-grained to pick up certain effects. As a case in point, fmri cannot detect the robust and early effect of face inversion reported in ERPs: a ten-millisecond delay in the N170 face-sensitive potential 10,11 that is recorded at temporo-occipital electrodes 12,13 or a similar inversion effect in intracranial recordings 14. A second model holds that category selectivity reflects the presence of a geometrically defined feature map in ventral temporal cortex 15,16. The hypothesis is that small, localized brain regions are activated by objects, generally from a single category, with similar shape and image features. Evidence for feature maps comes from monkey neurophysiology 17 suggesting a topography of features in inferior temporal cortex (IT) and from human fmri studies indicating that across a single task, different stimuli selectively activate different regions of the ventral temporal cortex. For instance, distinct brain areas respond preferentially to letter strings, chairs, buildings or faces 4,6,16,18. However, geometric How does the primate visual system process and identify objects? Central to this question is that visual recognition can occur at many levels, from coarse categories, for example a car, to highly specific individuals, my dad s 1960 TR3 convertible (subordinate-level processing). The processing demands exerted by these two tasks seem to require separable visual recognition systems, one dedicated to determining category membership and one dedicated to individuation. The most conspicuous version of this argument is one in which there is a functional and neuroanatomical division between the mechanisms supporting object at the basic level and face recognition at the individual level. Here we evaluate this claim and ask whether putatively face-specific mechanisms in a region of the visual cortex known as the Fusiform Face Area ( FFA ) are selective specifically to the geometry of faces or whether, through the operation of other factors, they extend to non-face stimuli. The comparison between faces and nonface objects is critical for evaluating three competing models. First, Kanwisher (this issue) suggests that face selectivity in the FFA as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) reflects a special-purpose, innate mechanism for detecting the geometry of faces, that is, a mechanism that determines only that a face is a face, but not the individual identity of the face 1 3. Evidence for this detection model comes from studies in which selectivity for faces in FFA has been established over a wide varisimilarity alone seems unlikely to account for category selectivity, in that task manipulations independent of image geometry can lead to activation maps indistinguishable from those produced by manipulating geometry. Third, our preferred model, the flexible process map 19, holds that an assortment of extrastriate areas support separable components of visual object processing and recognition. This model is distinguished from the detection and feature-map models in that specialization of large extrastriate areas (that is, those detectable with fmri) is not a priori a function of stimulus geometry. Rather, through experience, observers associate particular geometries which define object categories with task-appropriate recognition strategies that automatically recruit components of the process map. In contrast to other accounts, the processmap model provides a general explanation for category selectivity, as well as why the FFA responds to a wide range of stimuli, including faces and non-face objects. 764 nature neuroscience volume 3 no 8 august 2000

2 commentary ies (such as 300 ms with a 500 ms ISI 5 ) may not affect face processing nearly as much as object processing. Because extreme values for these two dimensions are typically associated with face recognition, any comparison between faces and non-face objects confounds these factors unless they are independently manipulated. When we do such manipulations, it is the level of categorization and the level of expertise, not the stimulus geometry, that are critical in determining selectivity in the FFA. Given such domain-general processes, we argue that the FFA may be better thought of as a flexible fusiform area a mechanism for visual recognition that exhibits a great deal of plasticity in response to both task demands and experience. Evidence from fmri Functional MRI provides an ideal tool for evaluating these models because brain areas can be identified in a functional manner. In comparison to cytoarchitectonic criteria (like Brodmann areas) or gross anatomical landmarks (such as the Talairach system), a region performing the same function in different individuals can be defined, allowing for variability in the mapping of function to anatomy. Building on a long history of neuropsychological findings indicating specialization in the primate visual system for face processing 20, both PET and fmri studies have localized face-selective regions in the ventral temporal lobe 4,18,23,24. Functional MRI has been used to identify a small number of faceselective voxels the FFA through a comparison of faces and non-face objects in a common task 4. Importantly, this neural pattern replicates within the same study and generalizes across experimental conditions. That is, candidate face-selective voxels are recruited more for faces than for non-face objects even when the task and stimuli are varied. These results from imaging seem to support earlier accounts of face selectivity in the brain (and provide a method of localizing it). Different interpretations are possible, however, when categorization level and experience are taken into account. To specifically address the roles of these two factors, several studies have manipulated the level of categorization and the level of expertise for non-face objects. These studies find that the response of FFA to non-face objects is affected by these manipulations independent of particular stimulus geometries Logically then, these others factors can account for most of the strong face selectivity in FFA activity. a b Manipulating categorization-level The FFA bilaterally shows an increased response when familiar artifactual and natural kind objects are seen in the context of visually similar stimuli 25. The FFA shows a similar response for animals, even when their faces are obscured 28 (but see ref. 5). Moreover, when the level of categorization is manipulated by different labels that subjects match to the same image (for example, TR3 versus car for the same picture of a TR3), the FFA is more active for judgments requiring classification at a subordinate level compared to more categorical judgments for a large variety of objects, living or artifactual 26,27. Thus, living things, including animals and animals with hidden faces 15, may recruit the FFA because of their visual homogeneity 29 and the level of visual discrimination that is most often applied to them (such as distinguishing a cow from a horse by default). Manipulating level of expertise The second factor influencing FFA activity for a wide range of objects is subject expertise. The FFA and a similarly selective occipital area ( OFA ) in the right hemisphere 4,7,30,31 are both recruited when observers become experts in discriminating objects from a visually homogeneous category (objects that share a common geometric structure). As detailed below, this occurs in subjects trained in the laboratory to be experts with novel objects called Greebles, as well as bird and car experts with 20 years of experience 9,25. Expertise here means more than practice or priming with specific instances of a class because the FFA exhibits an equivalent response for new Proportion Correct Proportion Correct Test Number Test Number Fig. 1. Examples of Greebles. (a) Greebles from a set that Greeble experts could learn to recognize faster than novices. (b) Another set of Greebles, which the same experts could not learn faster than novices, presumably because they are more visually homogeneous than Greebles in the training set. Filled squares denote data from novices, and open circles denote data from experts Greebles never shown during training. Such expertise effects indicate a high degree of flexibility with regard to acceptable image geometries in the neural network mediating object recognition. Moreover, this flexibility does not have a critical period and can be harnessed for relatively novel learning experiences (such as learning to read brain scans or individuate nonsense objects such as Greebles). Automatization by expertise Object processing in the FFA can be automatized by expertise. Consider that Greeble experts show FFA activation in response to Greebles even under passive viewing instructions 9. Moreover, whereas novices can easily focus on the top half of a composite Greeble (made out of the top of one and the bottom of another), Greeble experts cannot ignore the bottom distractor part. The behavioral changes measured by this composite effect show a significant correlation across subjects and training sessions with activity in the right FFA of Greeble experts (I.G. and M.J.T., unpublished data). Thus, neural changes with expertise, at least in the right FFA, seem to be tied strongly to holistic processing strategies. This is consistent with a PET study 32 reporting that the right FFA is more active when observers attend to whole faces than face parts, whereas the opposite pattern is found in the left FFA. An fmri study of bird and car experts further supports the idea that expertise automatizes processing in the right FFA 25. In novices, the homogeneous classes of birds and cars elicit more activity in the FFA than a set of various familiar objects, and this Mean Response Time (ms) Mean Response Time (ms) nature neuroscience volume 3 no 8 august

3 commentary a b Fig. 2. Complex selectivity of IT cells. Adapted from ref. 49. (a) This cell responded most to the picture of a teacup, and also responded more to the picture of a balloon than all other images tested. Although the selectivity of this cell may be based on color, this set of responses illustrates that any hypothesis about the selectivity of a neuron is highly dependent upon the stimulus set used in an experiment. (b) A cell that responds to a subset of animals, with no obvious measure of similarity separating the effective from ineffective stimuli. The authors 49 argue that selectivity emerges from experience and that images that activate IT neurons need not be especially similar to one another. effect is more pronounced when subjects attend to the identity of the objects than when they attend to their location. In contrast, experts show more activity in the right FFA for their category of expertise, birds or cars, regardless of whether they attend to the identity or location of the objects. In addition, a behavioral test of relative performance in matching birds or cars is highly correlated (r ~ 0.8) with activity in the right FFA for another set of birds or cars during the location judgments. Similarly, an ERP study of bird and dog experts found an enhanced N170 the negative potential typically associated with face-specific processing 10,11 for categorizing objects within their respective domains of expertise 33. Thus, both fmri 25 and ERP 33 studies of experts provide evidence that the neural basis of face recognition extends to other domains of expertise. Although these findings would already seem sufficient to reject the hypothesis of the FFA as a face detector 1,2, additional evidence is provided by fmri habituation procedures assessing the nature of automatized processing for faces in the FFA. A task in which subjects attended to the location of a face revealed that FFA and OFA activity habituates more to the repeated presentation of a single face than to different faces 31. Thus, this activity seems to reflect more than generic face detection. Also supporting the hypothesis that the FFA is involved in individuation of faces, attention to face identity (rather than to eye gaze) increases activity in the vicinity of the FFA 34. It does not follow, however, that this area is not involved in individual face processing when subjects attend to dimensions other than identity. Indeed, several studies find that the FFA activity level in response to faces, as well as to other categories of expertise, is relatively immune to whether the task is recognition or not 4,16,25. Thus, it seems that the FFA automatically processes objects for which observers are experts at the subordinate level, but, as with most visual processes, the efficacy of the system may be further modulated by attention. Evidence from neuropsychology The most salient neuropsychological example of impaired visual object recognition with brain injury is that of prosopagnosia. Patients with this disorder usually have suffered injury in the lingual and fusiform gyri, which typically encompass the FFA 35. The pattern of sparing and loss is striking although they can recognize a face as a face, these patients are remarkably impaired at face recognition at the individual level, but sometimes seem to be able to recognize non-face objects. Although this deficit is often cited as evidence for a face-specific neural substrate, such as the FFA, it actually says little about the origins of face selectivity. Thus, prosopagnosia as a face-specific deficit does not distinguish among the detection, feature-map and process-map models all three of which acknowledge the existence of and, indeed, attempt to explain category-selective neural substrates. To the extent that prosopagnosia speaks to these models at all, it is because of an intact ability. Even though the FFA is typically lesioned in prosopagnosia, face detection is spared when tested 35,36 ; thus, the detection model cannot account for face-selective activity in the FFA. Interestingly, autistic individuals do not show normal FFA activity when viewing faces and also have few problems with face detection 37. At the same time, a patient (CK) with the complementary pattern of sparing and loss preserved face recognition with impaired object recognition is widely held as the other side of a double dissociation demonstrating distinct face and object recognition mechanisms 38. As with prosopagnosia, at issue is not whether there exist category-selective brain regions, but rather, whether these regions are domaingeneral or domain-specific. That is, all three models predict that a patient such as CK could occur; the question is simply why. Unfortunately, testing the domainspecificity of CK s spared recognition abilities would be difficult in that it is his holistic processing mechanisms that remain intact (given that it is generally agreed that the FFA is involved in holistic processing for faces and possibly other objects 9,25,27,31 ). Within the framework of the process-map model, the recruitment of these holistic mechanisms results from the interaction between subordinate-level recognition and expertise training. CK, however, seems to have lost the ability to recognize objects in a non-holistic manner and, in particular, the ability to move from a novice to an expert for any new object class. Thus, unlike normal subjects 9,39, CK cannot be bootstrapped into applying the intact components of his visual recognition system to new, non-face objects. 766 nature neuroscience volume 3 no 8 august 2000

4 commentary Beyond its specific inconsistency with the detection model, the syndrome of prosopagnosia reinforces the point that processing in the FFA is domain-general. In particular, patients with prosopagnosia are impaired with more than faces. Although this was raised as a possibility some time ago 20, it was rejected by some researchers 40 based on a study in which a prosopagnosic patient was differentially impaired at recognizing previously studied faces as compared to recognizing previously studied homogeneous non-face objects (such as eyeglasses). This experiment, however, did not consider potential response biases, for instance, the frequency with which the patient responded old to individual items actually shown during study relative to those items not shown during study, and the amount of time and effort expended by the patient for faces as compared to objects. Indeed, if the patient believed (perhaps incorrectly) that he was more impaired with faces relative to other objects, he may spend less time and allocate fewer attentional resources in encoding and recalling faces 36. These concerns were addressed in a study that measured recognition response times in conjunction with a bias-free (d ) measure of recognition accuracy 36. Data from two patients with prosopagnosia were collected for their recognition of faces, visually similar non-face objects and Greebles. Although the patients did about as well as control subjects in recognizing objects and Greebles, they showed a speed accuracy tradeoff to achieve this level of performance. That is, the patients spent far more time than the controls to achieve the same level of recognition accuracy. Moreover, when patients were forced to spend the same amount of time for each stimulus category, accuracy as measured by d a statistic capturing a subject s ability to discriminate or remember items independent of response bias revealed that patients with prosopagnosia are most impaired whenever visually similar stimuli must be discriminated, regardless of category. Thus, prosopagnosia may actually be a domain-general deficit when assessed using a complete picture of performance. Critiques of the process map It is our contention that behavioral, neuroimaging, and neuropsychological results provide strong evidence in support of a domain-general model the process-map model for face processing and recognition. In particular, manipulations of the factors of level of categorization and level of expertise are able to account for nearly all of the apparent differences between face and object recognition. There are, however, skeptics. Next we review and address some critiques of a domain-general account. Several groups have attempted to refute the hypothesis that the reason that FFA seems face selective is because faces are recognized at the individual level, whereas other objects are recognized at a coarser level. One study compared passive viewing of faces and visually similar flowers appearing on a continuously changing background of nonsense patterns or non-face objects. When compared to a baseline of non-face objects, faces, but not flowers, produced activation in right fusiform gyrus 41. Similarly, another study compared identity judgments for faces and hands and found higher activation for faces than hands as compared to a fixation baseline 4. How can these results be reconciled with other studies indicating a role for subordinate-level processing in the FFA? First, consider that the first study used passive viewing and did not require subjects to process flowers at the subordinate level. The second study showed faces and hands for 500 ms each. As reviewed above, subordinate-level judgments not automatized by expertise typically require additional processing time 42. Second, these experiments only indicate that categorization level is not sufficient to account for all the specificity in the FFA. Note that this is already assumed by our model, in which the interaction of multiple factors (such as homogeneity, categorization level and expertise) constrain facerecognition performance. In sum, experiments using non-face objects can provide some evidence that a given factor influences FFA response. However, the exact contribution of a factor to the combination that leads to the maximum FFA response cannot be inferred based on the same experiment (unless it can be precisely equated for faces and objects and we know how it interacts with other factors). This also means that we cannot refute the possibility that image geometry is one of several factors that contribute to maximum selectivity at least not until maximum selectivity is obtained with non-face objects. Even accepting that the FFA is involved in subordinate-level recognition, it remains possible that this area is preferentially selective for a specific geometry that defining what sort of an image counts as a face. Refuting this argument is complex although we know that the FFA can become selective for other domains of expertise, for example, Greebles, birds and cars 9,25, it is often stated that Greebles look like faces and that experts can learn to apply faceselective mechanisms to other categories. These two claims seem reasonable a mechanism efficient for face processing might gain some of its efficiency by making assumptions regarding stimulus geometry. However, it is crucial that we be clear about what such claims imply, given that we have already established selectivity of the FFA in response to non-face stimuli. As a specific and oft cited example, consider FFA selectivity for Greebles. Several findings indicate that Greebles are not treated as face-like in terms of their geometry. First, Greeble novices do not show Greeblespecific activation in FFA, but Greeble experts do show such activation 9. Second, Greeble novices do not show face-like behavioral effects, but Greeble experts do show such effects 39,43. Third, CK, the agnosic patient who shows intact face recognition, but is dramatically impaired in non-face object recognition 38 cannot recognize Greebles any better than other objects even when he is told to think of Greebles as faces or little people (M. Behrmann, I. G. & M. J. T., unpublished observation). At most, subjects can only learn to treat Greebles like faces following an intensive training process that takes about 10 hours. A second point is that subjects can see objects as face-like without recruiting those processes thought to be face-specific. For example, inverted faces are not recognized using the same configural processes as upright faces, yet anybody, including prosopagnosics, can identify inverted faces as faces. Likewise, schematic faces (made out of small geometric shapes) clearly look like faces but elicit about half as much activity as shaded faces do (equal to the activation obtained with the back of heads in a different experiment 44 ). Finally, perhaps the best example comes from patients with prosopagnosia, who can identify faces as such, but have severe problems perceiving and recognizing them as individuals 35. This is consistent with our experience training Greeble experts. Neither behavioral nor neural effects of expertise with Greebles are found even when subjects are well advanced in the training but before they meet our criterion for expertise (to recognize Greebles as quickly at the individual as at the family level). Thus, seeing Greebles as face-like (which any novice can do easily) does not predict expertise effects, including recruitment of the FFA. In contrast, a specific measure of holistic processing (the composite effect) with Greebles correlates with the recruitment of the FFA for Greebles in a different task (I.G. & M.J.T., unpublished data). This method, combining precise psychophysics and fmri measurements of expertise acquisition, indicates that the nature neuroscience volume 3 no 8 august

5 commentary CONCLUSIONS From grandmothers to scientists (and the two are not mutually exclusive), there is a certain fondness for the idea that the difficulty of face recognition combined with its social importance has exerted unique adaptive pressures. Thus, it is often argued, it would not be particularly surprising if there were a distinct neural and functional module for face recognition. Although the adaptive pressures may be real, this implies only that the primate visual system should be capable of efficiently performing this task, not that whatever perceptual mechanisms represent the present end-state of evolution must be exclusive to faces. Put another way, speculating on the evolutionary reasons for why a particular neural system might have arisen does not define the limits of this system. In an effort to explore these limits, we have collected evidence that FFA processing is domain-general. Specifically, it is involved in processing subordinate-level information for all objects, including faces. This processing is not restricted to objects of a certain geometry (that is, faces), as this factor cannot account for the majority of responses in the FFA to non-face objects. On the other hand, manipulations of the level of categorization and the level of expertise are able to predict when and if FFA activity will be found in a given task. Thus, even if stimulus geometry is somehow involved in FFA selectivity, it is likely to remain only one of several factors that contribute to the specialization of this brain area. That faces generally elicit the maximum response in the FFA is interestcally, a localizer is used to define a FFA at the outset of the study, and all further analyses reflect the response of only these voxels. Little consideration is given to the potential variability of the FFA. For instance, both the peaks of face selectivity and the extent of the FFA as defined using a faces-minus-objects comparison can differ substantially within subjects during passive viewing as compared to an active identification task 25. This issue is highly relevant to the nature of the selectivity in the FFA, as differences between faces and nonface objects must be weighed against the variability of face selectivity itself. To the extent the microstructure of this variability can be assessed with current techniques, there is no indication that the face-selective part of cortex is in any sense mutually exclusive with the rest of the object-recognition system. In a fmri study of bird and car experts, several criteria for the definition of the FFA were compared (see The criterion most stringent in terms of size and face selectivity (face to object activity ratio, 12.8) led to a right FFA region of interest with an average size of 3 voxels (each voxel was mm 2 in plane) but could be found in all 19 subjects. In comparison, the criterion used in many prior studies 4 led to a larger area (average size of 6 voxels), which displayed more moderate face selectivity (face to object activity ratio, 5.2) but was less inclusive subject-wise (a right FFA only found in 7 of 19 subjects). Crucially, regardless of criterion, the FFA showed sensitivity to the level of categorization and expertise manipulations. This and several other analyses, such as center of mass comparisons and inspection of the peaks in unsmoothed activation maps, indicate that face expertise cannot be dissociated from object expertise at the spatial resolution of fmri. The existence of face cells in IT 45 is often cited as evidence for a distinct recognition mechanism for faces. On the other hand, studies in which monkeys learn to recognize novel objects have revealed a remarkable similarity between properties of cells tuned to these trained objects and face cells 46,47. One interpretation is that it would be surprising if neurons exist that respond strongly to both faces and cars (but not other complex objects) in car experts (for example) 48. The implication is that faces and objects would selectively activate independent populations of neurons, interspersed within the same fmri voxel. Even if this were the case, the intermingling of at least two populations of neurons in a relatively small region of IT would suggest that FFA is recruited by Greebles because experts acquire a more efficient strategy for discriminating Greebles, not because subjects learn to see Greebles as face-like. Reinforcing this point, even when Greeble experts successfully transfer their expert abilities to the recognition of new Greeble exemplars, their expertise may not apply to new Greebles that are more homogeneous than the original training set, even though the new Greebles clearly look equivalently face-like (Fig. 1). The evidence for selectivity on a purely geometric level in the FFA is not strong, although it is possible that the FFA tends to respond to objects that are face-like because our perceptual expertise with faces makes this geometry a cue that the computations executed in the FFA are required. However, our results indicate experience recognizing Greebles, cars or birds at the individual level can make the geometries of these objects equally valid cues. Selectivity for particular image geometry is learned a point driven home because objects of acceptable geometry do not activate the FFA by default. Thus, selectivity at a purely geometric level does not provide an adequate account of FFA specialization. In contrast, as already reviewed, we have established that level of categorization and expertise in a given object domain do affect the response of FFA independent of stimulus geometry. We argue that it is the manipulation of these factors, not stimulus geometry, that leads to recruitment of the FFA. Could different interpretations for the function of the FFA depend on how we define this area? One method consists of defining a face-selective area in a group of subjects and measuring its behavior in other conditions (in the same or another group). This averaging method is not optimal, but it may be useful when individual localizers are not available. A study comparing group-averaged with individual functional definitions revealed that even though a FFA can be found in the large majority of subjects, it is small enough and variable enough in its location that the former approach may be inadequate 27. Specifically, if the defined region is large enough to encompass the FFA for all subjects, it also includes many non-face-selective voxels, and if it is small enough to contain only face-selective voxels, it may not include the FFA of many subjects. Given these problems, many researchers have chosen to rely on individual localizers, leading to very small and highly selective FFAs 4,9. This approach sustains a certain illusion: that of a sharply defined area that is stable across conditions. Typithese neurons serve similar functional roles, but with somewhat different tuning (for instance, adjacent cortical columns are tuned to specific orientations, but the functional role of the entire area is a coding of orientation space arguing that faces are special in this context is equivalent to suggesting that vertical lines are special, too). Moreover, the finding that a given neuron responds most strongly to a specific stimulus does not imply that this same neuron is not functionally implicated in the processing of other stimuli. For example, neurophysiologists often rely on a reduction method 17 in which, beginning with an image from a restricted basis set, they incrementally simplify the test stimulus while attempting to maintain the same level of cell response. Thus, the reduction method is bound to produce a single preferred feature set per cell. In contrast, IT cells can have responses that defy simple explanations in terms of singular preferred features (Fig. 2). 768 nature neuroscience volume 3 no 8 august 2000

6 commentary ing, but only as an effect that is inherently confounded with the processing biases that, through experience, are automatically engaged when we see a face. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This work was a collaborative effort, and the order of authorship is arbitrary. We thank D. Sheinberg for his careful reading of this review. M.J.T. thanks S. Pinker for a challenging discussion on some of the issues raised here; I.G. thanks N. Kanwisher for many stimulating discussions on our diverging opinions. This work was supported by NSF Award SBR RECEIVED 4 APRIL; ACCEPTED 20 JUNE, Kanwisher, N., Tong, F. & Nakayama, K. The effect of face inversion on the human fusiform face area. Cognition 68, B1 11 (1998). 2. Bentin, S., Deouell, L. Y. & Soroker, N. Selective visual streaming in face recognition: evidence from developmental prosopagnosia. Neuroreport 10, (1999). 3. Puce, A., Allison, T. & McCarthy, G. Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. III: Effects of top-down processing on face-specific potentials. Cereb. Cortex 9, (1999). 4. Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J. & Chun, M. M. The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J. Neurosci. 17, (1997). 5. Kanwisher, N., Stanley, D. & Harris, A. The fusiform face area is selective for faces not animals. Neuroreport 10, (1999). 6. Aguirre, G. K., Zarahn, E. & D Esposito, M. An area within human ventral cortex sensitive to building stimuli: Evidence and implications. Neuron 21, (1998). 7. Haxby, J. V. et al. The effect of face inversion on activity in human neural systems for face and object perception. Neuron 22, (1999). 8. Yin, R. K. Looking at upside-down faces. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 81, (1969). 9. Gauthier, I., Tarr, M. J., Anderson, A. W., Skudlarski, P. & Gore, J. C. Activation of the middle fusiform face area increases with expertise in recognizing novel objects. Nat. Neurosci. 2, (1999). 10. Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, E. & McCarthy, G. Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 8, (1996). 11. Jeffreys, D. A. A face-responsive potential recorded from the human scalp. Exp. Brain Res. 78, (1989). 12. Liu, J., Higuchi, M., Marantz, A. & Kanwisher, N. The selectivity of the occipitotemporal M170 for faces. Neuroreport 7, (2000). 13. Rossion, B. et al. The N170 occipito-temporal component is delayed and enhanced to inverted faces but not to inverted objects: An electrophysiological account of face-specific processes in the human brain. Neuroreport 11, (2000). 14. McCarthy, G., Puce, A., Belger, A. & Allison, T. Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. II: Response properties of face-specific potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 5, (1999). 15. Chao, L. L., Haxby, J. V. & Martin, A. Attributebased neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects. Nat. Neurosci. 2, (1999). 16. Ishai, A., Ungerleider, L. G., Martin, A., Schouten, J. L. & Haxby, J. Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 96, (1999). 17. Tanaka, K. Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 19, (1996). 18. Puce, A., Allison, T., Asgari, M., Gore, J. C. & McCarthy, G. Differential sensitivity of human visual cortex to faces, letterstrings, and textures: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 16, (1996). 19. Gauthier, I. What constrains the organization of the ventral temporal cortex? Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 1 2 (2000). 20. Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H. & Van Hoesen, G. W. Prosopagnosia: Anatomical basis and behavioral mechanisms. Neurology 32, (1982). 21. Diamond, R. & Carey, S. Why faces are and are not special: An effect of expertise. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 115, (1986). 22. Tanaka, J. W. & Gauthier, I. in Mechanisms of Perceptual Learning (eds. Goldstone, R. L., Medin, D. L. & Schyns, P. G.) (Academic, San Diego, California, 1997). 23. Haxby, J. V. et al. Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways in human extrastriate cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 88, (1991). 24. Sergent, J., Ohta, S. & MacDonald, B. Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing: A positron emission tomography study. Brain 115, (1992). 25. Gauthier, I., Skudlarski, P., Gore, J. C. & Anderson, A. W. Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition. Nat. Neurosci. 3, (2000). 26. Gauthier, I., Anderson, A. W., Tarr, M. J., Skudlarski, P. & Gore, J. C. Levels of categorization in visual object studied with functional MRI. Curr. Biol. 7, (1997). 27. Gauthier, I. et al. Does visual subordinate-level categorisation engage the functionally defined Fusiform Face Area? Cognit. Neuropsychol. 17, (2000). 28. Chao, L. L., Martin, A. & Haxby, J. V. Are faceresponsive regions selective only for faces? Neuroreport 10, (1999). 29. Gaffan, D. & Heywood, C. A. A spurious category-specific agnosia for living things in normal human and nonhuman primates. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, (1994). 30. Halgren, E. et al. Location of human faceselective cortex with respect to retinotopic areas. Hum. Brain Mapp. 7, (1999). 31. Gauthier, I. et al. The fusiform face area is part of a network that processes faces at the individual level. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, (2000). 32. Rossion, B. et al. Hemispheric asymmetries for whole-based and part-based face processing in the human brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. (in press). 33. Tanaka, J. W. & Curran, T. A neural basis for expert object recognition. Psychol. Sci. (in press). 34. Hoffman, E. A. & Haxby, J. V. Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nat. Neurosci. 3, (2000). 35. Tranel, D., Damasio, A. R. & Damasio, H. Intact recognition of facial expression, gender and age in patients with impaired recognition of face identity. Neurology 38, (1998). 36. Gauthier, I., Behrmann, M. & Tarr, M. J. Can face recognition really be dissociated from object recognition? J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, (1999). 37. Schultz, R. T. et al. Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and asperger syndrome. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57, (2000). 38. Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G. & Behrmann, M. What is special about face recognition? Nineteen experiments on a person with visual object agnosia and dyslexia but normal face recognition. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, (1997). 39. Gauthier, I. & Tarr, M. J. Becoming a Greeble expert: Exploring the face recognition mechanism. Vision Res. 37, (1997). 40. Farah, M. J., Levinson, K. L. & Klein, K. L. Face perception and within-category discrimination in prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia 33, (1995). 41. McCarthy, G., Puce, A., Gore, J. C. & Allison, T. Face-specific processing in the human fusiform gyrus. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, (1997). 42. Jolicoeur, P., Gluck, M. & Kosslyn, S. M. Pictures and names: Making the connection. Cognit. Psychol. 16, (1984). 43. Gauthier, I., Williams, P., Tarr, M. J. & Tanaka, J. Training Greeble experts: A framework for studying expert object recognition processes. Vision Res. 38, (1998). 44. Tong, F., Nakayama, K., Moscovitch, M., Weinrib, O. & Kanwisher, N. Response properties of the human fusiform face area. Cognit. Neuropsychol. 17, (2000). 45. Perrett, D. I., Rolls, E. T. & Caan, W. Visual neurones responsive to faces in the monkey temporal cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 47, (1982). 46. Kobatake, E., Wang, G. & Tanaka, K. Effects of shape-discrimination training on the selectivity of inferotemporal cells in adult monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 80, (1998). 47. Logothetis, N. K. & Pauls, J. Psychophysical and physiological evidence for viewer-centered object representation in the primate. Cereb. Cortex 3, (1995). 48. Kanwisher, N., Downing, P., Epstein, R. & Kourtzi, Z. in Handbook of Functional Neuroimaging of Cognition (eds. Cabeza, R. & Kingstone, A.) (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, in press). 49. Sheinberg, D. L. & Logothetis, N. K. in Perceptual Learning (ed. Fahle, M.) (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 2000). nature neuroscience volume 3 no 8 august

Perception of Faces and Bodies

Perception of Faces and Bodies CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Perception of Faces and Bodies Similar or Different? Virginia Slaughter, 1 Valerie E. Stone, 2 and Catherine Reed 3 1 Early Cognitive Development Unit and 2

More information

Accounts for the N170 face-effect: a reply to Rossion, Curran, & Gauthier

Accounts for the N170 face-effect: a reply to Rossion, Curran, & Gauthier S. Bentin, D. Carmel / Cognition 85 (2002) 197 202 197 COGNITION Cognition 85 (2002) 197 202 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Discussion Accounts for the N170 face-effect: a reply to Rossion, Curran, & Gauthier

More information

A defense of the subordinate-level expertise account for the N170 component

A defense of the subordinate-level expertise account for the N170 component B. Rossion et al. / Cognition 85 (2002) 189 196 189 COGNITION Cognition 85 (2002) 189 196 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Discussion A defense of the subordinate-level expertise account for the N170 component

More information

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni VISUAL AGNOSIA -damage to the extrastriate visual regions (occipital, parietal and temporal lobes) disrupts recognition of complex visual stimuli

More information

(for Trends in Cognitive Science) Cindy Bukach 1,3. Isabel Gauthier 1,3. Michael J. Tarr 2,3. Research Center, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN

(for Trends in Cognitive Science) Cindy Bukach 1,3. Isabel Gauthier 1,3. Michael J. Tarr 2,3. Research Center, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN BEYOND FACES AND MODULARITY (for Trends in Cognitive Science) Cindy Bukach 1,3 Isabel Gauthier 1,3 Michael J. Tarr 2,3 1 Department of Psychology, Center for Integrative and Cognitive Neuroscience, Vanderbilt

More information

Are face-responsive regions selective only for faces?

Are face-responsive regions selective only for faces? Cognitive Neuroscience and Neurophysiology 10, 2945±2950 (1999) TO examine the speci city of face-responsive regions for face processing, we used fmri to measure the response of the fusiform gyrus and

More information

Revisiting the Role of the Fusiform Face Area in Visual Expertise

Revisiting the Role of the Fusiform Face Area in Visual Expertise Cerebral Cortex August 2005;15:1234--1242 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi006 Advance Access publication January 26, 2005 Revisiting the Role of the Fusiform Face Area in Visual Expertise Yaoda Xu Department of

More information

Normal Greeble Learning in a Severe Case of Developmental Prosopagnosia

Normal Greeble Learning in a Severe Case of Developmental Prosopagnosia Neuron, Vol. 43, 469 473, August 19, 2004, Copyright 2004 by Cell Press Normal Greeble Learning in a Severe Case of Developmental Prosopagnosia Case Study Bradley C. Duchaine, 1, * Kerry Dingle, 1 version

More information

Unraveling Mechanisms for Expert Object Recognition: Bridging Brain Activity and Behavior

Unraveling Mechanisms for Expert Object Recognition: Bridging Brain Activity and Behavior Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, Vol. 28, No. 2, 431 446 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.431

More information

Are Greebles like faces? Using the neuropsychological exception to test the rule

Are Greebles like faces? Using the neuropsychological exception to test the rule Neuropsychologia 42 (2004) 1961 1970 Are Greebles like faces? Using the neuropsychological exception to test the rule Isabel Gauthier a,, Marlene Behrmann b, Michael J. Tarr c a Department of Psychology,

More information

Beyond faces and modularity: the power of an expertise framework

Beyond faces and modularity: the power of an expertise framework Beyond faces and modularity: the power of an expertise framework Cindy M. Bukach 1, Isabel Gauthier 1 and Michael J. Tarr 2 1 Department of Psychology, Center for Integrative and Cognitive Neuroscience,

More information

The influence of feature conjunction on object inversion and conversion effects

The influence of feature conjunction on object inversion and conversion effects Perception, 2014, volume 43, pages 0000 0000 doi:10.1068/p7610 The influence of feature conjunction on object inversion and conversion effects Maxim A Bushmakin, Thomas W James Department of Psychological

More information

The functional organization of the ventral visual pathway and its relationship to object recognition

The functional organization of the ventral visual pathway and its relationship to object recognition Kanwisher-08 9/16/03 9:27 AM Page 169 Chapter 8 The functional organization of the ventral visual pathway and its relationship to object recognition Kalanit Grill-Spector Abstract Humans recognize objects

More information

Brain Activation during Face Perception: Evidence of a Developmental Change

Brain Activation during Face Perception: Evidence of a Developmental Change Brain Activation during Face Perception: Evidence of a Developmental Change E. H. Aylward 1, J. E. Park 1, K. M. Field 1, A. C. Parsons 1, T. L. Richards 1, S. C. Cramer 2, and A. N. Meltzoff 1 Abstract

More information

Impaired face discrimination in acquired prosopagnosia is associated with abnormal response to individual faces in the right middle fusiform gyrus

Impaired face discrimination in acquired prosopagnosia is associated with abnormal response to individual faces in the right middle fusiform gyrus Impaired face discrimination in acquired prosopagnosia is associated with abnormal response to individual faces in the right middle fusiform gyrus Christine Schiltz Bettina Sorger Roberto Caldara Fatima

More information

Selectivity for the Human Body in the Fusiform Gyrus

Selectivity for the Human Body in the Fusiform Gyrus J Neurophysiol 93: 603 608, 2005. First published August 4, 2004; doi:10.1152/jn.00513.2004. Selectivity for the Human Body in the Fusiform Gyrus Marius V. Peelen and Paul E. Downing School of Psychology,

More information

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Cortical Analysis of Visual Context Moshe Bar, Elissa Aminoff. 2003. Neuron, Volume 38, Issue 2, Pages 347 358. Visual objects in context Moshe Bar.

More information

BECOMING A GREEBLE EXPERT: EXPLORING MECHANISMS FOR FACE RECOGNITION

BECOMING A GREEBLE EXPERT: EXPLORING MECHANISMS FOR FACE RECOGNITION Vision Research 1997, Vol. 37, No. 12, 1673-1682 BECOMING A GREEBLE EXPERT: EXPLORING MECHANISMS FOR FACE RECOGNITION Isabel Gauthier Yale University Michael J. Tarr Brown University Sensitivity to configural

More information

Can face recognition really be dissociated from object recognition?

Can face recognition really be dissociated from object recognition? Can face recognition really be dissociated from object recognition? Isabel Gauthier Yale University Marlene Behrmann Carnegie Mellon University Michael J. Tarr Brown University We argue that the current

More information

PS3019 Cognitive and Clinical Neuropsychology

PS3019 Cognitive and Clinical Neuropsychology PS3019 Cognitive and Clinical Neuropsychology Carlo De Lillo HWB Room P05 Consultation time: Tue & Thu 2.30-3.30 E-mail: CDL2@le.ac.uk Overview Neuropsychology of visual processing Neuropsychology of spatial

More information

Processing Faces and Facial Expressions

Processing Faces and Facial Expressions in press 2003. Neuropsychology Review, 13(3), *** *** Processing Faces and Facial Expressions Mette T. Posamentier The University of Texas of Dallas Hervé Abdi The University of Texas of Dallas This paper

More information

Developmental prosopagnosia: a window to content-specific face processing Bradley C Duchaine 1 and Ken Nakayama 2

Developmental prosopagnosia: a window to content-specific face processing Bradley C Duchaine 1 and Ken Nakayama 2 Developmental prosopagnosia: a window to content-specific face processing Bradley C Duchaine 1 and Ken Nakayama 2 Developmental prosopagnosia is characterized by severely impaired face recognition. Individuals

More information

Identify these objects

Identify these objects Pattern Recognition The Amazing Flexibility of Human PR. What is PR and What Problems does it Solve? Three Heuristic Distinctions for Understanding PR. Top-down vs. Bottom-up Processing. Semantic Priming.

More information

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH. DEGREE (if applicable)

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH. DEGREE (if applicable) NAME: Isabel Gauthier, Ph.D. OMB No. 0925-0001/0002 (Rev. 08/12 Approved Through 8/31/2015) BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH Provide the following information for the Senior/key personnel and other significant contributors.

More information

Face Perception - An Overview Bozana Meinhardt-Injac Malte Persike

Face Perception - An Overview Bozana Meinhardt-Injac Malte Persike Face Perception - An Overview Bozana Meinhardt-Injac Malte Persike Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz 2 Interesting to mention Bahrik, Bahrik & Wittlinger,1977 The identification and matching of once

More information

Virtual Brain Reading: A Connectionist Approach to Understanding fmri

Virtual Brain Reading: A Connectionist Approach to Understanding fmri Virtual Brain Reading: A Connectionist Approach to Understanding fmri Rosemary A. Cowell (r.a.cowell@kent.ac.uk) Computing Laboratory, University of Kent, Canterbury, CT2 7NF, UK David E. Huber (dhuber@psy.ucsd.edu)

More information

Summary. Multiple Body Representations 11/6/2016. Visual Processing of Bodies. The Body is:

Summary. Multiple Body Representations 11/6/2016. Visual Processing of Bodies. The Body is: Visual Processing of Bodies Corps et cognition: l'embodiment Corrado Corradi-Dell Acqua corrado.corradi@unige.ch Theory of Pain Laboratory Summary Visual Processing of Bodies Category-specificity in ventral

More information

Nature versus Nurture in Ventral Visual Cortex: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Twins

Nature versus Nurture in Ventral Visual Cortex: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Twins The Journal of Neuroscience, December 19, 2007 27(51):13921 13925 13921 Brief Communications Nature versus Nurture in Ventral Visual Cortex: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Twins Thad

More information

Domain specificity versus expertise: factors influencing distinct processing of faces

Domain specificity versus expertise: factors influencing distinct processing of faces D. Carmel, S. Bentin / Cognition 83 (2002) 1 29 1 COGNITION Cognition 83 (2002) 1 29 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Domain specificity versus expertise: factors influencing distinct processing of faces

More information

Bodies capture attention when nothing is expected

Bodies capture attention when nothing is expected Cognition 93 (2004) B27 B38 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article Bodies capture attention when nothing is expected Paul E. Downing*, David Bray, Jack Rogers, Claire Childs School of Psychology,

More information

THE EFFECT OF DIFFERENT TRAINING EXPERIENCES ON OBJECT RECOGNITION IN THE VISUAL SYSTEM. Alan Chun-Nang Wong

THE EFFECT OF DIFFERENT TRAINING EXPERIENCES ON OBJECT RECOGNITION IN THE VISUAL SYSTEM. Alan Chun-Nang Wong THE EFFECT OF DIFFERENT TRAINING EXPERIENCES ON OBJECT RECOGNITION IN THE VISUAL SYSTEM By Alan Chun-Nang Wong Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate School of Vanderbilt University In partial

More information

fmri: What Does It Measure?

fmri: What Does It Measure? fmri: What Does It Measure? Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 04/02/2018: Lecture 02-1 Note: This Powerpoint presentation may contain macros that I wrote to help me create

More information

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age Infant and Child Development Inf. Child Dev. 10: 39 45 (2001) DOI: 10.1002/icd.245 Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age Olivier Pascalis a, *, Elisabeth

More information

Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them

Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2008, 61 (4), 552 557 Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them Jeffrey S. Bowers and Keely W. Jones University of Bristol, Bristol,

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report Research Report CHANGING FACES: A Detection Advantage in the Flicker Paradigm Tony Ro, 1 Charlotte Russell, 2 and Nilli Lavie 2 1 Rice University and 2 University College London, London, United Kingdom

More information

Seeing faces in the noise: Stochastic activity in perceptual regions of the brain may influence the perception of ambiguous stimuli

Seeing faces in the noise: Stochastic activity in perceptual regions of the brain may influence the perception of ambiguous stimuli R175B BC JP To printer - need negs for pgs, 3,, and Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,?? (?),???-??? Seeing faces in the noise: Stochastic activity in perceptual regions of the brain may influence the perception

More information

THE FUNCTIONAL ORGANIZATION OF THE VENTRAL VISUAL PATHWAY. In Attention and Performance XX: Functional Brain Imaging of Visual Cognition.

THE FUNCTIONAL ORGANIZATION OF THE VENTRAL VISUAL PATHWAY. In Attention and Performance XX: Functional Brain Imaging of Visual Cognition. THE FUNCTIONAL ORGANIZATION OF THE VENTRAL VISUAL PATHWAY AND ITS RELATIONSHIP TO OBJECT RECOGNITION. Kalanit Grill-Spector Stanford University, Stanford CA 94305 In Attention and Performance XX: Functional

More information

Translating From Perceptual to Cognitive Coding

Translating From Perceptual to Cognitive Coding Translating From Perceptual to Cognitive Coding Tyler Davis (thdavis@mail.utexas.edu) Bradley C. Love (brad_love@mail.utexas.edu) W. Todd Maddox (maddox@psy.utexas.edu) Department of Psychology, The University

More information

IS FACE RECOGNITION NOT SO UNIQUE

IS FACE RECOGNITION NOT SO UNIQUE COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2000, 17 (1/2/3), 125 142 IS FACE RECOGNITION NOT SO UNIQUE AFTER ALL? Isabel Gauthier Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA Nikos K.

More information

Linking Brain Response and Behavior to Reveal Top-Down. (and Inside-Out) Influences on Processing in Face Perception. Heather A. Wild. Thomas A.

Linking Brain Response and Behavior to Reveal Top-Down. (and Inside-Out) Influences on Processing in Face Perception. Heather A. Wild. Thomas A. Linking Brain Response and Behavior to Reveal Top-Down (and Inside-Out) Influences on Processing in Face Perception Heather A. Wild Thomas A. Busey Department of Psychology Indiana University Bloomington,

More information

A Biologically Plausible Approach to Cat and Dog Discrimination

A Biologically Plausible Approach to Cat and Dog Discrimination A Biologically Plausible Approach to Cat and Dog Discrimination Bruce A. Draper, Kyungim Baek, Jeff Boody Department of Computer Science Colorado State University Fort Collins, CO 80523-1873 U.S.A. draper,beak,boody@cs.colostate.edu

More information

A Biologically Plausible Approach to Cat and Dog Discrimination

A Biologically Plausible Approach to Cat and Dog Discrimination A Biologically Plausible Approach to Cat and Dog Discrimination Bruce A. Draper, Kyungim Baek, Jeff Boody Department of Computer Science Colorado State University Fort Collins, CO 80523-1873 U.S.A. draper,beak,boody@cs.colostate.edu

More information

A picture is worth thousands of trials: rendering the use of visual information from spiking neurons to recognition

A picture is worth thousands of trials: rendering the use of visual information from spiking neurons to recognition Cognitive Science 28 (2004) 141 146 A picture is worth thousands of trials: rendering the use of visual information from spiking neurons to recognition Frédéric Gosselin a,, Philippe G. Schyns b a Département

More information

Experience Matters: Modeling the Relationship Between Face and Object Recognition

Experience Matters: Modeling the Relationship Between Face and Object Recognition Experience Matters: Modeling the Relationship Between Face and Object Recognition Panqu Wang (pawang@ucsd.edu) Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of California San Diego 9500

More information

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544 Frank Tong Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544 Office: Room 3-N-2B Telephone: 609-258-2652 Fax: 609-258-1113 Email: ftong@princeton.edu Graduate School Applicants

More information

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: A Comparison of Electrophysiological and Other Neuroimaging Approaches Leun J. Otten Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology University

More information

Cognition xx (0000) xxx xxx. Brief article. A holistic account of the own-race effect in face recognition: evidence from a cross-cultural study

Cognition xx (0000) xxx xxx. Brief article. A holistic account of the own-race effect in face recognition: evidence from a cross-cultural study 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 Brief article A holistic account of the own-race effect in face recognition:

More information

Expertise in object and face recognition. James Tanaka Oberlin College. Isabel Gauthier Yale University

Expertise in object and face recognition. James Tanaka Oberlin College. Isabel Gauthier Yale University 1 Expertise in object and face recognition James Tanaka Oberlin College Isabel Gauthier Yale University in Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Mechanisms of Perceptual Learning (eds. Goldstone, Schyns

More information

Human Cognitive Developmental Neuroscience. Jan 27

Human Cognitive Developmental Neuroscience. Jan 27 Human Cognitive Developmental Neuroscience Jan 27 Wiki Definition Developmental cognitive neuroscience is an interdisciplinary scientific field that is situated at the boundaries of Neuroscience Psychology

More information

The Training and Transfer of Real-World Perceptual Expertise

The Training and Transfer of Real-World Perceptual Expertise PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article The and Transfer of Real-World Perceptual Expertise 1 2 3 James W. Tanaka, Tim Curran, and David L. Sheinberg 1 Department of Psychology, University of Victoria,

More information

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY Thomas Lachmann *, Gunjan Khera * and Cees van Leeuwen # * Psychology II, University of Kaiserslautern, Kaiserslautern, Germany # Laboratory

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Early lateralization and orientation tuning for face, word, and object processing in the visual cortex

Early lateralization and orientation tuning for face, word, and object processing in the visual cortex NeuroImage 20 (2003) 1609 1624 www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Early lateralization and orientation tuning for face, word, and object processing in the visual cortex Bruno Rossion, a,b, * Carrie A. Joyce,

More information

The Central Nervous System

The Central Nervous System The Central Nervous System Cellular Basis. Neural Communication. Major Structures. Principles & Methods. Principles of Neural Organization Big Question #1: Representation. How is the external world coded

More information

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention Goldsmiths Research Online. The Authors. Originally published: Science vol.291 2 March 2001 1803-1806. http://www.sciencemag.org. 11 October 2000; accepted 17 January 2001 The Role of Working Memory in

More information

Face-selective Activation in a Congenital Prosopagnosic Subject

Face-selective Activation in a Congenital Prosopagnosic Subject Face-selective Activation in a Congenital Prosopagnosic Subject Uri Hasson 1, Galia Avidan 1,2, Leon Y. Deouell 3, Shlomo Bentin 2,4, and Rafael Malach 1 Abstract & Congenital prosopagnosia is a severe

More information

RUNNNING HEAD: Pigeons Attend to Features of Human Faces. Pigeons and People Categorize Human Faces Using Similar Features

RUNNNING HEAD: Pigeons Attend to Features of Human Faces. Pigeons and People Categorize Human Faces Using Similar Features RUNNNING HEAD: Pigeons Attend to Features of Human Faces Pigeons and People Categorize Human Faces Using Similar Features Brett M. Gibson *, Edward A. Wasserman *, Frédéric Gosselin, and Philippe G. Schyns

More information

Separate Face and Body Selectivity on the Fusiform Gyrus

Separate Face and Body Selectivity on the Fusiform Gyrus The Journal of Neuroscience, November 23, 2005 25(47):11055 11059 11055 Brief Communication Separate Face and Body Selectivity on the Fusiform Gyrus Rebecca F. Schwarzlose, 1,2 Chris I. Baker, 1,2 and

More information

Word Count: Please address correspondence to: James Tanaka, Department of Psychology, University

Word Count: Please address correspondence to: James Tanaka, Department of Psychology, University 1 The training and transfer of real-world, perceptual expertise James W. Tanaka 1, Tim Curran 2 and David Sheinberg 3 1 Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, British Columbia, Canada 2 Department

More information

Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions! 131

Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions! 131 ! Chapter 5 Summary and Conclusions! 131 Chapter 5!!!! Summary of the main findings The present thesis investigated the sensory representation of natural sounds in the human auditory cortex. Specifically,

More information

Chapter 7: First steps into inferior temporal cortex

Chapter 7: First steps into inferior temporal cortex BEWARE: These are preliminary notes. In the future, they will become part of a textbook on Visual Object Recognition. Chapter 7: First steps into inferior temporal cortex Inferior temporal cortex (ITC)

More information

The neural basis of object perception Kalanit Grill-Spector

The neural basis of object perception Kalanit Grill-Spector 59 The neural basis of object perception Kalanit Grill-Spector Humans can recognize an object within a fraction of a second, even if there are no clues about what kind of object it might be. Recent findings

More information

Implementing the Expert Object Recognition Pathway

Implementing the Expert Object Recognition Pathway Implementing the Expert Object Recognition Pathway Bruce A. Draper, Kyungim Baek and Jeff Boody Department of Computer Science Colorado State University Fort Collins, CO, 80523, U.S.A. draper,baek,boody@cs.colostate.edu

More information

11, in nonhuman primates, different cell populations respond to facial identity and expression12, and

11, in nonhuman primates, different cell populations respond to facial identity and expression12, and UNDERSTANDING THE RECOGNITION OF FACIAL IDENTITY AND FACIAL EXPRESSION Andrew J. Calder* and Andrew W. Young Abstract Faces convey a wealth of social signals. A dominant view in face-perception research

More information

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J.

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J. The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem By Andrew Laguna, S.J. Outline I. Introduction II. The Visual System III. What is the Binding Problem? IV. Possible Theoretical Solutions

More information

Taking control of reflexive social attention

Taking control of reflexive social attention Cognition 94 (2005) B55 B65 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article Taking control of reflexive social attention Jelena Ristic*, Alan Kingstone Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia,

More information

Semantic Memory. Learning & Memory

Semantic Memory. Learning & Memory Semantic Memory Learning & Memory Semantic Memory General knowledge about the world, not linked to any time or context Some examples: What is the capital of North Dakota? Bismarck What is the population

More information

Differences of Face and Object Recognition in Utilizing Early Visual Information

Differences of Face and Object Recognition in Utilizing Early Visual Information Differences of Face and Object Recognition in Utilizing Early Visual Information Peter Kalocsai and Irving Biederman Department of Psychology and Computer Science University of Southern California Los

More information

Congenital prosopagnosia: faceblind

Congenital prosopagnosia: faceblind Congenital prosopagnosia: faceblind from birth Marlene Behrmann and Galia Avidan Department of Psychology and Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh PA 15213-3890,

More information

Behavioral Change and Its Neural Correlates in Visual Agnosia After Expertise Training

Behavioral Change and Its Neural Correlates in Visual Agnosia After Expertise Training Behavioral Change and Its Neural Correlates in Visual Agnosia After Expertise Training Marlene Behrmann 1, Jonathan Marotta 2, Isabel Gauthier 3, Michael J. Tarr 4, and Thomas J. McKeeff 5 Abstract & Agnosia,

More information

EDGE DETECTION. Edge Detectors. ICS 280: Visual Perception

EDGE DETECTION. Edge Detectors. ICS 280: Visual Perception EDGE DETECTION Edge Detectors Slide 2 Convolution & Feature Detection Slide 3 Finds the slope First derivative Direction dependent Need many edge detectors for all orientation Second order derivatives

More information

Object detection and basic-level categorization: Sometimes you know it is there before you know what it is

Object detection and basic-level categorization: Sometimes you know it is there before you know what it is R243B Time-Course of Object Detection and Categorization 1 PBR-BR-07-031.R2 Production Number R243B Object detection and basic-level categorization: Sometimes you know it is there before you know what

More information

Learning to see faces and objects

Learning to see faces and objects Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.7 No.1 January 2003 23 Learning to see faces and objects Michael J. Tarr and Yi D. Cheng Cognitive and Linguistic Sciences, Brown University, Box 1978, Providence,

More information

COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE HOW TO STUDY MORE EFFECTIVELY (P 187-189) Elaborate Think about the meaning of the information that you are learning Relate to what you already know Associate: link information together Generate and test

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Neural codes PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 12. COC illusion

Neural codes PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 12. COC illusion Neural codes PSY 310 Greg Francis Lecture 12 Is 100 billion neurons enough? COC illusion The COC illusion looks like real squares because the neural responses are similar True squares COC squares Ganglion

More information

Vision Research 51 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Vision Research 51 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage: Vision Research 51 (2011) 195 202 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Differences between perception of human faces and body shapes:

More information

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Selective Attention Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form,

More information

fmr-adaptation reveals a distributed representation of inanimate objects and places in human visual cortex

fmr-adaptation reveals a distributed representation of inanimate objects and places in human visual cortex www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage 28 (2005) 268 279 fmr-adaptation reveals a distributed representation of inanimate objects and places in human visual cortex Michael P. Ewbank, a Denis Schluppeck,

More information

Configural, holistic, and coordinate processing: The same or different?

Configural, holistic, and coordinate processing: The same or different? Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate College 2012 Configural, holistic, and coordinate processing: The same or different? Jonathan Taylor Kahl Iowa State University Follow this and additional works

More information

Contrast polarity and face recognition in the human fusiform gyrus

Contrast polarity and face recognition in the human fusiform gyrus Contrast polarity and face recognition in the human fusiform gyrus Nathalie George 1, Raymond J. Dolan 1, Gereon R. Fink 2, Gordon C. Baylis 3, Charlotte Russell 4 and Jon Driver 4 1 Wellcome Department

More information

DISORDERS OF PATTERN RECOGNITION

DISORDERS OF PATTERN RECOGNITION DISORDERS OF PATTERN RECOGNITION A. Visual agnosia inability to identify objects by sight Types (1) apperceptive agnosia unable to form stable [presemantic] representations of objects (2) associative agnosia

More information

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions Sarah J. Miles (smiles25@uwo.ca) and John Paul Minda (jpminda@uwo.ca) Department of Psychology The University of Western Ontario London, ON N6A 5C2 Abstract

More information

ASSAF HAREL, PHD CURRICULUM VITAE Department of Psychology Wright State University

ASSAF HAREL, PHD CURRICULUM VITAE Department of Psychology Wright State University Assaf Harel, August 2014 1 Contact Details 437 Fawcett Hall 3640 Colonel Glenn Highway Dayton, Ohio 45435 Email: assaf.harel@wright.edu Tel: (937) 775-3819 Cell: (240) 938-4020 ASSAF HAREL, PHD CURRICULUM

More information

Perceptual Disorders. Agnosias

Perceptual Disorders. Agnosias Perceptual Disorders Agnosias Disorders of Object Recognition AGNOSIA : a general term for a loss of ability to recognize objects, people, sounds, shapes, or smells. Agnosias result from damage to cortical

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception 1 Virginia R. de Sa Department of Cognitive Science UCSD Lecture 6: Beyond V1 - Extrastriate cortex Chapter 4 Course Information 2 Class web page: http://cogsci.ucsd.edu/

More information

Dissociation of Category-Learning Mechanisms via Brain Potentials

Dissociation of Category-Learning Mechanisms via Brain Potentials Dissociation of Category-Learning Mechanisms via rain Potentials Robert G. Morrison (rmorrison@luc.edu) Loyola University Chicago, Psychology Department, 6525 North Sheridan Road Chicago, IL 60626 USA

More information

PART 4. Social Processes

PART 4. Social Processes PART 4 Social Processes 1 c43.indd 1 2/25/9 5:59:41 PM c43.indd 2 2/25/9 5:59:42 PM 43 Face Perception N ANCY K ANWISHER AND G ALIT Y OVEL For highly social organisms like us, faces reign supreme among

More information

Chapter 10 Importance of Visual Cues in Hearing Restoration by Auditory Prosthesis

Chapter 10 Importance of Visual Cues in Hearing Restoration by Auditory Prosthesis Chapter 1 Importance of Visual Cues in Hearing Restoration by uditory Prosthesis Tetsuaki Kawase, Yoko Hori, Takenori Ogawa, Shuichi Sakamoto, Yôiti Suzuki, and Yukio Katori bstract uditory prostheses,

More information

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Journal: Psychological Science Manuscript ID: PSCI-0-0.R Manuscript Type: Short report Date Submitted by the Author:

More information

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems Chapter 3: 2 visual systems Overview Explain the significance of the turn to the brain in cognitive science Explain Mishkin and Ungerleider s hypothesis that there are two distinct visual systems Outline

More information

Individuating faces and common objects produces equal responses in putative face-processing areas in the ventral occipitotemporal cortex

Individuating faces and common objects produces equal responses in putative face-processing areas in the ventral occipitotemporal cortex HUMAN NEUROSCIENCE Original Research Article published: 08 October 2010 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00181 Individuating faces and common objects produces equal responses in putative face-processing areas in

More information

Perceptual Expertise Effects Are Not All or None: Spatially Limited Perceptual Expertise for Faces in a Case of Prosopagnosia

Perceptual Expertise Effects Are Not All or None: Spatially Limited Perceptual Expertise for Faces in a Case of Prosopagnosia Perceptual Expertise Effects Are Not All or None: Spatially Limited Perceptual Expertise for Faces in a Case of Prosopagnosia Cindy M. Bukach 1, Daniel N. Bub 2, Isabel Gauthier 1, and Michael J. Tarr

More information

Phil 490: Consciousness and the Self Handout [16] Jesse Prinz: Mental Pointing Phenomenal Knowledge Without Concepts

Phil 490: Consciousness and the Self Handout [16] Jesse Prinz: Mental Pointing Phenomenal Knowledge Without Concepts Phil 490: Consciousness and the Self Handout [16] Jesse Prinz: Mental Pointing Phenomenal Knowledge Without Concepts Main Goals of this Paper: Professor JeeLoo Liu 1. To present an account of phenomenal

More information

THE ENCODING OF PARTS AND WHOLES

THE ENCODING OF PARTS AND WHOLES THE ENCODING OF PARTS AND WHOLES IN THE VISUAL CORTICAL HIERARCHY JOHAN WAGEMANS LABORATORY OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY UNIVERSITY OF LEUVEN, BELGIUM DIPARTIMENTO DI PSICOLOGIA, UNIVERSITÀ DI MILANO-BICOCCA,

More information

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception Paul E. Hemeren (paul.hemeren@his.se) University of Skövde, School of Humanities and Informatics

More information

MSc Neuroimaging for Clinical & Cognitive Neuroscience

MSc Neuroimaging for Clinical & Cognitive Neuroscience MSc Neuroimaging for Clinical & Cognitive Neuroscience School of Psychological Sciences Faculty of Medical & Human Sciences Module Information *Please note that this is a sample guide to modules. The exact

More information

The fusiform face area is tuned for curvilinear patterns with more high-contrasted elements in the upper part

The fusiform face area is tuned for curvilinear patterns with more high-contrasted elements in the upper part www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage 31 (2006) 313 319 The fusiform face area is tuned for curvilinear patterns with more high-contrasted elements in the upper part Roberto Caldara, a, * Mohamed L.

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report Research Report SEMANTIC CATEGORIZATION IN THE HUMAN BRAIN: Spatiotemporal Dynamics Revealed by Magnetoencephalography Andreas Löw, 1 Shlomo Bentin, 2 Brigitte Rockstroh, 1 Yaron Silberman, 2 Annette Gomolla,

More information