Cooperative tapping: Time control under different feedback conditions

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Cooperative tapping: Time control under different feedback conditions"

Transcription

1 Perception & Psychophysics / (6), (1)/-74 Cooperative tapping: Time control under different feedback conditions JIRi MATES Ludwig-Maximilians University, Munich, Germany and Czechoslovak Academy ofsciences, Prague, Czechoslovakia ToMAs RADIL Czechoslovak Academy ofsciences, Prague, Czechoslovakia and ERNST POPPEL Ludwig-Maximilians University, Munich, Germany The same isochronous tone sequence was presented simultaneously to two mutually isolated subjects. In half the trials, accentuation in this sequence was accomplished by doubling the duration of the first and then of every fourth tone; in the other half, by doubling the frequency of those tones. The subjects' task was to follow the rhythm of the resulting four-tone patterns by finger tapping to tone onsets. There were four auditory feedback (FB) conditions: (1) no FB; (2) FB from the subject's own motor responses; (3) "alien" FB from the motor responses of the other pair member who, in turn, was listening to FB from his/her own tapping; (4) mutually "crossed" FB, where each pair member listened to FB from the tapping of the other. Tap onsets regularly preceded stimulus onsets. The observed order of the amount of this anticipation (from least to greatest) was: (1) own FB, (2) no FB, (3) alien FB, and (4) crossed FB. No mutual dynamic influence between simultaneously performing subjects was detected. Anticipation was more pronounced for sequences that were accentuated by frequency rather than by duration changes. The type of accent also influenced timing of intertap intervals in the rhythmic patterns. For the frequency accent, regular timing was produced, whereas for the durational accent, shortening of the second and lengthening of the fourth (the last) intertap interval were observed. The presence and source of feedback as well as the character of accentuation are therefore relevant factors in the timing of auditorally controlled rhythmic motor behavior. There are several possible internal sources of random error in the precise timing ofrepetitive movements, such as fluctuations in sensory transduction, variance in the motor system, and so forth (see, e.g., Vorberg & Hambuch, 1984; Vos & Ellermann, 1989; Wing, Keele, & Margolin, 1984; Wing & Kristofferson, 1973). The same sources also might be responsible for random and systematic errors indicating the limits in precision and accuracy of the underlying processes responsible for timing in sensorimotor synchronization. Subjective timing in synchronization of simple motor responses with repetitive sequences of stimuli has been studied by many authors over a long This experiment was performed within the framework of cooperation between the Institute of Medical Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians University of Munich, and the Institute of Physiology, Czechoslovak Academy of Sciences, Prague. The research was supported by Grant PO of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft. The text was revised during the period in which the first author's research work was supported by a fellowship from the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation, Germany. Correspondence should be addressed to J. Mates, Instituteof Medical Psychology, Ludwig-MaxirniliansUniversity, Goethestrasse 31, -8 Munich 2, Germany. period of time (see reviews by Fraisse, 1978, 1982). The synchronization of finger tapping with sequences of acoustical stimuli, for example, can be conceived of as a simple model ofsynchronization in music performance (see, e.g., Clynes & Walker, 1982; Fraisse, 1978, 1982; Franek, Mates, Radil, Beck, & Poppel, 1991a, 1991b; Franek et al., 1989). Many external factors may also influence the accuracy of timing when motor acts are used to follow or reproduce repetitive stimulus sequences; these factors include the modality of stimuli (Kolers & Brewster, 1985; Najenson, Ron, & Behroozi, 1989), the length of interstimulus intervals (Fraisse, 1978, 1982; Peters, 1989; Poppe I, Milller, & Mates, 199), the lateralizationofstimuli (Ilmberger, Muller, Poppel, Mates, & Radil, 199), or the rhythmic structure of the stimulus sequence (Franek et al., 1991b). The rhythmic structure of a stimulus sequence can be obtained by means of accentuation of some stimuli by differentiating their specific physical aspects, since accentuation is one ofthe factors ofgrouping stimuli into temporal, or rhythmic, patterns (Fraisse, 1978). It is probable that feedback on movement execution might be a further factor influencing stimulus-driven 691 Copyright 1992 Psychonomic Society, Inc.

2 692 MATES, RADIL, AND POPPEL repetitive movements. The influence offeedback on performance in a synchronization task has been investigated by Kolers and Brewster (1985), who used visual stimuli and visual feedback from motor responses. The variability of performance was larger than it was in tasks without additional feedback, but the mean values were uninfluenced. Kolers and Brewster assumed that different timing mechanisms operated in different sensory modalities, so it is difficult to determine from their results what would occur if both the stimuli and the feedback were in the auditory modality. Fraisse (1982) reported lower accuracy of foot-tapping in comparison with finger-tapping; because a different motor effector was used, different proprioceptive feedback was involved. The results of other experiments demonstrate that increased proprioceptive feedback improves the anticipatory timing of motor responses (Adams & Creamer, 1962; Schmidt & Christina, 1969). All these experimental results support the assumption about the role of feedback information. In the present study, we examined the effect of social feedback based on cross-linking of the motor and sensory systems of two individuals (Knight, 1987; Rosenberg & Hall, 1958; Sauter & K. U. Smith, 1971; Smelser, 1961; K. U. Smith & Arndt, 1969; K. U. Smith & Kao, 1971; T. J. Smith & K. U. Smith, 1987a, 1987b; Wagner & Zeaman, 1956). This is achieved by creating single or double feedback loops so that the movements of 1 subject constitute the source of sensory feedback to a 2nd subject and vice versa. Specifically, we examined the influence ofthe auditory feedback signal derived from the motor performance of 1 subject on another one, both of whom were working on the same task. Additionally, the effect oftheir mutual interaction was investigated under a crossed feedback condition. As a control condition, the individual performance of the subject was tested in isolation from any additional signal, as well as with additional auditory feedback from his/her own performance. Systematic distortions of the temporal relationships between stimulus and response sequences typical for individual performance are mostly reflected in stimulus anticipation (see, e.g., Hary & Moore, 1985; Kolers & Brewster, 1985; Radii, Mates, 11mberger, & Poppel, 199b). The aim of this experiment was therefore to analyze whether or not these systematic distortions might be compensated by means offeedback derived from another subject, whether crossed feedback would time-lock both subjects, and whether the presence or absence of an additional external feedback loop would influence the timing of individual performance. Thus the same stimulus sequences were presented simultaneously to 2 subjects sitting in separate rooms, their common task being to follow the stimuli by finger tapping under four different conditions. Either there was no additional auditory feedback available to the subjects at all, or the subjects were provided with additional external auditory feedback derived from their own motor responses, from the responses of the other subject, who, in tum, was provided with his or her own feedback, or the feedback was crossed between the subjects. Mutual stimulus and response timing was analyzed under these conditions. METHOD Stimuli Isochronous sequences of 22 tones were used. The first and every fourth tone in a stimulus sequence were accentuated either by their being of longer duration (D sequence) or by their having a higher frequency (F sequence) in comparison with the remaining tone pulses (see Figure I). This accentuation perceptually chunked the sequence into four-tone rhythmic patterns (Fraisse, 1978), so that each sequence comprised 55 equal patterns. The duration and the frequency of each nonaccentuated tone (the second, third, and fourth tone of each pattern) were 1 msec and 262 Hz, respectively. The accentuated tones (the first tone of each pattern) were either twice as long (2 rnsec) or double the frequency (524 Hz) of the remaining tones in the patterns. Identical patterns followed one after the other continuously. The temporal intervals from onset to onset of tones inside the patterns (the interstimulus intervals), as well as the interpattern intervals, were always 5 msec. It was found in previous experiments that synchronization is most regular and the variance of interresponse intervals length minimal if the tapping frequency falls into the range of interval durations between approximately 2 and 1, rnsec (e.g., Fraisse, 1982). Given the interstimulus interval above, one stimulus sequence of 22 tones lasted 11 sec. Procedure An identical stimulus sequence was presented simultaneously to a pair of subjects sitting in two acoustically shielded chambers. The subjects had no direct knowledge of each other's performance. The subject's task was to synchronize the tapping of his or her index finger on a response key with the onset of the stimuli according to the rhythm of the stimulus sequence. The subjects were asked not to let the duration of stimulus tones affect the duration of their taps. At the beginning of each sequence, the subjects had to listen without responding to the first three repetitions of the four-tone pattern, so that they could become familiar with the presented rhythm. Fraisse (1982) has reported that synchronization with repetitive patterns is usually established from the third pattern on. Thus, from D-SEQUENCE F-SEQUENCE 262Hz 262Hz 262Hz 262Hz ~ n n n 2 ms 1ms 1ms 1ms I SOOms SOOms SOOms SOOms I S23Hz 262Hz 262Hz 262Hz JI n n n 1ms 1ms looms looms I SSx SS x Figure 1. Schematic representation of tbe two rbythmic stimulus sequences. The rhythmic structure was created by accentuation of the first and tben of every fourth tone in a sequence: (1) using tone-duration difference (D sequence); (2) using tone-frequency difference (F sequence).

3 Table 1 Five Experimental Conditions as Sources for Four Data Analysis Groups Experimental Condition No feedback Own feedback Subject I feedback Subject 2 feedback Feedback Feedback Source Receiver Data Analysis (Subject) (Subject) Group none I no feedback none 2 no feedback I I own feedback 2 2 own feedback I 1 own feedback 1 2 alien feedback 2 1 alien feedback 2 2 own feedback Crossed feedback 2 I crossed feedback 1 2 crossed feedback Note-Eighteen individual subjects performed both types of stimulus sequences (D and F sequences; see Figure 1) in each of 5 conditions, as membersof one of 9 pairs of subjects. COOPERATIVE TAPPING 693 one stimulus sequence, we usually were able to record about 52 response patterns. A short training sequence of 2 pattern repetitions (4 sec) preceded the experiment proper. Each member of a subject pair was instructed separately; the subjects were not informed of the experimental hypotheses. For every tap in the sequence, the following three temporal intervals were measured and analyzed: tap duration, intertap interval (i.e., the temporal difference between successive tap onsets), and synchronization error (i.e., the temporal difference between the stimulus onset and the corresponding tap onset). Ifthe tap onset precedes ("anticipates") the stimulus onset in time, the synchronization error is called the anticipation interval and it is represented by a negative value of the synchronization error. Positive values of the synchronization error are connected with delayed taps. We prefer this definition, in accordance with the definition used by Hary and Moore (1985) for the identical quantity named "error intervals, " since it expresses the orientation in time of tap onset with respect to stimulus onset better. The "response time" defined by Najenson et al. (1989) also indicates the same quantity, but it bears negative values in case of delayed responses. Concurrently with the stimuli, an acoustical feedback signal was supplied to each subject. The feedback signal consisted of tones separated by pauses, with the feedback tones being triggered by tap onsets and the duration of feedback tones corresponding to tap durations. There were five different combinations of feedback signals for each pair of subjects (see Table I). It should be noticed, however, that in all feedback conditions (i.e., even in the nofeedback condition), the tactile-kinaesthetic feedback information from the tapping responses was present. Thus, the synchronization could always be judged on the basis of this information. The order of the experimental conditions was randomized among subject pairs. In each experimental condition, both the D sequence and the F sequence were used, also in a random order. Therefore, IOdifferent trials were presented altogether. Each subject pair performed all IO trials in one session, with the duration of a session being approximately 4 min. Apparatus The stimulus tone sequence was controlledand the responses were monitored by a special program on an ffim-pc-compatible computer (Mates, 199). The temporal resolution of interval measurement was 25 p.sec. The stimulus sequences were generated with the internal sound generator of the computer, producing a rectangular signal of a given frequency, and they were presented to the subjects over headphones. The signal was amplified to a comfortable level for each subject individually and then kept constant throughout the experiment. The acoustical feedback signals were produced by an external tone generator triggered directly by the response keys. The frequency of the feedback tones was always equal to 2 Hz, a frequency substantially higher than that of the stimulus itself. The feedback signal was supplied to each subject by means of a loudspeaker, which was positioned at a distance of approximately I m from the subject. The response keys themselves produced no sound and were identical to those commonly used in computer keyboards; this eliminated possible false response detection, which can result from mechanical switch bouncing. Subjects Nine paid subject pairs (i.e., 18 subjects) participated in the experiment (6 females, 12 males, from 26 to 55 years old). Sex and age were not taken into consideration when the pairs were formed. RESULTS From every individual response sequence, the first and the last pattern as well as incidental incomplete response patterns were removed before data processing. If an invalid pattern was removed from the response sequence of one member of a pair, the pattern ofthe same sequential order was also removed from the response sequence of the other member of the pair. The sequential correspondence of responses of both pair members was thus preserved. All response sequences ofeach pair recorded in the 1 trials were adjusted to the minimal common length by removing a necessary number of response patterns from the beginnings of longer sequences. Thus the statistics from all experimental conditions for both subjects of a pair were based on the same number of measurements. The numbers of response patterns used for the nine subject pairs were 48,43,45,45,46,37,45,49, and 47, respectively. The data were combined for evaluation into four groups, depending on the type of auditory feedback presented (see Table 1). The four groups were: no feedback (NFB), own feedback (OFB), alien feedback (AFB), and crossed feed-

4 694 MATES, RADIL, AND POPPEL Table 2 Degrees of Freedom, F Ratios and Probability Levels of Within-Subject Effects in Repeated Measures Analyses of Variance of Intertap Intervals (lti), Tap Durations (TD), and Synchronization Errors (SE) ITI TD SE Effect df F p* F P F P ST 1, FC 3, <.1 TP 3, < ST x FC 3, ST x TP 3, < FC x TP 9, ST x FC x TP 9, Note-ST, sequence type; FC, feedback condition; TP, tap position. probability. *Greenhouse-Geisser's adjusted back (CFB). Because of the likely within-trial correlations between the successive intervals measured in a stream of repetitive responses, the trial means were used in the statistical analyses. Such a correlation may result from an error compensation mechanism or from the properties of repetitive responding that follow from the timing model developed by Wing and Kristofferson (1973; see also Mates, 1991; Vos & Ellermann, 1989). Correlations between successive temporal parameters of a sequence of repetitive responses have already been observed in earlier experimental data (see, e.g., Franek.et al., 1991b; Kolers & Brewster, 1985; Peters, 1989; Vos & Ellermann, 1989). Intertap Intervals When the data were pooled for all subjects and all taps in the patterns, there was remarkably little variation in the duration of intertap intervals corresponding to interstimulus intervals (always 5 msec). The intraindividual standard deviations of intertap intervals varied in the range from 15 to 5 msec. Because a synchronization tapping task was used, the total mean value of intertap intervals equaled the duration of the interstimulus interval; otherwise there would have been a cumulative increase of synchronization errors. Detailed analysis, which took into account the sequential positionof taps in the patterns, was done with a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). The type of experimental sequence (D or F; df = 1), type of feedback condition (NFB, OFB, AFB, CFB; df = 3) and position ofthe tap (T) in the patterns (T1-T4; df = 3) were selected as trial (within-subject) factors, with subjects' mean intertap intervals computed over each trial as replicates (df = 17). The hypothesis ofhomogeneity of withincell variances, on the basis of Bartlett'stest, was rejected [x 2(31) = 77.53, P <.1], so the Greenhouse-Geisser's adjusted probabilities were used to judge the significance levels. The results of the ANOVA are presented in Table 2. There were no significant main effects. The only significant interaction occurred between sequence type and tap position in the patterns. The total mean values of intertap intervals grouped according to the detected differences are displayed in Figure U 55 OJ <J).E:. Ci3 > m c: 5 Q 2 I m :s To preserve a one-to-one correspondence of measures by one subject from different levels of within-subject (trial) factors, single-degree-of-freedom contrasts, instead of any multiple comparison test (Tukey, etc.), were used for all post hoc comparisons. Post hoc analysis of withinsubject differences showed no significant effect of tap position inside the patterns for the D sequence (p >.5) or for the F sequence (p >.5). Within-subject differences between the two sequence types were significant for the second and the fourth taps in the patterns [F(l,17) = 14.76forT2and13.51forT4,p <.1] and nonsignificant for the first and third taps (p >.5). Further analysis showed (see Figure 2) that the second intertap interval produced in the D sequence was signifir- ---} T J T2 Tap position ~- -- Figure 2. Total average values with between-trial-blocks standard errors of the mean of intertap intervals, plotted for both stimulus sequences (empty bars, D sequence; rjlled bars, F sequence) and for the four consecutive tap positions in the rhythmic patterns (Tl to T4) separately. Subjects' trial means were pooled over all feedback conditions, which revealed no significant effect on duration of intertap intervals. The dashed line represents the interstimulus interval (5 msec), T3 T4

5 COOPERATIVE TAPPING 695 cantly shorter than that produced in the F sequence, and also that it was shorter than the duration of the interstimu Ius interval (5 msec). The mean duration of the second intertap interval (495.4 msec) was significantly less than 5 msec [one-sample 1 test; 1(17) = 2.35, p <.5], being the (nonsignificantly) shortest in the patterns. This shortening was compensated mostly by lengthening ofthe fourth (i.e., the last) intertap interval in the patterns, whose mean for the D sequence (54.1 msec) was significantly longer than the mean for the F sequence and (nonsignificantly) longer than 5 msec [one-sample 1 test; 1(17) = 1.88, p >.5]. No significant lengthening of the first intertap interval "filled" in the D sequence with the accentuated double-lasting tone could be observed in comparison with its required duration. No significant deviations of intertap intervals from the duration of the interstimu Ius interval were found inside the patterns for the sequence accentuated by the higher frequency of the first tone in the patterns (F sequence). The individual results, pooled according to the significant effects observed in the preceding analysis, are shown in Figure 3. Since there were no effects of the feedback condition and since different individual strategies oftiming of intertap intervals together with the influence oftype of accentuation on them have already been investigated (Franek et ai., 1991b), no other tests on interindividual differences were performed. Tap Durations To investigate possible factors influencing these intervals, a repeated measures ANOV A with the same factorial design as that used for the intertap intervals was performed. On the basis of a Bartlett's test, the hypothesis ofoverall within-cell homogeneity of variances was accepted b: 2 (31) = 5.46, p >.5]. The results of the ANOVA are presented in Table 2. The main effect oftap position in the patterns and the interaction between sequence type and tap position were significant. The total mean values of tap durations grouped according to the detected differences are displayed in Figure 4. Post hoc within-subject comparisons showed a gradual decrease oftap duration in the patterns. The values ofthe F ratio and corresponding probability levels are shown in Table 3. There were no within-subject differences between the two types of sequences for any tap position in the patterns [F(1, 17), p >.5]. Significant within-subject differences with respect to the position of tap inside the patterns were found for the D sequence [F(3,51) = 12.68, P <.1] as well as for the F sequence [F(3,51) = 3.96, p <.5]. The relationship between tap durations inside the patterns for the D sequence was the same as it was for the global data. For the F sequence, the duration of only the fourth tap was significantly different (shorter) than the duration of the other taps in the patterns. The values ofthe F ratio and corresponding probability levels of the preceding two tests are also shown in Table 3. The individual results, pooled according to the significant effects observed in the preceding analysis, are shown in Figure 5. Since there were no effects of the feedback condition, and since interindividual differences in tap durations as a consequence of different musical skill have already been studied (Franek et al., 1991a), no other tests on interindividual differences were performed. Synchronization Errors The influence ofthe various feedback conditions on the synchronization errors (i.e., the temporal differences between stimulus onsets and corresponding tap onsets) was analyzed in a repeated measures ANOVA with the same factorial design as that for intertap intervals, in order to find out more about the variables influencing these intervals. The hypothesis of overall within-eell homogeneity of variances was retained on the basis of a Bartlett's test [x 2 (31) = 35.47,p >.5]. The results of the ANOVA are presented in Table 2. A significant main effect was found for both sequence type and feedback condition. The interaction between sequence type and tap position was also significant. The total mean values of synchronization errors, grouped according to the detected differences, are displayed in Figure 6. Comparison of the two types of stimulus sequences showed that anticipation intervals were longer for the accentuation by frequency of the first tone in the patterns (F sequence; M = -69 msec) than for the accentuation by duration (D sequence; M = -59 msec)(see Figure 6). Post hoc within-subject comparison of the influence of different feedback conditions showed that feedback conditions AFB and CFB resulted in a significant lengthening of these intervals in comparison with the conditions NFB and OFB. The values of the F ratio and corresponding probability levels are shown in Table 4. Since there was no interaction between sequence type and feedback condition, both of which revealed significant main effects, we conclude that the influence of feedback conditions on synchronization errors was similar for both sequence types. With regard to the interaction between sequence type and tap position, within-subject differences between the two types of sequences were significant for the first and second taps in the patterns [F(1,17) = for T1, p <.1, and 6.44 for T2, p <.5] and nonsignificant for the third and fourth taps (p >.5). Significant withinsubject differences with respect to the position of taps inside the patterns were found for the D sequence [F(3,51) = 2.87, p <.5], the first and the second taps being associated with a significantly shorter anticipation than was the third one. The values of the F ratio and corresponding probability levels are shown in Table 5. No differences according to the tap position were found for the F sequence (p >.5). The individual results, pooled according to the significant main effects observed in the preceding analysis, are shown in Figure 7. Using a Tukey's post hoc compari-

6 696 MATES, RADIL, AND POPPEL Pair - 1 Pair - 2 Pair ,---,.-,...--,---.-~~~-~~ &4 t ;f 1 -- ~.-+F -~;r------l- - i.;,..:~_ +-- L.1 r 1 o Q) if) '----'---'-_-'----'---'_ '_ ' DT1 Dr2 Dr3 Df4 Fr1 FT2 FT3 FT4 Pair r-,--, ,---,.-,...--,---.-~~ &4 63 Dr' Dr2 DT3 OH FT1 FT2 FTS FT4 Dr, Dr2 Dr3 Dr4 FT' FT2 FTS FH Pair - 5 Pair '----'---'-_-'-----'---'-_-'----'---'_..I.-...J on Dr2 DT3 OT4 FT1 FT2 Fr3 Fr4 DT1 DT2 DT3 DH FT1 FT2 FTS FT4 Dr' DT2 DTS DT4 FT' FT2 FTS FT4 Pair r-..,...,...-r-,---, ,---,.-..--~ &4 Pair - 8 Pair - 9 ".'I~"'-l..+'.i...-1 " TO. of -+~'-.:r,"":f:~ , 7<:' --\;L"'"':T' t...a.. "-l _\lt~.;..l:; ~ " -.'::' f -l-:'- r T "f" -'" '---'---'_..I.--I...---'_..I.--I...---'L.-.J...-.J on OT2 DT3 Of4 Fr, FT2 Fr3 FT4 or, DT2 DT3 OH FT1 FT2 FT3 FT4 DT1 DT2 OTS DT4 FT1 FT2 Fr3 FT4 Tap position Figure 3. Individual means with between-trial-blocks standard deviations of intertap intervals plotted for both stimulus sequences and for the four consecutive tap positions in the rhythmic patterns separately (DTI-DT4, Taps 1-4 for the D sequence; FTI-FT4, Taps 1-4 for the F sequence). The data are grouped according to the experimental pairs (Pairs 1-9; circles, Subject 1; triangles, Subject 2 in one pair). Subjects' trial means were pooled over all feedback conditions, which revealed no significant effect on the duration of intertap intervals. The dashed line represents the interstimulns interval (5 msec). (Tick marks on the abscissas are not a scale; points are connected with lines to aid the eye.)

7 COOPERATIVE TAPPING 697 (j) co E. c +C CIl 's D Q CIl f '--- - Tl T2 T3 T4 Tap position Figure 4. Total average values with between-trial-blocks standard errors of the mean of tap durations plotted for both stimulus 'sequences (empty bars, D sequence; filled bars, F sequence) and for the four consecutive tap positions in the rhythmic patterns (Tl-T4) separately. Subjects' trial means were pooled over all feedback conditions, which revealed no significant effect on tap durations. The corresponding stimulus-tone duration was for the first tone in the D sequence equal to 2 msec; otherwise it was 1 msec (represented by the dashed line). (Cf. Figure 1.) son, interindividual differences of synchronization errors were found (p <.(1). The subjects' total means and between-trials standard deviations are shown in Figure 8. The series of successive anticipation intervals in each trial were also tested with simple linear regression to examine trends in their duration during the sequence. With the use of the t statistic, the trend slopes were found to be significantly positive in 49% of cases for the D sequence and in 44 % ofcases for the F sequence (p <.5). The possible factors influencing the slopes ofthese trends were investigated by means of a repeated measures ANDVA. Type of experimental sequence (D or F; df = 1) and type offeedback condition (NFB, DFB, AFB, CFB; df = 3) were selected as trial (within-subject) factors with the slopes of trends computed in each trial for all subjects as replicates (df = 17). The hypothesis ofhomogeneity of within-eell variances, on the basis ofa Bartlett's test, was rejected (~(7) = 15.89, P <.5J, so again the Greenhouse-Geisser's adjusted probabilities were used to judge the significance levels. There was no significant main effect of sequence type (p >.5) or of feedback condition (p >.5), nor was their interaction significant (p >.5). The mutual dependencies between slopes of trends of series of anticipation intervals for the simultaneously performing members of a subject pair were tested by using correlation coefficients computed for all pairs ofsubjects and all experimental conditions. These correlations were not significant for any feedback condition or for any sequence. For conditions AFB and CFB for both the D sequence and the F sequence, the values of cross-correlation functions (for lag equal to and 1) between the series of successive anticipation intervals of pair members from each experimental pair in one trial were also computed. No systematic significant cross-correlations were found between pairs of subjects in the two feedback conditions or with the D sequence and the F sequence. DISCUSSION The present experiment, in a sense, represents a model of a maximally simplified musical duet with and without feedback between both performers, with the computer providing the external timing template and to a certain extent playing the role of a conductor. Fraisse (1982) hypothesized that the unity ofmusicians playing togetheris possible only when they are capable of anticipation. It is obvious Table 3 F Ratios and Probability Levels of Post Hoc Within-Subject Comparison of Tap Durations According to Tap Position (1-4) for Both Sequences Together and for the D and F Sequences Separately Tap Position F 4 Both Sequences D sequence p F Tap Position 3 2 P F P F sequence Note-The degrees of freedom in all cases are 1,17. Single-degree-of-freedomcontrasts were used for pairwise comparisons.

8 698 MATES, RADIL, AND POPPEL Pair - 1 Pair - 2 Pair - 3 ' f. 1..._... f--...",.""".""..1 +_.+--l--! t +,, +..f t,, --t- +..'t,..., o Q) C!) 8 Pair t-r---.-,.--,--,--, r--, 2 1 F:( r~r..r :F rt 1 --Lt;.+"~';f L~:l::-J::.1... DTl DT2 OT3 OU FT1 FT2 FT3 FT4 DTl OT2 DT3 DT4 FT1 FT2 FT3 FT4 t-+-t- f f Pair - 5 Pair - 6, -f. t f!""'i""'-f--"'t DT, DT2 OT3 OT4 DT1 OT2 DT3 DT4 FT, FT2 FT3 FT4 Pair - 7 Pair - 8 Pair _...._... f--t'l-f ;--+---t..;,oo,---+-_.,, -!- f, t"""f""'+_'-j f l.. t..f O' '----'_--'------'------L_.L-...L---J...J OT1 DT2 DT3 DT4 DT, DT2 OT3 DT4 FTl FT2 FT3 FT4 DT, OT2 DT3 DT4 FT, FT2 FT3 FT4 Tap position Figure 5. Individual means with between-trial-blocks standard deviations of tap durations plotted for both stimulus sequences and for the four consecutive tap positions in the rhythmic patterns separately (DTI-DT4, Taps 1-4 for the D sequence; FfI-Ff4, Taps 1-4 for the F sequence). The data are grouped according to the experimental pairs (pairs 1-9; circles, Subject I; triangles, Subject 2 in one pair). Subjects' trial means were pooled over all feedback conditions, which revealed no significant effect on tap durations. The corresponding stimulus-tone duration was for the first tone in the D sequence equal to 2 rnsec; otherwise it was 1 rnsec (represented by the dashed lines). (Cf. Figure 1.) (Tick marks on the abscissas are not a scale; points are connected with lines to aid the eye.)

9 COOPERATIVE TAPPING 699 U QJ U) ~ Q;. c 9 cd " c -6 1: u c.>, Ul I NFB OFB AFB CFB Feedback condition Figure 6. Total average values with between-trial-blocks standard errors of the mean of synchronization errors plotted for both stimulus sequences (empty bars, D sequence; filled bars, F sequence) and for the four feedback conditions (NFB, C. I, AFB, CFB) separately. Subjects' trial means were pooled over all consecutive tap positions in the rhythmic patterns, which revealed no significant main effect on synchronization errors. The dashed line represents the onset of stimuli; the zero level means perfect synchronization; and the area below this level corresponds to responses in advance of stimulus presentation. Table 4 F Ratios and Probability Levels of Post Hoc Within-Subject Comparison of Synchronization Errors According to the Feedback Condition (NFB, OFB, AFB, CFB) for Both Sequences Together CFB AFB OFB F p F p F p NFB OFB <.1 AFB Note-The degrees of freedom in all cases are 1,17. Single-degree-offreedom contrasts were used for pairwise comparisons. CFB, crossed feedback; NFB, no feedback; AFB, alien feedback; OFB, own feedback. j that not only the capability of anticipation, but also the ability to synchronize one's own activity with the activity of other players, is the necessary condition of unity. In this study, the effect of social feedback, or "cross-yoking" (see Smith & Kao, 1971)the sensory and motor systems of two individuals, performing identical tasks, by means of feedback loops, was examined. The results showed that unior bilateral cooperation of the type adopted in our experiment neither contributed to more accurate performance of the subjects (in the sense of reducing differences between stimulus and response timing) nor mutually synchronized the activity of both subjects. Thus, in the "social feedback" paradigm used in this study, which differs from those mentioned in the introduction mostly in the impossibility of adding up the effort of both participants in a direct way and in the lack of any type of learning and apparent adaptive behavior, no signs of real collaboration between the subjects could be found. Also, we did not observe the higher types of cooperation or competition detectable in and usually studied by means of two-person mixed-motive games (Gallo & McClintock, 1965; Marwell & Schmitt, 1975; Rapoport & Chammak, 1965; Smead, 1972; Wichman, 197). The significance of feedback in musical performance is a complex matter that has been studied relatively little (Shaffer, 1984). The stimulus sequences in experiments like ours should be more multifarious (multitonal and with more durations) to study this problem more adequately. On the basis of our observations, we would expect that the more complex a musical piece is (complex time structure and more musicians playing together), the greater the anticipation necessary in order to obtain subjectively acceptable synchronization. The results showed that the different feedback conditions used in our experiment primarily influenced the temporal difference between the stimulus onsets and the corresponding tap onsets (synchronization error), with the latter ones usually preceding the former ones. The other measured temporal characteristics ofthe subjects' performance did not vary systematically with respect to the social feedback conditions adopted. Table 5 F Ratios and Probability Levels of Post Hoc Within-Subject Comparison of Synchronization Errors According to Tap Position (1-4) for the D Sequence Tap Position Tap Position F p F p F 2 p Note-The degrees of freedom in all cases are 1,17. Single-degree-of-freedomcontrasts were used for pairwise comparisons.

10 7 MATES, RAOIL, AND POPPEL Pair - 1 Pair - 2 Pair eo -9-12,..., -115 Q) -18 C!) L..-J L~l:... - l f., CN oo OA oo t... \ 'f CN oo OA oo I.:: ~ "" \. \ " J \.....j.. : ;--- f.. f... "'. :' ",-» - CN oo OA oo t - I-, ~':~"'",. I ~.~ Pair - 4 F t\i-,.! 1 CN oo OA OC.:'..,... j\ /..'\ 'i"~ \ i" \-, '",.. '" t r"'-... CN oo CIA oo Pair - 5 '1._" J """'l-"'f t -.! +: :t- ' i CN oo OA oo Pair - 6 '-. ~ t -l> t---1 ~ o ~ ~ Q) c o rl +J nj N 'r-! C o ~..c: o c ~ (f) -3 Pair.j--;"'1 t!... -eo 1... ~ 7 f'-k i-.. :..t~.! Pair - 8 Pair rso A-.,." "'j t --! CN oo OA OC ON oo OA oo CN co CIA co FN FO FA Fe Experimental condition Figure 7. Individual trial means with between-trial-blocks standard deviations of synchronization errors plotted for both stimulus sequences and for the four feedback conditions separately (DN,, DA, DC: Conditions NFB, OFB, AFB, and CFB for the D sequence; FN, FO, It'A, FC: Conditions NFB, OFB, AFB, and CFB for the F sequence). The data are grouped according to the experimental pairs (Pairs 1-9;circles, Subject 1; triangles, Subject 2 in one pair). Subjects' trial means were pooled over all consecutive tap positions in the rhythmic patterns, which revealed no significant main effect on synchronization errors. The zero lines represent the onset of stimuli; the zero level means perfect synchronization; and the area below this level corresponds to responses in advance of stimulus presentation. (Tick marks on the abscissas are not a scale; points are connected with lines to aid the eye.)

11 COOPERATIVE TAPPING 71 5 Q) (j) E L 2-5 L Q) C cd N c L -'= c >- (f) l' l 'I"~ -, Subject Figure 8. Individual total means with between-trial-blocks standard deviations of synchronization errors. Subjects' trial means were pooled over all consecutive tap positions in the rhythmic patterns, for both stimulus sequences, and for the four feedback conditions. Abscissa: 11-91, Pairs 1-9, Subject 1; 12-92, Pairs 1-9, Subject 2. The dashed line represents the onset of stimuli; the zero level means perfect synchronization; and the area below this level corresponds to responses in advance of stimulus presentation. Synchronization Errors Linked (cooperative) performances. The systematic advance of responses with respect to stimulus occurrence has been observed in previous synchronization studies (see, e.g., Fraisse, 1978, 1982; Hary & Moore, 1985; Kolers & Brewster, 1985; Radil et al., 199b). Responses to predictable stimuli should be considered as anticipatory not only in the case when they appear objectively in advance with respect to stimuli, but also when the responses are delayed by an interval shorter than the usual reaction time of at least 15 msec (Adams & Creamer, 1962; Najenson et al., 1989; Schmidt, 1968; Schmidt & Christina, 1969). The cause of motor anticipation of a sequential predictable series of stimuli is still obscure (Radii et al., I99b) and deserves special analysis. Other studies have demonstrated that anticipation is suppressed by shortening the available interstimulus intervals, either through increasing the stimulation rate (Muller, Ilmberger, Poppel, Mates, & Radii, 199) or through increasing the stimulus duration during these intervals (Franek et al., 1989). A transition from anticipative toward pure reactive (delayed) responses has been observed when the interstimulus interval duration is lengthened above an individual threshold of a few seconds (Najenson et al., 1989; Poppel, MUller, & Mates, 199). On the other hand, anticipation remains present even when some of the tones in the patterns are missing. Moreover, a general tendency to lengthen anticipation under these conditions has been observed (Radil, Mates, Ilmberger, & Poppel, 199a). We also observed that anticipation intervals were significantly longer when sequences were accentuated by frequency rather than by duration changes, and significantly longer under Conditions AFB and CFB than under Conditions NFB and OFB. The findings above concerning anticipation intervals indicate that greater task "difficulty" (i.e., higher information load as expressed in the absence of some stimuli, or in different frequencies of some tones, or in unpredictability caused by alien feedback) might result in a higher activation, with a likely alteration of corresponding motor programs and/or further decision making, hence requiring additional processing time. Consequently, the duration of anticipation increases. We do not possess explicit knowledge about the temporal scheme of events taking place under the AFB and CFB conditions. Due to interindividual differences and uncorrelated variance in performance and brain timing in

12 72 MATES, RADIL, AND POPPEL both subjects, there might be in both cases a certain temporal incongruity between auditory and tactile-kinaesthetic feedback which makes the programming of rhythmic motor activity more difficult. An additional factor under the CFB condition could be the mutual dynamic influence between both incongruent systems, which further increases the complexity of the programming task for the brain and which is reflected in larger anticipation intervals. There were no differences in time trends of individual series of synchronization errors, no correlations between these trends, and no cross-correlations between the synchronization error series of "cooperating" subjects in a pair under the experimental conditions studied. There seems to have been no "cooperation" between subjects in the sense of instantaneous unidirectional or bidirectional influence on each other's performance. The influence from the other subject resulted only in an increase of mean anticipation interval without any relation to the instantaneous value of the anticipation interval of that subject. This influence seems to have been established at the beginning of each trial, and it did not change over the sequence. Separated performances. The objectively measured dissynchrony of motor response and stimulus does not necessarily mean that these events are also dissynchronous in the brain. Let us assume, as Hary and Moore (1985) did, that "the neural events corresponding to the two external events, observed to be dis-synchronous in the laboratory, could occur synchronously at some point in the nervous system" (p. 78). This synchronous occurrence may also be understood in terms of a "temporal window" of nonzero duration, which can perhaps be created by a neuronal oscillator. One period of such an oscillation may define a time zone (temporal window), within which the before-after relationship is not defined (Peppel, 197; Peppel, Schill, & von Steinbiichel, 199). The analogous hypothesis of a "psychological moment" (Fraisse, 1978) could be considered, too. Mutual timing in "temporal central availability" (Poppel, Schill, & von Steinbiichel, 199) of the feedback signals and proper auditory stimuli might just be the important factor that determines anticipation, although its exact mechanisms are not understood yet. It is ofpotential importance that the time of transduction ("conduction delay" in Hary & Moore, 1985; "perception latency" in Fraisse, 1978) to the cortex for the auditory pathway is shorter than that for the tactile-kinesthetic one, resulting in different temporal central availability of both signals. Under the NFB condition, the longer delay is in the feedback (tactile-kinesthetic) pathway, so that the response observed from the outside has to be in advance ofthe related stimulus in order that delayed feedback information about it may appear simultaneously (in some of the meanings of simultaneity given above) with the stimulus in the central nervous system. The faster auditory feedback pathway involved in the OFB condition may have supplied information about one's own response earlier than in the NFB condition, when the subject's brain did rely upon tactile-kinesthetic information only. Therefore, in the former case, the motor command could have been sent later, causing the anticipation intervals to be shorter. Fraisse (1982) mentioned a greater anticipation in synchronization performed by foot than by finger; this can also be explained by the longer time of transduction of peripheral information. Comparison of Conditions NFB (i.e., no additional feedback) and OFB (i.e., own feedback) showed that the auditory feedback derived from the subject's own taps could influence positively his or her performance in the sense that the synchronization errors became smaller; however, significance was not achieved. Kolers and Brewster (1985) have hypothesized that the use of the tactile feedback information is inconsistent, if there is any at all. Nevertheless, the tactile-kinesthetic feedback coming from the motor effector and from touching the response key remained constant across experimental conditions; only the external auditory feedback loop created additional variance. Thus, the tactile-kinesthetic feedback could have determined the results in all the experimental conditions and could have suppressed the expected positive influence of additional auditory feedback. It resulted in a nonzero anticipation even in the OFB condition, where both signals were coming through the same sensory pathway (i.e., with the same delay). Persisting anticipation when both the feedback signal and the stimulus were in the visual modality has also been observed by Kolers and Brewster. They found a larger variability ofsynchronization errors in the intramodal (visual) stimulus and feedback setting as opposed to the intermodal (visual-tactile) condition; the mean values were not different. It might be possible that subjects use the available feedback information according to the accuracy that it offers for their judgment about the stimulus-response synchrony. The larger variability mentioned can be a result of a more frequent intervention of feedback regarding the subject's strategy in the intramodalcondition, in accordance with this 'accuracy" -determined preference. Nevertheless, it is obvious that feedback can directly influence the timing of subsequent movements only and not those that have already been executed. Rather, the instantaneous feedback information in performing a sequence of repetitive motor acts is used to tune or update programs ofcorresponding motor- and timing-control systems. We found that in Conditions NFB and OFB the average anticipation interval was somewhat longer than what we observed in the previous study, in which the tapping of single subjects was investigated and no additional signals were provided (RadiI et al., 199Ob). This was probably caused by the fact that synchronization errors vary interindividually, as can be seen from the individual results shown in Figure 7. Intertap Intervals The individual intertap intervals shown in Figure 3 support the hypothesis that different strategies used for timing of rhythmic patterns result in subjective deformation

13 COOPERATIVE TAPPING 73 of regular patterns (Franek et al., 1991b). Franek and coworkers discussed the systematic shortening of the second intertap interval in comparison with the first one in the patterns as one possible strategy used in rhythmic patterns. Both the results of experiments performed by Franek et al. (l991b), in which various frequency accents were used, and the present observed differences in the timing of intertap intervals in the patterns between the D and F sequences show that these strategies are influenced by the type of accent. In 63 % of the cases, our subjects used the long-short strategy (i.e., they made the first intertap interval longer than the second one) for the D sequence. The prevailing use of this strategy might be connected with the differences in tap durations in the patterns observed for this sequence (see below). In the study by Franek et al. (l991b) a lengthening of the fifth intertap interval in six-tone patterns was also observed. It was suggested that the prolongation was caused by a subjective accentuation that was needed as a control point for timing at the end of a temporal interval, reflecting the duration of a "psychological present" (Fraisse, 1984) or the limited capacity of a "central integration mechanism" (e.g., Poppel, 1988). Such a subjective accentuation might be one of the sources ofthe (nonsignificant) prolongation of the fourth intertap interval that was observed in our experiment for the D sequence as well. The present observed distortions in intertap interval durations were not influenced by the type of auditory feedback condition used. Tap Durations A brief discussion of further distortions in performance timing that were not influenced by the type of external feedback adopted is in order. The subjects were always instructed to tap synchronously only with stimulus onsets and not to follow the duration of stimulus tones. Despite this instruction, we observed a systematic decrease of tap durations in the reproductions of the D sequence, where the first tap, corresponding to the accentuated tone, was the (significantly) longest tap in the patterns. No such phenomenon was observed for the F sequence, where the first tone was accentuated by frequency changes. However, in both sequence types, the fourth tap was the (significantly) shortest one. The technique that we used did not allow us to measure the tapping force; stronger taps could be assessed in terms of the lengthening ofthe tap durations only. The results could mean that the differences in duration of the differing tones are perceived as stronger accents than are the differences in frequency. On the basis of the observed (nonsignificant) prolongation of the last (i.e., the fourth) intertap interval in the patterns for the D sequence, it could be assumed that in connection with the perceived stronger accent, there is also a stronger tendency to group tones in the rhythmic patterns together. This grouping tendency is reflected by the separation of patterns by longer pauses. The shortening of the second intertap interval for the D sequence might be a subjective compensation for the longer first tap duration in the patterns, although the durations of the first intertap intervals did not differ from the interstimu Ius interval for this sequence. The striking interindividual differences in tap durations evident in Figure 5 might reflect different levels in the subjects' musical skill. It has already been reported (Franek et al., 1991a) that musical skill influences tap durations. REFERENCES ADAMS, J. A., &: CREAMER, L. R. (1962). Proprioception variables as determiners of anticipatory timing behavior. Human Factors, 4, CLYNES, C. M., &: WALKER, J. (1982). Neurobiologic functions of rhythm, time and pulse in music. In M. Clynes (Ed.), Music. mind and brain: The neuropsychology ofmusic (pp ). New York: Plenum. FRAISSE, P. (1978). Time and rhythm perception. In E. C. Carterette & M. Friedman (Eds.), Handbook ofperception: Vol. 8. Perceptual coding (pp ). New York: Academic Press. FRAISSE, P. (1982). Rhythm and tempo. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), Psychology of music (pp ). New York: Academic Press. FRAISSE, P. (1984). Perception and estimation of time. Annual Review / Psychology, 35, FRANJlK, M., MATES, J., RADlL, T., BECK, K., &: POPPEL, E. (l99la). Finger tapping in musicians and nonmusicians. International Journal / Psychophysiology, 11, FRAN K, M., MATES, J., RADlL, T., BECK, K., &: POPPEL, E. (l99lb). Sensorimotor synchronization: Motor responses to regular auditory patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 49, FRAN K, M., RADlL, T., POPPEL, E., BECK, K., MATES, J., &: ILM BERGER, J. (1989). Following of rhythmic tonal sequences by tapping and holding the response key. Activitas Nervosa Superior, 31, GALW, P. S., JR., &: McCUNTOCK, C. G. (1965). Cooperative and competitive behavior in mixed-motive games. Journal / Conflict Resolution, 9, HARY, D., &: MOORE, G. P. (1985). Temporal tracking and synchronization strategies. Human Neurobiology, 4, ILMBERGER, J., MULLER, U., POPPEL, E., MATES, J., &: RADlL, T. (199). Timing of rhythmic tapping and lateralization. Activitas Nervosa Superior, 32, KNIGHT, J. L., JR. (1987). Manual control and tracking. In G. Salvendy (Ed.), Handbook ofhuman factors (pp ). New York: Wiley. KOLERS, P. A., &: BREWSTER, J. M. (1985). Rhythms and responses. Journal / Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, ll, MARWELL, G., &: ScHMITT, D. R. (1975). Cooperation: An experimental analysis. New York: Academic Press. MATES, J. (199). A system of personal computer control programs for tapping experiments. Computer Methods & Programs in Biomedicine, 33, MATES, J. (1991). Extending the model ofself-paced periodic responding: Comment on Vos and Ellennann (1989). Journal ofexperimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 17, MULLER, U., ILMBERGER, J., POPPEL, E., MATES, J., &: RADlL, T. (199). Stimulus anticipation and the 3 ms basic timing unit during rhythmic tapping. Activitas Nervosa Superior, 32, NAJENSON, T., RON, S., &: BEHROOZI, K. (1989). Temporal characteristics of tapping responses in healthy subjects and in patients who sustained cerebrovascular accident. Brain, Behavior & Evolution, 33, PETERS, M. (1989). The relationship between variability of intertap intervals and interval duration. Psychological Research, 51, POPPEL, E. (197). Excitability cycles in central intennittency. Psychologische Forschung ; 34, 1-9. POPPEL, E. (1988). Mindworks: TImeand conscious experience. Boston: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. POPPEL, E., MULLER, U., &: MATES, J. (199, October). Temporalconstraints in synchronization/ motor responses to a regular sequence

14 74 MATES RADIL, AND POPPEL ofstimuli. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, SI. Louis. POPPEL, E., ScHILL, K., '" VON STEINBOCHEL, N. (199). Sensory integration within temporally neutral systems states: A hypothesis. Naturwissenschaften, 77, RADlL, T., MATES, J., ILMBERGER, J., '" POPPEL, E. (l99oa). Interpolation and extrapolation during rhythmical tapping. Activitas Nervosa Superior, 32, RADlL, T., MATES, J., ILMBERGER, J., '" POPPEL, E. (l99b). Stimulus anticipation in following rhythmic acoustical patterns by tapping. Experientia, 46, RAPOPORT, A., '" CHAMMAK, A. M. (1965). Prisoner'sdilemma: A study in conflict andcooperation. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. ROSENBERG, S., '" HALL, R. L. (1958). The effect of different social feedback conditions upon performance in dyadic teams. Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 57, SAUTER, S., '" SMITH, K. U. (1971). Social feedback: Quantitative division of labor in social interactions. Journal ofcybernetics, I, SHAFFER, L. H. (1984). Timing in solo and duet piano performances. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36A, ScHMIDT, R. A. (1968). Anticipation and timing in human motor performance. Psychological Bulletin, 7, ScHMIDT, R. A., '" CHRISTINA, R. W. (1969). Proprioception as a mediator in the timing of motor responses. Journal ofexperimental Psychology, 81, SMEAD, A. (1972). Cooperation and competition. In L. S. Wrightsman (Ed.), Social psychology in the seventies (pp ). Belmont: Brooks/Cole. SMELSER, W. T. (1961). Dominance as a factor in achievement and perception in cooperative problem solving interactions. Journal ofabnormal & Social Psychology, 62, SMITH, K. U., '" ARNDT, R. (1969). Experimental hybrid-computer automation in perceptual-motor and social behavioral research. Journal of Motor Behavior, I, SMITH, K. U., '" KAO, H. (1971). Social feedback: Determination of social learning. Journal ofnervous & Mental Disease, 152, SMITH, T. J., '" SMITH, K. U. (l987a). Feedback-control mechanisms of human behavior. In G. Salvendy (Ed.), Handbook ofhuman factors (pp ). New York: Wiley. SMITH, T. J., '" SMITH, K. U. (l987b). Motor feedback control of human cognition-implications for the cognitive inference. In G. Salvendy, S. L. Sauter, J. J. Hurrel, Jr. (Eds.), Social, ergonomic and stress aspects ofwork with computers (pp ). Amsterdam: Elsevier. VORBERG, D., '" HAMBUCH, R. (1984). Timing oftwo-handed rhythmic performance. In J. Gibbon & L. Allan (Eds.), Timing and time perception (Annals of The New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 423, pp. 39-4(6). New York: New York Academy of Sciences. Vos, P. G., '" ELLERMANN, H. H. (1989). Precision and accuracy in the reproduction of simple tone sequences. Journal ofexperimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 15, WAGNER, N., '" ZEAMAN, D. (1956). Team and individual performances on a motor learning task. Journal ofgeneral Psychology, 55, WICHMAN, H. (197). Effects of isolation and communicationon cooperation in a two-person game. Journal ofpersonality & Social Psychology, 16, WING, A. M., KEELE, S., '" MARGOLIN, D. I. (1984). Motor disorder and the timing of repetitive movements. In J. Gibbon & L. Allan (Eds.), Timing and time perception (Annals of The New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 423, pp ). New York: New York Academy of Sciences. WING, A. M., '" KRISTOFFERSON, A. B. (1973). Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses. Perception & Psychophysics, 14, (Manuscript received March 6, 1991; revision accepted for publication May 26, 1992.)

Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns

Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns Perception & Psychophysics 1986, 40 (2), 69-73 Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns PETER J. ESSENS TNO Institute for Perception, Soesterberg, The Netherlands In two reproduction experiments,

More information

The generation of two isochronous sequences in parallel

The generation of two isochronous sequences in parallel Perception & Psychophysics 1983, 34, 331-337 The generation of two isochronous sequences in parallel DIANA DEUTSCH University of California, La Jolla, California Subjects were presented with two parallel

More information

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2001, 61: 175-179 Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions El bieta Szel¹g 1, Krystyna Rymarczyk 1 and Ernst Pöppel 2 1 Department

More information

Imitation and production of interval ratios

Imitation and production of interval ratios Perception & Psychophysics 1986, 39 (6), 437-444 Imitation and production of interval ratios JEFFERY J. SUMMERS, SIMON R. HAWKINS, and HELEN MAYERS University ofmelbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia

More information

TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS

TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS Simon Grondin, Vincent Laflamme, Nicolas Bisson, Emi Hasuo, Tsuyoshi Kuroda Université Laval, Québec, Canada simon.grondin@psy.ulaval.ca Abstract

More information

Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion

Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion Perception & Psychophysics 1980.28 (5). 465-470 Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion GREGORY M. CORSO Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia 30332 A pair of pure-tone sine

More information

Auditory Scene Analysis

Auditory Scene Analysis 1 Auditory Scene Analysis Albert S. Bregman Department of Psychology McGill University 1205 Docteur Penfield Avenue Montreal, QC Canada H3A 1B1 E-mail: bregman@hebb.psych.mcgill.ca To appear in N.J. Smelzer

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Keywords: time perception; illusion; empty interval; filled intervals; cluster analysis

Keywords: time perception; illusion; empty interval; filled intervals; cluster analysis Journal of Sound and Vibration Manuscript Draft Manuscript Number: JSV-D-10-00826 Title: Does filled duration illusion occur for very short time intervals? Article Type: Rapid Communication Keywords: time

More information

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Ivaylo Vlaev (ivaylo.vlaev@psy.ox.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX1

More information

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity 1,2 Mark A. Elliott, 2 Zhuanghua Shi & 2,3 Fatma Sürer 1 Department of Psychology National University of Ireland Galway, Ireland.

More information

Rhythm Categorization in Context. Edward W. Large

Rhythm Categorization in Context. Edward W. Large Rhythm Categorization in Context Edward W. Large Center for Complex Systems Florida Atlantic University 777 Glades Road, P.O. Box 39 Boca Raton, FL 3343-99 USA large@walt.ccs.fau.edu Keywords: Rhythm,

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE Åke Hellström Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, S-106 91 Stockholm, Sweden Email: hellst@psychology.su.se Abstract Participants compared successive

More information

Temporal Control of Movements in Sensorimotor Synchronization

Temporal Control of Movements in Sensorimotor Synchronization Brain and Cognition 48, 66 79 (2002) doi:10.1006/brcg.2001.1304, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Temporal Control of Movements in Sensorimotor Synchronization Gisa Aschersleben Max Planck

More information

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 11 18 The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Abstract Ralph M. Reitan, Deborah Wolfson Reitan Neuropsychology

More information

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 BLL R. BROWN AND THOMAS J. REBBN2 UNlVERSTY OF LOUSVLLE This experiment required Ss to make Markov-generated histoforms that were

More information

Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities

Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities David P. McGovern, Andrew T. Astle, Sarah L. Clavin and Fiona N. Newell Figure S1: Group-averaged learning

More information

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),???-??? 794-799 What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? ULRICH MAYR University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon Schneider and

More information

Human Movement Science

Human Movement Science Human Movement Science xxx (2012) xxx xxx Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Human Movement Science journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/humov Anticipatory phase correction in sensorimotor

More information

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation The Role of in Categorisation Mark Suret (m.suret@psychol.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology; Downing Street Cambridge, CB2 3EB UK I.P.L. McLaren (iplm2@cus.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental

More information

Beat perception in 3D: A comparative analysis between sight, sound, and touch

Beat perception in 3D: A comparative analysis between sight, sound, and touch Beat perception in 3D: A comparative analysis between sight, sound, and touch Heather Khey Beldman (Dr. Jessica A. Grahn) The Brain and Mind Institute, Western University Physiology 4980E Honors Thesis

More information

Some methodological aspects for measuring asynchrony detection in audio-visual stimuli

Some methodological aspects for measuring asynchrony detection in audio-visual stimuli Some methodological aspects for measuring asynchrony detection in audio-visual stimuli Pacs Reference: 43.66.Mk, 43.66.Lj Van de Par, Steven ; Kohlrausch, Armin,2 ; and Juola, James F. 3 ) Philips Research

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Temporal Integration in Sensorimotor Synchronization

Temporal Integration in Sensorimotor Synchronization Temporal Integration in Sensorimotor Synchronization Jfii Mates Ludwig-Maximilians University and Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic Ulrike Muller Klinikum Passauer Wolf and Ludwig-Maximilians University

More information

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences 2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences Stanislas Dehaene Chair of Experimental Cognitive Psychology Lecture n 5 Bayesian Decision-Making Lecture material translated

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task?

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task? Bachelor s Thesis Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a sequential movement task? Nils Hatger Prof. Dr. Ing. Willem Verwey Jonathan Barnhoorn, PhD student BMS Faculty/Cognitive

More information

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14 CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines Assignment 5: Neural mechanisms of object-based attention Due: Friday, April 14 This Assignment is a guided reading of the 2014 paper, Neural Mechanisms of Object-Based

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Table 1 Patient characteristics Preoperative. language testing

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Table 1 Patient characteristics Preoperative. language testing Categorical Speech Representation in the Human Superior Temporal Gyrus Edward F. Chang, Jochem W. Rieger, Keith D. Johnson, Mitchel S. Berger, Nicholas M. Barbaro, Robert T. Knight SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

More information

Modeling Human Perception

Modeling Human Perception Modeling Human Perception Could Stevens Power Law be an Emergent Feature? Matthew L. Bolton Systems and Information Engineering University of Virginia Charlottesville, United States of America Mlb4b@Virginia.edu

More information

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER Memory & Cognition 2008, 36 (7), 1236-1247 doi: 10.3758/MC/36.7.1236 An action sequence held in memory can interfere with response selection of a target stimulus, but does not interfere with response activation

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

Perception and Production of Brief Durations: Beat-Based Versus Interval-Based Timing

Perception and Production of Brief Durations: Beat-Based Versus Interval-Based Timing Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2001, Vol. 27, No. 2, 485-493 Copyright 2001 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/01/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.27.2.485

More information

Concurrent Learning of Temporal and Spatial Sequences

Concurrent Learning of Temporal and Spatial Sequences Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2002, Vol. 28, No. 3, 445 457 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.3.445

More information

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Activation of effect codes in response planning - 1 - To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Michael Ziessler

More information

FATIGUE STUDIED IN REACTION TIME EXPERIMENTS. By. furnish results of a general kind, and does not enable one to arrive at a

FATIGUE STUDIED IN REACTION TIME EXPERIMENTS. By. furnish results of a general kind, and does not enable one to arrive at a FATIGUE STUDIED IN REACTION TIME EXPERIMENTS. By T. H. MILROY. (From the Physiology Laboratory, Queen's University, Belfast.) (Received for publication 5th April 1909.) MANY methods have been employed

More information

Behavioral generalization

Behavioral generalization Supplementary Figure 1 Behavioral generalization. a. Behavioral generalization curves in four Individual sessions. Shown is the conditioned response (CR, mean ± SEM), as a function of absolute (main) or

More information

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (2), February /98/103(2)/1080/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 1080

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (2), February /98/103(2)/1080/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 1080 Perceptual segregation of a harmonic from a vowel by interaural time difference in conjunction with mistuning and onset asynchrony C. J. Darwin and R. W. Hukin Experimental Psychology, University of Sussex,

More information

The Science. This work was performed by Dr. Psyche Loui at Wesleyan University under NSF-STTR # Please direct inquiries to

The Science. This work was performed by Dr. Psyche Loui at Wesleyan University under NSF-STTR # Please direct inquiries to The Science This work was performed by Dr. Psyche Loui at Wesleyan University under NSF-STTR #1720698. Please direct inquiries to research@brain.fm Summary Brain.fm designs music to help you do things.

More information

Image generation in a letter-classification task

Image generation in a letter-classification task Perception & Psychophysics 1976, Vol. 20 (3),215-219 Image generation in a letter-classification task THOMAS R. HERZOG Grand Valley State Colleges, Allandale, Michigan 49401 Subjects classified briefly

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Memory Search, Task Switching and Timing 1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Timing is affected by demands in memory search, but not by task switching Claudette

More information

9/3/2014. Which impairs the ability to integrate these experiences in an adaptive manner.

9/3/2014. Which impairs the ability to integrate these experiences in an adaptive manner. Presented by DaLene Forester Thacker, PhD Licensed Marriage and Family Therapist Licensed Professional Clinical Counselor Director and Trainer with AEI daleneforester@yahoo.com I was not able to use EMDR

More information

When do auditory/visual differences in duration judgements occur?

When do auditory/visual differences in duration judgements occur? THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2006, 59 (10), 1709 1724 When do auditory/visual differences in duration judgements occur? J. H. Wearden Keele University, Keele, UK N. P. M. Todd and L.

More information

The effect of stimulus duration on the persistence of gratings

The effect of stimulus duration on the persistence of gratings Perception & Psychophysics 1980,27 (6),574-578 The effect of stimulus duration on the persistence of gratings ALISON BOWLING and WILLIAM LOVEGROVE University oftasmania, Hobart, Tasmania, Australia 700/

More information

Frequency refers to how often something happens. Period refers to the time it takes something to happen.

Frequency refers to how often something happens. Period refers to the time it takes something to happen. Lecture 2 Properties of Waves Frequency and period are distinctly different, yet related, quantities. Frequency refers to how often something happens. Period refers to the time it takes something to happen.

More information

Is Clock Slowing General or Specific?

Is Clock Slowing General or Specific? Temporal Binding and Internal Clocks: Is Clock Slowing General or Specific? Richard Fereday (FeredayR@cardiff.ac.uk), Marc J. Buehner (BuehnerM@cardiff.ac.uk) Cardiff University, School of Psychology,

More information

ON THE ROLE OF IMPUTED VELOCITY IN THE AUDITORY KAPPA EFFECT. Molly J. Henry. A Thesis

ON THE ROLE OF IMPUTED VELOCITY IN THE AUDITORY KAPPA EFFECT. Molly J. Henry. A Thesis ON THE ROLE OF IMPUTED VELOCITY IN THE AUDITORY KAPPA EFFECT Molly J. Henry A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate College of Bowling Green State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the

More information

CONTRIBUTION OF DIRECTIONAL ENERGY COMPONENTS OF LATE SOUND TO LISTENER ENVELOPMENT

CONTRIBUTION OF DIRECTIONAL ENERGY COMPONENTS OF LATE SOUND TO LISTENER ENVELOPMENT CONTRIBUTION OF DIRECTIONAL ENERGY COMPONENTS OF LATE SOUND TO LISTENER ENVELOPMENT PACS:..Hy Furuya, Hiroshi ; Wakuda, Akiko ; Anai, Ken ; Fujimoto, Kazutoshi Faculty of Engineering, Kyushu Kyoritsu University

More information

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance Shikakeology: Designing Triggers for Behavior Change: Papers from the 2013 AAAI Spring Symposium The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance Shoko Yamane, Naohiro Matsumura Faculty of Economics, Kinki University;

More information

DOES FILLED DURATION ILLUSION TAKE PLACE FOR VERY SHORT TIME INTERVALS?

DOES FILLED DURATION ILLUSION TAKE PLACE FOR VERY SHORT TIME INTERVALS? DOES FILLED DURATION ILLUSION TAKE PLACE FOR VERY SHORT TIME INTERVALS? Emi Hasuo, Yoshitaka Nakajima, and Kazuo Ueda Graduate School of Design, Kyushu University -9- Shiobaru, Minami-ku, Fukuoka, - Japan

More information

The Coupled Oscillator Model of Between-Hand Coordination in Alternate-Hand Tapping: A Reappraisal

The Coupled Oscillator Model of Between-Hand Coordination in Alternate-Hand Tapping: A Reappraisal Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 200, Vol. 27, No. 2, 25-265 Copyright 200 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-523/0/$5.0O DOI: 0.037//0096-523.27.2.25

More information

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1 Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1 Hearing-aids Induce Plasticity in the Auditory System: Perspectives From Three Research Designs and Personal Speculations About the

More information

THE IMPORTANCE OF COORDINATIVE ABILITIES IN ACHIEVING ATHLETIC PERFORMANCE

THE IMPORTANCE OF COORDINATIVE ABILITIES IN ACHIEVING ATHLETIC PERFORMANCE THE IMPORTANCE OF COORDINATIVE ABILITIES IN ACHIEVING ATHLETIC PERFORMANCE Lecturer PhD Neluţa Smîdu E-mail: nelysmadu@yahoo.com D.E.F.S. A.S.E. Bucharest Abstract During the training process, the development

More information

Assessment of auditory temporal-order thresholds A comparison of different measurement procedures and the influences of age and gender

Assessment of auditory temporal-order thresholds A comparison of different measurement procedures and the influences of age and gender Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience 23 (2005) 281 296 281 IOS Press Assessment of auditory temporal-order thresholds A comparison of different measurement procedures and the influences of age and gender

More information

Vibrotactile masking:,a comparison of psychophysical procedures

Vibrotactile masking:,a comparison of psychophysical procedures Perception & Psychophysics 1977, Vol. 22 (5), 471-475 Vibrotactile masking:,a comparison of psychophysical procedures RITA E. SNYDER Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana 47401 The amounts of ipsilateral

More information

TESTING A NEW THEORY OF PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: TEMPORAL LOUDNESS INTEGRATION

TESTING A NEW THEORY OF PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: TEMPORAL LOUDNESS INTEGRATION TESTING A NEW THEORY OF PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: TEMPORAL LOUDNESS INTEGRATION Karin Zimmer, R. Duncan Luce and Wolfgang Ellermeier Institut für Kognitionsforschung der Universität Oldenburg, Germany Institute

More information

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance Shoko Yamane, Naohiro Matsumura Faculty of Economics, Kinki University; Graduate School of Economics, Osaka University syamane@kindai.ac.jp Abstract We examined

More information

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (2), 85-89 Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons CHARLES R. LEITH and WILLIAM S. MAKI, JR. University ofcalifornia, Berkeley, California

More information

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 13 (1), 6-65 Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects JULIE M. C. BAKER and JOHN

More information

Effects of time intervals and tone durations on auditory stream segregation

Effects of time intervals and tone durations on auditory stream segregation Perception & Psychophysics 2000, 62 (3), 626-636 Effects of time intervals and tone durations on auditory stream segregation ALBERT S. BREGMAN, PIERRE A. AHAD, POPPY A. C. CRUM, and JULIE O REILLY McGill

More information

THE PRESENT STUDY ADDRESSES two main questions:

THE PRESENT STUDY ADDRESSES two main questions: Rate Limits of On-Beat and Off-Beat Tapping with Simple Auditory Rhythms 165 RATE LIMITS OF ON-BEAT AND OFF-BEAT TAPPING WITH SIMPLE AUDITORY RHYTHMS: 2. The Roles of Different Kinds of Accent BRUNO H.

More information

Are the Referents Remembered in Temporal Bisection?

Are the Referents Remembered in Temporal Bisection? Learning and Motivation 33, 10 31 (2002) doi:10.1006/lmot.2001.1097, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Are the Referents Remembered in Temporal Bisection? Lorraine G. Allan McMaster University,

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

Separate What and Where Decision Mechanisms In Processing a Dichotic Tonal Sequence

Separate What and Where Decision Mechanisms In Processing a Dichotic Tonal Sequence Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1976, Vol. 2, No. 1, 23-29 Separate What and Where Decision Mechanisms In Processing a Dichotic Tonal Sequence Diana Deutsch and Philip

More information

Long-range dependence in human movements: detection, interpretation, and modeling perspectives

Long-range dependence in human movements: detection, interpretation, and modeling perspectives Long-range dependence in human movements: detection, interpretation, and modeling perspectives Didier Delignières E.A. 2991 Motor Efficiency and Deficiency University Montpellier I, France Telephone: +33

More information

innate mechanism of proportionality adaptation stage activation or recognition stage innate biological metrics acquired social metrics

innate mechanism of proportionality adaptation stage activation or recognition stage innate biological metrics acquired social metrics 1 PROCESSES OF THE CORRELATION OF SPACE (LENGTHS) AND TIME (DURATIONS) IN HUMAN PERCEPTION Lev I Soyfer To study the processes and mechanisms of the correlation between space and time, particularly between

More information

ơ ͷ! ǡ Ƭ Ǥ!ͺ Ǥ Ǥ ǡ Ǧ Ǥ ǡ Ǥ ǡ Ǥ ǡ ǡ Ǧ Ǥ !"#$!%&

ơ  ͷ! ǡ Ƭ Ǥ!ͺ Ǥ Ǥ ǡ Ǧ Ǥ ǡ Ǥ ǡ Ǥ ǡ ǡ Ǧ Ǥ !#$!%& D E L!! The perception of temporal regularities in auditory rhythms is central to many human activities. In music and dance, for example, the synchronization of movements and sounds is facilitated by the

More information

Measuring sensorimotor synchronization abilities

Measuring sensorimotor synchronization abilities TIMELY School on «Timing and Time Perception: Procedures, Measures, & Applications» Corfu, February 4-8, 2013 Measuring sensorimotor synchronization abilities Simone Dalla Bella EuroMov, M2H Laboratory,

More information

Processes involved in tempo perception: A CNV analysis

Processes involved in tempo perception: A CNV analysis Psychophysiology, 40 (2003), 69 76. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2003 Society for Psychophysiological Research Processes involved in tempo perception: A CNV analysis MICHA

More information

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Alessandro Couyoumdjian (alessandro.couyoumdjian@uniroma1.it) Faculty of Psychology 1, University La Sapienza via dei

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

The spacing and lag effect in free recall

The spacing and lag effect in free recall The spacing and lag effect in free recall Michael J. Kahana, Bradley R. Wellington & Marc W. Howard Center for Complex Systems and Department of Psychology Brandeis University Send correspondence to: Michael

More information

USE OF A DELAYED SIGNAL TO STOP A VISUAL REACTION-TIME RESPONSE *

USE OF A DELAYED SIGNAL TO STOP A VISUAL REACTION-TIME RESPONSE * Journal of Experimental Psychology 1966, Vol. 72, No. 6, 85-811 USE OF A DELAYED SIGNAL TO STOP A VISUAL REACTION-TIME RESPONSE * JOSEPH S. LAPPIN 2 AND CHARLES W. ERIKSEN University of Illinois In a visual

More information

Novel single trial movement classification based on temporal dynamics of EEG

Novel single trial movement classification based on temporal dynamics of EEG Novel single trial movement classification based on temporal dynamics of EEG Conference or Workshop Item Accepted Version Wairagkar, M., Daly, I., Hayashi, Y. and Nasuto, S. (2014) Novel single trial movement

More information

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison Ethan D. Montag Rochester Institute of Technology Munsell Color Science Laboratory Chester F. Carlson Center for Imaging Science

More information

Psychological Research Springer-Verlag 1989

Psychological Research Springer-Verlag 1989 Psychol Res (1989) 50:251-256 Psychological Research Springer-Verlag 1989 Mechanisms of perceptual timing: Beat-based or interval-based judgements? Steven W. Keele, Roberto Nicoletti, Richard I. lvry,

More information

Do monkeys' subjective clocks run faster in red light than in blue?

Do monkeys' subjective clocks run faster in red light than in blue? Perception, 1977, volume 6, pages 7-14 Do monkeys' subjective clocks run faster in red light than in blue? Nicholas K Humphrey, Graham R Keeble Sub-Department of Animal Behaviour, University of Cambridge,

More information

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations?

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? Keyed. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2002, 55B (1), 1 25 Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? J.H. Wearden, A. Parry, and L. Stamp University of

More information

Perceptual Plasticity in Spatial Auditory Displays

Perceptual Plasticity in Spatial Auditory Displays Perceptual Plasticity in Spatial Auditory Displays BARBARA G. SHINN-CUNNINGHAM, TIMOTHY STREETER, and JEAN-FRANÇOIS GYSS Hearing Research Center, Boston University Often, virtual acoustic environments

More information

Sensation is the conscious experience associated with an environmental stimulus. It is the acquisition of raw information by the body s sense organs

Sensation is the conscious experience associated with an environmental stimulus. It is the acquisition of raw information by the body s sense organs Sensation is the conscious experience associated with an environmental stimulus. It is the acquisition of raw information by the body s sense organs Perception is the conscious experience of things and

More information

Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm

Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2008, 15 (6), 1111-1116 doi:10.3758/pbr.15.6.1111 Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm Mei-Ching Lien Oregon

More information

REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l

REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l Journal oj Experimental Psychology 12, Vol. 63, No. 3, 289-293 REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l MAURICE HERSHENSON 2 Brooklyn College In measuring reaction time (RT) to simultaneously

More information

ENVIRONMENTAL REINFORCEMENT LEARNING: A Real-time Learning Architecture for Primitive Behavior Refinement

ENVIRONMENTAL REINFORCEMENT LEARNING: A Real-time Learning Architecture for Primitive Behavior Refinement ENVIRONMENTAL REINFORCEMENT LEARNING: A Real-time Learning Architecture for Primitive Behavior Refinement TaeHoon Anthony Choi, Eunbin Augustine Yim, and Keith L. Doty Machine Intelligence Laboratory Department

More information

Toward an objective measure for a stream segregation task

Toward an objective measure for a stream segregation task Toward an objective measure for a stream segregation task Virginia M. Richards, Eva Maria Carreira, and Yi Shen Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, 3151 Social Science Plaza,

More information

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention Chapter 6 Attention Attention William James, in 1890, wrote Everyone knows what attention is. Attention is the taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several simultaneously

More information

A Multimodal Paradigm for Investigating the Perisaccadic Temporal Inversion Effect in Vision

A Multimodal Paradigm for Investigating the Perisaccadic Temporal Inversion Effect in Vision A Multimodal Paradigm for Investigating the Perisaccadic Temporal Inversion Effect in Vision Leo G. Trottier (leo@cogsci.ucsd.edu) Virginia R. de Sa (desa@cogsci.ucsd.edu) Department of Cognitive Science,

More information

RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES

RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES by John W. Brelsford, Jr. and Richard C. Atkinson TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 July 21, 1967 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES!, Reproduction

More information

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION Jordan Schoenherr, Craig Leth-Steensen, and William M. Petrusic psychophysics.lab@gmail.com, craig_leth_steensen@carleton.ca, bpetrusi@carleton.ca Carleton

More information

Complementarity and the Relation Between Psychological and Neurophysiological Phenomena

Complementarity and the Relation Between Psychological and Neurophysiological Phenomena the Relation Between Psychological and Neurophysiological Phenomena Douglas M. Snyder Berkeley, California ABSTRACT In their recent article, Kirsch and Hyland questioned the relation between psychological

More information

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings J. Besle, P. Lakatos, C.A. Schevon, R.R. Goodman, G.M. McKhann, A. Mehta, R.G. Emerson, C.E.

More information

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons From M. L. Commons, R.J. Herrnstein, & A.R. Wagner (Eds.). 1983. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: Discrimination Processes. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger (Vol. IV, Chapter 1, pages 3-19). A Memory Model for

More information

Neurobiology of Hearing (Salamanca, 2012) Auditory Cortex (2) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang

Neurobiology of Hearing (Salamanca, 2012) Auditory Cortex (2) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang Neurobiology of Hearing (Salamanca, 2012) Auditory Cortex (2) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang Laboratory of Auditory Neurophysiology Department of Biomedical Engineering Johns Hopkins University web1.johnshopkins.edu/xwang

More information

The basic hearing abilities of absolute pitch possessors

The basic hearing abilities of absolute pitch possessors PAPER The basic hearing abilities of absolute pitch possessors Waka Fujisaki 1;2;* and Makio Kashino 2; { 1 Graduate School of Humanities and Sciences, Ochanomizu University, 2 1 1 Ootsuka, Bunkyo-ku,

More information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL 1 SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL Response time and signal detection time distributions SM Fig. 1. Correct response time (thick solid green curve) and error response time densities (dashed red curve), averaged across

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF BRAIN AREAS

REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF BRAIN AREAS REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF BRAIN AREAS Franziska Kopp* #, Erich Schröger* and Sigrid Lipka # *University of Leipzig, Institute of General Psychology # University of Leipzig,

More information

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization Acoust. Sci. & Tech. 24, 2 (23) PAPER The role of low components in median plane localization Masayuki Morimoto 1;, Motoki Yairi 1, Kazuhiro Iida 2 and Motokuni Itoh 1 1 Environmental Acoustics Laboratory,

More information