ARTICLE IN PRESS Biological Psychology xxx (2012) xxx xxx

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "ARTICLE IN PRESS Biological Psychology xxx (2012) xxx xxx"

Transcription

1 G Model ARTICLE IN PRESS Biological Psychology xxx (2012) xxx xxx Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Biological Psychology journa l h o me page: Emotion, Etmnooi, or Emitoon? Faster lexical access to emotional than to neutral words during reading Johanna Kissler a,b,, Cornelia Herbert a,c a Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany b Department of Psychology, University of Bielefeld, Bielefeld, Germany c Department of Psychology, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 3 August 2011 Accepted 26 September 2012 Available online xxx Keywords: Emotion Attention Word processing Lexical access ERPs Early posterior negativity a b s t r a c t Cortical processing of emotional words differs from that of neutral words. Using EEG event-related potentials (ERPs), the present study examines the functional stage(s) of this differentiation. Positive, negative, and neutral nouns were randomly mixed with pseudowords and letter strings derived from words within each valence and presented for reading while participants EEG was recorded. Results indicated emotion effects in the N1 ( ms), early posterior negativity (EPN, ) and late positive potential (LPP, ms) time windows. Across valence, orthographic word-form effects occurred from about 180 ms after stimulus presentation. Crucially, in emotional words, lexicality effects (real words versus pseudowords) were identified from 216 ms, words being more negative over posterior cortex, coinciding with EPN effects, whereas neutral words differed from pseudowords only after 320 ms. Emotional content affects word processing at pre-lexical, lexical and post-lexical levels, but remarkably lexical access to emotional words is faster than access to neutral words Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Visual processing of emotional words differs from visual processing of neutral words. Using electroencephalographic eventrelated potentials (ERP) emotional neutral differences have been identified at various temporal stages following word onset in lexical-decision, evaluation, or reading tasks (for review see Kissler et al., 2006). Several ERP studies have described more negativegoing ERPs for emotional words over occipital cortex between 200 and 300 ms (Herbert et al., 2008; Kissler et al., 2007; Kissler et al., 2009; Scott et al., 2009) or between 300 and 400 ms (Palazova et al., 2011; Schacht and Sommer, 2009a, 2009b) after word presentation. Collectively, these effects are referred to as emotion driven early posterior negativities (EPN). Morphologically analogous effects have been reported in emotional face (Schupp et al., 2004), scene (Junghofer et al., 2001), or gesture processing (Flaisch et al., 2011), and in explicit object-based attention tasks (Hillyard and Anllo-Vento, 1998; Schoenfeld et al., 2007), implying that early emotion effects in this time window reflect attentional highlighting of emotional stimuli in general (Schupp et al., 2006). Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, University of Bielefeld, Postfach , Bielefeld, Germany. address: johanna.kissler@uni-bielefeld.de (J. Kissler). Larger parietal positivities arising around 500 ms after word onset have also been reported in emotion word processing (Fischler and Bradley, 2006; Herbert et al., 2006; Schacht and Sommer, 2009b), as well as in the processing of emotional scenes, faces, or gestures. Again, parallel effects are found in explicit attention tasks. Occasionally, effects of emotional content on the N400 component, a classic index of contextual semantic integration, have been found (Herbert et al., 2008; Kiehl et al., 1999), suggesting facilitated semantic integration of emotional words after a first initial attentional processing stage. Also, emotion effects arising earlier than 200 ms after word onset have been documented (Begleiter and Platz, 1969; Hofmann et al., 2009; Ortigue et al., 2004; Skrandies, 1998; Skrandies et al., 1998). These very early effects, in particular, support the view that emotional processing can operate even pre-attentively, outside the boundaries set by other informationprocessing mechanisms, as proposed by automatic vigilance (Pratto and John, 1991) or automatic evaluation models (Zajonc, 1980). In the context of language processing, such very early effects suggest the possibility of pre-lexical responses to emotional content. At least under conditions of low to moderate competing cognitive load, emotional stimuli, including words, are preferentially processed. Still, it is debated whether ERP emotion effects are specific to a particular category or dimension of emotion. Whereas much data suggest a major impact of emotional intensity, i.e. stimulus arousal, on emotion word processing (Fischler and Bradley, 2006; Kissler et al., 2007; Schacht and Sommer, 2009b), some /$ see front matter 2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

2 ARTICLE IN PRESS G Model 2 J. Kissler, C. Herbert / Biological Psychology xxx (2012) xxx xxx results indicate specific effects for negative (Bernat et al., 2001) or positive words (Kissler and Koessler, 2011). These discrepancies are reflected in different theoretical accounts. According to one model, rapid automatic allocation of attention specifically to negative stimuli is biologically adaptive in facilitating rapid withdrawal from potentially dangerous environments. Therefore this mechanism may by-pass other cognitive processes (Pratto and John, 1991). Alternatively, attentional orienting to emotionally arousing stimuli in general may be needed to mobilize appropriate approach or avoidance behavior (Lang et al., 1997). Finally, appraisal theory proposes a cascade of processing steps where sequential checks influenced by situational demands determine the patterning of emotional responses, allowing for more flexibility and perhaps reconciling some empirical discrepancies (Grandjean and Scherer, 2008; Scherer, 2009). As outlined above, ERP effects of emotion in vision are often assumed to reflect spontaneous attentional highlighting of motivationally relevant emotional stimuli. This assumption is supported by analogous ERP effects in object-based selective attention tasks and in line with the general concept of emotionally motivated attention (Lang et al., 1997). Attention and emotion can affect the processing of perceptual objects, including words, at various temporal stages (Luck and Hillyard, 1999; Ruz and Nobre, 2008; Schupp et al., 2007; Vogel et al., 2005; Ziegler et al., 1997). Still, it is controversial, how much perceptual and cognitive processing has to be carried out, before a stimulus emotional significance is identified and how early in the processing stream emotion effects can occur. In other words, an important question still is: How much, if any, inference do preferences need? In emotion word processing the question can be put as: When do emotion effects occur in relation to different processing stages in the mental lexicon? To answer this question, emotion effects can be pitched against a time-line of word recognition derived from models of visual word processing. According to classic serial word processing models (e.g. Coltheart et al., 2001; McClelland and Rumelhart, 1981), increasingly abstract information, including orthographic word form and phonological and lexical properties of a word, is extracted from the visual percept as activation travels from primary visual to higher order, multimodal, association areas of the brain, where information is combined to achieve full comprehension of a word s meaning (for review see Dien, 2009). Word-processing stages are assumed to partly overlap and to be organized in an interactive and cascaded fashion, but the extent of parallel processing is a matter of considerable debate (Barber and Kutas, 2007; Grainger and Holcomb, 2009; Pulvermuller et al., 2009). Traditional models agree that some perceptual invariance has to be extracted from the physical signal, before a word s meaning can be accessed. It is traditionally assumed that orthographic processes take place within the first 250 ms, followed by lexical and semantic access from about 300 ms after stimulus onset, culminating in full semantic contextual integration around 400 ms (Grainger and Holcomb, 2009). However, some models argue for a considerably higher speed of word recognition with parallel or near parallel processing of different attributes and present evidence for semantic processing even within the first 200 ms (e.g. Pulvermuller et al., 2009). Because models and data differ regarding the speed and stages of word processing itself, identification of the locus of emotion effects along an empirically determined time-line of word processing is needed to inform models of word recognition and emotional processing. From a linguistic perspective, the extent to which emotional content can accelerate or by-pass other stages of word processing has implications for models of lexical access. Acceleration of word processing by emotion is suggested by consistent evidence of faster lexical decisions to emotional than to neutral words (Kousta et al., 2009; Schacht and Sommer, 2009b), but in lexical-decision emotional response facilitation may also play a role (Kissler and Koessler, 2011). Moreover, models such as the automatic vigilance model, developed from experiments with word stimuli, assume that emotion processing can operate preattentively (Pratto and John, 1991), suggesting the existence of short-cut routes, by-passing stages of perceptual analysis. A linguistic challenge is to account for such effects within models of word recognition and to determine whether they are unique to emotion words or also occur for other semantic classes, and whether they could be mimicked by attention manipulations. From the perspective of emotion theory, data will provide information regarding the temporal evolution of emotion cognition interactions and the direction of emotion effects at distinct processing stages. On-line measures such as ERPs can reveal such sequences that might be obscured in lexical-decision reaction times alone, which represent a compound measure, integrating the results of many different operations. So far, two ERP studies investigated the pre-, peri-, or postlexical status of emotion effects in word processing. Scott et al. (Scott et al., 2009) varied emotional content within high- and lowfrequency words in a lexical-decision task. Since frequency effects in visual word processing are considered indicative of lexical access, word frequency by emotion interactions was identified to determine the functional status of emotion effects. Interactions occurred on the N1 around 150 ms and in the subsequent EPN window ( ms). These N1 and EPN effects were assumed to indicate modulations of lexical access by emotional content. An earlier P1 amplitude reduction for negative words was interpreted as a pre-lexical perceptual defense mechanism. Palazova et al. (2011) examined ERP emotion effects in high- and low-frequency adjectives, verbs, and nouns, likewise using a lexical-decision task. Results confirmed an early frequency effect around ms which interacted with emotion. Word pseudoword differences were found between 250 and 550 ms. Crucially, main effects of emotion were reflected by an EPN potential and a centro-parietal positivity (LPP), whose temporal onset largely coincided (EPN) or followed (LPP) word pseudoword differentiation, supporting a lexical or post-lexical status of emotion effects. The present study further investigates the status of emotion effects along the time-line of word processing using a silent reading paradigm: For adults, silent reading is a highly automatic and very natural process (e.g. Kahneman and Chajczyk, 1983). Simultaneously measuring on-going brain activity during reading can reveal emotional modulations irrespective of additional effects of response preparation and execution, which may differ from wordprocessing-related activity itself. Lexical-decision reaction times are consistently faster for emotion words, with some controversy surrounding the relative role of valence versus arousal (Estes and Adelman, 2008; Kousta et al., 2009). However, in lexical decision many different processes could be affected by emotion. Moreover, lexical decision, while highly successful in identifying word nonword differences, draws explicit attention to a stimulus lexical status, thereby potentially affecting lexicality or emotion effects. In fact, Ziegler et al. (1997) demonstrated that the time course of lexical and semantic activation in word processing is task-dependent, with faster ERP word pseudoword differentiation in a semantic categorization task than during letter search. Here, emotion effects in reading are investigated in relation to effects of orthography and lexicality to determine the temporal and functional stage(s) of effects. We present positive, negative, and neutral nouns intermixed with letter strings and pseudowords to identify successively more refined processing stages. Under a serial model, differentiation of orthographically legal word forms (words or pseudowords) from illegal words forms (letter strings) should precede differentiation of real words (with a corresponding lexical

3 G Model ARTICLE IN PRESS J. Kissler, C. Herbert / Biological Psychology xxx (2012) xxx xxx 3 Table 1 Means and standard deviations (in brackets) of different stimulus dimensions. Word stimuli Positive Neutral Negative Valence 7.46 (.92) 5.20 (.36) 2.02 (.72) Arousal 5.84 (1.02) 2.18 (.77) 5.72 (.79) Concreteness 4.55 (1.16) 4.05 (2.04) 4.20 (1.42) Word Length 7.28 (2.70) 7.13 (1.97) 6.83 (1.95) Word Frequency (144.01) (20.91) (173.05) entry) from pseudowords (without lexical entry). Emotion effects will be assessed along the time-line of such effects. Our previous studies have used the rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) technique to study emotion effects in word processing. Although effects have been largely replicated in lexical-decision tasks (Hinojosa et al., 2010; Palazova et al., 2011; Schacht and Sommer, 2009a, 2009b; Scott et al., 2009), RSVP with its absence of baseline periods and inherent conceptual masking, may create a special experimental situation. Here, we examine emotion effects in single word reading outside RSVP. In particular, we address the timing of emotion effects in relation to orthographic word-form analysis and lexical access in positive, negative, and neutral words, investigating the possibility that emotional content may accelerate or by-pass some of these processes. 2. Methods 2.1. Participants Twenty-four native German students (12 women) from the University of Konstanz, Germany, took part in the experiment. Mean age was (SE =.62) years. All were right-handed according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971). They reported no history of neurological or psychiatric disease and their vision was normal or corrected to normal. Subjects signed written informed consent forms and received either a financial bonus of 7.50 D ( $) or course credit for their participation Material 138 German nouns (46 high arousing positive, 46 high arousing negative, 46 neutral) were used. From these nouns, for each of the three emotion categories, 46 pseudowords and 46 letter strings were created by within- and between-word permutation, resulting in a total of 138 words, 138 pseudowords and 138 letter strings as experimental stimuli. Positive and negative nouns differed in valence but not in arousal. They differed from neutral nouns in both valence and arousal. Words were matched across emotion categories for concreteness, length and word frequency and did not differ in any of these. Arousal, valence, and concreteness values were based on self-collected nine-point scale ratings given by 45 students who did not take part in this experiment. Frequency of written words was determined from the standardized word-database CELEX (Baayen et al., 1995) (see Table 1 for a summary of stimulus characteristics). Pseudowords were based on the original words within each emotion category and were generated by letter permutations within and between words to preclude simple perceptual repair and at the same time maintain compliance to orthographic and phonotactic rules of German. Original word length was never altered. Letter strings were likewise generated by within- and between-word letter permutation, but now violated German orthographic and phonotactic rules. The words, pseudowords, and letter strings, used in the experiment are listed in Appendix A Procedure Participants were familiarized with the laboratory, handedness was assessed (Oldfield, 1971) and they were asked about their past and current health using a standardized questionnaire. They were then seated in an electrically shielded room and a geodesic net containing 256 EEG electrodes was positioned on their head (GSN 200 v2.0; EGI: Electrical Geodesics, Inc., Eugene, Oregon). Participants were informed that they were taking part in a word-processing study and that their task was to read a sequence of words and word-like stimuli while their EEG was being recorded. They were instructed to refrain from head and eye movements during stimulation as much as possible and attend to each stimulus for its entire presentation time. Stimuli were presented for 600 ms, separated by a pseudo-randomly varying inter-stimulus interval of ms during which a fixation cross was shown. Stimulus sequence was constrained such that overall transition probability was equal across stimuli and that no more than three exemplars of the same Stimulus Type (word, pseudoword, letter string) appeared in sequence EEG recording and analysis EEG was recorded from 256 channels using EGI amplifiers and Netstation software (GSN 200 v2.0; EGI: Electrical Geodesics, Inc., Eugene, Oregon). Recording bandwidth was Hz; sampling rate was 250 Hz. To reduce mains interference, a 50 Hz notch filter was used. Impedance was held beneath 50 k and Cz was used as recording reference. Off-line, data were re-referenced to an average reference and band-pass filtered between 0.1 and 35 Hz. Eye movement and blink artifacts were corrected using the correction algorithm implemented in BESA (Brain Electrical Source Analysis, MEGIS Software GmbH). Remaining artifacts were reduced by individual channel-interpolation or the epochs were rejected (max. 10%). Data were segmented from 100 to 700 ms, baseline corrected using the first 100 ms, and averaged according to condition EEG analysis The Matlab-based EMEGs v2.2 package (Peyk et al., 2011) was used for ERP visualization and analysis. To specify time windows and regions of interest, point-wise ANOVAs were conducted across all time-points and sensors. In a first step, differences between positive, negative, and neutral words were determined to replicate previous effects. Then, effects of Stimulus Type (word, pseudoword, letter string) were examined within each individual valence to assess orthographic and lexical processing stages for each valence, fully controlling for basic perceptual characteristics (note that pseudowords and letter strings were created by permutation within individual valence). Separate analyses were conducted to account for the possibility that the temporal dynamics of lexical processes differ between neutral, negative, and positive words. Results were considered meaningful, if effects remained significant at a significance level of p <.01 for at least 8 sample points (32 ms) and could be seen, without additional spatial or temporal smoothing, in a cluster of at least 10 electrodes (Kissler and Koessler, 2011). Main effects were decomposed using linear contrasts or quadratic trends, depending on the hypothesis to be tested. Statistical maps for contrasts were interpreted only in time windows that first revealed significant topographically overlapping effects in the ANOVA. Such effects are marked with black boxes in the figures, although the statistical maps for the contrasts are shown across the entire time window to provide full information. Additionally, based on regions and time windows of interest typically reported in the literature and identified in the point-wise ANOVAs, effects were confirmed using ANOVAs assessing the effect of Valence (positive, negative, neutral) in words and the effect of Stimulus Type (Words, Pseudowords, Letter strings) within valence. ANOVAs compared mean activity in clusters of sensors covering regions and time windows of interests and were decomposed using t-test. The same clusters were used across valences, and sensor clusters were kept constant across effects as much as possible. Where appropriate according to previous reports, Hemisphere was included as a factor, comparing ERPs at two symmetrical left and right hemispheric electrode clusters. Sensor clusters are listed in Appendix B. 3. Results 3.1. Emotion in words Point-wise ANOVA Fig. 1 displays p-maps of the time course and topography for the main effect of Emotion in nouns (Fig. 1a), for the quadratic trend comparing emotional (positive and negative) with neutral nouns, testing the hypothesis that arousal accounts for emotional neutral differences in reading (Fig. 1b), and the linear contrast testing for valence-specific differences by comparing positive and negative nouns (Fig. 1c). ERP difference topographies for these effects are shown in Fig. 2 for negative minus neutral (a) and positive minus neutral (b) and positive minus negative nouns (c). ANOVA revealed a first significant effect between 108 and 140 ms over left centro-occipital scalp. This effect is apparent both in the ANOVA (Fig. 1a) and in the quadratic trend comparing emotional with neutral words (Fig. 1 b), but not in the linear contrast between positive and negative stimuli (Fig. 1c), indicating mainly an arousal effect. Still, the difference topographies show it to be driven mainly by negative nouns eliciting more negative-going ERPs than neutral nouns between 108 and 140 ms (Fig. 2a). Further emotion effects evolve from 216 ms after word onset, extending throughout the entire analysis window (Fig. 1a and b). They start over left lateral scalp for both negative and positive nouns compared to neutral ones and are due to a more pronounced posterior negativity and concomitant frontal positivity for

4 ARTICLE IN PRESS G Model 4 J. Kissler, C. Herbert / Biological Psychology xxx (2012) xxx xxx Fig. 1. Probability maps from 0 to 680 ms in steps of 32 ms (on average p <.01 for at least 32 ms) depicting the effects of emotional content on ERPs during reading. (a) Point-wise ANOVA comparing positive, negative, and neutral words. (b) Quadratic trends comparing ERPs elicited by positive and negative with neutral words. (c) t-tests contrasting positive and negative words. Black frames mark time windows that were significant already in the ANOVA and can therefore be interpreted in the post hoc comparison. emotional than for neutral words (Fig. 2a and b). This process mirrors the previously described EPN effect for emotional words. Directly comparing ERPs to positive and to negative words (Figs. 1c and 2c) reveals that from about 400 ms positive words elicit more right occipito-parietal positivity than negative or neutral words. This replicates the previously documented LPP effect, presently restricted to positive content Spatio-temporal cluster ANOVA: emotion effect in words ms Emotion was significant at two bilateral occipito-parietal electrode groups (F(2, 46) = 4.15, p <.05) due to more negative-going brain potentials for negative than for neutral words (p <.05). The other categories did not differ and although the effect appeared somewhat left lateralized, the interaction was far from significant (F < 1) ms Further Emotion effects (F(2, 46) = 4.84, p <.05) arose at two groups of occipito-parietal electrodes between 216 and 320 ms with both positive and negative words differing from neutral ones (ps <.05), but not from each other (F < 1). ERPs were generally more negative over the left hemisphere (F(1, 23) = 6.53, p <.05), but this did not interact with valence (F(2, 46) = 1.28, p <.19) ms Emotion further impacted on visual processing between 324 and 392 ms (F(2, 46) = 3.12, p =.05) due to both negative (p <.05) and positive (p <.05) words differing from neutral words. There was no effect of Hemisphere or an interaction between Emotion and Hemisphere (both p >.2) ms A centro-parietal effect of emotion (F(2, 46) = 4.72, p <.05) emerged between 468 and 608 ms. This was due to more positivegoing ERPs for positive than for negative words (p <.05). The effect differed between the hemispheres (F(2, 46) = 4.64, p <.05). Over the left hemisphere, both positive and neutral words had more positive-going ERPs than negative words (ps <.05), whereas over the right hemisphere positive words had more positive going ERPs than both positive and neutral words (ps <.01), the latter also eliciting more positivity than negative words (p <.05) Effects of Stimulus Type: neutral valence Point-wise ANOVA To determine the functional stage(s) at which emotion effects occur in neutral, negative, or positive words, the effect of Stimulus Type (Word, Pseudoword, Letter string) was analyzed within each valence. Fig. 3a displays the result of a point-wise ANOVA

5 G Model ARTICLE IN PRESS J. Kissler, C. Herbert / Biological Psychology xxx (2012) xxx xxx 5 Fig. 2. Topographic difference maps of ERPs elicited by words with different emotional content from 0 to 680 ms in steps of 32 ms. (a) Negative words minus neutral words. (b) Positive words minus neutral words. (c) Positive words minus negative words. Black frames mark time windows where significant effects were found in the respective statistical comparisons. comparing ERPs elicited by neutral words with pseudowords and letter strings Fig. 3b and c display the p-maps for the linear contrasts. First effects of Stimulus Type are seen over inferior occipital scalp between 180 and 248 ms. Further effects arise over right occipital scalp from 288 ms, extending bilaterally and more superior from 324 ms. Centro-parietal effects of Stimulus Type are seen from around 432 ms. The underlying difference topographies are shown in Figs. 6a and 7a Pseudowords versus letter strings. Differentiation between pseudowords and letter strings, reflecting a formal analysis stage, is first seen between 180 and 249 ms after stimulus onset (Fig. 3c). Pseudowords elicit more positive-going ERPs over bilateral inferior occipital scalp (Fig. 7a). A second significant difference between pseudowords and letter strings arises from 288 ms as a prolonged right occipital negativity for pseudowords compared to letter strings successively extending over the entire back of the brain (Fig. 7a). Finally, from 540 ms a left frontal positivity accompanies the retreating right occipital negativity (Fig. 7a) Words versus pseudowords. Linear contrasts reveal a difference between neutral words and pseudowords starting from 324 ms (Fig. 3b) as a left occipital negativity and right frontal positivity (Fig. 6a). This difference extends until 500 ms (Fig. 3b), evolving from a left occipito-temporal negativity into a central positivity for neutral words, resembling the N400 effect. This process extends for several hundred milliseconds, retreating over occipital cortex, where it reaches significance again, between 576 and 608 ms. Finally, a slightly left frontal negativity develops for words compared to pseudowords, significant between 648 and 680 ms after stimulus onset Spatio-temporal cluster ANOVA: effect of Stimulus Type for neutral valence To further validate the above effects, a region of interest analysis was conducted for individual time windows for the first 500 ms. Figs. 6a and 7a depict the underlying topographical differences. For consistency and because of considerable spatial overlap in the effects, all posterior effects were analyzed at the same channel groups as the above emotion effects ms An effect of Stimulus Type (F(2, 46) = 9.68, p <.01) was identified at two bilateral inferior-occipital electrode groups. It was due to more positive-going brain potentials for neutral words and pseudowords than for letter strings (p <.01 and p =.01, respectively). Words and pseudowords did not differ (p >.5) ms Stimulus Type (F(2, 46) = 5.25, p <.01) was significant: Pseudowords, and in tendency also words, were more positive than letter strings (p <.05 and p =.07, respectively, Fig. 7a).

6 ARTICLE IN PRESS G Model 6 J. Kissler, C. Herbert / Biological Psychology xxx (2012) xxx xxx Fig. 3. Probability maps from 0 to 680 ms in steps of 32 ms (on average p <.01 for at least 32 ms) depicting the effects of Stimulus Type on ERPs during reading of neutral words, pseudoword, or letter strings derived from neutral words. (a) Point-wise ANOVA comparing words, pseudowords, and letter strings. (b) t-tests comparing words and pseudowords. (c) t-tests contrasting pseudowords and letter strings. Black frames mark time windows that were significant already in the ANOVA and can therefore be interpreted in the post hoc comparison ms A significant effect of Hemisphere was due to more negativegoing potentials over the left hemisphere (F(1, 23) = 5.99, p <.05), but no effects of Stimulus Type (F < 1) or interactions were found (F < 1) ms A main effect of Stimulus Type (F(2, 46) = 8.86, p <.01) occurred with more negative-going potentials for words than for letter strings (p <.01). Pseudowords were likewise somewhat more negative than letter strings (p =.08) and did not differ from words (p >.2). ERPs were somewhat more negative over the left hemisphere (F(1, 23) = 2.82, p =.1), but the interaction was far from significant (F <.1) ms Between 324 and 392 ms a further effect of Stimulus Type emerged (F(2,46) = 13.43; p <.01), now differentiating words from pseudowords (Fig. 6a). Words were more negative than both pseudowords (p <.05) and letter strings (p <.01). The latter did not differ. Effects were somewhat larger over left than over right occipital areas (F(2, 46) = 2.64, p =.08) ms Finally, a centro-parietal effect was tested in the typical N400 time range. This yielded a main effect of Stimulus Type (F(2,46) = 10.12; p < 0.01). Figs. 6a and 7a) reveal that pseudowords were associated with more negative-going ERPs than both words (p <.01) and letter strings (p <.05). In tendency, letter strings were also more negative than words (p =.06) Effects of Stimulus Type: negative valence Point-wise ANOVA Fig. 4a displays the results of a point-wise ANOVA comparing ERPs for negative words and pseudowords and letter strings. First effects of Stimulus Type are seen over right inferior occipital scalp from 180 ms. From 216 ms these effect extend over the left hemisphere, spreading over bilateral occipital and frontal scalp by 392 ms. From 396 ms effects are seen fronto-centrally, lasting until about 540 ms. Underlying topographical differences are shown in Figs. 6b and 7b Pseudowords versus letter strings. Pseudowords and letter strings first differ right-occipitally from 180 after stimulus onset (Fig. 4 c). By 284 ms, more positive going ERPs are elicited by pseudowords, primarily over right inferior occipital scalp (Fig. 7 b). From 288 ms a prolonged bilateral occipital negativity for pseudowords compared to letter strings successively extends over the entire back of the brain (Fig. 7b). Finally, from 540 ms a left frontal positivity accompanies the retreating right occipital negativity (Fig. 7a).

7 G Model ARTICLE IN PRESS J. Kissler, C. Herbert / Biological Psychology xxx (2012) xxx xxx 7 Fig. 4. Probability maps from 0 to 680 ms in steps of 32 ms (on average p <.01 for at least 32 ms) depicting the effects of Stimulus Type on ERPs during reading of negative words, pseudoword, or letter strings derived from negative words. (a) Point-wise ANOVA comparing words, pseudowords, and letter strings. (b) t-tests comparing words and pseudowords. (c) t-tests contrasting pseudowords and letter strings. Black frames mark time windows that were significant already in the ANOVA and can therefore be interpreted in the post hoc comparison Words versus pseudowords. Differentiation between negative words and pseudowords starts from 252 ms (Fig. 4 b) as a left occipital negativity and left frontal positivity (Fig. 6b) lasting until 356 ms. From 360 ms a left frontal positivity develops that gradually extends posterior, spreading over much of the central scalp by 432 ms and by 536 ms retreating to left frontal regions. This process resembles the N400 effect Spatio-temporal cluster ANOVAs: effect of Stimulus Type for negative valence To further validate the above effects, a regions of interest analysis was conducted for the same sensor clusters and time windows as above. Topographical differences underlying these effects are shown in Figs. 6b and 7b ms A first effect of Stimulus Type (F(2, 46) = 3.79, p <.05) was due to somewhat more negative-going brain potentials for letter strings than for negative words and pseudowords (p <.1 and p <.2, respectively). Words and pseudowords did not differ (F < 1) ms No clear effects of Stimulus Type (F(2, 46) = 2.18, p =.12) or Hemisphere (F(2, 23) = 2.82, p =.11) and no interaction between the two (F(2, 46) = 1.4, p =.26) were observed ms A main effect of Stimulus Type (F(2, 46) = 3.93, p <.05) was found, with more negative-going potentials for words than for pseudowords (p <.05). The effect of Hemisphere (F(2, 23) = 2.49, p =.13) or the interaction between Stimulus Type and Hemisphere (F(2, 46) = 1.49, p =.23) were not significant ms ERPs differed according to Stimulus Type (F(2, 46) = 14.26, p <.01) with more negative-going potentials for words than for pseudowords (p <.05) and letter strings (p <.01). The effect of Hemisphere (F(2, 23) = 2.50, p =.13) or the interaction between Stimulus Type and Hemisphere (F < 1) were not significant ms Stimulus Type was significant (F(2,46) = 18.38; p <.01) due to words being more negative than both pseudowords (p <.05) and letter strings (p <.01). Pseudowords were also more negative than letter strings (p <.05). The hemispheres did not differ significantly (F(1, 23) = 2.64, p =.08) and the interaction was far from significant (F < 1) ms Finally, a centro-parietal, N400-like effect, was tested. This yielded a main effect of Stimulus Type (F(2,46) = 5.65; p < 0.01). Pseudowords were associated with more negative-going ERPs than

8 ARTICLE IN PRESS G Model 8 J. Kissler, C. Herbert / Biological Psychology xxx (2012) xxx xxx Fig. 5. Probability maps from 0 to 680 ms in steps of 32 ms (on average p <.01 for at least 32 ms) depicting the effects of Stimulus Type on ERPs during reading of positive words, pseudoword, or letter strings derived from negative words. (a) Point-wise ANOVA comparing words, pseudowords, and letter strings. (b) t-tests comparing words and pseudowords. (c) t-tests contrasting pseudowords and letter strings. Black frames mark time windows that were significant already in the ANOVA and can therefore be interpreted in the post hoc comparison. words (p <.01) and letter strings (p =.01). The latter did not differ (p >.2) Effects of Stimulus Type: positive valence Point-wise ANOVA Fig. 5a displays the result of a point-wise ANOVA comparing the cortical processing of positive words and pseudowords and letter strings. Fig. 6b illustrates the underlying topographic differences. Initial effects of Stimulus Type are seen over right frontal scalp from 216 ms. From 252 ms additional effects are found over left occipital and right parietal scalp, gradually extending in frontal and parietal regions until 356 ms. In the following time windows, effects gradually diminish and finally disappear by 612 ms Pseudowords versus Letter strings. Differentiation between pseudowords and letter strings, reflecting a formal analysis stage, first starts right-frontally from 216 ms (Fig. 5c). Although the occipital negativity for letter strings observed for the neutral and negative valences is likewise visible (Fig. 7c), here it falls short of significance. Instead, a left frontal negativity is significant until 284 ms. Subsequently, an initially right dominant posterior negativity for pseudowords appears ( ms) gradually becoming left-dominant, and moving more anterior by 500 ms (Figs. 5c and 7c) Words versus pseudowords. Linear contrasts reveal that the differentiation between positive words and pseudowords starts from 216 ms (Fig. 5 b). It is first significant as a frontal positivity (Fig. 6 b). The concomitant left occipital negativity is visible in this time window, but only reaches significance from 252 ms, lasting until 392 ms (Fig. 6b). From 432 ms the frontal positivity starts to extend centrally, reverting to frontal regions by 572 ms, this process reflecting the N400 effect. An additional brief occipital positivity is seen between 576 and 608 ms Spatio-temporal cluster ANOVA: effect of Stimulus Type for positive valence To further validate the above effects, the same regionsof-interest analysis as above was conducted. Topographical differences underlying the reported effects are shown in Figs. 6c and 7c ms Although over the right hemisphere words and pseudowords were somewhat more positive-going than letter strings, neither a clear effect of Stimulus Type (F(2, 46) = 1.7, p <.2), nor of Hemisphere (p >.4) or an interaction (F(2, 46) = 2.13, p <.2) were found ms No effects of Stimulus Type (F(2, 46) = 1.42, p <.2) or Hemisphere (F(2, 23) = 1.81, p =.19) were found, but an interaction between

9 G Model ARTICLE IN PRESS J. Kissler, C. Herbert / Biological Psychology xxx (2012) xxx xxx 9 Fig. 6. Topographic difference maps of ERPs elicited by words versus pseudowords in different emotion categories from 0 to 680 ms in steps of 32 ms. (a) Neutral words minus pseudowords. (b) Negative words minus pseudowords. (c) Positive words minus pseudowords. Black frames mark time windows where significant effects were found in the respective statistical comparisons. Stimulus Type X Hemisphere (F(2, 46) = 5.1, p =.01 emerged. It indicated that over the left hemisphere words were more negative than pseudowords (p <.01), whereas over the right hemisphere words were more positive than letter strings (p <.05), not differing from pseudowords. Words were also more negative over the left than over the right hemisphere (p <.01), whereas pseudowords or letter strings showed no asymmetry ms A main effect of Stimulus Type (F(2, 46) = 4.86, p =.01), with more negative-going potentials for words than for pseudowords (p <.05), as well as a significant effect of Hemisphere (F(1, 23) = 6.93, p <.05), with more negativity over the left hemisphere, were identified. The interaction between Stimulus Type and Hemisphere (F(2, 46) = 6.07, p <.01) indicated more negative-going potentials over the left hemisphere elicited by words than by pseudowords or letter strings (p <.01). No such difference was observed over the right hemisphere (p >.2) ms A main effect of Stimulus Type (F(2, 46) = 16.93, p <.01) with more negative-going potentials for words than for pseudowords (p =.05) and letter strings (p <.01) as well as more negative-going potentials for pseudowords than for words (p <.05) were found. The effect of Hemisphere or the interaction between Stimulus Type and Hemisphere were not significant (F < 1) ms ERPs differed depending on Stimulus Types (F(2,46) = 16.48; p <.01), words being more negative than both pseudowords (p <.01) and letter strings (p <.01). The latter were not significantly different. The hemispheres did not differ (F(1, 23) = 1.60, p >.2) and the interaction was far from significant (F < 1) ms Finally, a centro-parietal N400-like effect yielded a main effect of Stimulus Type (F(2,46) = 4.52; p < 0.05). Pseudowords were associated with more negative-going ERPs than words (p <.05). Letter strings fell between words and pseudowords but did not differ significantly from either (both p >.1). 4. Discussion The present study investigated the time course and topography of emotion and lexicality effects in silent reading. It replicates and extends several findings about neural mechanisms of word processing, in general, and emotion word processing in particular. But in combination of the two, the study yields a remarkable new finding: Word pseudoword differentiation is faster for emotional than for neutral words, indicating faster lexical access to emotional than to neutral words. Replicating previous research, an EPN to emotional compared to neutral words was found between 200 and 300 ms after stimulus onset (Herbert et al., 2008; Hinojosa et al., 2010; Kissler et al.,

10 ARTICLE IN PRESS G Model 10 J. Kissler, C. Herbert / Biological Psychology xxx (2012) xxx xxx Fig. 7. Topographic difference maps of ERPs elicited by pseudowords versus letter strings derived from different emotion categories from 0 to 680 ms in steps of 32 ms. (a) Pseudowords and letter strings derived from neutral words. (b) Pseudowords and letter strings derived from negative words. (c) Pseudowords and letter strings derived from positive words. Black frames mark time windows where significant effects were found in the respective statistical comparisons. 2007; Scott et al., 2009). Also, a second posterior emotion effect was identified between 300 and 400 ms, similar to the effects previously reported by Schacht and her colleagues (Schacht and Sommer, 2009a, 2009b). Perhaps, this later part of the effect is attenuated and obscured in RSVP designs. Regarding the time-line of reading, an occipital positivity and a concomitant left frontal negativity were found for pseudowords from about 180 ms after stimulus onset, reflecting orthographical word-form processing. Orthographical word-form processing was assessed comparing pseudowords with letter strings so as not to confound it further with emotion effects carried by the words. Pseudoword letter string differentiation occurred as a bilateral posterior negativity for pseudowords by about 200 ms lasting until about 500 ms. There was some variation in the onset of the orthographical differentiation in that the effect became significant later (from 216 ms) for stimuli derived from positive words than from negative or neutral words (from 180 ms). The reason for this is currently unclear, but because the topographic differences between pseudowords and letter strings were similar for all three valences (Fig. 7), it could be due to random noise effects. Crucially, however, the differentiation between words and pseudowords, that is, between word forms that possess an actual lexical entry, and word forms that do not, was found to differ according to emotional content, occurring in the early EPN window ( ms) for emotional words, but only later for neutral words. In detail, in neutral words, word pseudoword differentiation occurred as a left occipito-temporal negativity from 324 ms after stimulus onset. By contrast, in negative words a left occipito-temporal negativity significantly differentiated word from pseudowords from 252 ms. In positive words the effect was significant already from 216 ms. Since pseudowords and letter strings were derived from the words within one valence, an influence of simple perceptual features is unlikely, since physical content did not vary between stimulus types (words, pseudowords, letter strings) within one valence. Thus, lexical access in reading was found to be faster in emotional than in neutral words. The timing and topography of this effect suggests that, at least in silent reading, a timing-difference in lexical access between emotional and neutral stimuli drives at least the early ( ms) emotion-word EPN effect. In other words, emotional content accelerates lexical access. This acceleration may contribute to the faster lexical decisions for emotional words seen in lexical-decision tasks (Kousta et al., 2009; Schacht and Sommer, 2009b). Whether the difference in onset between positive and negative words is meaningful and replicable is open to future research. While valence-dependent effects are sometimes found in lexical decisions, occasionally also with faster reaction times for positive words (e.g. Kissler and Koessler, 2011), arousal-dependent effects with acceleration for both positive and negative valence are more typical (Kousta et al., 2009). In any case, comparing the topographies of the effects, the differences between positive and negative words and pseudowords are clearly more similar regarding timing and topography than the difference between neutral words and pseudowords. Fig. 8 illustrates this major result of this experiment as ERP time course at

11 G Model ARTICLE IN PRESS J. Kissler, C. Herbert / Biological Psychology xxx (2012) xxx xxx 11 Fig. 8. ERP time course at selected sensors for emotional words (red), neutral words (blue), pseudowords (black solid), and letter strings (black dashed). To facilitate inspection of major effects, ERPs are collapsed across valence (positive and negative) for emotional words. Pseudowords and letter strings are collapsed across valences of the original words. six representative sensors, collapsing across negative and positive words. Pre- and post-lexical effects of emotional content were also observed: A posterior negativity driven primarily by negative words was observed already from about 110 ms after word onset. This early emotion effect may replicate the posterior N1 effect for negative words reported by Scott et al. (2009). N1 emotion effects are reported less consistently than later effects but are in line with findings of a negativity bias towards high arousing material (Cacioppo, 2004; Ito and Cacioppo, 2000). This negativity bias is also compatible with Pratto and John s (1991) automatic vigilance model. From an emotion-theory perspective, it would support the long-held view that emotional responses or preferences, need no inferences (Zajonc, 1980), or at least fewer inferences than other types of processing, suggesting that emotional processing can operate outside the boundaries of cognitive information-processing. At any rate, recent work by Schacht and her group actually provide evidence that very early, presumably pre-lexical, emotion effects in word processing can be induced by associating a previously meaningless symbol with reward value in an operant learning procedure (Schacht et al., 2012). However, since the negative words have somewhat, although not significantly, lower frequencies than positive and neutral words, a covert frequency effect also needs to be considered. In this regard, Scott et al. (2009) report stronger P1 and N1 effects for high frequency than for low frequency words, suggesting that subtle differences in word frequency, with somewhat less frequent negative words should have attenuated, rather than accentuated these effects. Consistent with previous literature, emotion effects were also found in a later time window, the LPP window, and in the present, as well as in some previous studies (Herbert et al., 2008; Kissler et al., 2009), the more pronounced posterior-parietal positivity was restricted to positive contents. Again, the possibility of subtle imbalances in non-emotional stimulus properties needs to be considered, precluding firm conclusions. However the entire timing pattern of effects lends itself to an interpretation in terms of a plausible adaptive processing sequence, perhaps most consistent with appraisal theory (Grandjean and Scherer, 2008): It may be advantageous to initially (even pre-lexically) rapidly and selectively process negative stimuli. At a mid-latency stage, which here has been shown to coincide with lexical access to emotional words (EPN), the organism may process arousing stimuli regardless of valence. Finally, at a post-lexical stage, pleasant stimuli may enjoy an advantage. Whereas rapid processing of negative contents may be survival critical, later extended evaluation of pleasurable material may be important for subjective well being. The most important finding of the present study is that lexical access for emotional words is faster than lexical access for neutral words. This merits further discussion in the context of the word-processing literature in general. There is wide agreement about the existence of distinct orthographic, lexical, and

12 ARTICLE IN PRESS G Model 12 J. Kissler, C. Herbert / Biological Psychology xxx (2012) xxx xxx semantic processing levels, presently modeled by pseudoword versus letter string, word versus pseudoword, and emotional content manipulations, respectively. Considerable debate, however, concerns the timing of different processing steps in visual word processing and the degree to which these steps occur in sequence, in parallel, or in interaction. Perhaps the most influential view assumes interactive and cascaded processing, allowing for both serial processing steps and their interaction via feedback mechanisms (Coltheart et al., 2001). Empirically, some data support very rapid initial activation of the mental lexicon, chiefly supported by eye-movement and ERP evidence from lexical decision. Word frequency effects around 150 ms are taken as perhaps the first indicators of lexical access (Sereno and Rayner, 2003; Sereno et al., 1998). Early word frequency by emotion interactions have also been reported in two previous studies (Palazova et al., 2011; Scott et al., 2009), but the present study did not manipulate this variable. Although the mental lexicon clearly uses perceptual frequency as an organizational principle to access its content, this is a rather low-level principle that may not be specific to words. For instance, orthographic typicality effects are highly similar for words and pseudowords and can also been found around 100 ms (Hauk et al., 2006). Using other experimental manipulations, magnetoencephalography (MEG) data indicate lexical processing considerably later, only around 350 ms after word onset (Pylkkanen and Marantz, 2003; Pylkkanen et al., 2002). The present results found lexicality effects as reflected by word pseudoword differentiation for neutral words from about 324 ms onwards, similar to the above MEG evidence. However, effects occurred for negative words from 252 ms and for positive words already from 216 ms. Palazova et al. (2011) report word pseudoword differentiation for nouns from 270 ms in a lexical-decision task, but included the emotional words in their word pseudoword comparison. If averaged across valence, both studies would yield a similar timing of lexical access. Present results suggest that part of the variability of reports on the timing of lexical access may be due to differential lexical access for different semantic classes. It is open to future research, whether such effects are restricted to emotional content or might also occur for other semantic classes, if these are explicitly attended to. Future studies will also elucidate, whether faster lexical access for emotional words is specific to reading designs. There is the possibility that intrinsically motivated attention to emotion speeds up processing, but that this acceleration disappears when attention is explicitly drawn to the lexicality dimension, as in lexical-decision tasks, consistent with evidence that the timing of word pseudoword differentiation in the ERP can vary with the task at hand (Ziegler et al., 1997). Alternatively, faster lexical access to emotional words may be a fairly general phenomenon that at least partly accounts for faster lexical-decision times for emotional words. The present results support, at least during initial processing phases, sequentially initiated, partly overlapping processing stages with an initial orthographic (from 180 ms) and a later lexical phase (from about 220 ms, depending on valence). A similar sequence was described across various tasks by Bentin et al. (Bentin et al., 1999). They found in early time windows major differences between words and pseudowords on the one hand and letter strings on the other hand and only comparatively late identified word pseudoword differences, peaking around 400 ms, when semantic integration is also assumed to occur. Previous reports have linked the emotion-word EPN effect to semantic processing and indeed, the time window that carries the lexicality effect in emotional words also, and by virtue of this, comprises an emotion-neutral differentiation. Indeed, responses to stimulus semantics could in general coincide with or precede other forms of lexical analysis or task requirements might alter the time course and perhaps also the general occurrence of attribute-specific activations in word processing. On the one hand, distinct responses to words of different semantic categories have been reported between 200 and 300 ms after word onset (Dehaene, 1995; Hauk et al., 2008; Hinojosa et al., 2004). On the other hand, under certain experimental manipulations that limit attentional resources even a total absence of semantic processing, which is otherwise assumed to be automatic during reading, has been observed (Smith et al., 2001). Because at least in non-competitive situations all kinds of emotional stimuli attract attention, and because emotional stimuli often signal the need for rapid action, they may result in differential brain responses even in situations where otherwise apparently very little higher level analysis occurs early on. Experimental test should demonstrate whether the EPN effect represents early semantic analysis and determine the degree to which it depends on attentional engagement and current motivational demands. Demands imposed by the experimenter, for instance in the form of attention to a specific semantic category, might be able to mimic effects currently found for emotional words that possess an intrinsic attentional and motivational relevance. Still, present results demonstrate that emotional content accelerates lexical access in silent reading tasks. Several later effects of both stimulus type and emotional content also appeared, again replicating and extending previous findings. Regarding stimulus type, from about 360 to 500 ms after stimulus presentation, more negative-going centro-parietal ERPs were observed for pseudowords, essentially replicating the well-known N400 effect, presumably indicating extended lexical search processes for word-like stimuli for which no lexical entry has yet been found. Finally, although not the focus of the present analyses, around 600 ms post stimulus, words elicited relatively more positivity over the left hemisphere than letter strings and pseudowords which appears consistent with a P600 effect in word reading. Although the P600 is mostly seen in syntactic tasks, a general attentional interpretation of late positive potentials would suggest that words, because they contain more information than pseudowords and letter strings attract more attentional resources, resulting in a larger P600. Also, effects of semantics on the P600 have been observed (van Herten et al., 2005). In parallel with the later effects of stimulus type, further effects of emotional content were also observed. A second posterior negativity for emotional words appeared between 320 and 420 ms. It showed a similar topography as the previous EPN and was likewise driven by arousal, differentiating between positive and negative words on the one hand and neutral words on the other hand. Regarding its timing and topography, this effect may resemble the EPN effect described by Schacht and colleagues who described a posterior negativity for emotional words emerging in lexicaldecision or evaluation tasks, similar to the EPN (Palazova et al., 2011; Schacht and Sommer, 2009a, 2009b). Perhaps this effect is obscured in the RSVP designs that we have previously used. At any rate, this second EPN effect parallels word pseudoword differentiation. Finally, a right-lateralized late positive potential revealed an emotion effect around 500 ms. Replicating previous research (Herbert et al., 2008; Herbert et al., 2006; Kissler et al., 2009; Schapkin et al., 2000), this positivity was more pronounced for positive words and differentiated them from both neutral and negative ones. In this time window a frontal positivity was also identified, being larger for emotionally arousing than for neutral words. The effect, which to our knowledge has not been described before, may reflect the frontal cortex s well-documented role in the evaluation of emotional stimuli (for review, see Etkin et al., 2011). Its identification may be due to the higher spatial sampling of the present compared to previous studies. In sum, the present emotion effects replicate previous reports of N1, EPN and LPP modulations in single word reading outside the RSVP design. Remarkably, against a time-line of structural and

SOMETHING ALWAYS STICKS? HOW EMOTIONAL LANGUAGE MODULATES NEURAL PROCESSES INVOLVED IN FACE ENCODING AND RECOGNITION MEMORY AUTHOR'S COPY ABSTRACT

SOMETHING ALWAYS STICKS? HOW EMOTIONAL LANGUAGE MODULATES NEURAL PROCESSES INVOLVED IN FACE ENCODING AND RECOGNITION MEMORY AUTHOR'S COPY ABSTRACT Poznań Studies in Contemporary Linguistics 53(1), 2017, pp. 63 93 Faculty of English, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań, Poland doi: 10.1515/psicl-2017-0004 SOMETHING ALWAYS STICKS? HOW EMOTIONAL LANGUAGE

More information

Buzzwords. Early Cortical Responses to Emotional Words During Reading. Research Report

Buzzwords. Early Cortical Responses to Emotional Words During Reading. Research Report PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Buzzwords Early Cortical Responses to Emotional Words During Reading Johanna Kissler, 1 Cornelia Herbert, 1 Peter Peyk, 2 and Markus Junghofer 3 1 University of Konstanz,

More information

Emotional arousal enhances lexical decision times

Emotional arousal enhances lexical decision times Emotional arousal enhances lexical decision times CHAPTER 4 Lars Kuchinke, Melissa L.-H. Võ, Lars Michael, and Arthur M. Jacobs 5 Abstract In a lexical decision experiment emotional valence and emotional

More information

Event related potentials to emotional adjectives during reading

Event related potentials to emotional adjectives during reading 0 0 (BWUS PSYP 00.PDF -Dec-0 : Bytes PAGES n operator=ravishankar) PSYP 00 P S Y P 0 0 B Dispatch:..0 Journal: PSYP CE: Blackwell Journal Name Manuscript No. Author Received: No. of pages: PE: Triveni

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

SECTION II. Understanding Emotional Language Content (Edited by Johanna Kissler)

SECTION II. Understanding Emotional Language Content (Edited by Johanna Kissler) SECTION II Understanding Emotional Language Content (Edited by Johanna Kissler) Anders, Ende, Junghöfer, Kissler & Wildgruber (Eds.) Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 156 ISSN 0079-6123 Copyright r 2006

More information

Affective Prime and Target Picture Processing: An ERP Analysis of Early and Late Interference Effects

Affective Prime and Target Picture Processing: An ERP Analysis of Early and Late Interference Effects Brain Topogr (2008) 20:183 191 DOI 10.1007/s10548-008-0045-6 ORIGINAL PAPER Affective Prime and Target Picture Processing: An ERP Analysis of Early and Late Interference Effects Tobias Flaisch Æ Jessica

More information

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Cortical Analysis of Visual Context Moshe Bar, Elissa Aminoff. 2003. Neuron, Volume 38, Issue 2, Pages 347 358. Visual objects in context Moshe Bar.

More information

Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves

Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves SICE Annual Conference 27 Sept. 17-2, 27, Kagawa University, Japan Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves Seiji Nishifuji 1, Kentaro Fujisaki 1 and Shogo Tanaka 1 1

More information

Address for correspondence: School of Psychological Sciences, Zochonis Building, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, M139PL, Manchester, UK

Address for correspondence: School of Psychological Sciences, Zochonis Building, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, M139PL, Manchester, UK BBS-D-15-00891_ Mather_ Talmi & Barnacle Emotionally arousing context modulates the ERP correlates of neutral picture processing: An ERP test of the Glutamate Amplifies Noradrenergic Effects (GANE) model

More information

Two neurocognitive mechanisms of semantic integration during the comprehension of visual real-world events

Two neurocognitive mechanisms of semantic integration during the comprehension of visual real-world events Two neurocognitive mechanisms of semantic integration during the comprehension of visual real-world events Tatiana Sitnikova a, Phillip J. Holcomb b, and Gina R. Kuperberg ba Abstract How do comprehenders

More information

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Cerebral Cortex March 2006;16:415-424 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi121 Advance Access publication June 15, 2005 Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Mika

More information

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study Armita Faghani Jadidi, Raheleh Davoodi, Mohammad Hassan Moradi Department of Biomedical Engineering Amirkabir University

More information

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms Submitted to Current Biology Mental representation of number in different numerical forms Anna Plodowski, Rachel Swainson, Georgina M. Jackson, Chris Rorden and Stephen R. Jackson School of Psychology

More information

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Intro Examine attention response functions Compare an attention-demanding

More information

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice Multiple Choice 1. Which structure is not part of the visual pathway in the brain? a. occipital lobe b. optic chiasm c. lateral geniculate nucleus *d. frontal lobe Answer location: Visual Pathways 2. Which

More information

Emotion-induced blindness reflects competition at early and late processing stages: An ERP study

Emotion-induced blindness reflects competition at early and late processing stages: An ERP study DOI 10.3758/s13415-014-0303-x Emotion-induced blindness reflects competition at early and late processing stages: An ERP study Briana L. Kennedy & Jennifer Rawding & Steven B. Most & James E. Hoffman #

More information

Outline of Talk. Introduction to EEG and Event Related Potentials. Key points. My path to EEG

Outline of Talk. Introduction to EEG and Event Related Potentials. Key points. My path to EEG Outline of Talk Introduction to EEG and Event Related Potentials Shafali Spurling Jeste Assistant Professor in Psychiatry and Neurology UCLA Center for Autism Research and Treatment Basic definitions and

More information

Brain & Language 134 (2014) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Brain & Language. journal homepage:

Brain & Language 134 (2014) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Brain & Language. journal homepage: Brain & Language 134 (2014) 34 43 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Brain & Language journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/b&l On pleasure and thrill: The interplay between arousal and valence

More information

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 2008, 8 (2), 195-210 doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.2.195 The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies MIKA

More information

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),???-??? 794-799 What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? ULRICH MAYR University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon Schneider and

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Early lateralization and orientation tuning for face, word, and object processing in the visual cortex

Early lateralization and orientation tuning for face, word, and object processing in the visual cortex NeuroImage 20 (2003) 1609 1624 www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Early lateralization and orientation tuning for face, word, and object processing in the visual cortex Bruno Rossion, a,b, * Carrie A. Joyce,

More information

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention Chapter 6 Attention Attention William James, in 1890, wrote Everyone knows what attention is. Attention is the taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several simultaneously

More information

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln 1 DATA MANAGEMENT Backups Storage Identification Analyses 2 Data Analysis Pre-processing Statistical Analysis

More information

the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining

the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining 8 the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining items was included. Participants were asked to decide whether a different item was included in the array,

More information

The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan. Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1

The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan. Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1 The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1 Psycholinguistics / Neurolinguistics Happy Hour!!! Tuesdays 7/11, 7/18, 7/25 5:30-6:30 PM @ the Boone Center

More information

Experimental design for Cognitive fmri

Experimental design for Cognitive fmri Experimental design for Cognitive fmri Alexa Morcom Edinburgh SPM course 2017 Thanks to Rik Henson, Thomas Wolbers, Jody Culham, and the SPM authors for slides Overview Categorical designs Factorial designs

More information

Processed by HBI: Russia/Switzerland/USA

Processed by HBI: Russia/Switzerland/USA 1 CONTENTS I Personal and clinical data II Conclusion. III Recommendations for therapy IV Report. 1. Procedures of EEG recording and analysis 2. Search for paroxysms 3. Eyes Open background EEG rhythms

More information

Neural Response to Emotional Pictures Is Unaffected by Concurrent Task Difficulty: An Event-Related Potential Study

Neural Response to Emotional Pictures Is Unaffected by Concurrent Task Difficulty: An Event-Related Potential Study Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 121, No. 6, 1156 1162 0735-7044/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.6.1156 Neural Response to Emotional Pictures

More information

Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention.

Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention. Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention. All behaviors unfold over time; therefore, our ability to perceive and adapt our behavior according to the temporal constraints

More information

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity 1,2 Mark A. Elliott, 2 Zhuanghua Shi & 2,3 Fatma Sürer 1 Department of Psychology National University of Ireland Galway, Ireland.

More information

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events Psychophysiology, 40 (2003), 827 831. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2003 Society for Psychophysiological Research BRIEF REPT Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the

More information

Response Inhibition and Memory Retrieval of Emotional Target Words: Evidence from an Emotional Stop-Signal Task

Response Inhibition and Memory Retrieval of Emotional Target Words: Evidence from an Emotional Stop-Signal Task Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science, 2011, 1, 153-159 doi:10.4236/jbbs.2011.13020 Published Online August 2011 (http://www.scirp.org/journal/jbbs) Response Inhibition and Memory Retrieval of Emotional

More information

The path of visual attention

The path of visual attention Acta Psychologica 121 (2006) 199 209 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The path of visual attention James M. Brown a, *, Bruno G. Breitmeyer b, Katherine A. Leighty a, Hope I. Denney a a Department of Psychology,

More information

Threat Modulates Neural Responses to Looming Visual Stimuli

Threat Modulates Neural Responses to Looming Visual Stimuli RUNNING HEAD: Threat Modulates Neural Responses to Looming Visual Stimuli Eleonora Vagnoni 1, Stella F. Lourenco 2, and Matthew R. Longo 1 1 Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Cognitive Brain Research 18 (2004) 255 272 Research report Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Audrey Duarte a, *, Charan Ranganath

More information

NeuroImage. Affective responses to emotional words are boosted in communicative situations. Lana Rohr, Rasha Abdel Rahman

NeuroImage. Affective responses to emotional words are boosted in communicative situations. Lana Rohr, Rasha Abdel Rahman NeuroImage 109 (2015) 273 282 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Affective responses to emotional words are boosted in communicative situations

More information

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential Title of Thesis Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential 2014 September Yang Weiping The Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology (Doctor s Course)

More information

In press, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. On the Time-Course of Letter Perception: A Masked Priming ERP Investigation

In press, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. On the Time-Course of Letter Perception: A Masked Priming ERP Investigation In press, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review On the Time-Course of Letter Perception: A Masked Priming ERP Investigation Jean-Philippe Petit, a Katherine J. Midgley, a,b Phillip J. Holcomb, b and Jonathan

More information

The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences

The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences from the N170 adaptation effect Xiao-hua Cao 1, Chao Li 1, Carl M Gaspar 2, Bei Jiang 1 1. Department of Psychology,Zhejiang Normal

More information

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention Sleep Deprivation and Selective Attention An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention Logan T. Trujillo, PhD 1 ; Steve Kornguth,

More information

Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): ISSN X

Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): ISSN X Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): 359-364 ISSN 0970-938X http://www.biomedres.info Investigating relative strengths and positions of electrical activity in the left and right hemispheres of the human brain

More information

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: A Comparison of Electrophysiological and Other Neuroimaging Approaches Leun J. Otten Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology University

More information

Attentional Blink Paradigm

Attentional Blink Paradigm Attentional Blink Paradigm ATTENTIONAL BLINK 83 ms stimulus onset asychrony between all stimuli B T D A 3 N P Z F R K M R N Lag 3 Target 1 Target 2 After detection of a target in a rapid stream of visual

More information

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002 Autumn 2007 674107 Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Stroop Effect Dual processing causing selective attention. 674107 November 26, 2007 Abstract This document

More information

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes Biological Psychology 66 (2004) 1 20 The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes Bettina Forster, Martin Eimer School of

More information

The impact of verbal framing on brain activity evoked by emotional images

The impact of verbal framing on brain activity evoked by emotional images Journal of Integrative Neuroscience, Vol. 10, No. 4 (2011) 513 524 c Imperial College Press DOI: 10.1142/S0219635211002816 The impact of verbal framing on brain activity evoked by emotional images Michael

More information

Threat modulates neural responses to looming visual stimuli

Threat modulates neural responses to looming visual stimuli European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 42, pp. 2190 2202, 2015 doi:10.1111/ejn.12998 COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE Threat modulates neural responses to looming visual stimuli Eleonora Vagnoni, 1 Stella F. Lourenco

More information

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Selective Attention Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form,

More information

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION INTRODUCTION Research on the Deaf Brain is beginning to provide a new evidence base for policy and practice in relation to intervention with deaf children. This talk outlines the multi-channel nature of

More information

The Function and Organization of Lateral Prefrontal Cortex: A Test of Competing Hypotheses

The Function and Organization of Lateral Prefrontal Cortex: A Test of Competing Hypotheses The Function and Organization of Lateral Prefrontal Cortex: A Test of Competing Hypotheses Jeremy R. Reynolds 1 *, Randall C. O Reilly 2, Jonathan D. Cohen 3, Todd S. Braver 4 1 Department of Psychology,

More information

Electrophysiological Responses to Affective Stimuli in Neglectful Mothers

Electrophysiological Responses to Affective Stimuli in Neglectful Mothers Electrophysiological Responses to Affective Stimuli in Neglectful Mothers Inmaculada León 1, María José Rodrigo 1 *, Ileana Quiñones 2, Juan Andrés Hernández 1, Agustín Lage 3, Iván Padrón 1, María Antonieta

More information

ERP correlates of retrieval orientation: Direct versus indirect memory tasks

ERP correlates of retrieval orientation: Direct versus indirect memory tasks available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report ERP correlates of retrieval orientation: Direct versus indirect memory tasks Michael Hornberger a, Michael D. Rugg b,

More information

Langeslag, S.J.E. & Van Strien, J.W. (in press). Preferential processing of task-irrelevant

Langeslag, S.J.E. & Van Strien, J.W. (in press). Preferential processing of task-irrelevant Langeslag, S.J.E. & Van Strien, J.W. (in press). Preferential processing of task-irrelevant beloved-related information and task performance: Two event-related potential studies. Neuropsychologia. Author

More information

Stroop Dilution Depends on the Nature of the Color Carrier but Not on Its Location

Stroop Dilution Depends on the Nature of the Color Carrier but Not on Its Location Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2006, Vol. 32, No. 4, 826 839 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.4.826

More information

One Mechanism or Two: A Commentary on Reading Normal and Degraded Words

One Mechanism or Two: A Commentary on Reading Normal and Degraded Words 1 One Mechanism or Two: A Commentary on Reading Normal and Degraded Words Carol Whitney Abstract The SERIOL model of letter position encoding (Whitney, 2001) led to a precise, verified prediction on how

More information

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus)

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus) EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus) Introduction 17 subjects were tested to measure effects of a Brain.fm focus session on cognition. With 4 additional subjects, we recorded EEG data during baseline and while

More information

Discovering Processing Stages by combining EEG with Hidden Markov Models

Discovering Processing Stages by combining EEG with Hidden Markov Models Discovering Processing Stages by combining EEG with Hidden Markov Models Jelmer P. Borst (jelmer@cmu.edu) John R. Anderson (ja+@cmu.edu) Dept. of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University Abstract A new method

More information

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Journal: Psychological Science Manuscript ID: PSCI-0-0.R Manuscript Type: Short report Date Submitted by the Author:

More information

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT (1) Foreword (optional) (2) Table of Contents (if report is more than 10 pages) (3) List of Appendixes, Illustrations and Tables (if applicable) (4) Statement of the problem studied FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

The observation that the mere activation

The observation that the mere activation Preparing and Motivating Behavior Outside of Awareness Henk Aarts,* Ruud Custers, Hans Marien Force (N) 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 The observation that the mere activation of the idea of a behavioral act

More information

Semantic and perceptual effects on recognition memory: Evidence from ERP

Semantic and perceptual effects on recognition memory: Evidence from ERP available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Semantic and perceptual effects on recognition memory: Evidence from ERP Erika Nyhus, Tim Curran Department of Psychology

More information

Aging, Emotion, Attention, and Binding in the Taboo Stroop Task: Data and Theories

Aging, Emotion, Attention, and Binding in the Taboo Stroop Task: Data and Theories Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 12803-12833; doi:10.3390/ijerph121012803 OPEN ACCESS Article International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health ISSN 1660-4601 www.mdpi.com/journal/ijerph

More information

Early brain potentials link repetition blindness, priming and novelty detection

Early brain potentials link repetition blindness, priming and novelty detection Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology NeuroReport 8, 943 948 (997) TO study mechanisms of visual object identification in humans, event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded during successful or

More information

Provided by the author(s) and NUI Galway in accordance with publisher policies. Please cite the published version when available. Title Prefrontal cortex and the generation of oscillatory visual persistence

More information

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

The Meaning of the Mask Matters PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Meaning of the Mask Matters Evidence of Conceptual Interference in the Attentional Blink Paul E. Dux and Veronika Coltheart Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science,

More information

See no evil: Directing visual attention within unpleasant images modulates the electrocortical response

See no evil: Directing visual attention within unpleasant images modulates the electrocortical response Psychophysiology, 46 (2009), 28 33. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2008 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00723.x See no evil: Directing visual

More information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time David E. Huber (dhuber@psyc.umd.edu) Department of Psychology, 1147 Biology/Psychology Building College Park, MD 2742 USA Denis Cousineau (Denis.Cousineau@UMontreal.CA)

More information

Event-related potentials as an index of similarity between words and pictures

Event-related potentials as an index of similarity between words and pictures Psychophysiology, 42 (25), 361 368. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 25 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 1.1111/j.1469-8986.25.295.x BRIEF REPORT Event-related potentials

More information

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm Author's response to reviews Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm Authors: Julia Miro (juliamirollado@gmail.com) Pablo Ripollès (pablo.ripolles.vidal@gmail.com)

More information

Final Summary Project Title: Cognitive Workload During Prosthetic Use: A quantitative EEG outcome measure

Final Summary Project Title: Cognitive Workload During Prosthetic Use: A quantitative EEG outcome measure American Orthotic and Prosthetic Association (AOPA) Center for Orthotics and Prosthetics Leraning and Outcomes/Evidence-Based Practice (COPL) Final Summary 2-28-14 Project Title: Cognitive Workload During

More information

Online Publication Date: 15 th July 2012 Publisher: Asian Economic and Social Society

Online Publication Date: 15 th July 2012 Publisher: Asian Economic and Social Society Online Publication Date: 15 th July 2012 Publisher: Asian Economic and Social Society Event-Related Potentials Reveal Perceptual Simulation of Color in Language Comprehension Aitao Lu (Center for Studies

More information

Schizophrenia as a disorder of thought, or language? Evidence from brain and behavioral investigations

Schizophrenia as a disorder of thought, or language? Evidence from brain and behavioral investigations Schizophrenia as a disorder of thought, or language? Evidence from brain and behavioral investigations Karen Froud Teachers College, Columbia University New York City Outline Big questions, and why this

More information

Biological Psychology

Biological Psychology Biological Psychology xxx (2008) xxx xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Biological Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho Early emotion word processing: Evidence from

More information

Temporal attention enhances early visual processing: A review and new evidence from event-related potentials

Temporal attention enhances early visual processing: A review and new evidence from event-related potentials available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Temporal attention enhances early visual processing: A review and new evidence from event-related potentials Ángel Correa,

More information

PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH. University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK

PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH. University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH Marie Rivenez 1,2, Chris Darwin 1, Anne Guillaume 2 1 Department of Psychology University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK 2 Département Sciences Cognitives Institut

More information

Profiling Attention s Pure Effect on the Sensory-Evoked P1 and N1 Event-Related Potentials of Human Electroencephalography

Profiling Attention s Pure Effect on the Sensory-Evoked P1 and N1 Event-Related Potentials of Human Electroencephalography Profiling Attention s Pure Effect on the Sensory-Evoked P1 and N1 Event-Related Potentials of Human Electroencephalography by Allison Elisabeth Connell A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction

More information

Evoked alpha and early access to the knowledge system: The P1 inhibition timing hypothesis

Evoked alpha and early access to the knowledge system: The P1 inhibition timing hypothesis available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Evoked alpha and early access to the knowledge system: The P1 inhibition timing hypothesis Wolfgang Klimesch University

More information

Evaluating Models of Object-Decision Priming: Evidence From Event-Related Potential Repetition Effects

Evaluating Models of Object-Decision Priming: Evidence From Event-Related Potential Repetition Effects Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 2, 230 248 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.2.230

More information

Influence of Color on Perceptual Priming: A Picture Fragment Completion Paradigm

Influence of Color on Perceptual Priming: A Picture Fragment Completion Paradigm Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Procedia - Social and Behavioral Scien ce s 82 ( 2013 ) 482 486 World Conference on Psychology and Sociology 2012 Influence of Color on Perceptual Priming: A Picture

More information

Attention modulates the processing of emotional expression triggered by foveal faces

Attention modulates the processing of emotional expression triggered by foveal faces Neuroscience Letters xxx (2005) xxx xxx Attention modulates the processing of emotional expression triggered by foveal faces Amanda Holmes a,, Monika Kiss b, Martin Eimer b a School of Human and Life Sciences,

More information

International Journal of Neurology Research

International Journal of Neurology Research International Journal of Neurology Research Online Submissions: http://www.ghrnet.org/index./ijnr/ doi:1.1755/j.issn.313-511.1..5 Int. J. of Neurology Res. 1 March (1): 1-55 ISSN 313-511 ORIGINAL ARTICLE

More information

Seizure onset can be difficult to asses in scalp EEG. However, some tools can be used to increase the seizure onset activity over the EEG background:

Seizure onset can be difficult to asses in scalp EEG. However, some tools can be used to increase the seizure onset activity over the EEG background: This presentation was given during the Dianalund Summer School on EEG and Epilepsy, July 24, 2012. The main purpose of this introductory talk is to show the possibilities of improved seizure onset analysis

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

Power-Based Connectivity. JL Sanguinetti

Power-Based Connectivity. JL Sanguinetti Power-Based Connectivity JL Sanguinetti Power-based connectivity Correlating time-frequency power between two electrodes across time or over trials Gives you flexibility for analysis: Test specific hypotheses

More information

Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA

Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA 1 Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA CONTENTS I Personal and clinical data II Conclusion. III Recommendations for therapy IV Report. 1. Procedures of EEG recording and analysis 2. Search for

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Item and Category-Based Attentional Control During Search for Real-World Objects: Can You Find the Pants Among the Pans? Rebecca Nako,

More information

Neural Strategies for Selective Attention Distinguish Fast-Action Video Game Players

Neural Strategies for Selective Attention Distinguish Fast-Action Video Game Players Neural Strategies for Selective Attention Distinguish Fast-Action Video Game Players - Online First - Springer 7/6/1 1:4 PM Neural Strategies for Selective Attention Distinguish Fast-Action Video Game

More information

An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search

An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search Steven Phillips (steve@ni.aist.go.jp) Neuroscience Research Institute (AIST), Tsukuba Central 2, 1-1-1 Umezono, Tsukuba, Ibaraki 305-8568

More information

Visual Selection and Attention

Visual Selection and Attention Visual Selection and Attention Retrieve Information Select what to observe No time to focus on every object Overt Selections Performed by eye movements Covert Selections Performed by visual attention 2

More information

The Effects of Temporal Preparation on Reaction Time

The Effects of Temporal Preparation on Reaction Time University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School January 2013 The Effects of Temporal Preparation on Reaction Time Glen Robert Forester University of South

More information

The Color of Similarity

The Color of Similarity The Color of Similarity Brooke O. Breaux (bfo1493@louisiana.edu) Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Louisiana at Lafayette, Lafayette, LA 70504 USA Michele I. Feist (feist@louisiana.edu) Institute

More information

Concurrent Learning of Temporal and Spatial Sequences

Concurrent Learning of Temporal and Spatial Sequences Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2002, Vol. 28, No. 3, 445 457 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.3.445

More information

INVESTIGATION COGNITIVE AVEC LES EFRPS (EYE-FIXATION-RELATED POTENTIALS) Thierry Baccino CHART-LUTIN (EA 4004) Université de Paris VIII

INVESTIGATION COGNITIVE AVEC LES EFRPS (EYE-FIXATION-RELATED POTENTIALS) Thierry Baccino CHART-LUTIN (EA 4004) Université de Paris VIII INVESTIGATION COGNITIVE AVEC LES EFRPS (EYE-FIXATION-RELATED POTENTIALS) Thierry Baccino CHART-LUTIN (EA 4004) Université de Paris VIII MESURES OCULAIRES PRELIMINARY QUESTIONS TO EFRPS Postulate: Individual

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information ten Oever and Sack 10.1073/pnas.1517519112 SI Materials and Methods Experiment 1. Participants. A total of 20 participants (9 male; age range 18 32 y; mean age 25 y) participated

More information

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals Takako Mitsudo *1, Yoshitaka Nakajima 2, Gerard B. Remijn 3, Hiroshige Takeichi 4, Yoshinobu Goto

More information