Cross-modal transfer as a function of initial training level in classical conditioning with the rabbit

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Cross-modal transfer as a function of initial training level in classical conditioning with the rabbit"

Transcription

1 Animal Learning & Behavior 1987, 15 (1), Cross-modal transfer as a function of initial training level in classical conditioning with the rabbit BERNARD G. SCHREURS and E. JAMES KEHOE University ofnew South Wales, Kensington, New South Wales, Australia Initial reinforced training with a conditioned stimulus (CSA) from one sensory modality subsequently facilitates the rate of acquisition of the rabbit's nictitating membrane response to a second conditioned stimulus (CSB)from a different sensory modality. In Experiment 1, the level of transfer was a direct function of the number of CSA-US pairings (0, 15, 30, 60, and 120). In Experiment 2, cross-modal transfer appeared to be maximal after initial training was conducted to a performance criterion as small as two CRa to CSA. The results are discussed with respect to theories of transfer, particularly a layered network model of conditioning that suggests that CR acquisition to each CS depends on two sequentially organized associations, one unique to the CS and one that is common to all associations involving the target response system. Kehoe and Holt (1984) recently examined transfer of training in classical conditioning of the rabbit's nictitating membrane response (NMR). They first conducted training in which a conditioned stimulus (CSA) in one sensory modality (e.g., tone) was paired with an electricalpulse unconditioned stimulus (US). After conditioned responses (CRs) were established to CSA, transfer training was begun with a new stimulus (CSB) in another sensory modality (e.g., light). At the beginning of transfer training, CSB was presented four times to determine whether there was immediate transfer from CSA to CSB. In fact, immediate transfer was negligible; the mean level ofresponding on the four test presentations was less than 10% CRs, which failed to differ from that of control groups. Although immediate transfer was not detected, general transfer was evidenced by extremely rapid CR acquisition during CSB-US pairings, as compared with that of control groups, which received initial exposure either to the experimental chambers or to the CSA and US separated by a long CS-US interval. One of the boundary conditions yielding transfer of training across sensory modalities that remains to be delineated is the level of initial training. There are some rabbit NMR data to suggest indirectly that general transfer may occur with only minimal initial training. Kehoe, Morrow, and Holt (1984) found that general transfer was undiminished after an extinction procedure had virtually eradicated responding to the initial CS. Further afield, Scavio, Ross, and McLeod (1983) have found that as few as 15 CS-US pairings substantially enhanced subsequent CR acquisition to the same CS even after an extinction procedure involving 480 CS-alone presentations. In addition, Ross and Scavio (1983) reported that, although in- This research was supported by Grant A from the Australian Research Grants Committee. Correspondence should be sent to E. J. Kehoe, School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Kensington, N.S.W. 2033, Australia. sufficient to produce CR acquisition, as few as 15 CS US pairings at one CS-US interval influenced the rate of CR acquisition when subjects were shifted to another CS US interval. Accordingly, the primary aim ofthe present experiments was to explicitly manipulate the initial level of training with CSA in order to reveal the minimum amount of training that would produce general transfer. Another aim ofthe present experiments was to further examine the possibility that the facilitation of CR acquisition to CSB represents stimulus generalization along some unspecified dimension of similarity between CSB US training and prior CSA-US training. In previous experiments, stimulus generalization has been defined in terms of immediate transfer as assessed during a small number of presentations of CSB immediately prior to CSB-US training (Holt & Kehoe, 1985; Kehoe & Holt, 1984; Kehoe et al., 1984). In addition to the previous assessment procedure for immediate transfer, the present experiments included test presentations ofcsb during initial CSA-US training and, conversely, test presentations of CSA during the course of CSB-US transfer training. As a test for subthreshold generalization, Experiment 1 assessed whether CSB could modify an unconditioned response (DR) and, more importantly, whether there were changes in any effect ofcsb on the UR over the course ofcsa-us training. Specifically, CSB-US and US-alone trials were administered at the beginning and end ofcsa US training (cf. Wagner, Thomas, & Norton, 1967; Sears, Baker, & Frey, 1979). A third aim ofthe present experiments was to evaluate a network model ofconditioning that accounts for general transfer (Kehoe, 1985, 1986). In brief, the model (illustrated in Figure 1) contains three "sensory" elements, one each for CSA (A), CSB (B), and the US. The outputs of elements A and B project to an intermediate element (X), and the output from X projects to a "response" element (R), which in tum gives rise to the observed behavior 47 Copyright 1987 Psychonomic Society, Inc.

2 48 SCHREURS AND KEHOE en Cl -l W u.. w > i= a. w U wa: CR/UR Figure 1. A neural network model ofconditioning with "sensory" elementsa, B, and US, an intermediate adaptive element X, and an adaptive "response" element R. (CR/UR). Both ofthe non-sensory elements (X, K) receive an input from the US element. The A-X, B-X, and X-R connections are subject to changes governed by a linear operator process (Sutton & Barto, 1981). The firing of both X and R is an all-or-none event that is governed by a random threshold variable. Thus, for example, X fires in response to an input from A only if the A-Xconnection weight exceeds the threshold value on that trial. Likewise, R fires only if the X-R connection exceeds the threshold value on that trial. Inputs from the US are assumed to always trigger both X and R. According to the proposed network model, CR acquisition to CSA and subsequent general transfer to CSB proceed in the following fashion. At the beginning of training, only outputs from the US to X and R are effective. Initially, the CSA input is unable to trigger the intermediate element X, but each CSA input renders its connection with X eligible for modification for a briefperiod following CSA onset (Sutton & Barto, 1981). Ifa US input triggers X during that eligibility period, the A-X connection weight will receive an increment. As the A-Xconnection strengthens over successive CSA-US pairings, A will begin to trigger X and thus render the X-R connection eligible for increments by the US input to R. CRs to CSA will begin to occur only when the A-X and X-R connections become strong enough so that A triggers X and then X triggers R. In subsequent transfer training with CSB, immediate transfer would not appear, because the B-Xconnection is ineffective. However, once CSB-US pairings begin and the B-X connection starts to strengthen, the earliestfirings ofxbyb would be immediately translated into CRs via the previously established X-R connection, thus yielding a facilitation of CR acquisition to CSB. In summary, CRs to CSA rely upon the successive strengthening of the A-X and X-R connections, but CRs to CSB require only the establishment of the B-X connection, which then capitalizes on the existing X-R connection. In addition to accounting for general transfer, the proposed network model generates two predictions regarding the effects ofmanipulating the level ofinitial training with CSA. First, the model predicts that the magnitude of general transfer should be proportional to the number ofinitial CSA-US pairings, because general transfer rests upon the strength ofthe X-R connection, which, in tum, requires prior strengthening of the A-Xconnection. Thus, a few CSA-US pairings would strengthen only the A-X connection and thus fail to lead to general transfer. A larger number of CSA-US pairings would be needed to strengthen the A-Xconnection sufficiently to produce any strengthening of the crucial X-R connection. Because both CRs to CSA and general transfer presumably depend upon strengthening of the X-R connection, general transfer to CSB would not be expected until there had been enough CSA-US pairings to produce some CRs to CSA. Second, using the same logic, the model makes the novel prediction that if initial training is truncated after a few CSA US pairings, partial strengthening ofthe A-Xconnection could be revealed subsequently if CSA were tested during CSB-US training. Specifically, after CSB-US pairings strengthen the B-Xconnection and begin to progressively strengthen the X-R connection, presentations of CSA would begin to evoke CRs via the previously established A-X connection and the newly established X-R connection. More specifically, the emergence of CRs to CSA would be expected to parallel the emergence of CRs to CSB. EXPERIMENT 1 The present experiment examined general transfer as a function of the number of CSA-US pairings. Method Subjects. The subjects were 40 naive female albino rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus), each days old andweighing approximately 1.5 kg. The animals had free access to food and water in their home cages. Apparatus. The apparatus and recording procedure for the NMR were patterned after those ofgormezano (1966; Kehoe, Feyer, & Moses, 1981). The CSs were (1) a looo-hz, 85-dB (SPL) tone superimposed on an ambient noise level of 81 db, and (2) a 20-Hz

3 CROSS-MODAL TRANSFER 49 flashing ofthe neon tube that served as the houselight. The US was a 50-msec, 3-mA, 5o-Hz ac electrical pulse delivered via stainless steel Autoclip wound clips positioned 10 rom apart and 15 rom posterior to the dorsal canthus of the right eye. The sequence and timing ofstimulus events were controlled by an Apple ITcomputer equipped with interfaces and software developed by Scandrett and Gormezano (1980). To monitor movements ofthe nictitating membrane, a small tinned copper wire hook was attached to a silk loop sutured in the nictitating membrane of the rabbit's right eye. The other end of the hook contained a loop that fitted over the end of an L-shaped piano wire lever, which operated a photoelectric transducer. The signal from the transducer was amplified and transmitted to an analog/digital converter installed in the computer (Scandrett & Gormezano, 1980). No straps were used to restrain the external eyelids. Procedure. All rabbits received I day of preparation, 2 days of recovery, 1 day of adaptation, 2 days of Stage 1 training, and 4 days ofstage 2 training. On the preparation day, hair surrounding the rabbit's right eye was removed, a small loop of silk (000 Dynex) was sutured into the nictitating membrane, and the animals were returned to their home cages for 2 days of recovery. On the adaptation day, the animals were placed in theconditioning apparatus for 70 min, but neither a CS nor a US was presented. Following adaptation, theanimalswere assigned randomly to one offive groups (n=8). The groups varied in the number of CSA-US pairings presented during Stage 1 and were designated PO, PIS, P30, P60, and P120 according to the number of CSA-US pairings received. Half of the animals in each group received the tone as CSA and the light as CSB; the other half received the light as CSA and the tone as CSB. All subjects were restrained in the conditioning chambers for the same period of time on each day of Stage 1 (70 min). At the beginning of Day 1 and at the end of Day 2, all subjects received a series of test trials for immediate transfer to CSB; the test trials consisted of four CSB-alone trials, four CSB-US trials, and four US-alone trials. On the test trials, the intensity of the US was.5 ma, a value selected to be just above threshold for elicitation ofthe DR. Group PO received no other stimulus presentations during Stage I. Groups PIS, P30, and P60 received 15, 30, and 60 CSA-US pairings, respectively, on Day 2. Group PI20 received 60 CSA-US pairings on both Day I and Day 2. During Stage I, the CS-US interval was 300 msec, and trials were presented at a mean intertrial interval (1TI) of 60 sec (range sec). With the exception ofthe first day ofstage 2, each day ofstage 2 contained 60 CSB-US pairings, 3 CSB-alone test trials, and 3 CSAalone test trials. On the first day of Stage 2, Trials I, 2, 3, and 4 were CSB-alone test trials that provided a further assessment of immediate transfer from CSA to CSB. Throughout Stage 2, the duration of each CS was 600 msec, and the CSB-US interval was 600 msec, a value that produces a moderate rate ofcr acquisition and avoids both ceiling and floor effects in detecting transfer (cf. Kehoe & Holt, 1984). The mean m was 60 sec. A CR was defined as any extension ofthe nictitating membrane exceeding.5 rom and occurring after the onset ofthe CS but prior to its termination. Unless otherwise noted, planned contrasts were used to analyze the data, and the rejection level was set according to a Type I error rate of.05. Results and Discussion The level of CR acquisition to CSA in Stage 1 was a direct function of the number ofcsa-us pairings. Specifically, the mean terminal levels of responding at the end of CSA-US pairings were 0%,0%,5%,49%, and 89% CRs for Groups PO, PI5, P30, P60, and PI20, respectively. For Group PO, which was not exposed to CSA-US pairings, the terminal level of responding represents the base rate of spontaneous responses during periods corresponding to CSA in the other groups. There was no discernible immediate transfer from CSA to CSB. On CSB-alone test trials, no subject displayed any CRs to esbeither before or after CSA-US training. To assess any subthreshold CR-evoking capacity by CSB, we examined the magnitude ofa UR to a weak US (.5 rna) following CSB. Specifically, we computed the ratio of the UR magnitude on CSB-US trials to the UR magnitude on US-alone trials. There was no significant change in this ratio from the beginning of Day 1 (M=.98) to the end of Day 2 (M= 1.06) (F < 1). Furthermore, a comparison of the ratios for Group PO versus the pretrained groups failed to reveal any differences in the UR magnitude ratios (F < 1). The panels of Figure 2 depict the percentage of CRs on CSB-US trials (Panel a) and CSA test trials (Panel b) during Stage 2 training. In Panel a, the set ofdata points designated as "IT" indicates the level of immediate transfer as assessed by the level of responding on the first four presentations of CSB alone. The data revealed little, if any, immediate transfer from Stage 1 to Stage 2. In particular, Groups PO, PI5, and P30 all showed 0% CRs to CSB. One subject in Group P60 responded to every CSBalone trial (M= 13%), 1 subject in Group PI20 produced one response to CSB, and another subject in Group PI20 produced two responses to CSB (M=I2%). Anyapparent differences among groups in levels of responding to CSB failed to reach statistical significance [F(1,35) = 2.68, p >.10]. Further inspection of Panel a reveals strong evidence of general transfer, its magnitude being a direct function ofthe number ofprevious CSA-US pairings. In particular, the levels of responding during the first 30 trials of CSB-US training were 3%, 1%, 6%, 30%, and 42% CRs for Groups PO, PI5, P30, P60, and PI20, respectively. Statistical analysis corroborated the effect of initial training with a significant linear trend across groups [F(1,35) = 6.27], which interacted with a linear trend across 3Q-trial blocks [F(1,35) = 9.88]. Inspection of Panel b of Figure 2 reveals that, during the course of CSB-US pairings, responding on CSA test trials in Group POremained at base-rate levels (M = 0% CRs), thus providing another instance in which there was no detectable immediate transfer from a CS in one modality to a CS in another modality. In contrast, Groups PI5 and P30 showed progressive increases in responding on CSA test trials from base-rate levels less than 6 % CRs to asymptotic levels of 34 % and 62 % CRs, respectively. Finally, responding to CSA in Groups P60 and PI20, which entered Stage 2 training displaying considerable responding to CSA, showed marginal increases over the course of Stage 2, reaching levels of 85% and 90% CRs, respectively. Statistical comparisons confirmed that there was a significant direct effect ofthe number ofprior CSA-US pairings on responding to CSA during Stage 2, which was reflected by a linear trend across groups [F(I,35) = 51.89]. In addition, the observed increase in responding to CSA across days of CSB-US training was

4 50 SCHREURS AND KEHOE (a) CSB-US PAIRINGS CSA TEST TRIALS lr) 0:: o ~ UJ 40 o 0:: o 15 UJ A IT TRIAL BLOCKS TEST TRIAL BLOCKS Figure 2. Perceot CRs 00 CSB-US trials (Panel a) and CSA test trials (Panel b) during Stage 2 training in Experiment 1. corroborated by a significant linear trend across test-trial blocks [F(I,35) = 16.44]. The apparent interaction of groups X test-trial blocks failed to reach the declared level of significance [F(I,35) = 3.75, p <.06]. The pattern of responding to both CSB and CSA strongly supports predictions from the proposed network model ofconditioning. (1) According to the model, both the emergence of CRs to CSA in Stage 1 and the early appearance of CRs to CSB in Stage 2 rely upon the initial establishmentofthe X-R connection following establishment of the A-X connection. In agreement with the model, a considerableand corresponding number ofcsa US trials were needed to produce both the emergence of CRs to CSA and the occurrence of general transfer to CSB. Inspection of the terminal levels of responding to CSA in Stage 1 reveals that only Groups P60 and P120 showed an appreciable number ofcrs to CSA. Likewise, only Groups P60 and P120 showed obvious facilitation in the acquisition of CRs to CSB. (2) According to the network model, a small number of initial CSA-US pairings should strengthen the A-X connection without strengthening the X-R connection to any appreciable degree. However, as the X-R connection is strengthened during the subsequent CSB-US training, CSA should progressively become able to elicit CRs via the previously established A-Xconnection and newly establishedx-r connection. Consistent with this prediction, Groups Pl5 and P30, which showed virtually no CRs during their CSA US pairings, displayed progressive increases in responding to CSA during Stage 2 that paralleled the course of CR acquisition to CSB. Moreover, responding to CSA in both Groups P15 and P30 appeared to reach asymptotic levels that were considerably less than 100% CRs. These low asymptotic levels further support the proposed model, because they can be construed as reflecting the limit of CR evocation that would be imposed if the initial CSA US pairings only partially strengthened the A-Xconnection. Finally, where the A-X connection had either received no previous strengthening (Group PO) or virtually complete strengthening (Groups P60 and PI20), there was little evidence of change in responding to CSA during CSB-US training. EXPERIMENT 2 Experiment 2 examined general transfer as a function of a performance criterion during CSA-US training, namely the number of CRs evoked by CSA before training was terminated. At an empirical level, the use of a fixed performance criterion represents a conventional alternative to a fixed amount of training for manipulating the degree ofinitial learning (cf. Ellis, 1965, pp ). Method Subjects. The subjects were 28 naive female rabbits of the same age and weight as those used in Experiment 1. Apparatus and Procedure. Except where indicated, the apparatus and procedure were identical to those used in Experiment 1. All

5 CROSS-MODAL TRANSFER 51 rabbits received 1 day of preparation, 1 day of adaptation, 1 day ofstage 1 training, and 4 days ofstage 2 training. The tone served as CSA and the flashing light served as CSB. Four groups designated C2, C5, CIO, and C20 (n=6) were presented CSA-US pairings until the animals achieved a criterion of 2, 5, 10, or 20 total CRs, respectively. The required number of CRs did not have to beconsecutive in occurrence. Upon reaching the designated criterion, each subject was immediately removed from the conditioning chamber and returned to its home cage. All subjects achieved criterion within 180 CSA.-US pairings. A further 4 subjects were assigned to Group CO, which did not receive any CSA-US pairings during Stage 1. Instead, subjects in Group CO were restrained in their chambers for 180 min, the maximum time allowed for CSA US pairings in the other groups. Finally, Stage 2 training for all subjects was identical to that of Experiment 1. That is, each day consisted of 60 CSB-US pairings, 3 CSB-alone test trials, and 3 CSA-alone test trials. Immediate transfer was assessed on the first day ofstage 2 by CSB-alone presentations on Trials 1, 2, 3, and 4. Results and Discussion An examination ofstage 1 training indicated that once CRs began to occur, subsequent CRs followed in relatively rapid succession. Accordingly, the number oftrials required to achieve the different criteria differed only slightly. Specifically, thecriteriaof2, 5,10, and20crs were achieved in 62, 103,80, and 94 CSA-US pairings, respectively. Any apparent linear or quadratic trend in the group means failed to reach statistical significance (Fs < 1). Despite the relatively uniform mean number of trials to criterion, there was considerable betweensubjects variability in the number of trials required to reach criterion (range: 20 to 180 trials). In Stage 2 training, immediate transfer from CSA to CSB was less than 10% CRs across all five groups (F < 1). Once CSB-US pairings began, general transfer appeared to be in evidence for all groups that had received prior CSA-US training. In the first 3D-trialblock, the mean response level shown by Group CO was only 1% CRs; for Groups C2, C5, C10, andc20, they were 24%, 34%, 14%, and 38% CRs, respectively. A statistical comparison of Group CO versus Groups C2, C5, CIO, and C20 approached, but failed to reach, the declared level ofsignificance [F(1,23) = 3.69, p <.06]. However, a comparison between the extreme groups, COversus C20, did indicate a significant general transfer effect [F(1,23) = 4.96, p <.05]. In summary, the general transfer effects were not as reliable as those that have been seen when more extensive initial training has been given (Kehoe & Holt, 1984; Kehoe et al., 1984); nevertheless, initial training to even a minimal performance criterion appeared to yield nearly as great an effect as have the more extensive training regimes. Likewise, initial training to a minimal performance criterion also yielded high levels of responding on CSA test trials during Stage 2. Across tests for CSA administered during CSB-US training, Group CO showed only 2% CRs, whereas Groups C2, C5, CI0, and C20 showed 68%,68%,61 %, and 88% CRs, respectively. Statistical comparisons confirmed a significant difference between Group CO versus the other groups [F(1,23) = 25.81]. Among Groups C2, C5, CI0, and C20, any apparent trends failed to reach significance. GENERAL DISCUSSION The results of Experiments 1 and 2 provide a coherent picture regarding the amount of initial CSA-US training needed to facilitate CR acquisition during subsequent CSB-US training. Forthe conditioning parameters prevailing in the present experiments, somewhere between 30 and 60 CSA-US pairings appears to have been the minimum number that yielded discernible general transfer. In Experiment 1, Groups P15 and P30 displayed little, if any, facilitation of CR acquisition to CSB, whereas Groups P60 and P120 showed considerable facilitation of CR acquisition to CSB. A similar pattern appears to be true for Experiment 2 when its results are examined in terms of the number of initial CSA-US pairings. In Experiment 2, a meanof 62 CSA-US pairings were required for subjects to reach even the lowest criterion (2 CRs). In fact, only 1 subject reached its designated criterion (5 CRs) in fewer than 30 CSA-US pairings. Subsequently, there was almost a uniform rate ofcr acquisition to CSB among the groups that had received prior CSA-US training. Across Groups C2, C5, CIO, and C20 in Experiment 2, the mean percentage ofcrs in the first block of CSB-US training was 28% CRs, whereas it was a mean of 1% CRs in the rest control, Group CO. The magnitude of this difference agrees with that seen when Group P60 (M = 30% CRs) and Group PO (M = 3% CRs) in Experiment 1 are compared. Because the precise number ofcsa-us pairings needed to produce general transfer depends undoubtedly on other conditioning parameters, such as the CSA-US interval, a more general rule for determining the amount ofinitial training can be induced from the correspondence between the emergence of CRs to CSA and facilitation ofcr acquisition to CSB. In Experiment 1, only the groups that displayed an appreciable likelihood of CRs during CSA US training, namely Groups P60 and P120, showed facilitation of CR acquisition to CSB. By the same token, in Experiment 2, the CSA-US training to a criterion ofjust two ers appeared sufficient to facilitate CR acquisition to CSB. With regard to a theory ofgeneral transfer, the present results provide detailed support for the following key features of the proposed network model. (1) According to the model, the first few CSA-US pairings only strengthen the A-Xconnection, because the X-R connection does not become eligible for strengthening until an A input starts to trigger X in advance of a US input. Nevertheless, the A-Xconnection can presumably be detected by CSA test trials during subsequent CSB-US training as it strengthens the shared X-R connection, thus permitting CSA inputs to evoke a CR via the previously established A-Xconnection and the newly established X-R connection. In Experiment 1, this hypothesis was confirmed in Groups P15 and

6 52 SCHREURS AND KEHOE P30, in which CRs to CSA emerged only during CSB US training. (2) According to the model, a relatively large number ofinitial CSA-US pairings enables the successive strengthening ofthe A-Xand X-R connections, thus leading to both the appearance ofcrs to CSA and subsequent facilitation of CR acquisition to CSB. In agreement with the model, the emergence of CRs to CSA appeared to predict the subsequent facilitation of CR acquisition to CSB. With regard to the results of Experiment 2, it is perhaps surprising that even the occurrence of two CRs to CSA predicts facilitation of CR acquisition to CSB. However, on each trial, both the likelihood ofcsa's triggering X and the likelihood of X's triggering R depend on a variable threshold. Thus, according to the model, a CR represents the product of two probabilistic events. Ordinarily, both the A-Xand X-R connections would have to be moderately well established for both connections to exceed their respective thresholds on any given trial. Therefore, even the minimal CR criterion in Experiment 2 ensured that a moderate X-R connection had been established prior to CSB-US training. In addition to explaining general transfer and the effects ofinitial CSA-US training, the proposed layered network model can also explain the data that prompted the present study, namely the ability of general transfer to survive extinction ofthe CR to CSA (Kehoe et al., 1984). Accordingto computer simulations of the model (Kehoe, 1985, 1986), general transfer survives because the X-R connection remains largely intact during extinction. During the extinction procedure, the A-Xconnection declines at a steady rate. As the A-Xconnection weakens and X's frequency offiring declines, the X-R connection is eligible for modification less and less often. In this way, the X-R connection is largely protected from extinction and thus remains intact. With the X-R connection still in place, the introduction of CSB-US training is still able to take advantage of the X-R connection in yielding CRs as the B-X connection begins to strengthen. To the knowledge ofthe authors, the proposed layerednetwork model represents the first rigorous account of general transfer applicable to classical conditioning and related associative learning procedures. Nevertheless, there have been a large number of previous attempts to identify the sources of general transfer. The proposed sources have included (1) adaptation to the apparatus, (2) development of a "US representation" (Rescorla & Heth, 1975), (3) neutralization ofbackground stimuli that might otherwise compete with the CS for the subject's processing resources (Mackintosh, 1977; Seraganian, 1979; Westbrook & Homewood, 1982), and (4) stimulus generalization from the initial CS to the new CS based on shared temporal or stimulus patterns (Friedes, 1974; Meck & Church, 1982; Seraganian & Popova, 1976), and (5) superordinate learning in which the animal acquires not only specific associations but also a sensitivity to the structural relations among the stimuli, responses, and reinforcers (Holt & Kehoe, 1985; Levine, 1959; Rodgers & Thomas, 1982). Among the proposed sources of general transfer, previous investigations indicate little, if any, contribution from adaptation, the US representation, and neutralization of background stimuli. In controlling for these sources, previous demonstrations ofgeneral transfer with the rabbit NMR preparation, as well as other preparations, have included control conditions with the same exposure to the apparatus, the initial CS, and the US as the transfer condition (Holt & Kehoe, 1985; Kehoe & Holt, 1984; Kehoe et al., 1984; Thomas, Miller, & Svinicki, 1971; Westbrook & Homewood, 1982). In all cases, the rate of response acquisition by these control groups in transfer testing fell well below that of groups with a history of initial stimulus-reinforcerpairings. When a naive rest control group was also available, the rate of response acquisition in the control groups exposed to the CS and US differed little from that of the rest control group (Kehoe & Holt, 1984; Westbrook & Homewood, 1982). In addition to the controls used in transfer studies, there are converging lines of evidence to show that presentation of the US in the rabbit NMR preparation does not contribute to the general transfer effect through the development of a "US representation," sensitization, or pseudoconditioning. Explicitly unpaired or truly random presentations ofa US and CS have never yielded evidence of even transitory sensitization or pseudoconditioning in the rabbit NMR preparation (Gormezano, Kehoe, & Marshall, 1983; Ross & Scavio, 1983). In fact, US-alone presentations generally retard CR acquisition during subsequent CS-US pairings. As few as 50 US-alone presentations can produce some retardation (Mis & Moore, 1973, Experiment 1). With respect to stimulus generalization as a source of general transfer, immediate transferacross stimulus modalities can occur when the original and test stimuli share the same temporal pattern (Meck & Church, 1982; Seraganian & Popova, 1976). Nevertheless, overt stimulus generalization does not appear to be a necessary precursor to general transfer. The aggregate of cross-modal transfer data collected with the rabbit NMR preparation has failed to reveal any statistical evidence for immediate transfer that occurs in conjunction with the highly reliable general transfer effect. In the transfer experiments conducted so far, immediate transfer has been routinely assessed on the first four presentations of CSB in transfer training. Thus, any individual animal could show 0, 1, 2, 3, or 4 responses to those presentations ofcsb. Including the subjects from Groups P120 and POof Experiment 1, 126 experimental subjects hadreceived extensive prior training with a CSA at a relatively short CS-US interval, and 117 control subjects had received either no prior training or prior exposure to a long CS-US interval that failed to produce initial CR acquisition. Among the experimental subjects, 79%, 13%, 3%, 2%, and 2% showed 0, 1,2, 3, and 4 responses, respectively. Similarly, among the control subjects, 85%,9%, 3%, 0%, and 1% showed 0, 1,2,3, and 4 responses, respectively. Any apparent difference between the two distributions

7 CROSS-MODAL TRANSFER 53 failed to attain statistical significance [~(4) = 4.52, P >.30]. In the present experiments, more extensive attempts to detect immediatetransfer from CSA to CSB also failed to yield any evidence of stimulus generalization. In particular, testing ofcsb immediately before and immediately after CSA-US pairings yielded no responding. Likewise, in the naive rest control groups (PO and CO), repeated tests of CSA during CSB-US pairings failed to reveal any immediate transfer. Finally, attempts to deteet subthreshold immediate transfer in the pretrained groups by examining the ability ofcsb to modify the UR magnitude yielded negative results. Having eliminated other possible contributors to general transfer, there remains the possibility that some superordinate learning process occurs during CSA-US training. For example, Thomas (1970) has explained general transfer between discrimination learning problems by contending that the animal acquires not only a specific discrimination during initial training, but also a "general attentiveness" to stimulus differences. Thomas's hypothesis is a special case of a more general hypothesis that, during initial training, the animal acquires not only specific associations but also a sensitivity to similar structural relations among stimuli, responses, and/or reinforcers (Behar & LeBedda, 1974; Holt & Kehoe, 1985; Levine, 1959; Rodgers & Thomas, 1982). In agreement with this hypothesis, Holt and Kehoe (1985) found that contiguous CSA-US relations in initial training uniquely facilitated subsequent acquisition based on contiguous CSB US relations even when embedded in a discrimination task. Likewise, initial discrimination training along a visual dimension facilitated subsequent discrimination training along an auditory dimension. With respect to the superordinate learning hypotheses, the proposed layered network model can be viewed as a complementary theoretical development. Rather than dividing the consequences ofinitial learning into a hierarchy ofspecific and superordinate components, the proposed layered-network model divides initial learning into a sequence of two associative linkages, namely a specific component (A-X) and a common component (X-R), the latter being available in subsequent learning tasks. It now remains to be determined whether a layered-network model can account for generaltransferbetweenmorecomplextasks, such as discrimination learning procedures. REFERENCES BEHAR, I., & LI!BEDDA, J. M. (1974). Effects of differential pretraining on learning-set formation. Journal ofcomparative &:Physiological Psychology, 82, ELUS, H. (1965). The transfer oflearning. New York: Macmillan. FRIEDl!s, D. (1974). Human information processing and sensory m0 dality: Cross-modal functions, information complexity, memory, and deficit. Psychological Bulletin, 81, GoRMl!ZANO, I. (1966). Classical conditioning. In J. B. Sidowski (Ed.), Experimental methods and instrumentation in psychology (pp ). New York: McGraw-Hill. GoRMEZANO, I., KEHOE, E. J., & MARSHALL, B. S. (1983). Twenty years of classical conditioning research with the rabbit. In J. M. Sprague & A. N. Epstein (Bds.), Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology (Vol. 10, pp ). New York: Academic Press. HOLT, P. E., & KEHOI!, E. J. (1985). Cross-modal transfer as a function ofsimilarities between training tasks in classical conditioning of the rabbit. Animal Learning &: Behavior, 13, KEHOI!, E. J. (1985, November). A neural network model ofclassical conditioning. Paper presented at the meeting ofthe Psychonomic S0 ciety, Boston. KEHOE, E. J. (1986). A layered network model for learning-to-learn and configuration in classical conditioning (pp ). In C. Clifton (Ed.), Proceedingsofthe Eighth Annual Conferenceofthe Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. KEHOI!, E. J., FI!YI!R, A., '" Mosl!S, J. L. (1981). Second-order conditioning of the rabbit's nictitating membrane response as a function ofthe CS2-eSI and CSI-US intervals. Animal Learning &: Behavior, 9, KEHOE,E. J., & HOLT, P. E. (1984). Transfer across CS-US intervals and sensory modalities in classical conditioning ofthe rabbit. Animal Learning &: Behavior, 12, KEHOI!, E. J., MORROW, L. D., & HOLT, P. E. (1984). General transfer across sensory modalities survives reductions in the original conditioned reflex in the rabbit. Animal Learning &: Behavior, 12, LEVINE,M. (1959). A model ofhypothesis behavior in discrimination learning sets. Psychological Review, 66, MACKINTOSH, N. J. (1977). Stimulus control: Attentional factors. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Bds.), Handbook ofoperant behavior (pp ). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. MI!CK, W. H., & CHURCH, R. M. (1982). Abstraction of temporal attributes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 8, MIs, F. W., & MooRl!, J. W. (1973). Effect ofpreacquisition UCS exposure on classical conditioning ofthe rabbit's nictitating membrane response. Learning &: Motivation, 4, RI!scoRLA, R. A., & HEm, C. D. (1975). Reinstatement of fear to an extinguished conditioned stimulus. Joumal ofexperimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1, RODGI!RS, J. P., '" THoMAS, D. R. (1982). Task specificity in IlOIL'IpCCific transfer and in extradimensional stimulus generalization in pigeons. Journal ofexperimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 8, Ross, R. T., & ScAVIO,M. J., JR. (1983). Perseveration ofassociative strength in rabbit nictitaiing membraneresponse conditioning following lsi shifts. Animal Learning &: Behavior, 11, ScANDRI!1T, J., '" GoRMEZANO, I. (1980). Microprocessor control and A-D data acquisition in classical conditioning. Behavior Research Methods &: Instrumentation, 12, ScAVIO,M. J., JR., Ross, R. T., & McLEoD, L. M. (1983). Perseveration ofassociative strength in rabbit nictitating membrane response conditioning. Animal Learning &: Behavior, 11, SEARS, R. J., BAKER, J. S., '" frey, P. W. (1979). The eye blink as a time-locked response: Implications for serial and second-order conditioning. JOlU'IUJI of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 5, SI!RAGANlAN, P. (1979). Extradimensional transfer in the easy-to-hard effect. Learning &: Motivation, 10, SI!RAGANlAN, P., '" POPOVA, Y. I. (1976). Cross-modal transfer of a conditioned flexion response in dogs. Pavlovian JourntJl ofbiological Sciences, 11, SUTTON, R. S., '" BARTO, A. G. (1981). Toward a modem theory of adaptive networks: Expectation and predicti.on. PsychologicalReview, 88, THOMAS, D. R. (1970). Stimulus selection, attention, and related matters. In J. H. Reynierse (Ed.), Current issues in animal learning (pp ). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

8 54 SCHREURS AND KEHOE THOMAS, D. R., MILLER, J. T., 8< SVINICKI, J. G. (1971). Nonspecific transfer effects ofdiscrimination training in the rat. Journal ofcomparative & Physiological Psychology, 74, WAGNER, A. R., THOMAS, E., 8< NORTON, T. (1967). Conditioning with electrical stimulation of motor cortex: Evidence of a possible source of motivation. Journal ofcomparative & Physiological Psychology, 64, WESTBROOK, R. F., 8< HOMEWOOD, J. (1982). The effects of a flavour toxicosis pairing upon long-delay, flavour aversion learning. Quarterly Journal ofexperimental Psychology, 348, (Manuscript received May 27, 1986; revision accepted for publication September 24, 1986.)

Stimulus specificity of concurrent recovery in the rabbit nictitating membrane response

Stimulus specificity of concurrent recovery in the rabbit nictitating membrane response Learning & Behavior 2005, 33 (3), 343-362 Stimulus specificity of concurrent recovery in the rabbit nictitating membrane response GABRIELLE WEIDEMANN and E. JAMES KEHOE University of New South Wales, Sydney,

More information

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999 TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL A Senior Honors Thesis Presented By Vanessa E. Castagna June 999 999 by Vanessa E. Castagna ABSTRACT TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: A TEST OF

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Concurrent development of excitatory and inhibitory associations during backward conditioning

Concurrent development of excitatory and inhibitory associations during backward conditioning Animal Learning & Behavior 1986, 14 (2), 133-137 Concurrent development of excitatory and inhibitory associations during backward conditioning ROBERT W. TAIT and MICHAEL E. SALADIN University of Manitoba,

More information

Effects of modulating tone frequency, intensity, and duration on the classically conditioned rabbit nictitating membrane response

Effects of modulating tone frequency, intensity, and duration on the classically conditioned rabbit nictitating membrane response Psychobiology 1995,23 (2),103-115 Effects of modulating tone frequency, intensity, and duration on the classically conditioned rabbit nictitating membrane response E.JAMESKEHOE University oj New South

More information

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1976, Vol. 4 (3), 293-298 The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock THOMAS s. HYDE Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland,

More information

Second-Order Conditioning of the Rabbit's Nictitating Membrane Response

Second-Order Conditioning of the Rabbit's Nictitating Membrane Response Second-Order Conditioning of the Rabbit's Nictitating Membrane Response Interstimulus Interval and Frequency of CS-CS Pairings CHARLES M. GIBBS, VALERIE COOL, TAMIO LAND, The University of lowa E. JAMES

More information

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (2), 213-219 Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning THOMAS HIGGINS and ROBERT A. RESCORLA University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

More information

Temporal-Difference Prediction Errors and Pavlovian Fear Conditioning: Role of NMDA and Opioid Receptors

Temporal-Difference Prediction Errors and Pavlovian Fear Conditioning: Role of NMDA and Opioid Receptors Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 121, No. 5, 1043 1052 0735-7044/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.5.1043 Temporal-Difference Prediction Errors

More information

Effects of cross-modal transfer on direct and reversal learning in the rat

Effects of cross-modal transfer on direct and reversal learning in the rat Animal Learning & Behavior 1986, 14 (4), 427-434 Effects of cross-modal transfer on direct and reversal learning in the rat EUGENE R. DELAY Regis College, Denver, Colorado In the first experiment, using

More information

Blocking in eyelid conditioning: Effect of changing the CS-US interval and introducing an intertrial stimulus

Blocking in eyelid conditioning: Effect of changing the CS-US interval and introducing an intertrial stimulus Animal Learning& Behavior 1979, 7 (4), 452-456 Blocking in eyelid conditioning: Effect of changing the CS-US interval and introducing an intertrial stimulus ROBERT T. MALESKE and PETER W. FREY Northwestern

More information

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus.

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR EFFECTS OF DISCRETE-TRIAL AND FREE-OPERANT PROCEDURES ON THE ACQUISITION AND MAINTENANCE OF SUCCESSIVE DISCRIMINATION IN RATS SHIN HACHIYA AND MASATO ITO

More information

Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting

Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting Animal Learning & Behavior 1989, 17 (3), 269-279 Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting PETER C. HOLLAND Duke University, Durham, North Carolina In three experiments, transfer effects

More information

Transfer of visual and haptic maze learning in rats

Transfer of visual and haptic maze learning in rats Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (4), 421-426 Transfer of visual and haptic maze learning in rats TUAN D. TRAN, LAURA C ADDUCCI, LIZA D. DEBARTOLO, BETH A. BOWER, and EUGENE R. DELAY Regis University,

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

Unit 6 Learning.

Unit 6 Learning. Unit 6 Learning https://www.apstudynotes.org/psychology/outlines/chapter-6-learning/ 1. Overview 1. Learning 1. A long lasting change in behavior resulting from experience 2. Classical Conditioning 1.

More information

Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning

Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (3), 230-245 doi:10.3758/lb.37.3.230 Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning JUSTIN A. HARRIS, SABA A GHARA EI, AND CLINTON A. MOORE

More information

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(2),199-203 Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning PETER C. HOLLAND and DESWELL T. FORBES University ofpittsburgh, Pittsburgh,

More information

The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Computational Models of Neural Systems Lecture 5.1

The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Computational Models of Neural Systems Lecture 5.1 The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Lecture 5.1 David S. Touretzky Based on notes by Lisa M. Saksida November, 2015 Outline Classical and instrumental conditioning The Rescorla

More information

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1985, 439 235-241 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES LANNY FIELDS THE COLLEGE OF

More information

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats Animal Learning & Behavior 1992. 20 (3). 233-239 The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats JAMES S. MILLER and JOYCE A.

More information

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery L132 NT MJA/cla Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (1), 60-73 doi:10.3758/lb.37.1.60 Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery Gonzalo P. Urcelay University of Cambridge, Cambridge,

More information

Interval duration effects on blocking in appetitive conditioning

Interval duration effects on blocking in appetitive conditioning Behavioural Processes 71 (2006) 318 329 Interval duration effects on blocking in appetitive conditioning Dómhnall Jennings 1, Kimberly Kirkpatrick Department of Psychology, University of York, Heslington,

More information

An extinction trial as a reminder treatment following electroconvulsive shock

An extinction trial as a reminder treatment following electroconvulsive shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(3),363-367 An extinction trial as a reminder treatment following electroconvulsive shock WLLAM C. GORDON and ROBERT R. MOWRER University ofnew Mexico, Albuquerque, New

More information

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010 Notes Textbook used Week 1-3: Bouton, M.E. (2016). Learning and Behavior: A Contemporary Synthesis. 2nd Ed. Sinauer Week 4-6: Rieger, E. (Ed.) (2014) Abnormal Psychology:

More information

Partial reinforcement delayed extinction effect after regularly alternating reward training

Partial reinforcement delayed extinction effect after regularly alternating reward training Animal Learning & Behavior 1986, 14 (3), 293-300 Partial reinforcement delayed extinction effect after regularly alternating reward training MASATO ISHIDA Osaka University of Education, Osaka, Japan When

More information

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules Animal Learning& Behavior 1981,9 (3),406-410 Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules JOHN H. HULL, TIMOTHY J. BARTLETT, and ROBERT

More information

acquisition associative learning behaviorism B. F. Skinner biofeedback

acquisition associative learning behaviorism B. F. Skinner biofeedback acquisition associative learning in classical conditioning the initial stage when one links a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus so that the neutral stimulus begins triggering the conditioned

More information

The Role of Temporal Relationships in the Transfer of Conditioned Inhibition

The Role of Temporal Relationships in the Transfer of Conditioned Inhibition Denniston, James C., Robert P. Cole, and Ralph R. Miller. (1998). "The role of temporal relationships in the transfer of conditioned inhibition." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

Retardation of rabbit nictitating membrane conditioning following US preexposures depends on the distribution and number of US presentations

Retardation of rabbit nictitating membrane conditioning following US preexposures depends on the distribution and number of US presentations Animal Learning & Behavior J989, J7 (2), J79-J87 Retardation of rabbit nictitating membrane conditioning following US preexposures depends on the distribution and number of US presentations MICHAEL E.

More information

Systematic manipulation of CS-US pairings in negative CS-US correlation procedures in rats

Systematic manipulation of CS-US pairings in negative CS-US correlation procedures in rats Animal Learning & Behavior 1980, 8 (1), 67-74 Systematic manipulation of CS-US pairings in negative CS-US correlation procedures in rats ELIZABETH S. WITCHER and JOHN J. B. AYRES University ofmassachusetts,

More information

Chapter 6/9: Learning

Chapter 6/9: Learning Chapter 6/9: Learning Learning A relatively durable change in behavior or knowledge that is due to experience. The acquisition of knowledge, skills, and behavior through reinforcement, modeling and natural

More information

Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative mechanisms

Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative mechanisms Q0667 QJEP(B) si-b03/read as keyed THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 56B (1), 43 55 Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative

More information

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 322 328 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.3.322

More information

from extinction provided by a conditioned inhibitor.

from extinction provided by a conditioned inhibitor. Learning & Behavior 2010, 38 (1), 68-79 doi:10.3758/lb.38.1.68 Protection from extinction provided by a conditioned inhibitor BRIDGET L. MCCONNELL AND RAR LPH R. MILLER State University of New York, Binghamton,

More information

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1991, Vol. 17, No. 3, 231-248 Copyright 1991 by the Am n Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/91/53.00 Transfer of Control in Ambiguous

More information

COMPUTATIONAL MODELS OF CLASSICAL CONDITIONING: A COMPARATIVE STUDY

COMPUTATIONAL MODELS OF CLASSICAL CONDITIONING: A COMPARATIVE STUDY COMPUTATIONAL MODELS OF CLASSICAL CONDITIONING: A COMPARATIVE STUDY Christian Balkenius Jan Morén christian.balkenius@fil.lu.se jan.moren@fil.lu.se Lund University Cognitive Science Kungshuset, Lundagård

More information

Author. Published. Journal Title. Copyright Statement. Downloaded from. Link to published version. Griffith Research Online

Author. Published. Journal Title. Copyright Statement. Downloaded from. Link to published version. Griffith Research Online The use of an unpleasant sound as the unconditional stimulus in aversive Pavlovian conditioning experiments that involve children and adolescent participants Author Neumann, David, Waters, Allison, Westbury

More information

Some determinants of second-order conditioning

Some determinants of second-order conditioning Learn Behav (2011) 39:12 26 DOI 10.1007/s13420-010-0002-6 Some determinants of second-order conditioning James E. Witnauer & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010

More information

Lateral Inhibition Explains Savings in Conditioning and Extinction

Lateral Inhibition Explains Savings in Conditioning and Extinction Lateral Inhibition Explains Savings in Conditioning and Extinction Ashish Gupta & David C. Noelle ({ashish.gupta, david.noelle}@vanderbilt.edu) Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science

More information

1. A type of learning in which behavior is strengthened if followed by a reinforcer or diminished if followed by a punisher.

1. A type of learning in which behavior is strengthened if followed by a reinforcer or diminished if followed by a punisher. 1. A stimulus change that increases the future frequency of behavior that immediately precedes it. 2. In operant conditioning, a reinforcement schedule that reinforces a response only after a specified

More information

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997, 4 (1), 113-117 Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer JANICE E. WEAVER University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky JANICE N. STEIRN

More information

Learning = an enduring change in behavior, resulting from experience.

Learning = an enduring change in behavior, resulting from experience. Chapter 6: Learning Learning = an enduring change in behavior, resulting from experience. Conditioning = a process in which environmental stimuli and behavioral processes become connected Two types of

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

acquisition associative learning behaviorism A type of learning in which one learns to link two or more stimuli and anticipate events

acquisition associative learning behaviorism A type of learning in which one learns to link two or more stimuli and anticipate events acquisition associative learning In classical conditioning, the initial stage, when one links a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus so that the neutral stimulus begins triggering the conditioned

More information

A Simulation of Sutton and Barto s Temporal Difference Conditioning Model

A Simulation of Sutton and Barto s Temporal Difference Conditioning Model A Simulation of Sutton and Barto s Temporal Difference Conditioning Model Nick Schmansk Department of Cognitive and Neural Sstems Boston Universit Ma, Abstract A simulation of the Sutton and Barto [] model

More information

Effects of an aversive CS+ and CS- under deprivation upon successive classical appetitive and aversive conditioning

Effects of an aversive CS+ and CS- under deprivation upon successive classical appetitive and aversive conditioning Animal Learning & Behavior 1978,6 (1), 57-65 Effects of an aversive CS+ and CS- under deprivation upon successive classical appetitive and aversive conditioning BRUCE K. BROMAGE and MICHAEL J. SCAVIO,

More information

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes Journal Learning & Behavior 2005,?? 33 (?), (4),???-??? 464-478 Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes PETER C. HOLLAND

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

DISCRIMINATION OF SERIAL AUDITORY PATTERNS IN RATS - roles of configural and elemental associations

DISCRIMINATION OF SERIAL AUDITORY PATTERNS IN RATS - roles of configural and elemental associations DISCRIMINATION OF SERIAL AUDITORY PATTERNS IN RATS - roles of configural and elemental associations Tiina Nurmi Master s thesis Department of Psychology University of Jyväskylä January 2011 CONTENTS INTRODUCTION

More information

Modulatory mechanisms in Pavlovian conditioning

Modulatory mechanisms in Pavlovian conditioning Animal Learning & Behavior 1995,23 (2),123-143 Modulatory mechanisms in Pavlovian conditioning DALE SWARTZENTRUBER Ohio Wesleyan University, Delaware, Ohio Pavlovian conditioning has traditionally been

More information

Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response 1

Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response 1 Japanese Psychological Research 1998, Volume 40, No. 2, 104 110 Short Report Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

Interference in pigeons' long-term memory viewed as a retrieval problem

Interference in pigeons' long-term memory viewed as a retrieval problem Animal Learning & Behavior 1981,9 (4),581-586 Interference in pigeons' long-term memory viewed as a retrieval problem DAVID R. THOMAS, ALAN R. McKELVIE, MICHAEL RANNEY, and THOMAS B. MOYE University ofcolorado,

More information

Some effects of short-term immediate prior exposure to light change on responding for light change*

Some effects of short-term immediate prior exposure to light change on responding for light change* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2 (4), 262-266 Some effects of short-term immediate prior exposure to light change on responding for light change* ALAN RUSSELLt and PETER H. GLOW University ofadelaide,

More information

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior A. Learning-long lasting changes in the environmental guidance of behavior as a result of experience B. Learning emphasizes the fact that individual environments also play

More information

Unit 06 - Overview. Click on the any of the above hyperlinks to go to that section in the presentation.

Unit 06 - Overview. Click on the any of the above hyperlinks to go to that section in the presentation. Unit 06 - Overview How We Learn and Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Operant Conditioning s Applications, and Comparison to Classical Conditioning Biology, Cognition, and Learning Learning By

More information

I. Classical Conditioning

I. Classical Conditioning Learning Chapter 8 Learning A relatively permanent change in an organism that occur because of prior experience Psychologists must study overt behavior or physical changes to study learning Learning I.

More information

Transfer of Serial Reversal Learning in the Pigeon

Transfer of Serial Reversal Learning in the Pigeon The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (1986) 38B, 81-95 Transfer of Serial Reversal Learning in the Pigeon P. J. Durlach and N. J. Mackintosh Department of Experimental Psychology, University

More information

Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online

Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online Chapter Title Attention and Pavlovian Conditioning Copyright Year 2011 Copyright Holder Springer Science+Business Media, LLC Corresponding Author

More information

Psychology in Your Life

Psychology in Your Life Sarah Grison Todd Heatherton Michael Gazzaniga Psychology in Your Life FIRST EDITION Chapter 6 Learning 2014 W. W. Norton & Company, Inc. Section 6.1 How Do the Parts of Our Brains Function? 6.1 What Are

More information

TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES SN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION

TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES SN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES SN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION Tnesis for the Degree of M. A. MiCHiGAN STATE UNWERSITY RODNEY CHARLES HOWARD 1977 f L13 1-;- R Y Michiga: State gr? Umw 1y ABSTRACT TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES

More information

Chapter 5 Study Guide

Chapter 5 Study Guide Chapter 5 Study Guide Practice Exam Questions: Which of the following is not included in the definition of learning? It is demonstrated immediately Assuming you have eaten sour pickles before, imagine

More information

Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events.

Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events. Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events. behaviorism - the view that psychology (1) should be an objective science

More information

Associative learning

Associative learning Introduction to Learning Associative learning Event-event learning (Pavlovian/classical conditioning) Behavior-event learning (instrumental/ operant conditioning) Both are well-developed experimentally

More information

Predictive Accuracy and the Effects of Partial Reinforcement on Serial Autoshaping

Predictive Accuracy and the Effects of Partial Reinforcement on Serial Autoshaping Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1985 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 0097-7403/85/$00.75 1985, VOl. 11, No. 4, 548-564 Predictive Accuracy and the

More information

Classical Conditioning. AKA: Pavlovian conditioning

Classical Conditioning. AKA: Pavlovian conditioning Classical Conditioning AKA: Pavlovian conditioning What is it? A type of learning where a stimulus gains the power to cause a response because it predicts another stimulus that already produces that response.

More information

Acquired Equivalence and Distinctiveness of Cues

Acquired Equivalence and Distinctiveness of Cues Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1989 by lhe American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 0097-740389$00.75 1989, Vol. 15, No. 4, 338-346 Acquired Equivalence and Distinctiveness

More information

Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive relationships in the context in which the target stimulus occurs

Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive relationships in the context in which the target stimulus occurs The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology ISSN: 1747-0218 (Print) 1747-0226 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pqje20 Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive

More information

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING JOURNL OF THE EXPERMENTL NLYSS OF BEHVOR 1979, 32, 121-127 NUMBER (JULY) CQUSTON OF STMULUS CONTROL WHLE NTRODUCNG NEW STMUL N FDNG LNNY FELDS THE COLLEGE OF STTEN SLND fter establishing a discrimination

More information

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1971, 15, 243-247 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) POSITIVE CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION: EFFECTS OF CS DURATION' KLAUS A. MICZEK AND SEBASTIAN P. GROSSMAN UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO

More information

Acquired Relational Equivalence: Implications for the Nature of Associative Structures

Acquired Relational Equivalence: Implications for the Nature of Associative Structures Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 0097-7403/98/$3.00 1998, Vol. 24, No, 3,325-334 Acquired Relational Equivalence:

More information

Stimulus and Temporal Cues in Classical Conditioning

Stimulus and Temporal Cues in Classical Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 2 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 97-743//$5. DOI: 1.137//97-743.26.2.26 2, Vol. 26, No. 2, 26-219 Stimulus and Temporal

More information

EBCC Data Analysis Tool (EBCC DAT) Introduction

EBCC Data Analysis Tool (EBCC DAT) Introduction Instructor: Paul Wolfgang Faculty sponsor: Yuan Shi, Ph.D. Andrey Mavrichev CIS 4339 Project in Computer Science May 7, 2009 Research work was completed in collaboration with Michael Tobia, Kevin L. Brown,

More information

Chapter 5: How Do We Learn?

Chapter 5: How Do We Learn? Chapter 5: How Do We Learn? Defining Learning A relatively permanent change in behavior or the potential for behavior that results from experience Results from many life experiences, not just structured

More information

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues Animal Learning & Behavior 1995, 23 (3), 273-279 Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues LOU M. SHERBURNE and

More information

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (4), 299-304 doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.299 Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions REBECCA RAYBURN-REEVES

More information

Taste quality and extinction of a conditioned taste aversion in rats

Taste quality and extinction of a conditioned taste aversion in rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of September 1999 Taste quality and extinction of

More information

Conditioned inhibition: a below zero effect/

Conditioned inhibition: a below zero effect/ University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 1973 Conditioned inhibition: a below zero effect/ Horace G. Marchant University of Massachusetts Amherst

More information

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1965, Vol. 59, No. 1, 13-17 AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 GORDON BOWER, RONALD STARR, AND LEAH LAZAROVITZ Stanford

More information

Eyeblink cross-habituation between tactile and acoustic systems in humans

Eyeblink cross-habituation between tactile and acoustic systems in humans Psychobiology 1997,250), 66-70 Eyeblink cross-habituation between tactile and acoustic systems in humans WILLIAM A DYCUS and ALICE SCHADE POWERS St. John's University, Jamaica, New York To study cross-habituation

More information

Learning. Learning. Learning

Learning. Learning. Learning Learning PSY 1000: Introduction to Psychology Learning Monkeys beginning to wash their food before they eat it The rituals that athletes perform before/during contests Birds learning to flutter their wings

More information

Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association

Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association Learning and Motivation 32, 349 366 (2001) doi:10.1006/lmot.2001.1089, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association Charlotte Bonardi

More information

CATS IN SHORTS. Easy reader of definitions and formal expressions. Holger Ursin March Uni Helse Universitetet i Bergen.

CATS IN SHORTS. Easy reader of definitions and formal expressions. Holger Ursin March Uni Helse Universitetet i Bergen. CATS IN SHORTS Easy reader of definitions and formal expressions Holger Ursin March 2009 Uni Helse Universitetet i Bergen Alarm Activation Load Stress Response Stress Stressor Stimuli Stimulus expectancy

More information

Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives

Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(4),621-629 Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives THOMAS R. ZENTALL University ofkentucky, Lexington,

More information

Overshadowing by fixed and variable duration stimuli. Centre for Computational and Animal Learning Research

Overshadowing by fixed and variable duration stimuli. Centre for Computational and Animal Learning Research Overshadowing by fixed and variable duration stimuli Charlotte Bonardi 1, Esther Mondragón 2, Ben Brilot 3 & Dómhnall J. Jennings 3 1 School of Psychology, University of Nottingham 2 Centre for Computational

More information

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 23, 343-367 (1992) Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training CHARLOTTE BONARDI University of York, York, United Kingdom In four experiments rats received

More information

Pain-elicited aggression in the squirrel monkey: An implicit avoidance contingency

Pain-elicited aggression in the squirrel monkey: An implicit avoidance contingency Animal Learning & Behavior 1976, Vol. 4(lA), 89-95 Pain-elicited aggression in the squirrel monkey: An implicit avoidance contingency PHILIP J, DUNHAM and ADAM CARR Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova

More information

Transfer after Serial Feature Positive Discrimination Training

Transfer after Serial Feature Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 17, 243-268 (1986) Transfer after Serial Feature Positive Discrimination Training PETER C. HOLLAND University of Pittsburgh Three experiments with rat subjects examined the transfer

More information

3/7/2010. Theoretical Perspectives

3/7/2010. Theoretical Perspectives Theoretical Perspectives REBT (1955) Albert Ellis Action & Result Oriented Teaches how to identify self-defeating thoughts Replaces thoughts w/ life enhancing ones 1 A B C s of personality formation: A

More information

Basic characteristics

Basic characteristics Learning Basic characteristics The belief that the universe is lawful and orderly The occurrence of phenomena as a function of the operation of specific variables Objective observation Controlled experiments

More information

Cue competition as a retrieval deficit

Cue competition as a retrieval deficit Denniston, J. C., Savastano, H. I., Blaisdell, A. P., & Miller, R. R. (2003). Cue competition as a retrieval deficit. Learning and Motivation, 34(1): 1-31. (Feb 2003) Published by Elsevier (ISSN: 1095-9122).

More information

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition Scholarly Commons Psychology Faculty Publications 2015 Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition James F. Briggs Timothy A. Toth Brian P. Olson Jacob G.

More information

Relations Between Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer and Reinforcer Devaluation

Relations Between Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer and Reinforcer Devaluation Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2004, Vol. 30, No. 2, 104 117 Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.30.2.104

More information

Association. Operant Conditioning. Classical or Pavlovian Conditioning. Learning to associate two events. We learn to. associate two stimuli

Association. Operant Conditioning. Classical or Pavlovian Conditioning. Learning to associate two events. We learn to. associate two stimuli Myers PSYCHOLOGY (7th Ed) Chapter 8 Learning James A. McCubbin, PhD Clemson University Worth Publishers Learning Learning relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience Association

More information

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 05, 3, 3 45 NUMBER (JANUARY) WITHIN-SUBJECT TESTING OF THE SIGNALED-REINFORCEMENT EFFECT ON OPERANT RESPONDING AS MEASURED BY RESPONSE RATE AND RESISTANCE

More information

Summation and transfer of negative occasion setting

Summation and transfer of negative occasion setting nimal Learning & ehavior /993, 2/ (2), /45-/53 Summation and transfer of negative occasion setting JVIER R. MORELL and PETER C. HOLLND Duke University, Durham, North Carolina Two experiments examined the

More information