Developmental Differences in Preferences for Using Color, Size, and Location Information to Disambiguate Hiding Places

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Developmental Differences in Preferences for Using Color, Size, and Location Information to Disambiguate Hiding Places"

Transcription

1 JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT, 8(4), Copyright 2007, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Developmental Differences in Preferences for Using Color, Size, and Location Information to Disambiguate Hiding Places Jodie M. Plumert and Penney Nichols-Whitehead The University of Iowa We conducted four experiments to examine developmental differences in preferences for using color, size, and location information to disambiguate hiding places. Three- and 4-year-olds and adults described how to find a miniature mouse that was hidden in one of two highly similar small objects in a dollhouse. In Experiment 1, the hiding places could be disambiguated by either color or location. Three-year-olds preferred color to location whereas adults preferred location to color information. Four-year-olds showed no preferences. In Experiment 2, the hiding places could be disambiguated by either size or location. Four-year-olds preferred size to location information whereas adults preferred location to size information. Three-year-olds showed no preferences. In Experiment 3, the hiding places could be disambiguated by either color or size information. Adults preferred size to color information, but 3- and 4-year-olds showed no preference for either type of information. Experiment 4 revealed that when only location information was available for disambiguating the hiding places, 4-year-olds referred to disambiguating location information on a significantly greater percentage of trials than did 3-year-olds. Discussion focuses on the role of relational complexity and pragmatic knowledge in producing preferences for disambiguating information in spatial communication tasks. Children and adults alike are frequently asked to describe the whereabouts of missing objects. One of the challenges speakers confront when describing object locations is disambiguating between potentially confusable places (e.g., two or more couches in a house). Typically, several types of information are available for dis- Correspondence should be sent to Jodie M. Plumert, Department of Psychology, 11 SSH East, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA jodie-plumert@uiowa.edu

2 428 PLUMERT, NICHOLS-WHITEHEAD ambiguating hiding places. For example, one might distinguish the target place (e.g., a couch) from other confusable places by referring to location information (e.g., The shoes are by the couch in the living room ), color information (e.g., The shoes are by the green couch ), or size information ( The shoes are by the little couch ). In nearly all studies of the development of spatial communication, however, location information is the only type of information available for disambiguating hiding places (Plumert, Ewert, & Spear, 1995; Plumert & Hawkins, 2001; Plumert & Nichols-Whitehead, 1996; for an exception see Craton, Elicker, Plumert, & Pick, 1990). Thus, little is known about young children s preferences for disambiguating information when multiple sources of information are available. The goal of this investigation was to examine developmental differences in preferences for disambiguating information when multiple sources of information were available for distinguishing between two highly similar hiding places. What factors might play a role in governing preferences for disambiguating information? One possibility is the complexity of the disambiguating information. Consider a situation in which a child asks a 4-year-old sibling where a missing toy is located. In the playroom there are two boxes, one of which contains the missing toy. One way for the sibling to disambiguate the two boxes is by referring to location information (e.g., It s in the box that s on the table ). Another way for the sibling to disambiguate the two boxes is by referring to color information (e.g., It s in the box that s yellow ). To convey disambiguating location information, the sibling has to compare the spatial relations between the two sets of small and large landmarks (e.g., a box on the table vs. a box on the couch). To convey disambiguating color information, the sibling has to compare the colors of the two small landmarks (e.g., the yellow box vs. the red box). Clearly, comparing two relations between objects is more complex than comparing the properties of two objects. Young children may be more likely to choose a simpler source of disambiguating information over a more complex source of disambiguating information when both are available. This idea is consistent with previous research on developmental changes in young children s use of relational information (Andrews & Halford, 2002; Genter, 1988; Gentner & Ratterman, 1991; Goswami, 1989; Halford, Andrews, Dalton, Boag, & Zielinski, 2002; Halford, Wilson, & Phillips, 1998; Ratterman & Gentner, 1998). Research on analogical reasoning, conducted by Gentner and her colleagues, has shown that young children have difficulty making comparisons based solely on relational information (Gentner, 1988; Gentner & Ratterman, 1991; Goswami, 1989; Ratterman & Gentner, 1998). According to Gentner (1988), children between the ages of 3 and 5 undergo a shift from using object similarity to using relational similarity to solve analogical reasoning tasks. In one study, for example, children were presented with two arrays of objects arranged in an order of decreasing size (Gentner & Ratterman, 1991). Object similarity and relational similarity were placed in direct competition with each other by making the first ob-

3 PREFERENCES FOR DISAMBIGUATING INFORMATION 429 ject in one array identical in size to the second object in the other array. Children first watched as the experimenter placed a sticker under an object in one of the arrays. Children then searched for a sticker under the corresponding object in the second array. Five-year-olds preferred the relational match whereas 3-year-olds preferred the object match. These findings are consistent with the idea that children s understanding and use of relational information undergoes significant change over the preschool years. Work by Halford and his colleagues also supports the idea that relational complexity plays an important role in children s performance on a variety of reasoning tasks (Andrews & Halford, 2002, Halford et al., 1998, 2002). According to Halford, much of the developmental change seen in children s reasoning skills can be explained by changes in the ability to process more complex relations. Relational complexity is defined as the number of arguments or entities that must be simultaneously considered to solve a problem. Unary relations have a single argument as in The dog is brown. Binary relations have two arguments as in The dog is bigger than the cat. Ternary relations have three arguments as in If Mary is taller than Sue and Sue is taller than Ellen, then Mary is taller than Ellen. According to Halford, unary relations can be processed at approximately one year of age, binary relations at two years of age, and ternary relations at five years of age. How might this framework apply to the problem of disambiguating two confusable hiding places? Consider two hiding places (one containing the missing object) that differ in color, size, and location. To distinguish the two hiding places using color, the speaker need only consider two unary relations (e.g., the red box vs. the yellow box). To distinguish the two hiding places using size, the speaker must consider a single binary relation between two objects (e.g., the big box vs. the small box). To distinguish the two hiding places using location, the speaker must consider two binary relations (e.g., the box on the table vs. the box on the couch). According to this analysis, location information involves more complex relational information as compared to color or size information. As a result, young children may avoid location information when other, less complex choices are available. A second factor that may govern the choice of disambiguating information is pragmatic knowledge. Consider again the situation in which a child asks his 4-year-old sibling where a missing toy is located. Given that the listener s goal is to find the missing toy, one might expect that the speaker would favor location information over other types of competing information. Implicitly, location information may be more closely matched to the listener s goal of locating the missing toy than is color or size information. Thus, pragmatic knowledge about the listener s goal may also play a role in guiding the speaker s choice of disambiguating information. Adults appear to be quite sensitive to the goals of the listener in communication tasks (Klabunde & Porzel, 1998; Plumert, Carswell, DeVet, & Ihrig, 1995; Plumert, Spalding, & Nichols-Whitehead, 2001, Russell & Schober, 1999;

4 430 PLUMERT, NICHOLS-WHITEHEAD Schober, 1995). For example, Plumert et al. (1995, 2001) have shown that the ordering of spatial information in directions for finding missing objects appears to be governed by the needs of the listener. In particular, adults order landmarks and regions from large to small when giving directions for finding hidden objects (e.g., Go down two flights, turn right, on the right side of the room against the back wall there is a table, under the table is a box full of newspapers, under a small stack is the chips. ). Why might this be the case? When the task is to give directions to someone else about how to find something, the goal is to get the listener from his or her present position to the location of the object. Thus, speakers may feel constrained to convey units of spatial information in the order in which the listener will encounter them. Movement through the space generally dictates that the listener encounters spatial units (i.e., landmarks and regions) in an order of decreasing size. That is, the listener must reach the floor before the room, and once inside the room, is likely to notice large landmarks before small landmarks. The idea that adults order spatial information from large to small in their directions to accommodate the needs of the listener is further supported by research showing that adults access spatial information from memory in an order of small to large (Plumert et al., 2001). In other words, adults are faster to verify sentences describing object locations when the spatial units are ordered from smallest to largest, indicating that they recall object locations by starting at the most specific level of the hierarchy. This suggests that they reorder spatial information from large to small in their directions to accommodate the needs of the listener. What does previous research tell us about whether young children take into account the goals or intentions of the listener? Research on pragmatic development has clearly shown that preschoolers take into account some aspects of the listener in forming their utterances (e.g., Maratsos, 1973; Shatz & Gelman, 1973). For example, 3- to 5-year-olds used far less pointing in a referential communication task when the listener was blindfolded than when the listener was not blindfolded. Likewise, Shatz and Gelman (1973) found that 4-year-olds used less complex speech when talking with 2-year-olds than when talking with their peers or mothers. Thus, preschoolers respond to highly observable characteristics of the listener by modifying their speech to better fit the listener s needs. Other research, however, has shown that young children have difficulty taking into account less observable characteristics of the listener such as goals or intentions (e.g., Abbeduto, Nuccio, Al-Mabuk, Rotto, & Maas, 1992). Such difficulty could inhibit preschoolers use of location information (relative to adults ) to disambiguate hiding places, particularly when simpler types of disambiguating information (e.g., color and size) are available. The goal of this investigation was to examine developmental differences in preferences for location, color, and size information for disambiguating hiding places. Three- and 4-year-olds and a comparison group of adults described how to find a miniature mouse hidden in a small dollhouse. Several pairs of small objects

5 PREFERENCES FOR DISAMBIGUATING INFORMATION 431 served as hiding places (e.g., bags, boxes, plants). The mouse was always hidden with one member of the pair, while the other member remained empty. Thus, in order to communicate clearly about the hiding place of the mouse, participants had to disambiguate the target-hiding place from the nontarget hiding place. In Experiment 1, the hiding place could be disambiguated by referring to either color ( It s in the bag with the red stripe ) or location ( It s in the bag next to the couch ) information. Based on young children s difficulty with using more complex relational information, we expected that 3- and 4-year-olds would prefer color to location information. In contrast, we expected that adults would prefer location to color information based on their greater sensitivity to the pragmatics of the task. In Experiment 2, the hiding places could be disambiguated by referring to either size ( It s in the big bag ) or location ( It s in the bag next to the couch ). We expected that 3- and 4-year-olds would prefer size to location information, but that adults again would prefer location information. In Experiment 3, we contrasted disambiguating color and size information to determine whether children and adults exhibited a preference for either of these two types of nonspatial information. Finally, in Experiment 4, we examined 3- and 4-year-olds references to location information when only location information was available to disambiguate the hiding places. EXPERIMENT 1 Method Participants. Participants were 42 three-year-olds, four-year-olds, and adults from predominantly middle- to upper middle-class Caucasian families. The mean ages were 3 years and 10 months (range = 3; 9 3;11 ), 4 years and 10 months (range = 4;9 to 4;11), and 19 years and 3 months (range = 18;1 20;0). There were 7 boys and 7 girls in the 3-year-old group, 8 boys and 6 girls in the 4-year-old group, and 7 men and 7 women in the adult group. The children were recruited from a child research participant database. Parents received a letter describing the study followed by a phone call inviting their child to participate. The adults participated to fulfill research credit in an introductory psychology course. Apparatus and Materials. A 56-cm wide cm deep cm high model room was used as the experimental space (see Figure 1). A miniature mouse served as the hidden object and a 3.8-cm high troll figure served as the listener. Within the model room there were eight pairs of small objects that served as hiding places. These included pillows, bags, trashcans, towels, hats, pots, plants, and shoes. The two members of each pair of small objects differed by a single color feature. There was a pillow with a red heart and a pillow with a yellow heart, a grocery bag with red lettering and a grocery bag with green

6 432 PLUMERT, NICHOLS-WHITEHEAD FIGURE 1 Dollhouse used as the experimental space. lettering, a towel with a yellow ribbon and a towel with a blue ribbon, a hat with a blue ribbon and a hat with a green ribbon, a pot with a red lid and a pot with a blue lid, a plant with a red striped pot and a plant with a blue striped pot, and a shoe with yellow stripes and a shoe with green stripes. Eight pieces of furniture served as landmarks; a TV, chair, piano, couch, stove, table, refrigerator, and a sink, for one pair of small objects. One member of the pair was placed on the piece of furniture, and the other was placed next to and touching the furniture item. Thus, the target member of the pair (i.e., the hiding place) could be disambiguated from the nontarget member by using color information (e.g., The hat with the blue ribbon. ) or location information (e.g., The hat on the chair. ). A Plexiglas cover that could be raised and lowered over the front of the house was used to prevent children from pointing directly at hiding places or retrieving the mouse before they described its hiding place. The dollhouse was placed on a low table and the child was always seated directly in front of it. The experimenter sat on the child s left side. The entire session was videotaped via a Panasonic camcorder positioned above the dollhouse.

7 PREFERENCES FOR DISAMBIGUATING INFORMATION 433 Design and Procedure Familiarization. Participants were tested individually in the laboratory. During familiarization with the dollhouse, participants were shown the troll figure and told that they would be giving directions to the troll about how to find a mouse in the dollhouse. Participants were then asked to name each object in the dollhouse. The experimenter pointed out the objects in the dollhouse in a random order with the constraint that she pointed out both members of each pair of small objects together. That is, after the participant named the first member of a pair, the experimenter would say, Did you see there is another, just like it right here? This was done to ensure that participants were aware of the pairs of small objects. If participants could not name an item, the experimenter supplied the label and later questioned them about that item to make sure they remembered its name. Practice trial. After familiarization, children were instructed that the troll would hide behind the dollhouse while they helped the experimenter hide the mouse. The troll would then come back to the front of the house, and they would try to tell the troll exactly how to find the mouse. Children first were given a practice trial in which they hid the mouse under the couch and were asked to describe how to find the mouse to the troll. This practice trial was designed to engage children in the task of verbally describing a hiding place and the practice trial did not provide children with experience distinguishing between two highly confusable hiding places because there was only one couch in the dollhouse. After completing the practice trial, children were praised for their effort (most children had no trouble describing the hiding place). Test trials. There were eight test trials, each involving one of the eight target small objects. The member of each pair that served as the hiding place was counterbalanced across participants. Four of the target hiding places involved the spatial relation of support (i.e., on the furniture landmark) and four involved the relation of proximity (i.e., next to and touching the furniture landmark). The order in which participants described the hiding places was randomized across subjects. For each trial, the experimenter put the troll behind the dollhouse and then touched the hiding place with a pencil and instructed the participant: Hide the mouse right there. For all hiding places, the mouse was completely hidden from view. After the mouse was hidden, the experimenter closed the cover and reminded children not to point to the mouse s hiding place. Children were also instructed to either cross their arms or sit on their hands. The experimenter then brought the troll to the front of the dollhouse and said, Can you tell the troll exactly how to find the mouse? The delay between when participants finished hiding the mouse and when the experimenter finished asking them to describe how to find the mouse was approximately 10 s. If the participant s initial description was inadequate to spec-

8 434 PLUMERT, NICHOLS-WHITEHEAD ify how to find the mouse, the experimenter would give a series of structured prompts to elicit more information. The first prompt was always, Can you tell the troll anything else about how to find the mouse? If the participant only responded with a reference to the small object, (e.g., It s under the hat. ), the experimenter followed up the first prompt with, I see two hats. Can you tell the troll anything else about how to find the mouse? If the child only responded with a reference to the landmark (e.g., It s by the couch ), the experimenter would say, I see two things with the couch. Can you tell the troll anything else about how to find the mouse? The trial ended when the participant provided sufficient information to specify the hiding place of the mouse or the participant failed to provide sufficient information in response to the series of structured prompts. When participants provided sufficient disambiguating information, the experimenter searched for the mouse in the correct place. When participants failed to provide sufficient disambiguating information, however, the experimenter searched in the wrong hiding place (i.e., the wrong member of the pair of small objects). Upon failing to find the mouse, the experimenter would say, Hmmm, it s not here, and proceed to look in the correct hiding place. This aspect of the procedure was designed to further encourage children to provide disambiguating information. Color production test. Participants were given a color production test after the communication task to determine whether they could verbally disambiguate between objects on the basis of the colors used in this experiment (i.e., red, yellow, blue, and green). A 10.2 cm cm piece of colored construction paper was held up and the participant was asked to name the color. All children were able to name the colors. At the end of the session, children were allowed to select two prizes and were thanked for their participation. Adults were debriefed about the nature of the experiment and thanked for their participation. Coding and Measures All verbalizations were transcribed verbatim and coded for presence or absence of the targeted information. The following aspects of participants descriptions were coded: a) small object references, b) location references, and c) color references. All descriptions were coded, including those provided after the specific prompt. A small object reference was coded as present when children mentioned or described the object with which the mouse was hidden (e.g., The mouse is in the bag or The mouse is in the grocery thing ). Participants received a small object reference score representing the percentage of trials in which they mentioned the small object. A location reference was coded as present when children mentioned or described the spatial relation between the small object and a landmark (e.g., the shoe by the piano or the pot on the floor ). Although children sometimes re-

9 PREFERENCES FOR DISAMBIGUATING INFORMATION 435 ferred to the small object and a landmark in a single description, the two were often produced separately. For example, it was not uncommon for children to give the small object spontaneously (e.g., it s in the shoe ), and to provide a disambiguating landmark in response to a prompt (e.g., the one by the piano ). Children were given credit for a location reference in either case. Children received a location reference score that represented the percentage of trials in which they mentioned location information. A color reference was coded as present when children mentioned color information that differentiated the target from the nontarget member of the small object pair (e.g., the hat with the blue ribbon or the blue hat ). Again, although children sometimes referred to the small object and its color in a single description, the two were often produced separately. Children were given credit for a color reference in either case. Children received a color reference score that represented the percentage of trials in which they referred to the color of the target small object. Intercoder reliabilities were calculated on nine randomly selected participants (three from each age group). Exact percent agreement for small object references, location references, and color references were 100, 97, and 97%, respectively. Results References to the Small Object As expected, children and adults alike usually referred to the small object when describing how to find the mouse. On average, 3-year-olds, 4-year-olds, and adults referred to the small object on 96% (SD = 12%), 97% (SD = 7%), and 100% (SD = 0%), of trials, respectively. A one-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) revealed that thepercentageofsmallobjectreferencesdidnotdifferbyage,f(2,39)=.90,ns. References to Disambiguating Location and Color Information Were there developmental differences in preferences for using location and color information to disambiguate hiding places? We first examined the percentage of trials on which participants provided either kind of disambiguating information as an indication of their overall success in the task. Prior to prompting, 3-year-olds, 4-year-olds, and adults provided some form of disambiguating information on 62% (SD = 27%), 55% (SD = 36%), and 100% of trials, respectively. After prompting, 3-year-olds, 4-year-olds, and adults provided some form of disambiguating information on 80% (SD = 18.5%), 84% (SD = 29%), and 100% (SD = 0%) of trials, respectively. A one-way ANOVA on references to disambiguating information after prompting with age as a factor was significant, F (2, 39) = 4.17, p <.05. Post-hoc analyses using Tukey s Honestly Significant Difference (HSD) revealed that adults provided disambiguating information on a significantly greater

10 436 PLUMERT, NICHOLS-WHITEHEAD percentage of trials than did the 3-year-olds. The 4-year-olds and adults did not differ significantly, nor did the 3- and 4-year-olds. To examine whether there were developmental differences in preferences for the two types of disambiguating information, we entered the percentage of location references and color references into an Age (3 yrs vs. 4 yrs vs. adults) 3 Information Type (location vs. color) repeated measures ANOVA with the first factor as a between-subjects variable and the second as a within-subjects variable. 1 This analysis yielded a significant effect of age, F (2, 39) = 4.32, p <.05, and a significant Age 3 Information Type interaction, F (2, 39) = 13.31, p < We examined the Age 3 Information Type interaction in two ways. First, we examined whether there was a preference for either type of disambiguating information at each age. As shown in Figure 2, simple effects test revealed a significant preference for color over location information for 3-year-olds, F (1, 13) = 5.08, p <.05, no significant preference for either type of information for 4-year-olds, F (1, 13) =.53, ns, and a significant preference for location over color information for adults, F (1, 13) = 33.88, p < Thus, when both location and color information were available for disambiguating the hiding place, adults exhibited a clear preference for location information. In contrast, 3-year-olds exhibited a clear preference for color information. Four-year-olds did not exhibit a significant preference for either type of information. Second, we examined whether there were age differences in the percentage of references to each type of information. As shown in Figure 2, there was a significant effect of age for references to disambiguating location information, F (2, 39) = 21.93, p <.0001, and a significant effect of age for references to disambiguating color information, F (2, 39) = 3.25, p <.05. Follow-up analyses using Tukey s HSD test revealed that adults referred to location information on a much higher percentage of trials than did 3- and 4-year-olds. In fact, adults referred to location information on nearly all trials, whereas children referred to location information on less than half of all trials. Follow-up analyses also revealed that 3-year-olds referred to color information on a higher percentage of trials than did adults. Four-year-olds did not differ significantly from either 3-year-olds or adults. Thus, references to disambiguating location information increased with age and references to disambiguating color information decreased with age. Use of redundant disambiguating information. We also examined whether there were age differences in the propensity to provide redundant disambiguating information. In other words, when children and adults provided one type of disam- 1 Each of the percentage scores entered into the analyses was free to independently vary between zero and 100%. That is, use of one type of information did not preclude the use of the other type of information. Potentially, a participant could have scores of 100% for both types of information. Thus, the percentage scores were statistically independent.

11 PREFERENCES FOR DISAMBIGUATING INFORMATION 437 FIGURE 2 References to disambiguating color and location information by age. biguating information, did they also sometimes provide the other type of disambiguating information? To address this question, the percentage of trials in which participants referred to both location and color information (e.g., under the hat with the blue stripe on the chair ) was entered into a one-way ANOVA with age as a between-subjects factor. Although there was some tendency for adults to provide more redundant information than children, this analysis did not yield a significant effect of age, F (2, 39) = 1.74, ns. Three-year-olds, 4-year-olds, and adults provided redundant location and color information on 9% (SD = 19%), 12% (SD = 18%), and 25% (SD = 33%) of trials, respectively. Individual patterns of performance. Finally, we examined individual patterns of performance to explore whether participants in each age group showed strong biases toward color or location information. Across the eight test trials, we calculated the percentage of participants who referred only to color or only to location information, or referred at least once to each type of information (but not necessarily in the same trial). As shown in Table 1, about half of the 3-year-olds showed a strong bias toward color (they never mentioned location on any trial) and half of the adults showed a strong bias toward location (they never mentioned color on any trial). The remaining 3-year-olds and adults used some of each type of in-

12 438 PLUMERT, NICHOLS-WHITEHEAD TABLE 1 Number of Participants Referring to Only Color, Only Location, or to Color and Location Across Trials Information Type Age Group Color Only Location Only Color and Location 3-year-olds 6 (43%) 2 (14%) 6 (43%) 4-year-olds 3 (21%) 3 (21%) 8 (57%) Adults 0 (0%) 7 (50%) 7 (50%) formation. The 4-year-olds fell somewhere in between, with some exhibiting a strong bias toward color and some exhibiting a strong bias toward location, but over half using some of each type of information. Thus, the individual patterns of performance generally mirrored the patterns of group performance above. Discussion The results of this experiment clearly show that when both location and color information were available for disambiguating hiding places, 3-year-olds preferred to use color over location information and adults preferred to use location over color information. Four-year-olds showed no significant preference for either source of information. Moreover, children s use of location information to disambiguate hiding places was much lower than that of adults. Adults used location information on virtually all trials whereas children used location information on less than half of the trials. What accounts for this developmental difference in the use of disambiguating location information? We hypothesize that adults prefer location information to other types of information because they are sensitive to the pragmatics of the task. In other words, location information is more closely matched the goal of finding the hidden mouse than did color information. We hypothesize that young children s preferences are driven by the complexity of the disambiguating information. That is, young children were less likely to use disambiguating location information than were adults because location information involved making complex relational comparisons, with which they might have difficulty. In Experiment 2, we tested whether young children avoid using disambiguating location information because they have difficulty with making relational comparisons in general or whether their avoidance of disambiguating location information stems more specifically from difficulty with making complex relational comparisons. In this experiment, we pitted disambiguating size information (e.g., It s under the big hat ) against disambiguating location information (e.g., It s under the

13 PREFERENCES FOR DISAMBIGUATING INFORMATION 439 hat next to the couch ). Using size information to disambiguate two hiding places requires that children compare the single, binary relation between the two hats (e.g., the big hat vs. the little hat). Using location information to disambiguate two hiding places requires that children compare the binary relation between the target hat and its corresponding landmark (e.g., the couch) to the binary relation between the nontarget hat and its corresponding landmark (e.g., the TV). Within Halford s framework, considering two binary relations is more complex than considering a single binary relation. If young children s difficulty with using disambiguating location information stems from difficulty with more complex relations, then they should show a preference for size information over location information. In contrast, adults should show a preference for location information over size information based on their sensitivity to the pragmatics of the task. EXPERIMENT 2 Method Participants Participants were 42 each of three-year-olds, four-year-olds, and adults from predominantly middle- to upper middle-class Caucasian families. The mean ages were 3 years and 7 months (range = 3;6 3;8), 4 years and 6 months (range = 4;3 4;10), and 20 years and 4 months (range = 18;9 27;7). There were 8 boys and 6 girls in the 3-year-old group, 7 boys and 7 girls in the 4-year-old group, and 7 men and 7 women in the adult group. The children and adults were recruited in the same manner as in Experiment 1. Apparatus and Materials The same dollhouse and furniture landmarks were used as in Experiment 1. The eight pairs of small objects included bags, pillows, pots, plants, baskets, boxes, hats, and pitchers. The two members of each small object pair were identical except that one was large and one was small. The large object was approximately 1.5 times the size of the small object (range = ). Design and Procedure All aspects of the procedure were identical to those used in Experiment 1. After the communication task, we gave participants a size production test to determine whether they could verbally distinguish between objects on the basis of size. Participants were shown a novel pair of objects that were identical except for size. The experimenter said, See these? Can you tell me about them? If the participant did not spontaneously volunteer information about the size difference, the experi-

14 440 PLUMERT, NICHOLS-WHITEHEAD menter said, Can you tell me how they are different? This procedure was repeated for a second pair of novel objects. All children were able to verbally note the size difference between the objects. Coding and Measures The coding and measures were identical to those used in Experiment 1 except that rather than color references; we coded size references in addition to location references and small object references. A size reference was coded as present when participants mentioned something about the size of the target small object (e.g., the big pillow or not the little pillow, the big pillow ). Intercoder reliabilities were calculated on nine randomly selected participants (three from each age group). Exact percent agreement for small object references, location references, and size references was 100%. Results References to the Small Object As in Experiment 1, children and adults alike almost always referred to the small object when describing how to find the mouse. On average, 3-year-olds, 4-year-olds, and adults referred to the small object on 99% (SD = 3%), 100% (SD = 0%), and 100% (SD = 0%) of trials, respectively. A one-way ANOVA revealed that the percentage of small object references did not differ by age, F (2, 39) = 1.00, ns. References to Disambiguating Location and Size Information The primary question of interest was whether there were developmental differences in preferences for using location and size information to disambiguate the target from the nontarget hiding place. We first examined the percentage of trials on which participants provided either kind of disambiguating information as an indication of their overall success in the task. Prior to prompting, 3-year-olds, 4-year-olds, and adults provided some form of disambiguating information on 59% (SD = 31%), 62% (SD = 32%), and 100% of trials, respectively. After prompting, however, 3-year-olds, 4-year-olds, and adults provided some form of disambiguating information on 83% (SD = 28%), 88% (SD = 15%), and 100% of trials, respectively. A one-way ANOVA on references to disambiguating information after prompting with age as a factor was marginally significant, F (2, 39) = 3.17, p =.053. To examine whether there were developmental differences in preferences for the two types of disambiguating information, we entered the percentage of location references and size references into an Age (3) 3 Information Type (2) repeated measures ANOVA with the first factor as a between-subjects variable and

15 PREFERENCES FOR DISAMBIGUATING INFORMATION 441 the second as a within-subjects variable. This analysis yielded a significant effect of age, F (2, 39) = 12.51, p <.0001, and a significant Age 3 Information Type interaction, F (2, 39) = 11.27, p <.001. We examined the Age 3 Information Type interaction in two ways. First, we examined whether there was a preference for either type of disambiguating information at each age. As shown in Figure 3, simple effects tests revealed no significant preference for location or size for 3-year-olds, F (1, 13) =.06, ns, a significant preference for size over location information for 4-year-olds, F (1, 13) = 11.95, p <.01, and a significant preference for location over size information for adults, F (1, 13) = 24.21, p <.001. Thus, when both location and size information were available for disambiguating the hiding place, adults exhibited a clear preference for location information. In contrast, 4-year-olds exhibited a clear preference for size information. Three-year-olds did not exhibit a significant preference for either type of information. Second, we examined whether there were age differences in the percentage of references to each type of information. As shown in Figure 3, there was a significant effect of age for references to disambiguating location information, F (2, 39) = 23.75, p <.0001, and a significant effect of age for references to disambiguating size information, F (2, 39) = 3.78, p <.05. Follow-up analyses revealed that adults referred to location information on a much higher percentage of trials than did 3- and 4-year-olds. In addition, 3-year-olds referred to location information on a higher percentage of trials than did 4-year-olds. As in Experiment 1, adults referred to location information on nearly all trials whereas chil- FIGURE 3 References to disambiguating size and location information by age.

16 442 PLUMERT, NICHOLS-WHITEHEAD dren referred to location information on less than half of all trials. Follow-up analyses also revealed that 4-year-olds referred to size information on a higher percentage of trials than did adults. Three-year-olds did not differ significantly from either 4-year-olds or adults. Use of redundant disambiguating information We also examined whether there were age differences in the propensity to provide redundant disambiguating information (i.e., both location and size information). To address this issue, the percentage of trials in which participants referred to both location and size information (e.g., under the big box on the chair ) was entered into a one-way ANOVA with age as a between-subjects factor. This analysis yielded a significant effect of age, F (2, 39) = 9.96, p <.001. Follow-up analyses revealed that adults provided redundant information on a significantly greater percentage of trials than did 3- and 4-year-olds. Three-year-olds, 4-year-olds, and adults provided redundant location and size information on 1% (SD = 4%), 2% (SD = 5%), and 32% (SD = 36%) of trials, respectively. Individual patterns of performance. Finally, we examined individual patterns of performance to explore whether participants in each age group showed strong biases toward size or location information. Across the eight test trials, we calculated the percentage of participants who referred only to size or only to location information, or referred at least once to both types of information (but not necessarily in the same trial). As shown in Table 2, nearly two-thirds of the 4-year-olds showed a strong bias toward size (they never mentioned location on any trial). In contrast to Experiment 1, however, relatively few of the adults showed a strong bias toward location (they never mentioned size on any trial). Most of the adults referred to size on at least one trial, although none of them referred exclusively to size whereas some referred exclusively to location. The 3-year-olds were about evenly split between using size exclusively, location exclusively, or referring to both size and location. Thus, although the group data show a strong bias toward TABLE 2 Number of Participants Referring to Only Size, Only Location, or to Size and Location Across Trials Information Type Age Group Size Only Location Only Size and Location 3-year-olds 3 (21%) 5 (36%) 5 (36%) 4-year-olds 9 (64%) 2 (14%) 3 (21%) 0 (0%) 4 (29%) 10 (71%) Note. There were 12 participants in each age group. One 3-year-old provided no references to either size or location information.

17 PREFERENCES FOR DISAMBIGUATING INFORMATION 443 location in adults, the individual patterns of performance indicate that most adults referred to size information at least once. Discussion When both location and size informations were available for disambiguating hiding places, 4-year-olds preferred to use size to location information. However, adults preferred to use location to size information. Three-year-olds showed no significant preference for either source of information. Moreover, children s use of location information to disambiguate hiding places was much lower than that of adults. Adults used location information on virtually all trials whereas children used location information on less than half the trials. These results suggest that young children s difficulty with using disambiguating location information stems from their difficulty with making more complex relational comparisons rather than difficulty with making any kind of relational comparison. We further addressed this hypothesis in a third experiment in which we pitted size information against color information. If young children s avoidance of location information stems from difficulty with more complex relations, then they should show no preference for color or size information. In other words, because neither size nor color information makes two binary comparisons, young children should show little or no preference for one source of information over the other. Likewise, adults should show no preference for color or size information. That is, unlike location information, neither size nor color information seems particularly well matched to the goal of finding a hidden object. EXPERIMENT 3 Method Participants Participants were 42 each of three-year-olds, four-year-olds, and adults from predominantly middle- to upper middle-class Caucasian families. The mean ages were 3 years and 9 months (range = 3;5 4;0), 4 years and 9 months (range = 4;1 5;0), and 19 years and 9 months (range = 18;4 22;11). There were 8 boys and 6 girls in the 3-year-old group, 7 boys and 7 girls in the 4-year-old group, and 7 men and 7 women in the adult group. The children and adults were recruited in the same manner as in the previous experiments. Apparatus and Materials The same dollhouse and furniture landmarks were used as in the previous experiments. The same eight pairs of small objects were used as in Experiment 2. In

18 444 PLUMERT, NICHOLS-WHITEHEAD this experiment, however, the two members of each small object pair differed both in size and in color. In addition, both members of each pair of small objects were placed next to the same piece of furniture. Thus, the hiding place could not be disambiguated using location information because both members of the pair were on the floor next to the same piece of furniture. Design and Procedure All aspects of the procedure were identical to those used in the previous experiments. After completing the communication task, participants were given the color and the size production tests used in the previous experiments. All children were able to name the four colors and distinguish between objects based on size. Coding and Measures Small object references, color references, and size references were coded in the same manner as in the previous experiments. Intercoder reliabilities were calculated on nine randomly selected participants (three from each age group). Exact percent agreement for small object references, color references, and size references were 100, 100, and 99%, respectively. Results References to the Small Object As in the previous experiments, children and adults alike usually referred to the small object when describing how to find the mouse. On average, 3-year-olds, 4-year-olds, and adults referred to the small object on 99% (SD = 3%), 99% (SD = 3%), and 99% (SD = 3%) of trials, respectively. A one-way ANOVA revealed that the percentage of small object references did not differ by age, F (2, 39) = 0.0, ns. References to Disambiguating Color and Size Information Were there developmental differences in preferences for using color and size information to disambiguate the hiding place? We first examined the percentage of trials on which participants provided any kind of disambiguating information as a measure of their overall success in the task. Prior to prompting, 3-year-olds, 4-year-olds, and adults provided some form of disambiguating information on 67% (SD = 26%), 64% (SD = 17%), and 99% (SD = 3%) of trials, respectively. After prompting, however, 3-year-olds, 4-year-olds, and adults provided some form of disambiguating information on 87% (SD = 16%), 92% (SD = 12%), and 99% (SD =.5%) of trials, respectively. A one-way ANOVA on references to disambiguating information after prompting with age as a factor was significant, F (2, 39) =

19 PREFERENCES FOR DISAMBIGUATING INFORMATION , p <.05. Tukey s HSD tests revealed that adults provided disambiguating information on a significantly greater percentage of trials than did the 3-year-olds. The 4-year-olds and adults did not differ significantly, nor did the 3- and 4-year-olds. To examine whether there were developmental differences in preferences for the two types of disambiguating information, we entered the percentage of color references and size references into an Age (3) x Information Type (2) repeated measures ANOVA. This analysis yielded significant effects of age, F (2, 39) = 24.10, p <.0001, and information type, F (1, 39) = 5.58, p <.05. Unlike the previous two experiments, this overall analysis did not yield a significant Age x Information Type interaction, F (2, 39) =.59, ns. However, because our goal was to examine whether there are age differences in preferences for using color and size information to disambiguate hiding places, we carried out a series of planned comparisons parallel to the analyses used in the previous experiments to examine Age x Information Type interactions. First, we examined whether there was a preference for either type of disambiguating information at each age. As shown in Figure 4, simple effects tests revealed a significant preference for size over color information for adults, F (1, 13) = 10.04, p <.01, but no significant preference for either type of information for 3-year-olds, F (1, 13) = 1.89, ns, or for 4-year-olds, F (1, 13) =.21, ns. Thus, when both color and size, but no location information was available for disambiguating the hiding place, adults exhibited a clear preference for size information. In contrast, 3- and 4-year-olds exhib- FIGURE 4 References to disambiguating size and color information by age.

20 446 PLUMERT, NICHOLS-WHITEHEAD ited no significant preference for either type of information. Second, we examined whether there were age differences in the percentage of references to each type of information. As shown in Figure 4, there was a significant effect of age for references to disambiguating size information, F (2, 39) = 7.03, p <.01, but no significant effect of age for references to disambiguating color information, F (2, 39) = 2.06, ns. Follow-up analyses revealed that adults referred to size information on a much higher percentage of trials than did 3- and 4-year-olds. Three- and 4-year-olds did not differ significantly. Use of redundant disambiguating information. We also examined whether there were age differences in the propensity to provide redundant disambiguating information (i.e., both size and color information). To address this issue, the percentage of trials in which participants referred to both color and size information (e.g., under the big pot with the red lid ) was entered into a one-way ANOVA with age as a between-subjects factor. This analysis yielded a significant effect of age, F (2, 39) = 24.10, p < Follow-up analyses revealed that adults provided redundant information on a significantly greater percentage of trials than did 3- and 4-year-olds. Three-year-olds, 4-year-olds, and adults provided redundant color and size information on 3% (SD = 7%), 3% (SD = 10%), and 49% (SD = 33%), of trials, respectively. Individual patterns of performance. Finally, we examined individual patterns of performance to determine whether participants in each age group showed strong biases toward color or size information. Across the eight test trials, we calculated the percentage of participants who referred only to color or only to size information, or referred at least once to each type of information (but not necessarily in the same trial). As shown in Table 3, most participants in the three age groups referred at least once to each type of information across the eight trials. A few 3- and 4-year-olds showed a bias toward size information (they never referred to color information on any trial). Thus, although the group data show a strong bias toward TABLE 3 Number of Participants Referring to Only Color, Only Size, or to Color and Size Across Trials Information Type Age Group Color Only Size Only Color and Size 3-year-olds 1 (7%) 3 (21%) 10 (71%) 4-year-olds 1 (7%) 4 (29%) 9 (64%) Adults 0 (0%) 1 (7%) 13 (93%) Note. There were 12 participants in each age group.

How do mothers communicate to young children about location

How do mothers communicate to young children about location University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Theses and Dissertations Spring 2010 How do mothers communicate to young children about location Kathryn Ann Haggerty University of Iowa Copyright 2010 Kathryn Ann

More information

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Leslee J. Martin (martin.1103@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University 216 Lazenby

More information

Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints

Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints Jean-Pierre Thibaut (jean-pierre.thibaut@univ-poitiers.fr) University of Poitiers, CeRCA, CNRS UMR 634, 99 avenue du recteur Pineau

More information

The Development of Memory for Location: What Role Do Spatial Prototypes Play?

The Development of Memory for Location: What Role Do Spatial Prototypes Play? Child Development, March/April 2001, Volume 72, Number 2, Pages 370 384 The Development of Memory for Location: What Role Do Spatial Prototypes Play? Jodie M. Plumert and Alycia M. Hund Two experiments

More information

Meeting someone with disabilities etiquette

Meeting someone with disabilities etiquette Meeting someone with disabilities etiquette Many people unsure how to go about meeting someone with a disability because they don t want to say or do the wrong thing. Here are a few tips to keep in mind

More information

Speech Spatial Qualities -C

Speech Spatial Qualities -C Speech Spatial Qualities -C Advice about answering the questions The following questions inquire about aspects of your ability and experience hearing and listening in different situations. You answered

More information

Introduction. Help your students learn how to learn!

Introduction. Help your students learn how to learn! Introduction Help your students learn how to learn! Lay a strong foundation in listening skills, the ability to follow directions, and in the ability to remember what one sees and hears important skills

More information

Case Study 1 Aimee Burns, Stephanie Kramer, Courtney Scholl, Diona Ysaac

Case Study 1 Aimee Burns, Stephanie Kramer, Courtney Scholl, Diona Ysaac Case Study 1 Aimee Burns, Stephanie Kramer, Courtney Scholl, Diona Ysaac Mrs. Jones is an 89-year-old right-handed female who presented with a severe fluent aphasia (Wernicke s) after a left parietal CVA

More information

The Emergence of Visual and Conceptual Perspective-Taking Abilities in Three and Four- Year-Old Children

The Emergence of Visual and Conceptual Perspective-Taking Abilities in Three and Four- Year-Old Children Carnegie Mellon University Research Showcase @ CMU The Sloping Halls Review Journals 1996 The Emergence of Visual and Conceptual Perspective-Taking Abilities in Three and Four- Year-Old Children Alisa

More information

Speech Spatial Qualities

Speech Spatial Qualities Speech Spatial Qualities Advice about answering the questions The following questions inquire about aspects of your ability and experience hearing and listening in different situations. For each question,

More information

Tips When Meeting A Person Who Has A Disability

Tips When Meeting A Person Who Has A Disability Tips When Meeting A Person Who Has A Disability Many people find meeting someone with a disability to be an awkward experience because they are afraid they will say or do the wrong thing; perhaps you are

More information

Try using a number as an adjective when talking to children. Let s take three books home or There are two chairs at this table.

Try using a number as an adjective when talking to children. Let s take three books home or There are two chairs at this table. Ages 0-18 mos. Try using a number as an adjective when talking to children. Let s take three books home or There are two chairs at this table. Ages 0-18 mos. Use the words more and less to describe stacks

More information

Creating Condom Confidence

Creating Condom Confidence A Lesson Plan from Rights, Respect, Responsibility: A K-12 Curriculum Fostering responsibility by respecting young people s rights to honest sexuality education. NSES ALIGNMENT: By the end of 12th grade,

More information

Running Head: VISUAL SCHEDULES FOR STUDENTS WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER

Running Head: VISUAL SCHEDULES FOR STUDENTS WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER Running Head: VISUAL SCHEDULES FOR STUDENTS WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER Visual Schedules for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorder Taylor Herback 200309600 University of Regina VISUAL SCHEDULES FOR

More information

dementia work training

dementia work training dementia friendly @ work training Participant s Guide In our communities, nearly 60 percent of people with Alzheimer s disease, a form of dementia, live in their own homes and need support from families

More information

Elements of Communication

Elements of Communication Elements of Communication Elements of Communication 6 Elements of Communication 1. Verbal messages 2. Nonverbal messages 3. Perception 4. Channel 5. Feedback 6. Context Elements of Communication 1. Verbal

More information

P.I.E.C.E.S. Dementia Care Series Approach September, 2011

P.I.E.C.E.S. Dementia Care Series Approach September, 2011 P.I.E.C.E.S. Dementia Care Series September, 2011 1 Objectives: The learner will be able to discuss: what is meant by approach strategies to use to facilitate positive interactions (approach) What would

More information

How to Conduct Direct Preference Assessments for Persons with. Developmental Disabilities Using a Multiple-Stimulus Without Replacement

How to Conduct Direct Preference Assessments for Persons with. Developmental Disabilities Using a Multiple-Stimulus Without Replacement How to Conduct Direct Preference Assessments for Persons with Developmental Disabilities Using a Multiple-Stimulus Without Replacement Procedure: A Self-Instruction Manual Duong Ramon and C.T. Yu University

More information

Chapter 7: Descriptive Statistics

Chapter 7: Descriptive Statistics Chapter Overview Chapter 7 provides an introduction to basic strategies for describing groups statistically. Statistical concepts around normal distributions are discussed. The statistical procedures of

More information

Overview. Meeting Length 90 minutes. Senses [Meeting 1]

Overview. Meeting Length 90 minutes. Senses [Meeting 1] Overview Girls have fun exploring their five senses and how they use them in everyday life. They also learn about what life would be like without one of their senses. Note to Volunteers A few agenda activities

More information

Using Typical Peer Mentors to Teach Children with Autism Social Skills: A Model for Teaching Pro-social Behaviors to Children with Special Needs

Using Typical Peer Mentors to Teach Children with Autism Social Skills: A Model for Teaching Pro-social Behaviors to Children with Special Needs Using Typical Peer Mentors to Teach Children with Autism Social Skills: A Model for Teaching Pro-social Behaviors to Children with Special Needs Elika Shahrestani, M.S., BCBA Michelle Coulter, M.A., BCBA

More information

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Alex Rice M.Cl.Sc SLP Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication

More information

There are often questions and, sometimes, confusion when looking at services to a child who is deaf or hard of hearing. Because very young children

There are often questions and, sometimes, confusion when looking at services to a child who is deaf or hard of hearing. Because very young children There are often questions and, sometimes, confusion when looking at services to a child who is deaf or hard of hearing. Because very young children are not yet ready to work on specific strategies for

More information

Full file at

Full file at Chapter Outline Chapter 2 Observing and Recording Behavior Direct and Indirect Assessment Defining the Target Behaviors The Logistics of Recording The Observer When and Where to Record Choosing a Recording

More information

Tips on How to Better Serve Customers with Various Disabilities

Tips on How to Better Serve Customers with Various Disabilities FREDERICTON AGE-FRIENDLY COMMUNITY ADVISORY COMMITTEE Tips on How to Better Serve Customers with Various Disabilities Fredericton - A Community for All Ages How To Welcome Customers With Disabilities People

More information

The Vine Assessment System by LifeCubby

The Vine Assessment System by LifeCubby The Vine Assessment System by LifeCubby A Fully Integrated Platform for Observation, Daily Reporting, Communications and Assessment For Early Childhood Professionals and the Families that they Serve Alignment

More information

Accessibility. Serving Clients with Disabilities

Accessibility. Serving Clients with Disabilities Accessibility Serving Clients with Disabilities Did you know that just over 15.5% of Ontarians have a disability? That s 1 in every 7 Ontarians and as the population ages that number will grow. People

More information

The truth about lying

The truth about lying Reading Practice The truth about lying Over the years Richard Wiseman has tried to unravel the truth about deception - investigating the signs that give away a liar. A In the 1970s, as part of a large-scale

More information

(p) (f) Echolalia. What is it, and how to help your child with Echolalia?

(p) (f) Echolalia. What is it, and how to help your child with Echolalia? (p) 406-690-6996 (f) 406-206-5262 info@advancedtherapyclinic.com Echolalia What is it, and how to help your child with Echolalia? Echolalia is repeating or echoing what another person has said. Children

More information

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing. Social Consequences as Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Hilary A.

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing. Social Consequences as Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Hilary A. Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing Social Consequences as Reinforcers A Thesis Presented by Hilary A. Gibson The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational

More information

Kids Booklet 5 & on Autism. Create an autism awareness ribbon! Tips for parents & teachers. Activities puzzles

Kids Booklet 5 & on Autism. Create an autism awareness ribbon! Tips for parents & teachers. Activities puzzles Kids Booklet on Autism Create an autism awareness ribbon! Tips for parents & teachers 5 & Activities puzzles Take a look at what s inside! Questions and Answers About Autism page 2 Brothers and Sisters

More information

Lecture 9: Sound Localization

Lecture 9: Sound Localization Lecture 9: Sound Localization Localization refers to the process of using the information about a sound which you get from your ears, to work out where the sound came from (above, below, in front, behind,

More information

Early Learning About the World From Symbolic Media

Early Learning About the World From Symbolic Media Early Learning About the World From Symbolic Media Georgene Troseth Department of Psychology & Human Development Vanderbilt University Vanderbilt Kennedy Center Talk Plan The role of symbols Models and

More information

RESOURCES FOR THE JOURNEY OF GRIEF. Is There Anything I Can Do? Supporting a Friend Who Is Grieving

RESOURCES FOR THE JOURNEY OF GRIEF. Is There Anything I Can Do? Supporting a Friend Who Is Grieving RESOURCES FOR THE JOURNEY OF GRIEF Is There Anything I Can Do? Supporting a Friend Who Is Grieving I can t even imagine what they re going through... Being a friend to someone whose loved one has died

More information

Unit 5. Thinking Statistically

Unit 5. Thinking Statistically Unit 5. Thinking Statistically Supplementary text for this unit: Darrell Huff, How to Lie with Statistics. Most important chapters this week: 1-4. Finish the book next week. Most important chapters: 8-10.

More information

T. Kushnir & A. Gopnik (2005 ). Young children infer causal strength from probabilities and interventions. Psychological Science 16 (9):

T. Kushnir & A. Gopnik (2005 ). Young children infer causal strength from probabilities and interventions. Psychological Science 16 (9): Probabilities and Interventions 1 Running Head: PROBABILITIES AND INTERVENTIONS T. Kushnir & A. Gopnik (2005 ). Young children infer causal strength from probabilities and interventions. Psychological

More information

Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering" GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755

Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755 Memory & Cognition 1974, Vol. 2, No.3, 533 538 Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering" GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755 Ss were required to learn linear

More information

Emily A. Jones, PhD, BCBA 1

Emily A. Jones, PhD, BCBA 1 Joint Attention Intervention for Children with Autism Emily A. Jones, Ph.D., BCBA CW Post, Long Island University Joint Attention an early social-communicative behavior in which two people share al focus

More information

Information Session. What is Dementia? People with dementia need to be understood and supported in their communities.

Information Session. What is Dementia? People with dementia need to be understood and supported in their communities. Information Session People with dementia need to be understood and supported in their communities. You can help by becoming a Dementia Friend. Visit www.actonalz.org/dementia-friends to learn more! Dementia

More information

THIS SET OF LESSONS PROVIDES

THIS SET OF LESSONS PROVIDES Foundational Skills WEEK ONE THIS SET OF LESSONS PROVIDES games and activities to help children develop fitness, strength, body awareness, spatial concepts, adaptive skills (following directions, imitating

More information

Thank you for your time and dedication to our industry and community.

Thank you for your time and dedication to our industry and community. secti To: AFP International Fundraising Conference Speakers From: Troy P. Coalman, Board Member, AFP Advancement Northwest, and Member, AFP International Committee for Diversity & Inclusion Date: February

More information

Running head: INFLUENCE OF LABELS ON JUDGMENTS OF PERFORMANCE

Running head: INFLUENCE OF LABELS ON JUDGMENTS OF PERFORMANCE The influence of 1 Running head: INFLUENCE OF LABELS ON JUDGMENTS OF PERFORMANCE The Influence of Stigmatizing Labels on Participants Judgments of Children s Overall Performance and Ability to Focus on

More information

Communication. Jess Walsh

Communication. Jess Walsh Communication Jess Walsh Introduction. Douglas Bank is a home for young adults with severe learning disabilities. Good communication is important for the service users because it s easy to understand the

More information

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions Hidehito Honda (hito@ky.hum.titech.ac.jp) Kimihiko Yamagishi (kimihiko@ky.hum.titech.ac.jp) Graduate School of Decision Science

More information

Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use

Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use Michael Romano (mrr2@nyu.edu) Department of Psychology, New York University, 6 Washington Place New York, NY 1000 USA Abstract Traditional

More information

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 1 Running Head: THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-EFFICACY The Role of Modeling and Feedback in Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 2 Abstract Participants

More information

Fahrenheit 451 Comprehension Questions

Fahrenheit 451 Comprehension Questions Name: Fahrenheit 451 Comprehension Questions Directions: Use the following questions to help check your understanding while reading. If you don t know an answer, look back at the book, ask a friend, or

More information

Developing symbolic capacity one step at a time q

Developing symbolic capacity one step at a time q Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Cognition 106 (2008) 1 12 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Developing symbolic capacity one step at a time q Janellen Huttenlocher a, *, Marina Vasilyeva b, Nora

More information

The Comprehension of Left and Right in a Referential Communication Task

The Comprehension of Left and Right in a Referential Communication Task The Comprehension of Left and Right in a Referential Communication Task Stanka A. Fitneva (Fitneva@Queensu.ca) Department of Psychology, Queen s University Kingston, ON K7L 3N6, Canada Yi Song (7ys4@Queensu.ca)

More information

TEN early signs of a Dementia

TEN early signs of a Dementia TEN early signs of a Dementia 1. Memory loss on a day-to-day basis Memory loss, especially forgetting things that happened only hours or days ago. forgetting important dates or events repeatedly asking

More information

Keeping Home Safe WHAT CAN YOU DO?

Keeping Home Safe WHAT CAN YOU DO? Keeping Home Safe or dementia may have trouble knowing what is dangerous or making safe decisions. By helping him or her feel more relaxed and less confused at home, you can help stop accidents. trip because

More information

This is a large part of coaching presence as it helps create a special and strong bond between coach and client.

This is a large part of coaching presence as it helps create a special and strong bond between coach and client. Page 1 Confidence People have presence when their outer behavior and appearance conveys confidence and authenticity and is in sync with their intent. It is about being comfortable and confident with who

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

Microarray Comparative Genomic Hybridisation (array CGH)

Microarray Comparative Genomic Hybridisation (array CGH) Saint Mary s Hospital Manchester Centre for Genomic Medicine Information for Patients Microarray Comparative Genomic Hybridisation (array CGH) An array CGH test looks for small changes in a person s chromosomes,

More information

Cognitive Changes Workshop Outcomes

Cognitive Changes Workshop Outcomes HO 4.1 Cognitive Changes Workshop Outcomes At the end of this session, participants should be able to: define Neuropsychology and the role of the Neuropsychologist (optional) recognise normal difficulties

More information

Learning Objective: To perform a roll with good body tension To evaluate their partners roll To complete a roll into a stretched position

Learning Objective: To perform a roll with good body tension To evaluate their partners roll To complete a roll into a stretched position There are different types of rolls in gymnastics, which include, forward roll, sideways roll, curled egg roll and a teddy bear roll. Rolls need to be smooth and flow at the same speed throughout. For a

More information

What makes us special? Ages 3-5

What makes us special? Ages 3-5 What makes us special? Ages 3-5 Pudsey s learning aims I can think about how we are different and how we are the same. I can think about how we are special and what makes us unique. Key Words Special Unique

More information

Leader Guide. Session 8. FOOD is Fuel. Lesson 1: Welcome Back & Recap (10 minutes) Lesson 2: FOOD Choices in Your Day (5 Minutes)

Leader Guide. Session 8. FOOD is Fuel. Lesson 1: Welcome Back & Recap (10 minutes) Lesson 2: FOOD Choices in Your Day (5 Minutes) Leader Guide Session 8 FOOD is Fuel Lesson 1: Welcome Back & Recap (10 minutes) Lesson 2: FOOD Choices in Your Day (5 Minutes) Lesson 3: FOOD and MOOD Relay (10 Minutes) Lesson 4: Fitness Challenge Dance

More information

For more than 100 years, extremely hyperactive

For more than 100 years, extremely hyperactive 8 W H A T W E K N O W AD/HD Predominantly Inattentive Type For more than 100 years, extremely hyperactive children have been recognized as having behavioral problems. In the 1970s, doctors recognized that

More information

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 12 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2009 We want to understand the reasons

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 12 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2009 We want to understand the reasons Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 12 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2009 We want to understand the reasons behind the lifestyles of our non-human primate relatives

More information

Activities for Someone in Early in Dementia

Activities for Someone in Early in Dementia Diamonds Still Clear Sharp - Can Cut Hard - Rigid - Inflexible Many Facets Can Really Shine Activities for Someone in Early in Dementia Diamond Characteristics Know Who s in Charge Respect Authority Can

More information

Information on ADHD for Children, Question and Answer - long version

Information on ADHD for Children, Question and Answer - long version Information on ADHD for Children, Question and Answer - long version What is Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder or ADHD? People with ADHD have brains that may function a little differently in some

More information

Types of questions. You need to know. Short question. Short question. Measurement Scale: Ordinal Scale

Types of questions. You need to know. Short question. Short question. Measurement Scale: Ordinal Scale You need to know Materials in the slides Materials in the 5 coglab presented in class Textbooks chapters Information/explanation given in class you can have all these documents with you + your notes during

More information

The Role of Gender in Physically Interactive Group Play: An Observation Study

The Role of Gender in Physically Interactive Group Play: An Observation Study MICUSP Version 1.0 - PSY.G0.16.1 - Psychology - Final Year Undergraduate - Female - Native Speaker - Research Paper 1 The Role of Gender in Physically Interactive Group Play: An Observation Study Through

More information

THEORIES OF PERSONALITY II Psychodynamic Assessment 1/1/2014 SESSION 6 PSYCHODYNAMIC ASSESSMENT

THEORIES OF PERSONALITY II Psychodynamic Assessment 1/1/2014 SESSION 6 PSYCHODYNAMIC ASSESSMENT THEORIES OF PERSONALITY II Psychodynamic Assessment 1/1/2014 SESSION 6 PSYCHODYNAMIC ASSESSMENT THEORIES OF PERSONALITY II SESSION 6: Psychodynamic Assessment Psychodynamic Assessment Assessing the specific

More information

The Clean Environment Commission. Public Participation in the Environmental Review Process

The Clean Environment Commission. Public Participation in the Environmental Review Process The Clean Environment Commission Public Participation in the Environmental Review Process Manitoba Clean Environment Commission Who are we? The Manitoba Clean Environment Commission is an arms-length agency

More information

University of Oregon HEDCO Clinic Fluency Center. Diagnostic Intake Form for Adults Who Stutter

University of Oregon HEDCO Clinic Fluency Center. Diagnostic Intake Form for Adults Who Stutter University of Oregon HEDCO Clinic Fluency Center Phone 541-346-0923 Fax 541-346-6772 Physical Address: Mailing Address: HEDCO Education Complex HEDCO Clinic 1655 Alder Street, Eugene, OR 97403 5207 University

More information

Progress Monitoring Handouts 1

Progress Monitoring Handouts 1 Progress Monitoring Handouts Teacher Administration Scripts, Teacher Sheets, and Student Sheets Reading Letter Sound Fluency (LSF)..2 Word Identification Fluency (WIF)...5 Passage Reading Fluency (PRF)

More information

Alien Life Form (ALF)

Alien Life Form (ALF) Alien Life Form (ALF) Closely related siblings are most often different in both genotype (the actual genes) and phenotype (the appearance of the genes). This is because of the great variety of traits in

More information

Welcome participants, introduce the expert, and briefly outline today s session.

Welcome participants, introduce the expert, and briefly outline today s session. SESSION CONTENT 5.1 Welcome and outline (2 min) Welcome participants, introduce the expert, and briefly outline today s session. 5.2 Review homework and exercises (15 min) Review participants personal

More information

Views of autistic adults on assessment in the early years

Views of autistic adults on assessment in the early years Views of autistic adults on what should be assessed and how assessment should be conducted on children with autism in the early years Summary of autistic adults views on assessment 1. Avoid drawing negative

More information

ILLINOIS VOICES LEADERSHIP TRAINING GUIDE

ILLINOIS VOICES LEADERSHIP TRAINING GUIDE ILLINOIS VOICES LEADERSHIP TRAINING GUIDE Written by: Lorraine Kendall Scottie Mitchell Maria Sinclair Claude Whitehouse Illinois Voices: Putting together the pieces of the self-advocacy puzzle. Learn

More information

How should you study for Friday's exam?

How should you study for Friday's exam? How should you study for Friday's exam? re-read textbook? re-read lecture slides? study guide? NO! these are passive. Use active study. Test yourself by Take the practice quizzes in Moodle Create your

More information

Fun and Fit Forever: Exercise. May 21, :00-7:30 p.m. All About Kids

Fun and Fit Forever: Exercise. May 21, :00-7:30 p.m. All About Kids Fun and Fit Forever: Exercise May 21, 2009 6:00-7:30 p.m. All About Kids The Florida Institute of Education at the University of North Florida 1 Standards, Outline and Materials Program Goal: To combat

More information

ADHD. What you need to know

ADHD. What you need to know ADHD What you need to know At Teva, we help to improve the health of 200 million people every day by providing innovative treatments and access to the world s largest medicine cabinet of generic and specialty

More information

Review Session!1

Review Session!1 Review Session!1 Review Four types of variables: nominal ordinal interval ratio score Values of variables are distributed Important goal: characterizing the distribution Graphs Bar graphs for nominal variables

More information

Interacting with people

Interacting with people Learning Guide Interacting with people 28518 Interact with people to provide support in a health or wellbeing setting Level 2 5 credits Name: Workplace: Issue 1.0 Copyright 2017 Careerforce All rights

More information

Volunteer Instructions

Volunteer Instructions Body Mass Index (1 Volunteer / 2 Minutes) The athlete s weight and height will be measured to determine their Body Mass Index (BMI). Portable Weighing System or Scales, Measuring Tape Set-Up: Remove Portable

More information

Experimental and survey design

Experimental and survey design Friday, October 12, 2001 Page: 1 Experimental and survey design 1. There is a positive association between the number of drownings and ice cream sales. This is an example of an association likely caused

More information

DEMENTIA RESOURCE PACK

DEMENTIA RESOURCE PACK DEMENTIA RESOURCE PACK CONTENTS THE A MILLION HANDS BIG MOMENT 2 UNDERSTAND THE ISSUE 3 PLAN ACTION 4 TAKE ACTION 5 LEARN AND DO MORE 7 TELL THE WORLD 8 1 THE A MILLION HANDS BIG MOMENT We are asking Cub

More information

What is stress? Stress is an emotional/ bodily reaction to

What is stress? Stress is an emotional/ bodily reaction to The Basics What is stress? What are some causes of stress? Stress is an emotional/ bodily reaction to a physical, psychological or emotional demand We all display stress in different ways Some stress is

More information

INTRODUCTION TO STATISTICS

INTRODUCTION TO STATISTICS Basic Statistics Introduction to Statistics Basic Statistical Formulas Commonly used Ecological Equations INTRODUCTION TO STATISTICS Statistics is the branch of mathematics that deals with the techniques

More information

ACCESSIBILITY CHECKLIST FOR EVENT PLANNING

ACCESSIBILITY CHECKLIST FOR EVENT PLANNING ACCESSIBILITY CHECKLIST FOR EVENT PLANNING Planning Event scheduled at accessible site (determined by site visit or audit), including: accessible parking spaces (adequate number of level, marked spaces)

More information

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design 11-1 Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design Advantages and Limitations Comparing Two Groups Comparing t Test to ANOVA Independent Samples t Test Independent Samples ANOVA Comparing

More information

Safeguarding adults: mediation and family group conferences: Information for people who use services

Safeguarding adults: mediation and family group conferences: Information for people who use services Safeguarding adults: mediation and family group conferences: Information for people who use services The Social Care Institute for Excellence (SCIE) was established by Government in 2001 to improve social

More information

THE EFFECTS OF THERAPIST-MEDIATED SIGNALS AND PARTICIPANT-MEDIATED RESPONSES ON THE DELAY TOLERANCE OF TYPICALLY DEVELOPING PRESCHOOL CHILDREN

THE EFFECTS OF THERAPIST-MEDIATED SIGNALS AND PARTICIPANT-MEDIATED RESPONSES ON THE DELAY TOLERANCE OF TYPICALLY DEVELOPING PRESCHOOL CHILDREN THE EFFECTS OF THERAPIST-MEDIATED SIGNALS AND PARTICIPANT-MEDIATED RESPONSES ON THE DELAY TOLERANCE OF TYPICALLY DEVELOPING PRESCHOOL CHILDREN BY Copyright 2010 MATTHEW H. NEWQUIST Submitted to the graduate

More information

Mindful Beings: Visitors Descriptions of Emotions, Intelligence, Consciousness, Creativity, and Mind. Joyce Ma. January 2002

Mindful Beings: Visitors Descriptions of Emotions, Intelligence, Consciousness, Creativity, and Mind. Joyce Ma. January 2002 Mindful Beings: Visitors Descriptions of Emotions, Intelligence, Consciousness, Creativity, and Mind Joyce Ma January 2002 Keywords: 1 Mind and Learning - Front-End Evaluation

More information

OPWDD: Putting People First. Redefining the Role of the DSP Trainer s Manual. Developed by Regional Centers for Workforce Transformation

OPWDD: Putting People First. Redefining the Role of the DSP Trainer s Manual. Developed by Regional Centers for Workforce Transformation Redefining the Role of the DSP Trainer s Manual Developed by Regional Centers for Workforce Transformation Core Competencies This Video Trainer s Manual is designed to guide you through the Workforce Transformation

More information

SERVING STUDENTS WITH AUTISM IN SCHOOLS 1

SERVING STUDENTS WITH AUTISM IN SCHOOLS 1 SERVING STUDENTS WITH AUTISM IN SCHOOLS 1 Serving Students with Autism in Schools Beth Kost Abnormal Psychology (PSY 406) SERVING STUDENTS WITH AUTISM IN SCHOOLS 2 Serving Students with Autism in Schools

More information

LEARNED HELPLESSNESS IN TODDLERS OF DEPRESSED AND NONDEPRESSED MOTHERS. Sue A. Kelley, Kay D. Jennings, & Amy J. Dran. April, 1999

LEARNED HELPLESSNESS IN TODDLERS OF DEPRESSED AND NONDEPRESSED MOTHERS. Sue A. Kelley, Kay D. Jennings, & Amy J. Dran. April, 1999 LEARNED HELPLESSNESS IN TODDLERS OF DEPRESSED AND NONDEPRESSED MOTHERS Sue A. Kelley, Kay D. Jennings, & Amy J. Dran April, 1999 405 Langley Hall Department of Psychology Pittsburgh, PA 15260 Poster presented

More information

Desirability Bias: Do Desires Influence Expectations? It Depends on How You Ask.

Desirability Bias: Do Desires Influence Expectations? It Depends on How You Ask. University of Iowa Honors Theses University of Iowa Honors Program Spring 2018 Desirability Bias: Do Desires Influence Expectations? It Depends on How You Ask. Mark Biangmano Follow this and additional

More information

This is an edited transcript of a telephone interview recorded in March 2010.

This is an edited transcript of a telephone interview recorded in March 2010. Sound Advice This is an edited transcript of a telephone interview recorded in March 2010. Dr. Patricia Manning-Courtney is a developmental pediatrician and is director of the Kelly O Leary Center for

More information

AFSP SURVIVOR OUTREACH PROGRAM VOLUNTEER TRAINING HANDOUT

AFSP SURVIVOR OUTREACH PROGRAM VOLUNTEER TRAINING HANDOUT AFSP SURVIVOR OUTREACH PROGRAM VOLUNTEER TRAINING HANDOUT Goals of the AFSP Survivor Outreach Program Suggested Answers To Frequently Asked Questions on Visits Roadblocks to Communication During Visits

More information

YMCA of Oakville. Accessibility Standard for Customer Service. Training Workbook

YMCA of Oakville. Accessibility Standard for Customer Service. Training Workbook YMCA of Oakville Accessibility Standard for Customer Service Training Workbook Contents The following workbook contains valuable information about the Accessibility Standard for Customer Service. Information

More information

2018 Municipal Elections Accessibility Plan

2018 Municipal Elections Accessibility Plan Dated this 17 th day of September, 2018. This Plan is for use in the 2018 Municipal Elections in conjunction with the Municipality of St.-Charles Multi-Year Accessibility Policy. Page 1 of 11 Table of

More information

Fahrenheit 451 Comprehension Questions

Fahrenheit 451 Comprehension Questions Fahrenheit 451 Comprehension Questions Part 1: The Hearth and the Salamander [Pages 3-33] 1. How are the books that are being burnt described? (3) 2. What is Montag s job? Does he like his job? (3) 3.

More information

PREPARING FOR THE ELEVENTH TRADITION

PREPARING FOR THE ELEVENTH TRADITION PREPARING FOR THE ELEVENTH TRADITION (Read pp. 180-183 of the 12 & 12.) THE ELEVENTH TRADITION: Our public relations policy is based on attraction rather than promotion; we need always maintain personal

More information

WARNING, DISTRACTION, AND RESISTANCE TO INFLUENCE 1

WARNING, DISTRACTION, AND RESISTANCE TO INFLUENCE 1 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1965, Vol. 1, No. 3, 262-266 WARNING, DISTRACTION, AND RESISTANCE TO INFLUENCE 1 JONATHAN L. FREEDMAN Stanford DAVID 0. SEARS of California, Los Angeles 2 hypotheses

More information

Among the Hidden: Questions Ch. 1-5

Among the Hidden: Questions Ch. 1-5 # Name Date Among the Hidden: Questions Ch. 1-5 BC # 1. After Luke's mother calls him to come into the house from the woods, what does Luke do for the first time in his life? a) He comes immediately. b)

More information