Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor"

Transcription

1 Learn Behav (2012) 40:24 33 DOI /s Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor Cody W. Polack & Mario A. Laborda & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 July 2011 # Psychonomic Society, Inc Abstract Two lick suppression experiments using rats were conducted to determine whether extinction of a punctate excitor in a particular context would result in that context becoming a conditioned inhibitor, as defined by passing both summation and retardation tests. The role of extinction trial spacing was investigated as a possible determinant of whether the extinction context would become inhibitory. Experiment 1 demonstrated that, although inhibition was evident using either massed or spaced extinction trials, spaced trials reduced measurable inhibition as assessed by the summation test, but trial spacing had no influence on retardation test performance. Experiment 2 confirmed Experiment 1 s conclusions while controlling for the influence of latent inhibition on the retardation test. In Experiment 2, the context proved inhibitory only following massed extinction trials. These data suggest that, at least with select parameters, an extinction context can become inhibitory. Keywords Conditioned inhibition. Context. Extinction. Pavlovian conditioning. Renewal. Trial spacing Contexts have been viewed as either modulating the retrieval of conditioned stimulus unconditioned stimulus (CS US) and conditioned stimulus no unconditioned stimulus (CS NoUS) associations or having direct associations with the US (e.g., Urcelay & Miller, 2010). In the former C. W. Polack : M. A. Laborda : R. R. Miller (*) Department of Psychology, State University of New York at Binghamton, Binghamton, NY , USA rmiller@binghamton.edu M. A. Laborda Departamento de Psicología, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile framework, the context is regarded as modulating the association between two events (i.e., as a positive or negative occasion setter; Holland, 1983, 1992; Miller & Oberling, 1998). In the latter framework, the context is like a punctate CS that can compete with the target stimulus for behavioral control during training and summate with it at test. At least under some conditions, the context appears to control behavior like a CS and does not require additional assumptions about contexts having special properties. Although much of this work has used physical contexts because they are easily controlled, we should not forget that contexts can also be situated in time and the organism s internal state (Bouton, 2010). For example, using alcohol as a negative discriminative cue has been shown to have inhibitory properties (Cunningham, 1979). This CS-like role of the context has been assessed in at least two ways: evaluating the influence of the context on responding to punctate cues in cue competition situations (e.g., Randich, 1981) and demonstrating that this competition is directly related to the measured excitatory value of the context (Urushihara & Miller, 2009). Thus, contexts are like punctate cues in that they can serve either as occasion setters or as CSs. Additionally, parameters that encourage occasion-setting properties in punctate cues could be viewed as instances of the potential context-like role of cues. Consequently, the distinction between occasion setters and simple Pavlovian CSs is not necessarily a matter of whether a stimulus is localized in time and space (i.e., a punctate cue) or diffuse (i.e., a context), but of how those stimuli are treated during training. An extinction context, serving as a stimulus that enters into cue competition with a target punctate stimulus, has been shown to influence behavior on a test of extinction when testing occurs in an associatively neutral context (e.g., Laborda, Witnauer, & Miller, 2011). This suggests that,

2 Learn Behav (2012) 40: although the role of the context in extinction has been emphasized as one in which the context modulates retrieval of CS US and CS NoUS associations (Bouton, 1993), one should not overlook the potential influence of the context as a Pavlovian CS in these situations. The CS-like role of contexts is especially important for certain theoretical accounts of the renewal effect (e.g., Rescorla & Wagner, 1972). Renewal, the recovery of a conditioned response (CR) following extinction treatment (presentations of the CS-alone) when testing occurs outside the context of extinction, can be explained by viewing the extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor that protects the target cue from complete extinction (e.g., Rescorla, 2003) and suppresses responding to the target CS when testing occurs in the extinction context (e.g., when acquisition occurs in a different context from extinction and testing; an ABB control situation). Recovery is then expected when the assessment of responding occurs in a context in which extinction has not occurred (i.e., outside the inhibitory influence of the extinction context; e.g., when acquisition, extinction, and testing all occur in separate contexts, an ABC renewal situation). Simply put, if the context of extinction is a functional conditioned inhibitor, negative summation should occur between the target cue and the test context in the ABB condition, whereas if testing occurs in an associatively neutral context (i.e., ABC renewal), no such reduction in responding should occur, and greater responding is expected. However, it is generally assumed that renewal is not the result of contextual conditioned inhibition, but rather that the context modulates responding much like an occasion setter. A major problem with an account of renewal based on contextual conditioned inhibition is that all previous attempts to detect the inhibitory properties of extinction contexts have failed. For example, Bouton and King (1983; Bouton & Swartzentruber, 1986; see also Nelson, Sanjuan, Vadillo-Ruiz, Pérez, & León, 2011) assessed whether the context of extinction could pass a summation test (Rescorla, 1969) with a transfer excitor that was not extinguished, and they also assessed the associative value of the context using context preference scores. In both assessments, no evidence for inhibitory properties of the context was found, despite their observing renewal effects. Thus, Bouton and King s findings are problematic for the view that renewal can be entirely explained by the extinction context becoming a conditioned inhibitor. Certainly, their findings indicate that at least in some preparations an extinction context functions as a negative occasion setter rather than a Pavlovian inhibitor in producing renewal. However, there are a number of considerations suggesting that the role of the context as a conditioned inhibitor should not be categorically rejected as a potential explanation of some of the published renewal data. For instance, Bouton and King used a cue that had a relatively strong associative status as a transfer excitor in their summation test. If the context of extinction was working as an inhibitor that summated with the extinguished target CS in the ABB condition, then for maximum sensitivity the appropriate summation test should be conducted with a stimulus having an associative history parallel to that of the target cue. These demonstrations have led some to define renewal as recovery that occurs in the absence of inhibitory or excitory contexts (e.g., Nelson et al., 2011). This confuses the empirical phenomenon of renewal with one particular mechanism that is thought to contribute to the phenomenon under some circumstances (i.e., occasion setting by the extinction context). We propose that the extinction context becoming a conditioned inhibitor is yet another potential mechanism that under some circumstances can contribute to the renewal phenomenon. The present experiments assessed whether extinction treatment outside of the acquisition context enables the extinction context to become a conditioned inhibitor, at least with some parameters. Specifically, the present experiments were designed to evaluate whether an extinction context could pass summation and retardation tests for inhibition using a design that approximates those commonly used to assess the conditioned inhibition properties of punctate cues. Additionally, we investigated whether the intertrial interval during extinction training plays a role in determining whether the extinction context becomes a conditioned inhibitor. According to the Rescorla Wagner (1972) model, spaced intertrial intervals (like those used by Bouton & King, 1983) should allow any inhibitory properties of the context to extinguish (due to a negative US expectancy and an outcome during extinction that is zero), thereby producing a positive incremental shift in associative value (i.e., extinction of conditioned inhibition; Polack, Laborda, & Miller, 2011; butseezimmer-hart& Rescorla, 1974). In contrast, using massed extinction trials should limit the amount of intertrial time during extinction training, potentially preserving the inhibitory status of the context. Consequently, to manipulate the associative value of the context, we used massed and spaced extinction trials, which are analogous to the explicit context extinction procedures previously demonstrated to influence the associative value of contexts in extinction (Laborda et al., 2011). Experiment 1 In the present experiment, we assessed whether a context in which a punctate excitor was extinguished can pass summation and retardation tests for conditioned inhibition. To understand prior observations of an extinction context failing to display inhibitory properties, we assessed whether total extinction-context exposure, which was manipulated

3 26 Learn Behav (2012) 40:24 33 using different extinction trial spacings, determines whether the extinction context becomes a conditioned inhibitor. Because we did not intend to directly compare inhibition to renewal in this series, we did not equate experience between the transfer excitor and the to-be-extinguished cue. In the introduction, we pointed out that failing to equate this experience might explain differences in sensitivity between those stimuli with respect to negative summation. In the present research, we selected parameters that would produce a moderate responding to our transfer excitor in order to achieve sensitivity to negative summation with the context. Future experiments designed to make direct comparisons between conditioned inhibition and renewal should control for experience with the tested stimuli, but that is not our present purpose. Here, we simply tried to identify whether an extinction context can ever function as a conditioned inhibitor under specific parameters. Method Subjects The subjects were 24 male and 24 female experimentally naive Sprague-Dawley-descended rats obtained from our own breeding colony. Body-weight ranges were g for males and g for females. Subjects were randomly assigned to one of four groups (ns = 12), counterbalanced within groups for sex. The animals were individually housed in standard hanging stainless-steel wire-mesh cages in a vivarium maintained on a 16:8 h light:dark cycle. Experimental manipulations occurred near the middle portion of the light phase. The animals received free access to Purina Lab Chow, whereas water availability was limited to 30 min per day following a progressive deprivation schedule initiated 4 days prior to the start of the study. From the time of weaning until the start of the study, all animals were handled for 30 s, three times per week. Apparatus We used 24 experimental chambers, of three different types. Chamber O was cm (l w h). The sidewalls of the chamber were made of stainless steel sheet metal, and the front wall, back wall, and ceiling of the chamber were made of clear Plexiglas. The floor was constructed of 0.3 cm diameter rods, spaced 1.3 cm center to center, and connected by NE-2 neon bulbs that allowed a 0.7-mA, 0.5-s constant-current footshock to be delivered by means of a high-voltage AC circuit in series with a 1.0-MΩ resistor. Each of 12 copies of Chamber O was housed in an environmental isolation chest that could be dimly illuminated by a houselight (1.12-W #1820 incandescent bulb) mounted on one wall of the experimental chamber. Chamber R was rectangular, measuring cm (l w h). The walls and ceiling were clear Plexiglas, and the floor was comprised of stainless steel rods measuring 0.5 cm in diameter, spaced 1.3 cm apart (center to center). The rods were connected by NE-2 bulbs, which allowed for the delivery of a 0.7-mA, 0.5-s constantcurrent footshock. Each of six copies of Chamber R was housed in separate light- and sound-attenuating environmental isolation chambers. Chamber V was 27 cm long, 29.5 cm high, and 21.5 cm wide at the top, and 5.5 cm wide at the bottom. The floor was comprised of two 27-cm-long plates, 2 cm wide, with a 1.5-cm gap between the two plates. A 0.7-mA, 0.5-s constantcurrent footshock could be delivered through the metal walls and floor of the chamber. The ceiling was clear Plexiglas, the front and back walls were black Plexiglas, and the sidewalls were stainless steel. Each of six copies of Chamber V was housed in a separate sound- and light-attenuating environmental isolation chest. Both R and V chambers could be equipped with a waterfilled lick tube that extended 1 cm into a cylindrical niche, which was 4.5 cm in diameter, left right centered, with its bottom 1.75 cm above the floor of the apparatus and 5.0 cm deep. In all chambers, there was a horizontal photobeam detector 1 cm in front of the lick tube that was broken whenever the subject licked the tube. Three 45-Ω speakers on the inside and back walls of all isolation chests could deliver a click train (6 Hz), a complex tone (450 and 550 Hz, presented simultaneously), and a white noise, all 6 db above background. Ventilation fans in each enclosure provided a constant 76-dB background noise. AllauditorycuesweremeasuredontheC-scale.A10-sduration click train served as our transfer excitor, X, and a 30-s-duration complex tone served as our extinguished cue, Z. A longer-duration stimulus was used for Z in order to facilitate a within-compound association with the extinction context, which we expected would favor making the context inhibitory. A 0.7-mA footshock of 0.5-s duration served as the US. The light intensities inside the three types of experimental chambers were approximately equal due to the difference in the opaqueness of the walls. Context A consisted of an instance of Chamber O with the houselight (HL) off and a block of wood with two drops of methyl salicylate located inside the isolation chest. Context D consisted of an instance of Chamber O (different from the one used as Context A) with the HL on and a block of wood with two drops of banana concentrate located inside the isolation chest. The physical contexts used as Context B and C were counterbalanced between an instance of Chamber R and an instance of Chamber V. No odor cue was used for physical contexts B or C.

4 Learn Behav (2012) 40: Procedures Acclimation On Day 1, all subjects received a 30-min session of acclimation to Context B and another to Context C, with a 2-h gap between sessions. The order of exposure was counterbalanced within groups. The lick tube was accessible in each context. Following this phase, the lick tubes were removed until the summation test. Transfer excitor training On Day 2, all subjects received four presentations of the transfer excitor X coterminating with the US, in a 60-min session in Context A. Stimulus onsets occurred at 6, 16, 36, and 51 min into the session. Acquisition On Days 3 5, all subjects received eight daily presentations of Z coterminating with the US, in a 120-min session in Context D. Stimulus onsets occurred at 4, 24, 39, 54, 64, 79, 89, and 109 min into the session. Extinction On Days 6 9, subjects in the massed condition (see Table 1) received 10 daily nonreinforced presentations of Z over a 6-min session in Context B (mean intertrial interval = 6 s). Stimulus onset occurred at 3, 39, 75, 111, 147, 183, 219, 255, 291, and 327 s into the session. The relatively short intertrial interval was intended to reduce extinction of the Context B CS Z association during the intertrial interval. Subjects in the spaced condition received the same 10 daily nonreinforced presentations of Z in Context B, but over a 240-min session (mean intertrial interval = 23.5 min). Stimulus onset occurred at 12, 36, 60, 84, 108, 132, 156, 180, 204, and 228 min into the session. Summation test On Day 10, the lick tubes were reinserted into Contexts B and C, and all subjects were tested in either Context B or C for a summation test with transfer excitor X. The test stimulus (X) was presented immediately upon placing a subject into the context, and the time to drink for five cumulative seconds was recorded. We were interested in the response to the combined presentation of the excitatory punctate transfer cue (X) and the context. Consequently, we omitted any pre-cs period during the session in order to have simultaneous onset of the cue and the context. This procedure is an adaptation of that used by Urushihara and Miller (2009) to assess excitatory conditioning to the context alone. Subjects in the summation condition (Sum) were tested in Context B, whereas those in the retardation condition (Ret) were tested in Context C. The sessions were 15 min in duration, with an upper limit of 15 min recorded for subjects that did not complete five cumulative seconds of drinking. Higher lick latencies represent a behavioral measure of greater expectancy of the aversive shock (i.e., freezing). All test scores were converted to log scores to better approximate the within-group normal distributions necessary for parametric statistical analysis. Retardation training On Day 11, subjects in Condition Sum received four US presentations at 65, 110, 165, and 230 s into a 260-s session in Context C, which was associatively neutral (i.e., the experimental subjects from the summation test were now the control subjects for the retardation test). Subjects in Condition Ret received the same treatment in Context B, the extinction context. The lick tubes were not accessible during this phase. Retardation test Prior to Day 12, the lick tubes were reinserted into Contexts B and C. On Day 12, all subjects were tested in the context of retardation training from the previous phase. Upon placing a subject in the chamber, the time to drink five cumulative seconds was recorded. The sessions were 30 min in duration, with an upper limit score of 30 min recorded for subjects that did not complete five cumulative seconds of drinking. As in the summation test, all test scores were converted to log scores to better approximate within-group normal distributions. Results and discussion Summation test As can be seen in Fig. 1, Condition Sum stopped suppressing faster than Condition Ret, indicating that Table 1 Design of Experiment 1 Groups Transfer Training Acquisition Extinction Summation Test Retardation Training Retardation Test Sum Massed (4X+) A (24 Z+) D (40 Z ) B (Massed) (X) B (4 +) C ( ) C Ret Massed (4X+) A (24 Z+) D (40 Z ) B (Massed) (X) C (4 +) B ( ) B Sum Spaced (4X+) A (24 Z+) D (40 Z ) B (Spaced) (X) B (4 +) C ( ) C Ret Spaced (4X+) A (24 Z+) D (40 Z ) B (Spaced) (X) C (4 +) B ( ) B Subscripts indicate that the variable is a context. X = clicks; Z = tone; Sum = experimental condition for summation test and control condition for retardation test; Ret = control condition for summation test and experimental condition for retardation test. + indicates footshock, indicates nonreinforcement. Contexts B and C were counterbalanced. Numbers refer to the number of trials. Massed and spaced manipulations refer to the trial spacing during extinction

5 28 Learn Behav (2012) 40:24 33 Lick latency (log10 s) Massed Sum Ret Spaced Lick latency (log10 s) Massed Sum Ret Spaced Fig. 1 Mean log times to complete five cumulative seconds of licking during the summation test of Experiment 1. Brackets represent SEMs. White bars represent testing of X in the extinction context (B). Black bars represent testing of X in an associatively neutral context (C). Massed and spaced manipulations refer to trial spacing during extinction. See the text for details and Table 1 for the experimental design Fig. 2 Mean log times to complete five cumulative seconds of licking during the retardation test of Experiment 1. Brackets represent SEMs. White bars represent testing in the control context (C). Black bars represent testing in the extinction context (B). Massed and spaced manipulations refer to trial spacing during extinction. See the text for details and Table 1 for the experimental design negative summation occurred when subjects were tested in the extinction context relative to an associatively neutral context. Negative summation was more pronounced when the extinction trials had been massed relative to when they had been spaced. This suggests that under some conditions an extinction context can pass a negative summation test for conditioned inhibition and that extinction trial spacing has an influence on the amount of negative summation observed. The following statistics support these conclusions. A 2 (Sum vs. Ret) 2 (massed vs. spaced) ANOVA was conducted on the log lick latencies from the summation test. A two-tailed criterion of.05 was used to identify significant differences for both summation and retardation data analyses. A significant interaction, F(1, 44) = 10.04, MSE =.05, Cohen s f =.44, was detected. Planned comparisons were conducted in order to determine the source of the interaction. Within the massed condition, we found that summation with the extinction context (Condition Sum) resulted in weaker suppression than summation with the neutral context (Condition Ret), F(1, 44) = Weaker suppression to the extinction context was also observed within the spaced condition, F(1, 44) = Negative summation with the extinction context produced less suppression in Group Sum Massed than Group Sum Spaced, F(1, 44) = 12.89; however, suppression in Condition Ret was not significantly influenced by the spacing of the extinction trials, F(1, 44) < 1. Retardation test As can be seen in Fig. 2, the results of this test were not entirely consistent with the results of the summation test. Those groups that received retardation training in the extinction context (i.e., Condition Ret) exhibited weaker suppression at test than those groups that received training in the control context (C; i.e., Condition Sum), and whether the extinction trials were massed or not appears to have had little influence on the level of suppression to the context at test. The following statistics support these assertions. A 2 (Sum vs. Ret) 2 (massed vs. spaced) ANOVA was conducted on the log lick latencies from the retardation test. This analysis revealed a main effect of context of testing (Sum vs. Ret), F(1, 44) = 9.84, MSE =.51, Cohen s f =.43, but no effect of the extinction trial spacing, F(1, 44) = 1.36, p >.24, and no interaction between these variables, F(1, 44) < 1. Planned comparisons revealed that retarded acquisition to the extinction context was observed when extinction trials were massed, F(1, 44) = 4.62, and when they were spaced, F(1, 44) = The suggestion that conditioned inhibition may be more pronounced when extinction trials were massed rather than spaced, supported by the summation results, was not supported by the present retardation data. However, the observation of retardation in the extinction context but not in Context C supports the view that the extinction context became inhibitory. It should be noted that the prior summation test location perfectly confounded the retardation test, but there is no theoretical or empirical reason to think that the location of the summation test would influence retardation test performance. Conclusions The results of the summation and retardation tests demonstrated that an extinction context can become a conditioned inhibitor, at least with some parameters. The summation test suggested that one of the parameters that influence the extent to which an extinction context becomes inhibitory is the spacing of the extinction trials. However, this claim was not supported by the retardation data. One possible reason why retarded acquisition to the extinction context was observed in both the massed and spaced conditions is that the spaced trials may have

6 Learn Behav (2012) 40: produced latent inhibition due to the extra exposure to that context (Lubow & Moore, 1959; Takigasaki, 1993). Thus, Group Ret Massed may have exhibited retarded acquisition due to the intended conditioned inhibition, whereas Group Ret Spaced may have showed retarded acquisition due to latent inhibition. This provides a plausible explanation for why responding was similarly retarded following massed and spaced extinction. To control for any influence of latent inhibition, Experiment 2 equated total exposure to the two testing contexts. Experiment 2 The results of Experiment 1 suggested that the extinction context became inhibitory, but this inhibition may have been due to discriminative inhibition training, rather than to Pavlovian conditioned inhibition training as predicted by the Rescorla Wagner (1972) model. That is, in Experiment 1 rats received shocks in Contexts A and D, but nonreinforced experience in Context B. If the contexts were being processed like punctate cues, this treatment would be analogous to sequential differential inhibition training. To determine whether the extinction treatment produced Pavlovian conditioned inhibition or differential training to Context B, we equated the numbers of stimulus presentations in each test context (B and C), and to eliminate the potential differences in latent inhibition to the two test contexts, the total amounts of exposure to Contexts B and C were also equivalent in Experiment 2. Reduced responding in Context B relative to Context C would support the conclusion that the results of Experiment 1 were due at least in part to the context becoming inhibitory through Pavlovian conditioned inhibition, with CS Z serving as the excitor for inhibition training (i.e., not due to prior nonreinforced experience of Context B). If inhibition was produced by discriminative conditioning alone, then Context B and Context C should be equivalently inhibitory. Method Subjects and apparatus The subjects were 48 male and 48 female experimentally naive Sprague-Dawley-descended rats obtained from our own breeding colony. Body-weight ranges were g for males and g for females. The larger number of subjects in Experiment 2 relative to Experiment 1 was required in order to obtain results that did not depend on marginal significance. Experiment 2 was run in two replications that used identical numbers of subjects and parameters. The housing, apparatus, and contexts were as stated in Experiment 1. The only difference from Experiment 1 was that the tone and the white noise served as the extinguished stimulus Z or the control stimulus Y, counterbalanced within groups. Procedures Acclimation, transfer excitor training, and acquisition On Days 1 5, acclimation, transfer excitor training, and the acquisition phase were conducted as described in Experiment 1, with the exception that during acquisition nonreinforced presentations of Stimulus Y were interspersed with the reinforced presentations of Z during acquisition (see Table 2). Y presentations occurred at 10, 20, 35, 50, 70, 85, 105, and 115 min into the three 120-min sessions. Extinction On Days 6 13, half of the subjects in each group received 10 nonreinforced presentations of Z in Context B on even-numbered days and 10 nonreinforced presentations of Y in Context C on oddnumbered days, whereas the other half received the same treatment with the days reversed. All subjects received extinction treatment of Stimulus Z with the same intertrial intervals used in Experiment 1, and Stimulus Y was presented equivalently to Stimulus Z. The lick tubes were not accessible during this phase. Table 2 Design of Experiment 2 Groups Transfer Training Acquisition Extinction Summation Test Retardation Training Retardation Test Sum Massed (4 X+) A (24 Z+ / 24 Y ) D (40 Z ) B / (40 Y ) C (Massed) (X) B (4 +) C ( ) C Ret Massed (4 X+) A (24 Z+ / 24 Y ) D (40 Z ) B / (40 Y ) C (Massed) (X) C (4 +) B ( ) B Sum Spaced (4 X+) A (24 Z+ / 24 Y ) D (40 Z ) B / (40 Y ) C (Spaced) (X) B (4 +) C ( ) C Ret Spaced (4 X+) A (24 Z+ / 24 Y ) D (40 Z ) B / (40 Y ) C (Spaced) (X) C (4 +) B ( ) B Subscripts indicate that the variable is a context. X = clicks; Z & Y = tone & white noise, counterbalanced; Sum = experimental condition for summation test and control condition for retardation test; Ret = control condition for summation test and experimental condition for retardation test. + indicates footshock, indicates nonreinforcement. Contexts B and C were counterbalanced. Numbers refer to the number of trials. Massed and spaced manipulations refer to the trial spacing during extinction

7 30 Learn Behav (2012) 40:24 33 Summation test, retardation training, and retardation test On Days 14 16, the summation test, retardation training, and retardation test were conducted exactly as described in Experiment 1. Results and discussion Summation test As can be seen in Fig. 3, negative summation occurred in the massed condition, thereby replicating the findings of the massed condition in Experiment 1. However, in the spaced condition there appeared to be a lack of any observable negative summation, indicating that in Experiment 1 the relative novelty of Context C may have influenced the results of the summation test. Importantly, the summation test data from both Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated that an extinction context, under some conditions (e.g., massed trials during extinction), can pass a summation test for conditioned inhibition. The following statistics support these conclusions. A 2 (replication) 2 (Sum vs. Ret) 2 (massed vs. spaced) ANOVA was conducted on the log lick latencies from the summation test. A main effect of replication (p <.05) was observed, which arose from reduced suppression in Replication 2 relative to Replication 1. This effect was likely due to the greater weight of the rats in Replication 2. Importantly, the replication did not interact with either of the critical manipulations; therefore, it is omitted from the rest of the discussion of the results. We observed an interaction between context of testing and extinction trial spacing, F(1, 88) = 5.60, MSE =.10, Cohen s f =.22. Planned comparisons were conducted in order to identify the source of the interaction. We observed less responding in Group Sum Massed than in Group Ret Massed, F(1, 88) = 5.11, demonstrating negative summation when subjects were tested in the extinction context after massed extinction training. However, when the extinction trials were spaced, there appeared to be no appreciable difference in responding based on the context of testing (i.e., Groups Sum Spaced vs. Ret Spaced), F(1, 88) = As in Experiment 1, Group Sum Massed demonstrated weaker responding (i.e., more negative summation) than Group Sum Spaced, F(1, 88) = 10.72, whereas there was no observable difference between Group Ret Massed and Group Ret Spaced, F(1, 88) < 1. Retardation test As can be seen in Fig. 4, the data from the retardation test of Experiment 2 are congruent with the summation tests from Experiments 1 and 2. The extinction context exhibited retarded behavioral control (relative to a group in which contextual latent inhibition was controlled) after massed extinction but not following spaced extinction trials. This finding supports our speculation that in Experiment 1 retardation was observed in Group Ret Spaced largely due to strong latent inhibition. The following statistics support this interpretation. A 2 (replication) 2 (Sum vs. Ret) 2 (massed vs. spaced) ANOVA was conducted on the log lick latencies from the retardation test. Again, we observed a main effect of replication, with reduced suppression in all groups of Replication 2, but a lack of any interaction with replication. Therefore, we further analyzed only the effects of the critical manipulations, test context and extinction trial spacing. The analysis revealed an interaction between extinction trial spacing and test context, F(1, 88) = 5.08, MSE =.35, Cohen s f =.21. Planned comparisons were conducted to assess the source of the interaction. These Lick latency (log10 s) Sum Ret Lick latency (log10 s) Sum Ret Massed Spaced Massed Spaced Fig. 3 Mean log times to complete five cumulative seconds of licking during the summation test of Experiment 2. Brackets represent SEMs. White bars represent testing of X in the extinction context (B). Black bars represent testing of X in an associatively neutral context (C). Massed and spaced manipulations refer to trial spacing during extinction. See the text for details and Table 2 for the experimental design Fig. 4 Mean log times to complete five cumulative seconds of licking during the retardation test of Experiment 2. Brackets represent SEMs. White bars represent testing in the control context (C). Black bars represent testing in the extinction context (B). Massed and spaced manipulations refer to trial spacing during extinction. See the text for details and Table 2 for the experimental design

8 Learn Behav (2012) 40: planned comparisons revealed that in the spaced condition appreciable retardation did not occur, F(1, 88) < 1; however, when extinction trials were massed, responding to the context of extinction (Group Ret Massed) was retarded in acquiring excitatory control of behavior, as compared to responding to the control context (Group Sum Massed), F(1, 88) = Additionally, Group Ret Massed demonstrated greater retarded acquisition than Group Ret Spaced, F(1, 88) = 21.52, whereas Groups Sum Massed and Sum Spaced did not differ, F(1, 88) < 1. Conclusions The summation and retardation data from Experiment 2 suggest that extinction of a punctate cue outside of the acquisition context can result in that extinction context acquiring inhibitory properties when extinction is conducted with massed trials. Moreover, Experiment 2 demonstrated that trial spacing during extinction influences the extinction context s inhibitory potential. Massed extinction trials resulted in the extinction context passing both summation and retardation tests, whereas spaced extinction trials seemingly attenuated the context s becoming a conditioned inhibitor. General discussion The present observations suggest that the frequently cited, but usually rejected, explanation of renewal in terms of the extinction context becoming inhibitory may in fact be valid in some situations. The present data suggest that extinction contexts can become inhibitory, at least when the extinction trials are massed. These results have long been proposed as a potential account of renewal, but to our knowledge, the extinction context becoming an inhibitor has never before been clearly demonstrated. Bouton and King (1983) failed to observe conditioned inhibition in their extinction contexts, presumably because they used parameters comparable to our spaced condition, in which negative summation was much weaker and only observed relative to a context that was relatively novel in addition to being associatively neutral. Spaced extinction trials are commonly used in an attempt to limit any potential inhibitory role of the extinction context. A possible distinction between spaced relative to massed extinction trials is that spaced trials may be perceived as more like sequential presentations of the context and the extinguished cue, whereas massed trials are perceived more like simultaneous presentations. Lamarre and Holland (1987; see also Holland & Lamarre, 1984) observed that negative discriminative stimuli failed to transfer to novel excitors if the discriminative stimuli had been presented sequentially with respect to the excitatory CS during training, but did transfer if they were presented simultaneously with the excitatory CS during training. Essentially, sequential presentations during discrimination training produced occasion setters, whereas simultaneous presentations produced conditioned inhibitors. Additionally, attentional mechanisms have been proposed to account for renewal effects (Larrauri & Schmajuk 2008), and reduced attention to the context in the spaced condition could possibly account for the present findings. According to the Rescorla Wagner (1972) model, conditioned inhibitors generate negative expectancy of the US, so exposure to the inhibitor alone generates a negatively valued expectancy of the US that is incompatible with the actual outcome (zero value). Consequently, the error correction rule should reduce the negative expectancy generated by the inhibitor (i.e., extinction of an inhibitor). The long intertrial intervals used by Bouton and King (1983) during extinction can be viewed as context (inhibitor) alone trials, which according to the Rescorla Wagner model should extinguish the inhibitory strength of the extinction context. It should be noted that there are numerous failures to observe attenuation of a punctate conditioned inhibitor through additional exposure to the inhibitor alone (for a review, see Williams, Overmier, & LoLordo, 1992); however, it has been recently observed that massive exposure to a punctate inhibitor alone can attenuate its inhibitory properties (Polack et al., 2011). Extinction of conditioned inhibition may explain why extinction contexts are rarely observed to be conditioned inhibitors, but this alone cannot explain why renewal is still observed when inhibition is not. This is problematic for attempts to explain all renewal through an inhibition-based account. In Polack et al. s (2011) series of experiments, after a punctate inhibitor was massively exposed, it continued to negatively summate with the initial training excitor, but not with a transfer excitor that had training equivalent to that of the training excitor. This implies that extinction of a conditioned inhibitor does not completely eliminate inhibition, but instead makes the conditioned inhibitor more stimulus specific. This stimulus-specific inhibition differs from occasion setting in that occasion setters modulate novel cues that have previously been occasion set by other stimuli, whereas a stimulus-specific inhibitor failed to generalize to a comparably treated cue. Thus, previous failures to show that an extinction context is a conditioned inhibitor may be misleading. Additional exposure to the extinction context (e.g., long intertrial intervals during extinction) potentially attenuates negative summation between the extinction context and a novel excitor but has little impact on negative summation with the specific cue that was extinguished in that context. The stimulus-specific inhibition that remains

9 32 Learn Behav (2012) 40:24 33 after extinction of a conditioned inhibitor is one of the attributes that define occasion setters. This parallel blurs the distinction between Pavlovian inhibitors and negative modulators. Lastly, a confound in Bouton and King s (1983) study is that the control group received extinction training in the acquisition context and was then shifted to a neutral context to test the transfer excitor, relative to the experimental group, which received extinction in a neutral context. Consequently, the two groups received different experiences due to extinction occurring in contexts of different associative values. Extinction treatment administered in the acquisition context is known to produce a strong reduction in behavioral control, even when testing occurs outside of the extinction context (Laborda et al., 2011; Tamai & Nakajima, 2000; Tamai, Nakajima, Kitaguchi, & Imada, 2001). Thus, Bouton and King s observation of renewal may be explained simply in terms of enhanced extinction produced by extinguishing a cue in the presence of an excitatory context (Rescorla, 2000; but see Pearce & Wilson, 1991). The present experiments demonstrate that extinguishing a punctate cue in a neutral context can cause that context to become a conditioned inhibitor. Although the present experiments did not assess renewal of the extinguished cue, as Bouton and King (1983) had, they did demonstrate that the context was inhibitory when extinction trials were massed. The finding that trial spacing is a determinant of whether the context becomes a conditioned inhibitor, in conjunction with recent findings that exposure to the conditioned inhibitor alone attenuates negative summation with transfer excitors (Polack et al., 2011), supports accounts of renewal based on contextual conditioned inhibition (e.g., Rescorla & Wagner, 1972). As extinction contexts tend to become inhibitory only with massed extinction trials, the present data do not provide a full account of renewal in terms of the extinction context becoming a conditioned inhibitor. However, the present data indicate that, at least under some circumstances, the inhibitory value of the extinction context can contribute to renewal. Author note National Institute of Mental Health Grant supported this research. Mario Laborda was supported by the Comisión Nacional de Investigación Científica y Tecnológica (CONICYT-Chile) and the Department of Psychology of the Universidad de Chile. The authors would like to thank Bridget L. McConnell, Gonzalo Miguez, Henry Cham, Liam O Hehir, Jessica Ray, and Christina Talev for their comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. References Bouton, M. E. (1993). Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychological Bulletin, 114, doi: / Bouton, M. E. (2010). The multiple forms of context in associative learning theory. In B. Mesquita, L. F. Barrett, & E. R. Smith (Eds.), The mind in context (pp ). New York: Guilford Press. Bouton, M. E., & King, D. A. (1983). Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear: Tests for the associative value of the context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, doi: / Bouton, M. E., & Swartzentruber, D. (1986). Analysis of the associative and occasion-setting properties of contexts participating in a Pavlovian discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 12, doi: / Cunningham, C. L. (1979). Alcohol as a cue for extinction: State dependency produced by conditioned inhibition. Animal Learning & Behavior, 7, doi: /bf Holland, P. C. (1983). Occasion-setting in Pavlovian feature positive discriminations. In M. L. Commons, R. J. Herrnstein, & A. R. Wagner (Eds.), Quantitative analysis of behavior: Discrimination processes (Vol. 4, pp ). New York: Ballinger. Holland, P. C. (1992). Occasion setting in Pavlovian conditioning. In D. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 28, pp ). San Diego: Academic Press. Holland, P. C., & Lamarre, J. (1984). Transfer of inhibition after serial and simultaneous feature negative discrimination training. Learning and Motivation, 15, doi: / (84) Laborda, M. A., Witnauer, J. E., & Miller, R. R. (2011). Contrasting AAC and ABC renewal: The role of contextual associations. Learning & Behavior, 39, doi: /s Lamarre, J., & Holland, P. C. (1987). Transfer of inhibition after serial feature negative discrimination training. Learning and Motivation, 18, doi: / (87) Larrauri, J. A., & Schmajuk, N. A. (2008). Attentional, associative, and configural mechanisms in extinction. Psychological Review, 115, doi: / x Lubow, R. E., & Moore, A. U. (1959). Latent inhibition: The effect of nonreinforced pre-exposure to the conditional stimulus. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52, doi: /h Miller, R. R., & Oberling, P. (1998). Analogies between occasion setting and Pavlovian conditioning. In N. A. Schmajuk & P. C. Holland (Eds.), Occasion setting: Associative learning and cognition in animals (pp. 3 35). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Nelson, J. B., Sanjuan, M. C., Vadillo-Ruiz, S., Pérez, J., & León, S. P. (2011). Experimental renewal in human participants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 37, doi: /a Pearce, J. M., & Wilson, P. N. (1991). Effects of extinction with a compound conditioned stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, doi: / Polack, C. W., Laborda, M. A., & Miller, R. R. (2011). Extinction of a conditioned inhibitor. Manuscript submitted for publication. Randich, A. (1981). The US preexposure phenomenon in the conditioned suppression paradigm: A role for conditioned situational stimuli. Learning and Motivation, 12, doi: / (81) Rescorla, R. A. (1969). Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 72, doi: /h Rescorla, R. A. (2000). Extinction can be enhanced by a concurrent excitor. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 26, doi: / Rescorla, R. A. (2003). Protection from extinction. Learning & Behavior, 31, doi: /bf

10 Learn Behav (2012) 40: Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp ). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Takigasaki, T. (1993). Effects of contextual preexposure on the context shock conditioning in rats. Japanese Psychological Research, 35, Tamai, N., & Nakajima, S. (2000). Renewal of formerly conditioned fear in rats after extensive extinction training. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 13, Tamai, N., Nakajima, S., Kitaguchi, K., & Imada, H. (2001). Renewal of extinguished fear by context-shifting in rats conditioned lick suppression. Japanese Journal of Psychology, 71, Urcelay, G. P., & Miller, R. R. (2010). Two roles of the context in Pavlovian fear conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 36, doi: /a Urushihara, K., & Miller, R. R. (2009). Stimulus competition between a discrete cue and a training context: Cue competition does not result from the division of a limited resource. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 35, doi: /a Williams, D. A., Overmier, J. B., & LoLordo, V. M. (1992). A reevaluation of Rescorla s early dictums about Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 111, doi: / Zimmer-Hart, C. L., & Rescorla, R. A. (1974). Extinction of a Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 86, doi: /h

Some determinants of second-order conditioning

Some determinants of second-order conditioning Learn Behav (2011) 39:12 26 DOI 10.1007/s13420-010-0002-6 Some determinants of second-order conditioning James E. Witnauer & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010

More information

from extinction provided by a conditioned inhibitor.

from extinction provided by a conditioned inhibitor. Learning & Behavior 2010, 38 (1), 68-79 doi:10.3758/lb.38.1.68 Protection from extinction provided by a conditioned inhibitor BRIDGET L. MCCONNELL AND RAR LPH R. MILLER State University of New York, Binghamton,

More information

Contrasting AAC and ABC renewal: the role of context associations

Contrasting AAC and ABC renewal: the role of context associations Learn Behav (2011) 39:46 56 DOI 10.3758/s13420-010-0007-1 Contrasting AAC and ABC renewal: the role of context associations Mario A. Laborda & James E. Witnauer & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 2 December

More information

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery L132 NT MJA/cla Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (1), 60-73 doi:10.3758/lb.37.1.60 Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery Gonzalo P. Urcelay University of Cambridge, Cambridge,

More information

The Role of Temporal Relationships in the Transfer of Conditioned Inhibition

The Role of Temporal Relationships in the Transfer of Conditioned Inhibition Denniston, James C., Robert P. Cole, and Ralph R. Miller. (1998). "The role of temporal relationships in the transfer of conditioned inhibition." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes

More information

Cue competition as a retrieval deficit

Cue competition as a retrieval deficit Denniston, J. C., Savastano, H. I., Blaisdell, A. P., & Miller, R. R. (2003). Cue competition as a retrieval deficit. Learning and Motivation, 34(1): 1-31. (Feb 2003) Published by Elsevier (ISSN: 1095-9122).

More information

S-R Associations, Their Extinction, and Recovery in an Animal Model of Anxiety: A New Associative Account of Phobias Without Recall of Original Trauma

S-R Associations, Their Extinction, and Recovery in an Animal Model of Anxiety: A New Associative Account of Phobias Without Recall of Original Trauma Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Behavior Therapy 42 (2011) 153 169 www.elsevier.com/locate/bt S-R Associations, Their Extinction, and Recovery in an Animal Model of Anxiety: A New Associative

More information

Spontaneous Recovery From Forward and Backward Blocking

Spontaneous Recovery From Forward and Backward Blocking Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2005, Vol. 31, No. 2, 172 183 Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.31.2.172

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Cue competition as a retrieval deficit q

Cue competition as a retrieval deficit q Learning and Motivation 34 (2003) 1 31 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Cue competition as a retrieval deficit q James C. Denniston, a, * Hernan I. Savastano, b Aaron P. Blaisdell, c and Ralph R. Miller d a

More information

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 23, 343-367 (1992) Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training CHARLOTTE BONARDI University of York, York, United Kingdom In four experiments rats received

More information

Counterconditioning of an Overshadowed Cue Attenuates Overshadowing

Counterconditioning of an Overshadowed Cue Attenuates Overshadowing Journal of F.xperirmm~ Psycholosy: Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological hau~ocialioa, Inc. Animall~ehavioa'Procc~es 0097-7403/00/$5.110 DOI: 10.10371/0097-7403.26.1.74 2000, VoL 26, No. 1, 74-86

More information

Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning

Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (3), 230-245 doi:10.3758/lb.37.3.230 Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning JUSTIN A. HARRIS, SABA A GHARA EI, AND CLINTON A. MOORE

More information

Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement

Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement Psicológica (2004), 25, 45-65. Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement Matías López*, Raúl Cantora*, and Luis Aguado** 1 * Universidad de

More information

Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting

Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting Animal Learning & Behavior 1989, 17 (3), 269-279 Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting PETER C. HOLLAND Duke University, Durham, North Carolina In three experiments, transfer effects

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology Title Renewal of Formerly Conditioned Fear in Rats after Extensive Extinction Training Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/7d41p8fj Journal International

More information

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1991, Vol. 17, No. 3, 231-248 Copyright 1991 by the Am n Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/91/53.00 Transfer of Control in Ambiguous

More information

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(2),199-203 Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning PETER C. HOLLAND and DESWELL T. FORBES University ofpittsburgh, Pittsburgh,

More information

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition Scholarly Commons Psychology Faculty Publications 2015 Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition James F. Briggs Timothy A. Toth Brian P. Olson Jacob G.

More information

Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning

Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning Learn Behav (2011) 39:79 86 DOI 10.3758/s13420-011-0022-x Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning Gabriel Rodríguez & Gumersinda Alonso Published

More information

The Influence of the Initial Associative Strength on the Rescorla-Wagner Predictions: Relative Validity

The Influence of the Initial Associative Strength on the Rescorla-Wagner Predictions: Relative Validity Methods of Psychological Research Online 4, Vol. 9, No. Internet: http://www.mpr-online.de Fachbereich Psychologie 4 Universität Koblenz-Landau The Influence of the Initial Associative Strength on the

More information

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION. William Travis Suits

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION. William Travis Suits TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION Except where reference is made to the works of others, the work described in this dissertation is my own or was done in collaboration

More information

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999 TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL A Senior Honors Thesis Presented By Vanessa E. Castagna June 999 999 by Vanessa E. Castagna ABSTRACT TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: A TEST OF

More information

Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association

Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association Learning and Motivation 32, 349 366 (2001) doi:10.1006/lmot.2001.1089, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association Charlotte Bonardi

More information

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats Animal Learning & Behavior 1992. 20 (3). 233-239 The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats JAMES S. MILLER and JOYCE A.

More information

Renewal after the extinction of free operant behavior

Renewal after the extinction of free operant behavior Learn Behav (211) 39:57 67 DOI 1.3758/s1342-11-18-6 Renewal after the extinction of free operant behavior Mark E. Bouton & Travis P. Todd & Drina Vurbic & Neil E. Winterbauer Published online: 29 January

More information

Contextual Effects in Conditioning, Latent Inhibition, and Habituation: Associative and Retrieval Functions of Contextual Cues

Contextual Effects in Conditioning, Latent Inhibition, and Habituation: Associative and Retrieval Functions of Contextual Cues Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1989, Vol. 15, No. 3, 232-241 Copyright 1989 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-740389$00.75 Contextual Effects in Conditioning,

More information

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1976, Vol. 4 (3), 293-298 The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock THOMAS s. HYDE Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland,

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

Context and Pavlovian conditioning

Context and Pavlovian conditioning Context Brazilian conditioning Journal of Medical and Biological Research (1996) 29: 149-173 ISSN 0100-879X 149 Context and Pavlovian conditioning Departamento de Psicologia, Pontifícia Universidade Católica

More information

INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT

INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT A thesis submitted to Kent State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts By Erin Marie Fleming

More information

Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response 1

Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response 1 Japanese Psychological Research 1998, Volume 40, No. 2, 104 110 Short Report Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response

More information

Second-order conditioning during a compound extinction treatment

Second-order conditioning during a compound extinction treatment Learning and Motivation 38 (2007) 172 192 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Second-order conditioning during a compound extinction treatment Oskar Pineño a, *, Jessica M. Zilski a, Todd R. Schachtman b a Dpto.

More information

Generalization of Fear Effects in Reinstatement to a Discrete Stimulus

Generalization of Fear Effects in Reinstatement to a Discrete Stimulus University of South Carolina Scholar Commons USC Aiken Psychology Theses Psychology Department 8-2014 Generalization of Fear Effects in Reinstatement to a Discrete Stimulus Lauren Best University of South

More information

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2016 American Psychological Association 2016, Vol. 42, No. 4, 401 414 2329-8456/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000112 Contextual

More information

Transfer after Serial Feature Positive Discrimination Training

Transfer after Serial Feature Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 17, 243-268 (1986) Transfer after Serial Feature Positive Discrimination Training PETER C. HOLLAND University of Pittsburgh Three experiments with rat subjects examined the transfer

More information

Primacy and recency effects in extinction and latent inhibition: A selective review with implications for models of learning

Primacy and recency effects in extinction and latent inhibition: A selective review with implications for models of learning Behavioural Processes 69 (2005) 223 235 Primacy and recency effects in extinction and latent inhibition: A selective review with implications for models of learning Oskar Pineño, Ralph R. Miller Department

More information

The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Computational Models of Neural Systems Lecture 5.1

The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Computational Models of Neural Systems Lecture 5.1 The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Lecture 5.1 David S. Touretzky Based on notes by Lisa M. Saksida November, 2015 Outline Classical and instrumental conditioning The Rescorla

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

Magazine approach during a signal for food depends on Pavlovian, not instrumental, conditioning.

Magazine approach during a signal for food depends on Pavlovian, not instrumental, conditioning. In Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/xan/index.aspx 2013, vol. 39 (2), pp 107 116 2013 American Psychological Association DOI: 10.1037/a0031315

More information

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes Journal Learning & Behavior 2005,?? 33 (?), (4),???-??? 464-478 Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes PETER C. HOLLAND

More information

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Eric A. Thrailkill 1, Alex Kacelnik 2, Fay Porritt 3 & Mark E. Bouton

More information

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (2), 213-219 Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning THOMAS HIGGINS and ROBERT A. RESCORLA University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

More information

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT The Psychological Record, 2007, 57, 577 592 PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, AMBER HULS, and ASHLEY KULLAND University

More information

Covariation in conditioned response strength between stimuli trained in compound

Covariation in conditioned response strength between stimuli trained in compound Animal Learning & Behavior /987, 15 (4), 439-447 Covariation in conditioned response strength between stimuli trained in compound LOUIS D. MATZEL, KARL SHUSTER, and RALPH R. MILLER State University ofnew

More information

THEORIES OF ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING

THEORIES OF ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2001. 52:111 39 Copyright c 2001 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved THEORIES OF ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING IN ANIMALS John M. Pearce School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/319/5871/1849/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Rule Learning by Rats Robin A. Murphy,* Esther Mondragón, Victoria A. Murphy This PDF file includes: *To whom correspondence

More information

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 322 328 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.3.322

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 90 (2012) 311 322 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journa l h omepa g e: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc US specificity of occasion setting:

More information

Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative mechanisms

Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative mechanisms Q0667 QJEP(B) si-b03/read as keyed THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 56B (1), 43 55 Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative

More information

The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning

The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning Behavioural Brain Research 110 (2000) 97 108 www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning Stephen Maren *, William Holt Department of Psychology

More information

Taste quality and extinction of a conditioned taste aversion in rats

Taste quality and extinction of a conditioned taste aversion in rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of September 1999 Taste quality and extinction of

More information

Contrast and the justification of effort

Contrast and the justification of effort Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (2), 335-339 Contrast and the justification of effort EMILY D. KLEIN, RAMESH S. BHATT, and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky When humans

More information

Stimulus competition in the absence of compound conditioning

Stimulus competition in the absence of compound conditioning Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (1), 3-14 Stimulus competition in the absence of compound conditioning HELENA MATUTE and OSKAR PINEÑO Universidad de Deusto, Bilbao, Spain Most associative theories

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

Contextual Conditioning with Lithium-Induced Nausea as the US: Evidence from a Blocking Procedure

Contextual Conditioning with Lithium-Induced Nausea as the US: Evidence from a Blocking Procedure LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 28, 200 215 (1997) ARTICLE NO. LM960958 Contextual Conditioning with Lithium-Induced Nausea as the US: Evidence from a Blocking Procedure MICHELLE SYMONDS AND GEOFFREY HALL University

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 05, 3, 3 45 NUMBER (JANUARY) WITHIN-SUBJECT TESTING OF THE SIGNALED-REINFORCEMENT EFFECT ON OPERANT RESPONDING AS MEASURED BY RESPONSE RATE AND RESISTANCE

More information

Transfer of inhibition and facilitation mediated by the original target stimulus

Transfer of inhibition and facilitation mediated by the original target stimulus Animal Leaming & Behavior 1991,19 (1), 65-70 Transfer of inhibition and facilitation mediated by the original target stimulus ROBERT A. RESCORLA University of Pennsyloania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania An

More information

University of Southampton Research Repository eprints Soton

University of Southampton Research Repository eprints Soton University of Southampton Research Repository eprints Soton Copyright and Moral Rights for this thesis are retained by the author and/or other copyright owners. A copy can be downloaded for personal non-commercial

More information

Blocking in eyelid conditioning: Effect of changing the CS-US interval and introducing an intertrial stimulus

Blocking in eyelid conditioning: Effect of changing the CS-US interval and introducing an intertrial stimulus Animal Learning& Behavior 1979, 7 (4), 452-456 Blocking in eyelid conditioning: Effect of changing the CS-US interval and introducing an intertrial stimulus ROBERT T. MALESKE and PETER W. FREY Northwestern

More information

Occasional reinforced trials during extinction can slow the rate of rapid reacquisition

Occasional reinforced trials during extinction can slow the rate of rapid reacquisition Learning and Motivation 35 (2004) 371 390 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Occasional reinforced trials during extinction can slow the rate of rapid reacquisition Mark E. Bouton, Amanda M. Woods, and Oskar

More information

Model Uncertainty in Classical Conditioning

Model Uncertainty in Classical Conditioning Model Uncertainty in Classical Conditioning A. C. Courville* 1,3, N. D. Daw 2,3, G. J. Gordon 4, and D. S. Touretzky 2,3 1 Robotics Institute, 2 Computer Science Department, 3 Center for the Neural Basis

More information

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1) JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 23, 79, 233 242 NUMBER 2(MARCH) PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY Rats

More information

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1965, Vol. 59, No. 1, 13-17 AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 GORDON BOWER, RONALD STARR, AND LEAH LAZAROVITZ Stanford

More information

An extinction trial as a reminder treatment following electroconvulsive shock

An extinction trial as a reminder treatment following electroconvulsive shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(3),363-367 An extinction trial as a reminder treatment following electroconvulsive shock WLLAM C. GORDON and ROBERT R. MOWRER University ofnew Mexico, Albuquerque, New

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR LICK-SHOCK CONTINGENCIES IN THE RATT1 VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY 1972, 17, 119-125 NUMBER I (JANUARY) Hungry rats were allowed to lick an 8%

More information

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1971, 15, 243-247 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) POSITIVE CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION: EFFECTS OF CS DURATION' KLAUS A. MICZEK AND SEBASTIAN P. GROSSMAN UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO

More information

Two Kinds of Attention in Pavlovian Conditioning: Evidence for a Hybrid Model of Learning

Two Kinds of Attention in Pavlovian Conditioning: Evidence for a Hybrid Model of Learning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2010, Vol. 36, No. 4, 456 470 2010 American Psychological Association 0097-7403/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0018528 Two Kinds of Attention in Pavlovian

More information

Extinction of the Context and Latent Inhibition

Extinction of the Context and Latent Inhibition LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 13, 391-416 (1982) Extinction of the Context and Latent Inhibition A. G. BAKER AND PIERRE MERCIER McGill University The hypothesis that latent inhibition could be reduced by extinguishing

More information

Perceptual learning transfer in an appetitive Pavlovian task

Perceptual learning transfer in an appetitive Pavlovian task Learn Behav (2017) 45:115 123 DOI 10.3758/s13420-016-0245-y Perceptual learning transfer in an appetitive Pavlovian task Antonio A. Artigas 1 & Jose Prados 2 Published online: 5 August 2016 # Psychonomic

More information

Effects of an extinguished CS on competition with another CS

Effects of an extinguished CS on competition with another CS Behavioural Processes 72 (2006) 14 22 Effects of an extinguished CS on competition with another CS Carla H. Bills a, Marsha Dopheide a, Oskar Pineño b, Todd R. Schachtman a, a University of Missouri, USA

More information

Conditioned inhibition of fear from negative Cs-us contingencies with shocks in the inhibitory Cs.

Conditioned inhibition of fear from negative Cs-us contingencies with shocks in the inhibitory Cs. University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 1974 Conditioned inhibition of fear from negative Cs-us contingencies with shocks in the inhibitory Cs.

More information

Modulatory mechanisms in Pavlovian conditioning

Modulatory mechanisms in Pavlovian conditioning Animal Learning & Behavior 1995,23 (2),123-143 Modulatory mechanisms in Pavlovian conditioning DALE SWARTZENTRUBER Ohio Wesleyan University, Delaware, Ohio Pavlovian conditioning has traditionally been

More information

A response rule for positive and negative stimulus interaction in associative learning and performance

A response rule for positive and negative stimulus interaction in associative learning and performance Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (6, 1115-1124 A response rule for positive and negative stimulus interaction in associative learning and performance Oskar Pineño Hofstra University, Hempstead, New

More information

Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive relationships in the context in which the target stimulus occurs

Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive relationships in the context in which the target stimulus occurs The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology ISSN: 1747-0218 (Print) 1747-0226 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pqje20 Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive

More information

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (4), 299-304 doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.299 Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions REBECCA RAYBURN-REEVES

More information

The contribution of latent inhibition to reduced generalization after pre-exposure to the test stimulus

The contribution of latent inhibition to reduced generalization after pre-exposure to the test stimulus Behavioural Processes 71 (2006) 21 28 The contribution of latent inhibition to reduced generalization after pre-exposure to the test stimulus Maria del Carmen Sanjuan a,, Gumersinda Alonso b, James Byron

More information

Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online

Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online Chapter Title Attention and Pavlovian Conditioning Copyright Year 2011 Copyright Holder Springer Science+Business Media, LLC Corresponding Author

More information

Context specificity of sensory preconditioning: Implications for processes of within-event learning

Context specificity of sensory preconditioning: Implications for processes of within-event learning Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (2), 225-232 Context specificity of sensory preconditioning: Implications for processes of within-event learning JASPER WARD-ROBINSON, MICHELLE SYMONDS, and GEOFFREY

More information

Overshadowing by fixed and variable duration stimuli. Centre for Computational and Animal Learning Research

Overshadowing by fixed and variable duration stimuli. Centre for Computational and Animal Learning Research Overshadowing by fixed and variable duration stimuli Charlotte Bonardi 1, Esther Mondragón 2, Ben Brilot 3 & Dómhnall J. Jennings 3 1 School of Psychology, University of Nottingham 2 Centre for Computational

More information

Overshadowing not potentiation of illness-based contextual conditioning by a novel taste

Overshadowing not potentiation of illness-based contextual conditioning by a novel taste Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (4), 379-390 Overshadowing not potentiation of illness-based contextual conditioning by a novel taste MICHELLE SYMONDS and GEOFFREY HALL University of York, York, England

More information

DISCRIMINATION OF SERIAL AUDITORY PATTERNS IN RATS - roles of configural and elemental associations

DISCRIMINATION OF SERIAL AUDITORY PATTERNS IN RATS - roles of configural and elemental associations DISCRIMINATION OF SERIAL AUDITORY PATTERNS IN RATS - roles of configural and elemental associations Tiina Nurmi Master s thesis Department of Psychology University of Jyväskylä January 2011 CONTENTS INTRODUCTION

More information

Systematic manipulation of CS-US pairings in negative CS-US correlation procedures in rats

Systematic manipulation of CS-US pairings in negative CS-US correlation procedures in rats Animal Learning & Behavior 1980, 8 (1), 67-74 Systematic manipulation of CS-US pairings in negative CS-US correlation procedures in rats ELIZABETH S. WITCHER and JOHN J. B. AYRES University ofmassachusetts,

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

Temporal-Difference Prediction Errors and Pavlovian Fear Conditioning: Role of NMDA and Opioid Receptors

Temporal-Difference Prediction Errors and Pavlovian Fear Conditioning: Role of NMDA and Opioid Receptors Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 121, No. 5, 1043 1052 0735-7044/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.5.1043 Temporal-Difference Prediction Errors

More information

Blocking by fixed and variable stimuli: Effects of stimulus distribution on blocking.

Blocking by fixed and variable stimuli: Effects of stimulus distribution on blocking. Stimulus distribution effects on blocking 1 Blocking by fixed and variable stimuli: Effects of stimulus distribution on blocking. Dómhnall J. Jennings 1 & Charlotte Bonardi 2 1 Institute of Neuroscience,

More information

Recovery from blocking achieved by extinguishing the blocking CS

Recovery from blocking achieved by extinguishing the blocking CS Animal Learning & Behavior 1999,27 (1),63-76 Recovery from blocking achieved by extinguishing the blocking CS AARON P. BLAISDELL State University ojnew York, Binghamton, New York USAM. GUNTHER Greensboro

More information

Evidence for recovery of fear following immediate extinction in rats and humans

Evidence for recovery of fear following immediate extinction in rats and humans Research Evidence for recovery of fear following immediate extinction in rats and humans Daniela Schiller, 1,2,3 Christopher K. Cain, 2,3 Nina G. Curley, 1 Jennifer S. Schwartz, 1 Sarah A. Stern, 2 Joseph

More information

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus Behavioral Neuroscience 1995. Vol. 109, No. 2, 366-370 Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/95/$3.00 BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With

More information

RETENTION OF SPECIFICITY OF MEMORY FOR CONTEXT USING REINSTATEMENT

RETENTION OF SPECIFICITY OF MEMORY FOR CONTEXT USING REINSTATEMENT RETENTION OF SPECIFICITY OF MEMORY FOR CONTEXT USING REINSTATEMENT A thesis submitted to Kent State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts by Christie Lee

More information

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1985, 439 235-241 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES LANNY FIELDS THE COLLEGE OF

More information

Learning and Motivation

Learning and Motivation Learning and Motivation 4 (29) 178 18 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Learning and Motivation journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Context blocking in rat autoshaping: Sign-tracking

More information

Excitation and Inhibition in Unblocking

Excitation and Inhibition in Unblocking Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1988, Vol. 14, No. 3, 261-279 Copyright 1988 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/88/S00.75 Excitation and Inhibition

More information

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 319-332 DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, KELSEY

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information