Learning and Motivation

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Learning and Motivation"

Transcription

1 Learning and Motivation 4 (29) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Learning and Motivation journal homepage: Context blocking in rat autoshaping: Sign-tracking versus goal-tracking Daniel S.J. Costa, Robert A. Boakes * School of Psychology (A18), University of Sydney, NSW 26, Australia article info abstract Article history: Received 2 August 28 Revised November 28 Available online 16 December 28 Keywords: Context blocking Autoshaping Sign-tracking Goal-tracking Trace conditioning Rats Prior experience of unsignaled food can interfere with subsequent acquisition by birds of autoshaped key-pecking at a signal light. This has been understood to indicate that unsignaled food results in context conditioning, which blocks subsequent learning about the keylight food relationship. In the present experiment with rats lever insertion as the conditioned stimulus (CS) preceded sucrose delivery as the unconditioned stimulus (US). Half the rats initially received unsignaled USs in the conditioning context, while the remainder did not. Both lever-presses (sign-tracking) and magazine-entries (goal-tracking) were recorded. Under immediate reinforcement conditions, prior unsignaled US interfered with signtracking, but had no effect on goal-tracking. In two further groups, a trace condition prevented development of sign-tracking. In this case, prior context conditioning interfered with goal-tracking. These results suggest that interference with sign-tracking may reflect response competition, while interference with goal-tracking under trace conditions may reflect failure to acquire a CS US association. Ó 28 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Contemporary analyses of classical conditioning propose that the pairing of a conditioned stimulus (CS) and an unconditioned stimulus (US) can result not only in the formation of associations between CS and US, but also between contextual cues and the US (Bouton, 27). Furthermore, the strength of the conditioned response appears to be inversely related to the strength of the context-us association (Balsam & Schwartz, 1981). One source of evidence is the US pre-exposure effect, in which prior to the arrangement of a classical contingency between a CS and a US, the US is first presented unsignaled in the conditioning context; such pre-exposure has been found to result in slower acquisition of respond- * Corresponding author. Fax: address: bobb@psych.usyd.edu.au (R. A. Boakes) /$ - see front matter Ó 28 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:.16/j.lmot

2 D.S.J. Costa, R.A. Boakes / Learning and Motivation 4 (29) ing to the CS (Baker, Mercier, Gabel, & Baker, 1981; Randich & LoLordo, 1979). In an early example of this effect involving autoshaping with pigeons, the delivery of unsignaled response-independent food prior to conditioning substantially retarded acquisition of pecking at the keylight, a phenomenon then labeled learned laziness (Engberg, Hansen, Welker, & Thomas, 1972). The contribution of context conditioning to this effect was indicated by Tomie s (1976) finding that extensive prior exposure to unsignaled food deliveries retarded the acquisition of autoshaped key-pecking only if the food was delivered in the same context as conditioning. Balsam and Schwartz (1981) obtained a similar result with ring doves and in addition found that both acquisition and maintenance of conditioned responding were inversely and monotonically related to the number of previous non-contingent food presentations. Pecking at the keylight, an example of sign-tracking, was the only response recorded by Engberg et al. (1972), Tomie (1976) and Balsam and Schwartz (1981). An alternative explanation to associative blocking for the effect of unsignaled USs on subsequent conditioning is that the context comes to control some form of conditioned behavior that competes with the development of sign-tracking when a localized CS is introduced. This response competition possibility was discussed by Balsam (198) who suggested that a context containing unsignaled USs comes to elicit general activity. In his experiments, this was measured in terms of movement across hinged panels comprising the floor of a conditioning chamber used for pigeons and doves. Although he did not report a US pre-exposure experiment using this measure, he did show that a keylight paired with food could come to elicit general activity under conditions that failed to produce key-pecking, namely, when the inter-trial interval was no longer than the keylight duration of 36 s (Balsam, 198; pp. 14 1). A more specific response than general activity that has been recorded in the course of autoshaping experiments is approach to the site of reinforcement following presentation of the CS, a phenomenon termed goal-tracking (Boakes, 1977). The occurrence of goal-tracking can indicate that animals have learned to associate a localized CS with the US, despite the absence of responding directed at the CS (sign-tracking). It is therefore possible that interference with sign-tracking produced by US preexposure results at least partly from response competition, as suggested by Balsam (198), but specifically from a change from sign- to goal-tracking in the way the CS US association is expressed. There have been at least two examples where goal-tracking measures have revealed learning about the predictive relationship between a localized CS and a US that did not result in sign-tracking. One example comes from experiments that have introduced a trace interval. Thus, in a pigeon autoshaping experiment in which unusually magazine-entries were recorded as well as keypecks, Brown, Hemmes, Cabeza de Vaca, and Pagano (1993) arranged that food followed the offset of a keylight by 6 s. The key-pecking that occurred with a zero trace failed to appear under these trace conditions. However, magazine-entries during the keylight CS developed as rapidly in the trace as in the zero trace condition. When a similar comparison was made in a rat autoshaping preparation of the kind used in the present experiment and with the same length of the trace period, s, Costa and Boakes (27) found, like Brown et al. (1993), that this trace effectively abolished sign-tracking in this case, lever-pressing but had no detectable effect on goal-tracking. These results suggest that at least under autoshaping conditions the introduction of trace intervals of this magnitude can have a greater effect on the nature of the conditioned response (CR) than on acquisition of the CS US association. It should be noted, however, that, when a sexual US was used in an autoshaping experiment using quail, introducing a trace interval reduced sign-tracking but did not produce goal-tracking (Burns & Domjan, 1996; Experiment 3). A second example in which goal-tracking and sign-tracking measures diverged comes from an experiment that gave rats repeated exposure to a light before it served as the CS in an autoshaping procedure. This pre-exposure retarded sign-tracking relative to a control group for which the light was novel thus appearing to produce a latent inhibition effect but had no detectable effect on the development of goal-tracking (Boughner & Papini, 23). In the light of the above examples it seemed possible that the effects of prior exposure to the US on the autoshaped behavior of rats might differ according to whether goal-tracking or sign-tracking is considered. The present experiment was designed to test this possibility. The preliminary aim was to establish whether in rats prior context conditioning would attenuate sign-tracking under zero trace (immediate reinforcement) conditions, as previously obtained in birds (e.g. Balsam & Schwartz, 1981).

3 18 D.S.J. Costa, R.A. Boakes / Learning and Motivation 4 (29) The second aim was to determine whether prior context conditioning would attenuate magazine-entry as well as sign-tracking. We gave one group unsignaled food pellets in the conditioning context, i.e. context conditioning, prior to introducing an autoshaping procedure in which lever insertion signaled pellet delivery immediately after the lever retracted (Block-Immediate group) and compared this group to a group that had not been given food in the conditioning context prior to the autoshaping procedure (Control-Immediate). Confirmation of a US pre-exposure effect for sign-tracking would be shown by less lever pressing in the Block-Immediate than in the Control-Immediate group, while little difference in goal-tracking between the two groups would suggest that there was not an associative blocking effect. Following our previous finding that rats tend to goal-track rather than sign-track with a trace interval of s (Costa & Boakes, 27), the third aim of the experiment was to test whether prior context conditioning would affect acquisition of goal-tracking under trace conditions. Thus a further group (Block-Trace group) was given the same context conditioning as the Block-Immediate group prior to introduction of a trace autoshaping procedure in which pellet deliveries occurred s after the lever had been retracted. This group was compared to a control group (Control-Trace) that was not given context conditioning prior to the trace autoshaping. As neither group was predicted to display signtracking, our main interest was whether in this case context conditioning would block the development of goal-tracking. The lack of any indication of conditioned behavior in the Block-Trace group would be consistent with an associative blocking effect. As in any experiment involving such appetitive reinforcers, an initial phase was required in order to train rats to drink sucrose promptly from the dipper before lever-sucrose pairings were introduced. Given that such magazine training inevitably involves context conditioning, we followed Tomie (1976) by giving this training to all rats in an altered version of the conditioning context, achieved in the present case by manipulating visual, auditory, tactile and olfactory cues in the conditioning chambers with the aim of reducing to a minimum generalization from the altered to the normal context. Methods Subjects The experiment was run in two replications. At the start of the first, 14 female hooded Wistar rats were 212 days old, with a mean weight of 221 g, range g, and at the start of the second replication, 14 female hooded Wistar rats were 137 days old, with a mean weight of 213 g, range g. All animals had previously participated in an experiment in which they were exposed to different flavors and allowed to run in an activity wheel; they received no instrumental or classical training that resembled the present procedure. The rats were housed in the colony room in large acrylic tubs measuring cm, in groups of eight, although only seven from each tub were used in the experiment. Eight rats from the first replication were allocated to the two Trace conditions (each n = 4), while six were allocated to the Imm conditions (each n = 3). These numbers were reversed for the second replication, such that, after both replications, all groups comprised seven rats. They were maintained on a food deprivation schedule providing 2-h access to food each day following an experimental session. Apparatus Seven conditioning chambers (skinner boxes), measuring cm, were contained within sound and light attenuating wooden cabinets. These were each fitted with a ceiling-mounted 4 W houselight and a fan providing some masking noise, and both were switched on throughout every experimental session, except as noted below for the altered context. In each chamber the two end walls and ceiling were aluminum, the side walls were clear Plexiglas and the floor was composed of 16 stainless steel rods. A dipper could be used for deliveries of. ml of 2% sucrose in tap water into a magazine aperture located in the center of one end wall. The same wall contained two 48-mm

4 D.S.J. Costa, R.A. Boakes / Learning and Motivation 4 (29) wide retractable levers (MED Instruments Inc.) on either side of the magazine aperture, but only the right-hand lever was used in this experiment. This lever was mounted such that, when extended, the lever projected 19 mm into the chamber, with its top surface 6 mm above the floor of the chamber. The distance from the left-hand edge of the lever to the center of the magazine aperture was 6 mm. An infrared sensor was fitted across the magazine aperture to allow recording of magazine-entries. For initial habituation and magazine training an altered context was created by lining the floor of each operant chamber with chicken mesh and placing an open jar of Vicks Vaporub above the chamber ceiling. s involving this altered context were run with houselight and fan switched off. Procedure All animals first received a -min habituation session in the altered context with a Petri dish containing ml of 2% sucrose solution. Three sessions of magazine training followed, each consisting of 2 sucrose deliveries on a VT 3-s schedule, also in the altered context. In the context conditioning phase, the conditioning chambers were returned to their normal state and remained unaltered for the rest of the experiment. With the levers retracted, rats in the Block-Imm and Block-Trace groups received 2 noncontingent deliveries of sucrose with a dipper time of -s on a variable 6-s (VT 6-s) schedule in each of two sessions. During these sessions, rats in the Ctrl-Imm and Ctrl-Trace groups were not given any sucrose but spent the same amount of time in the conditioning chambers as the other rats. In the subsequent classical conditioning phase, each session contained 2 trials, each trial commencing with -s insertion of the right-hand lever and concluding with response-independent delivery of sucrose (-s dipper time). A variable inter-trial interval of 6 s (VT 6-s) was used, where the inter-trial interval was defined as the period commencing with dipper-retraction in one trial and ending with lever insertion in the next trial. For subjects in groups Ctrl-Imm and Block-Imm, sucrose delivery followed immediately after lever-retraction, while for subjects in groups Ctrl-Trace and Block-Trace sucrose was delivered s after lever-retraction. Data analysis Since, as previously (e.g. Costa & Boakes, 27), lever-pressing rates followed a skewed distribution, the non-parametric Mann Whitney U-test was used to analyze group differences on this measure. On the other hand, since magazine-entry data were approximately normally distributed, parametric analyses were applied to the number of magazine-entries made during the s prior to lever insertion (Pre-CS), during the s of lever insertion (CS) and to CS pre-cs difference scores for each trial, calculated by subtracting pre-cs magazine-entries from CS entries and for trace conditions from entries during the -s trace interval. A 2 () mixed ANOVA applied to these data compared Blocked with Control condition as the between-group factor (separately for each trace condition) and trend over sessions as the within-subject factor. Results Sign-tracking As seen in Fig. 1, only group Ctrl-Imm acquired lever-pressing. By response rates in this group were reliably greater than those in the Block-Imm group, U(7, 7) = 9, p =.4, thus confirming that the context conditioning procedure had reduced sign-tracking. In the trace condition, only 2 rats out of 14 showed any tendency to press the lever (none of the other rats exceeded an average of 1. responses/min), and, as suggested by the right-hand panel of Fig. 1, there was no detectable difference in this response in between the Block-Trace and Ctrl-Trace groups, U(7, 7) = 7, p >.. Goal-tracking during lever insertion Magazine-entries during the s prior to lever insertion (pre-cs) and during lever insertion (CS) are shown in Fig. 2. A mixed ANOVA applied to these data from the Immediate groups (left-hand

5 182 D.S.J. Costa, R.A. Boakes / Learning and Motivation 4 (29) Block-Imm Block-Trace Ctrl-Imm Ctrl-Trace Median lever-presses/min Fig. 1. Left-hand panel: median lever-press rates for the Block-Imm and Ctrl-Imm groups during zero trace conditioning whereby sucrose was delivered immediately following -s lever insertion. The groups differed in that only the Block-Imm group had received prior context conditioning. Right-hand panel: Median lever-press rates for the Block-Trace and Ctrl-Trace groups during trace conditioning whereby sucrose was delivered s after the lever was retracted. These groups also differed in that only the Block-Trace group had received prior context conditioning. panel) revealed that the difference between pre-cs and CS scores approached significance, F(1,12) = 3.79, p =.8, and, importantly, this difference interacted with, F(1,12) = 4.96, p =.2, The main effect of Group (Block vs Control) approached significance, F(1,12) = 4.3, p =.7, but neither the main effect of nor any interaction came close to significance, largest F = 1.8. A subsequent analysis of CS pre-cs difference scores for magazine-entries by the two Imme- Mean number of magazineentries per trial 1 Block-Imm CS Ctrl-Imm CS Block-Imm Pre-CS Ctrl-Imm Pre-CS 1 Block-Trace CS Ctrl-Trace CS Block-Trace Pre-CS Ctrl-Trace Pre-CS Fig. 2. Left-hand panel: mean (±SEM) number of magazine-entries in the -s pre-cs period (unfilled markers) and during the -s CS period when the lever was present (filled markers) for the Block-Imm and Ctrl-Imm groups. Right-hand panel: Mean (±SEM) number of magazine-entries in the -s pre-cs period (unfilled markers) and during the -s CS period (filled markers) for the Block-Trace and Ctrl-Trace groups.

6 D.S.J. Costa, R.A. Boakes / Learning and Motivation 4 (29) Block-Trace Trace Ctrl-Trace Trace Block-Trace Pre-CS Ctrl-Trace Pre-CS Mean number of magazineentries per trial Fig. 3. Mean (±SEM) number of magazine-entries during the -s trace interval and in the -s pre-cs period for the Block- Trace and Ctrl-Trace groups. Note that the pre-cs data shown here are identical to those shown in the right-hand panel of Fig. 2. diate groups revealed a linear trend, F(1, 12) = 6.77, p =.23, but, as suggested by the left-hand panel of Fig. 2, no main effect of Group, F < 1, and no interaction between this factor and trend, F(1, 12) = 1.2, p >.. Thus, although at the start of training the Block-Immediate group tended to make more magazine-entries than the Control-Immediate group both before and during lever insertion, goal-tracking, as measured by CS pre-cs scores, was acquired by both groups at a comparable rate. As for the two Trace groups, analysis of the CS and pre-cs magazine-entries shown in the righthand panel of Fig. 2 revealed a significant linear trend over sessions, F(1, 12) = 6.76, p =.2, but no other main effects or interactions, largest F = Although inspection of these data suggested that the Ctrl-Trace, but not the Block-Trace, group developed some goal-tracking during the CS, analysis of CS pre-cs difference scores in the two trace groups did not reveal any main effect or interaction, largest F(1,12) = Goal-tracking during trace intervals The mean number of magazine-entries during the trace interval and during the s prior to lever insertion are illustrated in Fig. 3. The trace pre-cs difference score was subjected to a mixed ANOVA (Group ). These data could, of course, be collected only from the two trace groups, where the important result was a main effect of Group, F(1, 12) =.16, p =.4. Other than the suggestion of a quadratic trend, F(1,12) = 6.22; Bonferroni F crit = 6., there were no trends or interaction between Group and trend, largest F(1, 12) = Rats in Ctrl-Trace made more magazine-entries during the trace interval than in the -s preceding the CS, F(1,6) = 13.22, p =.1, while the number of magazine-entries made by rats in Block-Trace did not differ from pre-cs to trace, F < 1. Thus, there was no evidence even on this measure that the latter group had learned to associate the lever with sucrose delivery. Discussion The main result in terms of lever-pressing was to confirm for the rat autoshaping procedure the context blocking effect previously reported for pigeons (Tomie, 1976) and ring doves

7 184 D.S.J. Costa, R.A. Boakes / Learning and Motivation 4 (29) (Balsam & Schwartz, 1981): previous unsignaled deliveries of sucrose in the conditioning context prevented the subsequent development of sign-tracking that was otherwise produced by pairing lever insertion with immediate sucrose delivery. Such blocking of sign-tracking was not found under - s trace conditions for the simple reason that, as previously found (Costa & Boakes, 27), sign-tracking did not develop under these conditions, even in the absence of prior context conditioning. The main new results from this experiment arose from recording magazine-entries in order to measure goal-tracking. For the groups given the immediate reinforcement condition, goal-tracking during lever insertion developed in a comparable way whether or not the rats had been given the context conditioning treatment, as seen in the left-hand panel of Fig. 2. Thus, although the sign-tracking results are consistent with those from previous context blocking studies, these goal-tracking results suggest that the acquisition of CS US associations under immediate reinforcement conditions was not detectably affected by prior US-only sessions. The marginal finding that early in conditioning Block rats given Immediate training (see Fig. 2, left-hand panel) showed a greater tendency than their Controls to enter the magazine both before and during lever insertion supports the proposal that context conditioning interferes with subsequent sign-tracking because it establishes a competing response rather than because of associative blocking (Balsam, 198). As noted in the Introduction, a similar response competition account has been proposed for a CS pre-exposure effect in an autoshaping experiment (Boughner & Papini, 23). More generally these findings provide further evidence that recording more than one kind of response can be important in classical conditioning experiments (cf. Holland, 1977, 198). A quite different pattern of results was seen in the groups given the trace condition. The control group not given prior context conditioning, Group Ctrl-Trace, showed some goal-tracking during the CS (Fig. 2) and even more during the trace interval (Fig. 3). On the other hand there was no indication of goal-tracking by Group Block-Trace during either the CS or the trace interval. The complete absence of both sign- and goal-tracking in this group suggests that these rats had failed to acquire a lever-sucrose association and thus that the difference between Group Block-Trace and Group Ctrl- Trace was the result of associative blocking by prior context conditioning. Although not the direct concern of this experiment, an obvious question posed by the lever-press data from this and our previous experiments (Costa & Boakes, 27) and by key-pecking data from a comparable pigeon experiment (Brown et al., 1993), is why the introduction of a trace interval can abolish sign-tracking. A possible answer comes from making the distinction between sign-tracking as approach to a stimulus that has acquired positive hedonic value and goal-tracking as an anticipatory response (cf. Konorski, 1967; Wagner & Brandon, 21) and by assuming that sign-tracking will occur only if hedonic conditioning of the signal CS is strong enough to compete successfully with goal-tracking. Recent evidence supporting this assumption comes from an analysis of individual differences in rat autoshaping: This revealed that for sign-trackers rats that pressed at a high rate a lever whose insertion signaled food lever insertion subsequently served as a more effective conditioned reinforcer of a novel instrumental response (nose-poking) than for goal-trackers, rats that were trained under identical conditions but that displayed a high rate of magazine-entry in the presence of the lever (Robinson & Flagel, in press). This approach suggests that immediate reinforcement makes the lever in the present case or the lit key in pigeon autoshaping sufficiently attractive or gives it sufficient incentive salience (Berridge, 21; Robinson & Flagel, in press) via strong hedonic conditioning to produce sign-tracking but that with a trace interval hedonic conditioning is weaker (cf. Mazur, 1997) so that goal-tracking becomes the dominant response to the CS. In addition to the above distinction between sign-tracking as a result of hedonic conditioning and goal-tracking as an anticipatory conditioned response, it should be noted that there are two fundamental asymmetries between these two types of response. First, a goal-tracking response (magazine-entry, in this case) can occur at any time during a conditioning session, while sign-tracking can only occur in the presence of the CS (the lever in the present case). Second, magazine-entry is necessary to obtain the US, while lever-pressing is not; thus, instrumental conditioning may play a role in the acquisition and maintenance of goal-tracking (Farwell & Ayres, 1979; Holland, 1977). Whether or not the above account is correct, the main conclusion to be drawn from this experiment is that prior context conditioning can affect the outcome of a subsequent autoshaping procedure in two ways. It can shift behavior from sign-tracking towards goal-tracking a performance effect, as

8 D.S.J. Costa, R.A. Boakes / Learning and Motivation 4 (29) seen here under immediate conditions and it can interfere with learning about the CS US relationship associative blocking, as appears to have taken place here under trace conditions. Acknowledgments This research was partly supported by an Australian Postgraduate Award to DSJC and by an Australian Research Council grant to RAB. We thank Geoff Hall and Justin Harris for their helpful comments on an earlier draft of this paper. References Baker, A. G., Mercier, P., Gabel, J., & Baker, P. A. (1981). Contextual conditioning and the US preexposure effect in conditioned fear. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 7, Balsam, P. D. (198). The functions of the context in learning and performance. In P. D. Balsam & A. Tomie (Eds.), Context and learning (pp. 1 21). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Balsam, P. D., & Schwartz, A. L. (1981). Rapid contextual conditioning in autoshaping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 7, Berridge, K. C. (21). Reward learning: Reinforcement, incentives and expectations. In D. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp ). New York: Academic Press. Boakes, R. A. (1977). Performance on learning to associate a stimulus with positive reinforcement. In H. Davis & H. M. B. Hurwitz (Eds.), Operant pavlovian interactions (pp. 67 1). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Boughner, R. L., & Papini, M. R. (23). Appetitive latent inhibition in rats: Now you see it (sign tracking), now you don t (goal tracking). Learning & Behavior, 31, Bouton, M. E. (27). Learning and behavior: A contemporary synthesis. Sunderland, Mass: Sinauer Associates. Brown, B. L., Hemmes, N. S., Cabeza de Vaca, S., & Pagano, C. (1993). Sign and goal tracking during delay and trace autoshaping in pigeons. Animal Learning & Behavior, 21, Burns, M., & Domjan, M. (1996). Sign tracking versus goal tracking in the sexual conditioning of male Japanese quail (coturnix japonica). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 22, Costa, D. S. J., & Boakes, R. A. (27). Maintenance of responding when reinforcement becomes delayed. Learning & Behavior, 3, 9. Engberg, L. A., Hansen, G., Welker, R. L., & Thomas, D. R. (1972). Acquisition of key-pecking via autoshaping as a function of prior experience: Learned laziness? Science, 178, 2 4. Farwell, B. J., & Ayres, J. J. B. (1979). Stimulus reinforcer and response reinforcer relations in the control of conditioned appetitive head poking ( goal tracking ) in rats. Learning and Motivation,, Holland, P. C. (1977). Conditioned stimulus as a determinant of the form of the Pavlovian conditioned response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 3, Holland, P. C. (198). The influence of visual stimulus characteristics on the form of Pavlovian appetitive conditioned responding in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 6, Konorski, J. (1967). Integrative activity of the brain. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Mazur, J. E. (1997). Choice, delay, probability and conditioned reinforcement. Animal Learning & Behavior, 2, Randich, A., & LoLordo, V. M. (1979). Associative and non-associative theories of the US preexposure phenomenon: Implications for Pavlovian conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 86, Robinson, T. E., Flagel, S. B. (in press). Dissociating the predictive and incentive motivational properties of reward-related cues through the study of individual differences. Biological Psychiatry. Tomie, A. (1976). Interference with autoshaping by prior context conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2(4), Wagner, A. R., & Brandon, S. E. (21). A componential theory of Pavlovian conditioning. In R. R. Mowrer & S. B. Klein (Eds.), Handbook of contemporary learning theories (pp ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 23, 343-367 (1992) Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training CHARLOTTE BONARDI University of York, York, United Kingdom In four experiments rats received

More information

Magazine approach during a signal for food depends on Pavlovian, not instrumental, conditioning.

Magazine approach during a signal for food depends on Pavlovian, not instrumental, conditioning. In Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/xan/index.aspx 2013, vol. 39 (2), pp 107 116 2013 American Psychological Association DOI: 10.1037/a0031315

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2003, 80, 43 58 NUMBER 1(JULY) ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT WASHINGTON

More information

Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning

Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (3), 230-245 doi:10.3758/lb.37.3.230 Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning JUSTIN A. HARRIS, SABA A GHARA EI, AND CLINTON A. MOORE

More information

Contextual Conditioning with Lithium-Induced Nausea as the US: Evidence from a Blocking Procedure

Contextual Conditioning with Lithium-Induced Nausea as the US: Evidence from a Blocking Procedure LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 28, 200 215 (1997) ARTICLE NO. LM960958 Contextual Conditioning with Lithium-Induced Nausea as the US: Evidence from a Blocking Procedure MICHELLE SYMONDS AND GEOFFREY HALL University

More information

Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response 1

Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response 1 Japanese Psychological Research 1998, Volume 40, No. 2, 104 110 Short Report Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response

More information

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1976, Vol. 4 (3), 293-298 The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock THOMAS s. HYDE Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland,

More information

Occasional reinforced trials during extinction can slow the rate of rapid reacquisition

Occasional reinforced trials during extinction can slow the rate of rapid reacquisition Learning and Motivation 35 (2004) 371 390 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Occasional reinforced trials during extinction can slow the rate of rapid reacquisition Mark E. Bouton, Amanda M. Woods, and Oskar

More information

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (2), 213-219 Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning THOMAS HIGGINS and ROBERT A. RESCORLA University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

More information

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2016 American Psychological Association 2016, Vol. 42, No. 4, 401 414 2329-8456/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000112 Contextual

More information

Some determinants of second-order conditioning

Some determinants of second-order conditioning Learn Behav (2011) 39:12 26 DOI 10.1007/s13420-010-0002-6 Some determinants of second-order conditioning James E. Witnauer & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010

More information

Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association

Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association Learning and Motivation 32, 349 366 (2001) doi:10.1006/lmot.2001.1089, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association Charlotte Bonardi

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(2),199-203 Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning PETER C. HOLLAND and DESWELL T. FORBES University ofpittsburgh, Pittsburgh,

More information

The Form of a Conditioned Stimulus Can Influence the Degree to Which It Acquires Incentive Motivational Properties

The Form of a Conditioned Stimulus Can Influence the Degree to Which It Acquires Incentive Motivational Properties The Form of a Conditioned Stimulus Can Influence the Degree to Which It Acquires Incentive Motivational Properties Paul J. Meyer*, Elizabeth S. Cogan, Terry E. Robinson Department of Psychology, University

More information

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1) JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 23, 79, 233 242 NUMBER 2(MARCH) PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY Rats

More information

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT The Psychological Record, 2007, 57, 577 592 PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, AMBER HULS, and ASHLEY KULLAND University

More information

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Eric A. Thrailkill 1, Alex Kacelnik 2, Fay Porritt 3 & Mark E. Bouton

More information

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 05, 3, 3 45 NUMBER (JANUARY) WITHIN-SUBJECT TESTING OF THE SIGNALED-REINFORCEMENT EFFECT ON OPERANT RESPONDING AS MEASURED BY RESPONSE RATE AND RESISTANCE

More information

Stimulus and Temporal Cues in Classical Conditioning

Stimulus and Temporal Cues in Classical Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 2 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 97-743//$5. DOI: 1.137//97-743.26.2.26 2, Vol. 26, No. 2, 26-219 Stimulus and Temporal

More information

Contextual Effects in Conditioning, Latent Inhibition, and Habituation: Associative and Retrieval Functions of Contextual Cues

Contextual Effects in Conditioning, Latent Inhibition, and Habituation: Associative and Retrieval Functions of Contextual Cues Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1989, Vol. 15, No. 3, 232-241 Copyright 1989 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-740389$00.75 Contextual Effects in Conditioning,

More information

Predictive Accuracy and the Effects of Partial Reinforcement on Serial Autoshaping

Predictive Accuracy and the Effects of Partial Reinforcement on Serial Autoshaping Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1985 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 0097-7403/85/$00.75 1985, VOl. 11, No. 4, 548-564 Predictive Accuracy and the

More information

Some effects of short-term immediate prior exposure to light change on responding for light change*

Some effects of short-term immediate prior exposure to light change on responding for light change* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2 (4), 262-266 Some effects of short-term immediate prior exposure to light change on responding for light change* ALAN RUSSELLt and PETER H. GLOW University ofadelaide,

More information

Overshadowing by fixed and variable duration stimuli. Centre for Computational and Animal Learning Research

Overshadowing by fixed and variable duration stimuli. Centre for Computational and Animal Learning Research Overshadowing by fixed and variable duration stimuli Charlotte Bonardi 1, Esther Mondragón 2, Ben Brilot 3 & Dómhnall J. Jennings 3 1 School of Psychology, University of Nottingham 2 Centre for Computational

More information

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 322 328 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.3.322

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 90 (2012) 311 322 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journa l h omepa g e: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc US specificity of occasion setting:

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

A learned flavor preference persists despite the extinction of conditioned hedonic reactions to the cue flavors

A learned flavor preference persists despite the extinction of conditioned hedonic reactions to the cue flavors Learning & Behavior 29, 37 (4), 35-31 doi:1.3758/lb.37.4.35 A learned flavor preference persists despite the extinction of conditioned hedonic reactions to the cue flavors DOMINIC M. DWYER Cardiff University,

More information

GRAHAM C. L. DAVEY, GARY G. CLELAND, and DAVID A. OAKLEY The City University, London, England

GRAHAM C. L. DAVEY, GARY G. CLELAND, and DAVID A. OAKLEY The City University, London, England Animal Learning & Behavior 1982, 10 (2), 257-262 Applying Konorski's model of classical conditioning to signal-centered behavior in the rat: Some functional similarities between hunger CRs and sign-tracking

More information

Context specificity of sensory preconditioning: Implications for processes of within-event learning

Context specificity of sensory preconditioning: Implications for processes of within-event learning Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (2), 225-232 Context specificity of sensory preconditioning: Implications for processes of within-event learning JASPER WARD-ROBINSON, MICHELLE SYMONDS, and GEOFFREY

More information

Transfer after Serial Feature Positive Discrimination Training

Transfer after Serial Feature Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 17, 243-268 (1986) Transfer after Serial Feature Positive Discrimination Training PETER C. HOLLAND University of Pittsburgh Three experiments with rat subjects examined the transfer

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 319-332 DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, KELSEY

More information

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes Journal Learning & Behavior 2005,?? 33 (?), (4),???-??? 464-478 Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes PETER C. HOLLAND

More information

500 KAWAI AND NAKAJIMA

500 KAWAI AND NAKAJIMA 500 KAWAI AND NAKAJIMA also Capaldi, 1991; Capaldi, Owens, & Palmer, 1994; Deems, Oetting, Sherman, & Garcia, 1986; Mehiel, 1991). This finding is consistent with the results reported in other Pavlovian

More information

Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive relationships in the context in which the target stimulus occurs

Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive relationships in the context in which the target stimulus occurs The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology ISSN: 1747-0218 (Print) 1747-0226 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pqje20 Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Flavor avoidance learning based on missing calories but not on palatability reduction

Flavor avoidance learning based on missing calories but not on palatability reduction Learn Behav (212) 4:542 55 DOI.3758/s1342-12-74-6 Flavor avoidance learning based on missing calories but not on palatability reduction Robert A. Boakes & Angela E. Patterson & Dorothy W. S. Kwok Published

More information

Renewal after the extinction of free operant behavior

Renewal after the extinction of free operant behavior Learn Behav (211) 39:57 67 DOI 1.3758/s1342-11-18-6 Renewal after the extinction of free operant behavior Mark E. Bouton & Travis P. Todd & Drina Vurbic & Neil E. Winterbauer Published online: 29 January

More information

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1991, Vol. 17, No. 3, 231-248 Copyright 1991 by the Am n Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/91/53.00 Transfer of Control in Ambiguous

More information

Overshadowing by fixed- and variable-duration stimuli

Overshadowing by fixed- and variable-duration stimuli THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2015 Vol. 68, No. 3, 523 542, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.960875 Overshadowing by fixed- and variable-duration stimuli Charlotte Bonardi 1,

More information

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (4), 299-304 doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.299 Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions REBECCA RAYBURN-REEVES

More information

Asymmetry in the discrimination of quantity by rats: The role of the intertrial interval

Asymmetry in the discrimination of quantity by rats: The role of the intertrial interval DOI.378/s1342-1-191- Asymmetry in the discrimination of quantity by rats: The role of the intertrial interval R. A. Inman 1 & R. C. Honey 1 & G. L. Eccles 1 & J. M. Pearce 1 # The Author(s) 21. This article

More information

Relations Between Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer and Reinforcer Devaluation

Relations Between Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer and Reinforcer Devaluation Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2004, Vol. 30, No. 2, 104 117 Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.30.2.104

More information

Contrast and the justification of effort

Contrast and the justification of effort Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (2), 335-339 Contrast and the justification of effort EMILY D. KLEIN, RAMESH S. BHATT, and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky When humans

More information

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION. William Travis Suits

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION. William Travis Suits TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION Except where reference is made to the works of others, the work described in this dissertation is my own or was done in collaboration

More information

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus.

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR EFFECTS OF DISCRETE-TRIAL AND FREE-OPERANT PROCEDURES ON THE ACQUISITION AND MAINTENANCE OF SUCCESSIVE DISCRIMINATION IN RATS SHIN HACHIYA AND MASATO ITO

More information

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues Animal Learning & Behavior 1995, 23 (3), 273-279 Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues LOU M. SHERBURNE and

More information

Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative mechanisms

Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative mechanisms Q0667 QJEP(B) si-b03/read as keyed THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 56B (1), 43 55 Learned changes in the sensitivity of stimulus representations: Associative and nonassociative

More information

Differential dependence of Pavlovian incentive motivation and instrumental incentive learning processes on dopamine signaling

Differential dependence of Pavlovian incentive motivation and instrumental incentive learning processes on dopamine signaling Research Differential dependence of Pavlovian incentive motivation and instrumental incentive learning processes on dopamine signaling Kate M. Wassum, 1,2,4 Sean B. Ostlund, 1,2 Bernard W. Balleine, 3

More information

Extinction of the Context and Latent Inhibition

Extinction of the Context and Latent Inhibition LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 13, 391-416 (1982) Extinction of the Context and Latent Inhibition A. G. BAKER AND PIERRE MERCIER McGill University The hypothesis that latent inhibition could be reduced by extinguishing

More information

TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES SN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION

TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES SN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES SN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION Tnesis for the Degree of M. A. MiCHiGAN STATE UNWERSITY RODNEY CHARLES HOWARD 1977 f L13 1-;- R Y Michiga: State gr? Umw 1y ABSTRACT TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES

More information

Publication Info: International Journal of Comparative Psychology, UCLA Department of Psychology, UCLA

Publication Info: International Journal of Comparative Psychology, UCLA Department of Psychology, UCLA Peer Reviewed Title: Trends in the Study of Pavlovian Conditioning Journal Issue: International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 17(2) Author: Timberlake, William, Indiana University, U.S.A. Publication

More information

Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events.

Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events. Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events. behaviorism - the view that psychology (1) should be an objective science

More information

Lesions of the Basolateral Amygdala Disrupt Selective Aspects of Reinforcer Representation in Rats

Lesions of the Basolateral Amygdala Disrupt Selective Aspects of Reinforcer Representation in Rats The Journal of Neuroscience, November 15, 2001, 21(22):9018 9026 Lesions of the Basolateral Amygdala Disrupt Selective Aspects of Reinforcer Representation in Rats Pam Blundell, 1 Geoffrey Hall, 1 and

More information

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR LICK-SHOCK CONTINGENCIES IN THE RATT1 VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY 1972, 17, 119-125 NUMBER I (JANUARY) Hungry rats were allowed to lick an 8%

More information

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli in rats RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson, H Djafari Marbini, AJ Toner, TW Robbins, BJ Everitt Departments of

More information

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules Animal Learning& Behavior 1981,9 (3),406-410 Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules JOHN H. HULL, TIMOTHY J. BARTLETT, and ROBERT

More information

Contextual Control of Conditioned Responding in Rats With Dorsal Hippocampal Lesions

Contextual Control of Conditioned Responding in Rats With Dorsal Hippocampal Lesions Behavioral Neuroscience 1996, Vol. 110, No. 5, 933-945 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/96/S3.00 Contextual Control of Conditioned Responding in Rats With Dorsal

More information

Perceptual Learning in Flavor Aversion: Evidence for Learned Changes in Stimulus Effectiveness

Perceptual Learning in Flavor Aversion: Evidence for Learned Changes in Stimulus Effectiveness Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2003, Vol. 29, No. 1, 39 48 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.29.1.39

More information

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010 Notes Textbook used Week 1-3: Bouton, M.E. (2016). Learning and Behavior: A Contemporary Synthesis. 2nd Ed. Sinauer Week 4-6: Rieger, E. (Ed.) (2014) Abnormal Psychology:

More information

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 479-490 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The experiment reported herein was conducted to determine

More information

Taste quality and extinction of a conditioned taste aversion in rats

Taste quality and extinction of a conditioned taste aversion in rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of September 1999 Taste quality and extinction of

More information

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 24, 191-197 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) PERFORMANCE IN CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION' BRENDA LOBB AND M. C. DAVISON UNIVERSITY

More information

Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor

Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor Learn Behav (2012) 40:24 33 DOI 10.3758/s13420-011-0039-1 Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor Cody W. Polack & Mario A. Laborda & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 July 2011 # Psychonomic

More information

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997, 4 (1), 113-117 Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer JANICE E. WEAVER University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky JANICE N. STEIRN

More information

Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting

Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting Animal Learning & Behavior 1989, 17 (3), 269-279 Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting PETER C. HOLLAND Duke University, Durham, North Carolina In three experiments, transfer effects

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/319/5871/1849/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Rule Learning by Rats Robin A. Murphy,* Esther Mondragón, Victoria A. Murphy This PDF file includes: *To whom correspondence

More information

Context and Pavlovian conditioning

Context and Pavlovian conditioning Context Brazilian conditioning Journal of Medical and Biological Research (1996) 29: 149-173 ISSN 0100-879X 149 Context and Pavlovian conditioning Departamento de Psicologia, Pontifícia Universidade Católica

More information

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning Operant Conditioning The nature of reinforcement: Premack s probability differential theory Response deprivation

More information

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (4), 477-490 Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons DOUGLAS S. GRANT and DIANE C. TALARICO University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada Pigeons were trained

More information

Overshadowing not potentiation of illness-based contextual conditioning by a novel taste

Overshadowing not potentiation of illness-based contextual conditioning by a novel taste Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (4), 379-390 Overshadowing not potentiation of illness-based contextual conditioning by a novel taste MICHELLE SYMONDS and GEOFFREY HALL University of York, York, England

More information

The Influence of an Appetitive Conditioned Stimulus on Interval Timing Behavior

The Influence of an Appetitive Conditioned Stimulus on Interval Timing Behavior City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects Graduate Center 10-2014 The Influence of an Appetitive Conditioned Stimulus on Interval Timing Behavior

More information

Excitation and Inhibition in Unblocking

Excitation and Inhibition in Unblocking Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1988, Vol. 14, No. 3, 261-279 Copyright 1988 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/88/S00.75 Excitation and Inhibition

More information

Conditioned Stimulus Familiarity Determines Effects of MK-801 on Fear Extinction

Conditioned Stimulus Familiarity Determines Effects of MK-801 on Fear Extinction Behavioral Neuroscience 2009 American Psychological Association 2009, Vol. 123, No. 2, 303 314 0735-7044/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014988 Conditioned Stimulus Familiarity Determines Effects of MK-801 on

More information

The attribution of incentive salience to a stimulus that signals an intravenous injection of cocaine

The attribution of incentive salience to a stimulus that signals an intravenous injection of cocaine Behavioural Brain Research 169 (2006) 320 324 Research report The attribution of incentive salience to a stimulus that signals an intravenous injection of cocaine Jason M. Uslaner, Martin J. Acerbo, Samantha

More information

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition Scholarly Commons Psychology Faculty Publications 2015 Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition James F. Briggs Timothy A. Toth Brian P. Olson Jacob G.

More information

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli Georgia Southern University Digital Commons@Georgia Southern Electronic Theses & Dissertations Graduate Studies, Jack N. Averitt College of Summer 2005 Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli William

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

REWARD DEVALUATION IN A TWO-LINK CHAIN SCHEDULE: EFFECTS OF REWARD DENSITY IN THE PROXIMAL LINK AND FOOD RESTRICTION PROTOCOL IN RATS

REWARD DEVALUATION IN A TWO-LINK CHAIN SCHEDULE: EFFECTS OF REWARD DENSITY IN THE PROXIMAL LINK AND FOOD RESTRICTION PROTOCOL IN RATS REWARD DEVALUATION IN A TWO-LINK CHAIN SCHEDULE: EFFECTS OF REWARD DENSITY IN THE PROXIMAL LINK AND FOOD RESTRICTION PROTOCOL IN RATS by Tyson William Baker A thesis submitted to the Department of Psychology

More information

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives?

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives? East Tennessee State University Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University Undergraduate Honors Theses 5-2014 Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives? Tarra L. Baker Follow

More information

Two Kinds of Attention in Pavlovian Conditioning: Evidence for a Hybrid Model of Learning

Two Kinds of Attention in Pavlovian Conditioning: Evidence for a Hybrid Model of Learning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2010, Vol. 36, No. 4, 456 470 2010 American Psychological Association 0097-7403/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0018528 Two Kinds of Attention in Pavlovian

More information

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1971, 15, 243-247 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) POSITIVE CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION: EFFECTS OF CS DURATION' KLAUS A. MICZEK AND SEBASTIAN P. GROSSMAN UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO

More information

Backward associations: Differential learning about stimuli that follow the presence versus the absence of food in pigeons

Backward associations: Differential learning about stimuli that follow the presence versus the absence of food in pigeons Antmal Learntng & Behavtor 1989, 17 (3), 280-290 Backward associations: Differential learning about stimuli that follow the presence versus the absence of food in pigeons ELIOT HEARST Indiana University,

More information

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1998, 5 (3), 516-522 Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals LOU M. SHERBURNE Wabash College, Crawfordsville,

More information

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery L132 NT MJA/cla Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (1), 60-73 doi:10.3758/lb.37.1.60 Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery Gonzalo P. Urcelay University of Cambridge, Cambridge,

More information

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson *, TW Robbins, A Dickinson, BJ Everitt Departments of Experimental

More information

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information

What is Learned? Lecture 9

What is Learned? Lecture 9 What is Learned? Lecture 9 1 Classical and Instrumental Conditioning Compared Classical Reinforcement Not Contingent on Behavior Behavior Elicited by US Involuntary Response (Reflex) Few Conditionable

More information

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus Behavioral Neuroscience 1995. Vol. 109, No. 2, 366-370 Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/95/$3.00 BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With

More information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information Animal Learning & Behavior 1978,6 (4),380-384 Copyright 1978 by The Psychonornic Society, nc. Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information BRUCE A. WALD Utah State University, Logan,

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Learn Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 26.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Learn Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 26. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Learn Behav. 2009 November ; 37(4): 357 364. doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.357. Behavioral Momentum and Relapse of Extinguished Operant Responding

More information

The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine

The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-009 The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine Wesley Paul Thomas Utah

More information

MCMASTER UNIVERSITY. more values independently of food delivery. In any case, the well known result of Skinner's

MCMASTER UNIVERSITY. more values independently of food delivery. In any case, the well known result of Skinner's JOURNAt, OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1968, 1 1, 1-8 NUMBER I (JANUARY) A UTO-SHAPING OF THE PIGEON'S KEY-PECK' PAUL L. BROWN AND HERBERT M. JENKINS MCMASTER UNIVERSITY Reliable acquistion

More information

Provided for non-commercial research and educational use. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use.

Provided for non-commercial research and educational use. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use. Provided for non-commercial research and educational use. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use. This article was originally published in the Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference,

More information

A behavior systems view of responding to probe stimuli during an interfood clock

A behavior systems view of responding to probe stimuli during an interfood clock Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 313 325 A behavior systems view of responding to probe stimuli during an interfood clock KATHLEEN M. SILVA and WILLIAM TIMBERLAKE Indiana University, Bloomington,

More information