Fifth-Grade Children s Daily Experiences of Peer Victimization and Negative Emotions: Moderating Effects of Sex and Peer Rejection

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Fifth-Grade Children s Daily Experiences of Peer Victimization and Negative Emotions: Moderating Effects of Sex and Peer Rejection"

Transcription

1 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2014) 42: DOI /s Fifth-Grade Children s Daily Experiences of Peer Victimization and Negative Emotions: Moderating Effects of Sex and Peer Rejection Michael T. Morrow & Julie A. Hubbard & Lydia J. Barhight & Amanda K. Thomson Published online: 27 March 2014 # Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014 Abstract This study examined the relations of fifth-grade children s (181 boys and girls) daily experiences of peer victimization with their daily negative emotions. Children completed daily reports of peer victimization and negative emotions (sadness, anger, embarrassment, and nervousness) on up to eight school days. The daily peer victimization checklist was best represented by five factors: physical victimization, verbal victimization, social manipulation, property attacks, and social rebuff. All five types were associated with increased negative daily emotions, and several types were independently linked to increased daily negative emotions, particularly physical victimization. Girls demonstrated greater emotional reactivity in sadness to social manipulation than did boys, and higher levels of peer rejection were linked to greater emotional reactivity to multiple types of victimization. Sex and peer rejection also interacted, such that greater rejection was a stronger indicator of emotional reactivity to victimization in boys than in girls. Keywords Daily diary. Peer victimization. Negative emotion. Peer rejection Peer victimization is a common and painful experience in the daily lives of many children. Roughly 10 % of youth are regularly victimized by peers (Olweus 1993; Perry et al. M. T. Morrow (*): A. K. Thomson Department of Psychology, Arcadia University, 124 Boyer Hall, 450 S. Easton Road, Glenside, PA 19038, USA morrowm@arcadia.edu J. A. Hubbard Department of Psychology, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA L. J. Barhight Behavior Medicine and Clinical Psychology, Cincinnati Children s Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, OH, USA 1988), and these experiences appear increasingly stable by late elementary school (Perry et al. 2001). Moreover, decades of research reveal that peer victimization is linked to numerous adjustment problems (Card and Hodges 2008; Grills and Ollendick 2002; Hawker and Boulton 2000) and several psychiatric disorders (Kumpulainen 2008). However, few theorists have articulated the process through which peer victimization contributes to psychopathology. Previous research has also shed scant light on the nature and impact of children s day-to-day experiences of victimization. Background on Peer Victimization and Emotional Functioning Despite these limitations, Coie (1990) offers a helpful framework for conceptualizing a pathway between children s daily peer victimization and daily psychological functioning. In this model, peer rejection (the experience of being disliked and not liked by peers) promotes negative treatment by peers, which in turn, impairs daily psychological functioning. With repeated exposure, the daily effects of these experiences give rise to internalizing and externalizing patterns of psychopathology. Thus, recurrent experiences of peer victimization may contribute to serious and stable adjustment problems through their cumulative impact on daily functioning. With regard to daily emotional functioning, children are theorized to experience and engage in a variety of affective processes during social encounters (Lemerise and Arsenio 2000). While some processes occur automatically (e.g., increased arousal when physically threatened), others result from effortful processing (e.g., modulating the expression of fear to save face among peers). Negative social experiences are likely to activate basic emotions (e.g., sadness, anger, or fear; Izard 2007), as well as self-conscious emotions (e.g., embarrassment or shame; Lewis 1992). When peer

2 1090 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2014) 42: victimization recurs, repeated negative daily emotions may cumulate to contribute to stable maladaptive patterns of emotional functioning. Numerous studies have linked peer victimization to negative emotions (e.g., anxiety, sadness, shame, anger, and general negative emotionality). However, a majority of this research assessed children s emotional experiences retrospectively for the past few weeks or months or as stable, trait-like dimensions (e.g., Dill et al. 2004; Hanish et al. 2004; Hawker and Boulton 2000). Therefore, we are unable to draw conclusions from this work regarding the daily connections of peer victimization and emotions, which is important for understanding the day-to-day process through which peer victimization may give rise to emotional maladjustment. The Present Study In this study, we build on past research by adopting a dailyreport methodology to assess children s daily experiences of peer victimization and negative emotions. Compared to traditional self-report tools, daily reports offer a more fine-grained and ecologically valid assessment of daily events (Iida et al. 2012). They also reduce retrospective bias by minimizing the time between participants experiences and their reports of these events (Bolger et al. 2003). Thus, daily measurement is likely to offer a more comprehensive and reliable snapshot of children s experiences of victimization and emotions. We also expand upon past work by addressing three notable gaps. First, we examine multiple types of daily peer victimization and evaluate their distinguishability as unique experiences. Second, we explore the daily relations of multiple types of victimization with several negative emotions. In doing so, we hope to offer a clearer picture of the relations of specific types of peer victimization with certain negative emotions. Third, we evaluate two potential moderators of emotional reactivity to peer victimization in order to identify child characteristics linked to stronger emotional reactions to peer victimization. Multiple Types of Daily Peer Victimization A majority of the research reviewed above assessed peer victimization as a singular construct; however, theorists have argued the importance of studying multiple types of peer victimization (Sandstrom and Cillessen 2003). Several researchers have performed factor analyses to distinguish different subtypes of victimization (e.g., Crick et al. 1999; Crick and Grotpeter 1996; Mynard and Joseph 2000; Sandstrom and Cillessen 2003). Collectively, they have identified four subtypes: physical victimization, verbal victimization, social victimization, and property attacks. Regarding social victimization, multiple terms (e.g., indirect, relational, social) have been used to characterize peer aggression aimed to harm children s social functioning that occurs directly or indirectly (for a discussion, see Card et al. 2008). In this study, we examine two potential subtypes: social manipulation and social rebuff. Social manipulation is intended to damage children s peer relations or social status and occurs indirectly (Mynard and Joseph 2000); that is, it is orchestrated by peers who influence others to treat children in negative ways (e.g., peers make a child s friends turn against him). In contrast, social rebuff occurs directly between perpetrators and targets and captures experiences of being ignored, left out, and excluded (Dodge et al. 1982; Putallaz and Wasserman 1989). For the current study, we assessed the five types of peer victimization experiences outlined above (physical victimization, verbal victimization, property attacks, social manipulation, and social rebuff) at the daily level. To accomplish this, we constructed a new checklist to capture day-to-day variation in children s experiences of each type of peer victimization. In evaluating this measure, we hypothesized that the five types of victimization would co-occur on a daily basis, yet also emerge as distinct daily experiences. Daily Peer Victimization and Negative Emotions At least two studies have simultaneously assessed children s daily peer victimization and daily emotional functioning. Nishina and Juvonen (2005) collected daily reports of sixth-graders peer victimization and negative emotions over four or five school days. Across two samples, they found that children reported greater anxiety, anger, and humiliation on days they also reported peer victimization. Nishina (2012) assessed sixthand ninth-graders daily experiences of peer victimization, worry, and humiliation. Students reported greater humiliation and worry on days they experienced victimization by peers. Accordingly, children s daily negative emotions appear to fluctuate with their daily experiences of peer victimization; however, these studies do not reveal whether different types of victimization are more strongly associated with specific emotions. If certain types of daily peer victimization are more powerful predictors of children s daily negative emotions, these experiences may confer greater risk for long-term emotional difficulties. This information could guide prevention programs to target the most potent types of peer victimization. Accordingly, we evaluated the daily relations of the five victimization subtypes with four negative emotions (sadness, anger, embarrassment, and nervousness). We carefully assessed the separate relations of each type of victimization with every negative emotion, as well as their relative associations, by simultaneously testing the relations of all five subtypes with each emotion. As a result, we were able to assess whether certain daily victimization experiences are uniquely linked to day-to-day variation in particular negative emotions. We generally hypothesized that children would

3 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2014) 42: report greater negative emotion on days they encountered more instances of peer victimization. Moderators of Emotional Reactivity to Daily Peer Victimization When faced with peer stress, some children are more likely to experience negative emotions or feel them more intensely (Lemerise and Arsenio 2000). Studies have linked peer victimization to abnormal emotional functioning, including elevated physiological arousal, heightened anger intensity, and emotion dysregulation (Champion and Clay 2007;Hanish et al. 2004; Shields and Cicchetti 2001; Woods and White 2005). However, little is known about the specific factors that influence emotional reactivity to peer victimization. By exploring such factors, it may be possible to identify children who are more susceptible to the emotional impact of peer victimization and require greater support in coping with their victimization experiences. We evaluated whether two child characteristics, sex and peer rejection, moderate the daily relations of peer victimization with negative emotions. Specifically, we tested whether certain types of victimization are more strongly linked to daily reactivity in negative emotions for boys versus girls and across children at different levels of peer rejection. We also examined the interactive effect of these two characteristics to assess whether peer rejection is associated with differing levels of emotional reactivity to victimization across gender. These tests allowed us to examine whether gender, peer rejection, and their combination, may be helpful in identifying children at greatest risk for the emotional maladjustment linked to peer victimization. We investigated these variables because both have been found to moderate various relations between peer relations and psychosocial adjustment (Coie 1990; RoseandRudolph2006). Sex Rose and Rudolph (2006) theorize that boys and girls are socialized via same-sex interactions to react and respond to stressful peer encounters in distinct ways. Research indicates that girls value friendships, hold communal goals, and receive emotional benefits from relationships more than boys do (Benenson and Benarroch 1998; Bukowski et al. 1994; Chung and Asher 1996; Rose and Asher 2004). In contrast, boys value strength and dominance and hold more selfinterested goals (Chung and Asher 1996; Jarvinen and Nicholls 1996; Rose and Asher 2004). Accordingly, girls may be more concerned about the status of their relationships and peers evaluations of them (Rose and Rudolph 2006), whereas boys may be more concerned with their status in the larger peer group (Bakker et al. 2010). Therefore, peer victimization aimed to damage relationships (e.g., social manipulation) may be more distressing to girls, while victimization intended to challenge strength and dominance (e.g., physical victimization) may be more upsetting to boys. In line with these ideas, Galen and Underwood (1997) found that girls rated hypothetical vignettes of social victimization, which included experiences of social manipulation and social rebuff, as more hurtful than boys did. However, girls rated social and physical victimization as equally hurtful, whereas boys rated physical victimization as more hurtful than social victimization. Additionally, Paquette and Underwood (1999) observed that girls rated social victimization as more emotionally distressing than boys, and the relation between social victimization and self-concept was stronger for girls than for boys. However, Rudolph (2002) found that social and overt (physical and verbal) types of peer victimization both contribute to greater emotional difficulties in girls than in boys. Despite these mixed findings, we hypothesized that girls would display greater reactivity in their negative emotions to social manipulation, while boys would show more emotional reactivity to physical victimization. Peer Rejection Peer-rejected children are often excluded from peer interaction (Buhs et al. 2006), which may limit their opportunities to practice and develop critical social skills (Hartup 1983), such as modulating their emotions among peers. According to multiple theorists, rejected children tend to be more reactive and less able to self-regulate during distressing social situations (Coie 1990; Halberstadt et al. 2001; Hubbard and Dearing 2004). The links between peer rejection and emotional functioning may be bidirectional in nature (Underwood 1997); for instance, strong emotional displays may invite rejection by peers, which in turn, may exacerbate future emotional reactivity. According to research, peer-rejected children tend to express more anger in response to peer-induced stress than average-status children (Hubbard 2001). Peer rejection has also been linked to less adaptive emotion regulation, as rated by others (Maszk et al. 1999) and observed during a stressful task (Trentacosta and Shaw 2009). In addition, short-term longitudinal studies point toward two-way paths between peer rejection and emotional functioning (e.g., Dill et al. 2004; Maszk et al. 1999). Based on this theory and research, we hypothesized that children higher in peer rejection would display greater reactivity to peer victimization in their negative daily emotions. Sex by Peer Rejection Finally, we explored whether sex and peer rejection interact to moderate emotional reactivity to peer victimization. That is, we tested whether peer rejection is a stronger indicator of emotional reactivity to different types of victimization for one sex versus the other. While boys and girls both experience rejection by peers, boys tend to experience higher rates of peer rejection (Coie et al. 1990). Different patterns of social cognitions and social behaviors are also associated with peer rejection in boys than in girls (Dodge and Feldman 1990). However, we were unable to locate

4 1092 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2014) 42: studies that specifically assessed the relations among children s sex, peer rejection, and emotional functioning. Per Rose and Rudolph (2006), certain social behaviors have different consequences across sex. Specifically, sexdeviant behaviors are more likely to contribute to problematic peer relations. Pertaining to this study, if certain emotional displays are less normative for one sex, such as crying by boys (Zeman and Garber 1996) or angry outbursts by girls (Hubbard 2001), they may be more strongly associated with peer rejection for that sex. For instance, intense displays of anger could be a stronger correlate of peer rejection in girls, while prominent fear responses could be a stronger correlate in boys. Thus, peer rejection might be a marker of elevated reactivity to peer victimization in different emotions for boys and girls. Given this complexity and lack of previous research, we explored whether sex and peer rejection interacted to moderate emotional reactivity to victimization, without making specific hypotheses. Summary of Study Aims and Hypotheses The current study had three aims. First, we first tested our hypothesis that the five proposed types of peer victimization would emerge as separate yet co-occurring daily experiences. Second, we evaluated our prediction that a variety of daily peer victimization experiences would be linked to increased negative emotion during the school day. Third, we examined whether sex and peer rejection moderate children semotional reactivity to daily victimization. We specifically tested whether girls would be more reactive to social manipulation, while boys would be more reactive to physical victimization, and that children higher in peer rejection would be generally more reactive to peer victimization. Method Participants were recruited from eight fifth-grade public school classrooms from four schools in one school district within a Mid-Atlantic state. Two schools are located in suburban areas, while the other two are situated in more urban settings. Consent forms were distributed to 225 children, and 201 returned them. Of these children, 188 received their parents permission to participate. Prior to data collection, six of the children transferred out of the participating classrooms, and one child was removed due to missing background information. The final sample consisted of 181 children (104 boys and 77 girls). The following racial/ethnic groups were reported: 35 % White, 31 % Black, 17 % Hispanic, 9 % Mixed, 3 % Asian, and 1 % American Indian; 4 % of the children were missing data on race-ethnicity due to their parents decision to skip this question on a demographics questionnaire. Across the school district, 39 % of students qualified for free or reduced-price lunch, and all participating schools were designated by the district as low income. Procedure Data collection took place in the late fall and early winter of one school year. We spent 4 to 5 weeks collecting data from each classroom. In the first 2 weeks, daily data were gathered from each child for up to 8 days. A majority of these data were collected across consecutive school days; however, several children missed daily collections. We returned to their schools over the next 1 or 2 weeks to collect as many daily observations as possible (up to eight total). Overall, 166 children provided data on eight school days, whereas 15 children provided data on at least three (but fewer than 8) days. In the 4th or 5th week, we returned to each classroom one final time to collect peer nominations of peer rejection. The principal investigator or a graduate-level research assistant (accompanied by two undergraduate assistants) groupadministered all measures to participating children in each classroom. The undergraduate assistants circulated within the classrooms to answer questions or read measures aloud to small groups of children identified by their teachers as having reading difficulties. Approximately 10 to 15 children required assistance with reading. All measures were collected in the final 30 min of each school day and took roughly 10 min to complete. A child assent form was administered on the first daily collection, and we verbally reminded participants of their right to withdraw during each subsequent collection. Measures Daily Negative Emotions Four items were used to assess children s daily sadness, anger, embarrassment, and nervousness. Three items (sad, mad, and nervous) were drawn from the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule for Children (PANAS-C; Laurent et al. 1999) and adapted to assess daily sadness, anger, and nervousness, respectively. The PANAS-C has been adapted for daily reporting in several previous studies involving preadolescents (e.g., Silk et al. 2003). One additional item (embarrassed) was added to capture feelings of daily embarrassment. Children were asked to rate the degree to which they felt each emotion today at school on a five-point scale: 1 (not at all), 2 (a little bit), 3 (some), 4 (pretty much), and 5 (very much). Higher scores indicate stronger feelings of each negative emotion. Daily Peer Victimization Children reported their daily peer victimization experiences using a newly developed checklist with five proposed subscales: physical victimization, verbal

5 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2014) 42: victimization, social manipulation, property attacks, and social rebuff. To construct this checklist, we borrowed 16 items from Mynard and Joseph s (2000) self-report scale, which includes four, four-item subscales (physical victimization, verbal victimization, social manipulation, and property attacks). Because these items were developed for English youth, we reworded several to sound more familiar to U.S. children. We borrowed three items from Sandstrom and Cillessen s (2003) checklist to create a social rebuff subscale and moved one of Mynard and Joseph s social manipulation items (A kid refused to talk to me) to our social rebuff subscale. Finally, we added one item (When I tried to play with one kid, another kid would not let me) from Sandstrom and Cillessen s checklist to our social manipulation subscale. By taking these steps, the daily checklist included five, four-item subscales representing each of the five proposed subtypes of peer victimization (Table 1). For each item, children were asked to circle the number of times (0, 1, 2, 3, 4 or more) that they experienced a victimization event today at school. As described in the Results section, we conducted a confirmatory factor analysis of this measure at the level of daily items, which supported our hypothesized five-factor solution. Accordingly, daily scores were computed for each subtype of peer victimization by summing children s responses within subscale for each day. We examined the reliability of the daily peer victimization subscales using two methods (Table 2). First, we calculated an aggregate index of internal consistency by averaging the daily alphas for each subscale. The average daily internal consistency was acceptable for all five types of peer victimization (average daily α= ). Second, we performed a novel procedure to evaluate the psychometric properties of daily report measures (Cranford et al. 2006; Wenze et al. 2009). The first step is to run an analysis of variance model to decompose the variance of a daily measure into four components: between-person, within-person, item, and error. Next, these variances are used to compute reliability estimates utilizing formulas developed by Cranford and colleagues (2006). We computed between- and within-person reliability coefficients for each victimization subscale. The between-person coefficients represent how reliably each scale captured childlevel differences in peer victimization between children, while the within-person coefficients reflect how reliably each scale measured day-to-day fluctuation of peer victimization within children. Both coefficients were acceptable for all five types of victimization (Iida et al. 2012). More specifically, 70 to 79 % of between-child variation in victimization was reliably captured by the subscales, while 62 to 79 % of daily change in victimization was captured reliably. Peer Rejection Peer rejection was assessed using unlimited peer nominations of liking and disliking. Children nominated classmates whom they like a lot and those whom they don t Table 1 Daily peer victimization items Type Number Item Physical victimization 1 A kid hit or pushed me. 2 Akidkickedme. 3 Another kid beat me up. 4 A kid hurt my body in some other way. Verbal victimization 5 A kid called me mean names. 6 A kid said something mean about me. 7 A kid made fun of the way I look. 8 A kid made fun of me for other some reason. Social manipulation 9 A kid tried to get me in trouble with my friends. 10 A kid tried to make my friends turn against me. 11 When I tried to play with one kid, another kid would not let me. 12 A kid made other people not talk to me. Property attacks 13 A kid took something of mine without permission. 14 A kid tried to break something of mine. 15 Another kid stole something from me. 16 A kid damaged something of mine on purpose. Social rebuff 17 A kid ignored me. 18 Akidrefusedtotalktome. 19 Kids wouldn t let me join their game. 20 Kids had a secret and would not tell me.

6 1094 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2014) 42: Table 2 Descriptive statistics and reliability estimates Level-1 (Daily) M (SD) Level-2 (Child) M (SD) Level-2 (Child) Range Level-2 (Child) Skewness Internal consistency Between-person reliability Within-person reliability Sadness 1.33 (0.83) 1.33 (0.50) Anger 1.64 (1.12) 1.64 (0.71) Embarrassment 1.20 (0.62) 1.20 (0.37) Nervousness 1.34 (0.81) 1.33 (0.50) Physical victimization 0.55 (1.69) 0.57 (1.24) Verbal victimization 0.94 (2.54) 0.98 (1.94) Social manipulation 0.52 (1.74) 0.53 (1.36) Property attacks 0.50 (1.58) 0.51 (1.17) Social rebuff 0.72 (1.99) 0.74 (1.58) Peer rejection 0.03 (1.00) At Level 1, descriptive statistics were computed from the full matrix of daily data. At Level 2, descriptive statistics were computed after aggregating all available daily data within each child to yield one average daily score for each variable for every child. Internal consistency was estimated by calculating separate alphas for all eight days and then averaging these values. Between-person reliability is an estimate of how reliably a daily scale captures traitlevel variability; within-person reliability is an estimate of reliably a scale captures within-person variability across days (Cranford et al. 2006) like very much. These nominations have been validated in many previous studies (e.g., Parker and Asher 1993). A class participation rate of at least 50 % is indicated to collect accurate data for sociometric rejection (McKown et al. 2011). In this study, at least 72 % of the children in each classroom completed these nominations. Peer rejection scores were computed following procedures validated by multiple researchers (e.g., Coie and Dodge 1983; Crick and Grotpeter 1995). First, we tallied the number of liking and disliking nominations received by each child and then standardized these scores within each classroom. Next, we calculated social preference scores by subtracting children s standardized disliking score from their standardized liking scores. Lastly, we multiplied the social preference scores by negative one and re-standardized them in each classroom; higher scores indicate greater levels of peer rejection. Results Our data are organized in two levels. Level 1 (daily level) includes 1,413 daily reports, and Level 2 (child level) includes 181 children who provided the daily reports. Thus, the daily observations are nested within children. Nested data are typically interdependent; that is, data from the same higher-level unit (e.g., within one child) tend to correlate more strongly than data from different units (e.g., between two children). Failure to account for interdependence yields biased standard error estimates and increased risk of Type I errors (Raudenbush and Bryk 2002). Therefore, we took multiple steps in our analyses to account for interdependence in our data. Multiple Types of Daily Peer Victimization We investigated the factor structure of the daily peer victimization measure by performing several confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) in Mplus 5.1 (Muthén and Muthén 2007). Each model included 20 indicators corresponding to the 20 daily items. To account for the nested structure of our data, we group centered all daily items by subtracting children s mean item scores (aggregated across all available days) from their daily item scores. This approach removed Level-2 variation from each indicator and allowed us to examine a single matrix of daily data that included only Level-1 variation (for a discussion, see Thompson and Bolger 1999). With this approach, it was not necessary to run separate models for each individual day. All 20 indicators were significantly skewed (skewness = ); we attempted to normalize them with three transformations (logarithmic, square root, and negative reciprocal), but none reduced skewness across all indicators. Therefore, we tested each model with the raw indicators using maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors (MLR). This approach allowed us to obtain parameter estimates, standard errors, and chi-square statistics that were robust to nonnormality. To evaluate model fit, we examined the chisquare statistic, along with three alternate indices: the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). RMSEA values less than or equal to 0.06, CFI values greater than or equal to 0.95, and SRMR values less than 0.08 indicate reasonable model fit (Hu and Bentler 1999). We began by testing our hypothesized model (Fig. 1) that included five correlated factors each measured by four

7 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2014) 42: , p<0.001; RMSEA=0.03; CFI=0.85; SRMR=0.06; however, the hypothesized model again fit significantly better, SB Δχ 2 (4)=34.11, p< Collectively, these results suggest that the hypothesized five-factor model provides a reasonable representation of the structure of the daily peer victimization measure. Thus, we summed the daily items within each of the five victimization subtypes and used these variables for all subsequent analyses. Descriptive Analyses, Demographic Tests, and Correlations Fig. 1 Hypothesized five-factor CFA model for the daily peer victimization checklist. Indicator labels correspond with the item numbers in Table 1. Factor loadings are standardized. All loadings and correlations are significant at p<0.01 indicators. This model fit the data relatively well, χ 2 (160)= , p<0.001; RMSEA=0.03; CFI=0.88; SRMR=0.06. All standardized factor loadings were significant and greater than Additionally, all factor correlations were positive and significant, yet did not indicate excessive overlap ( ). We then tested two competing models and compared them to the hypothesized model using Satorra and Bentler s (2001) formula for evaluating chi-square differences with MLR (SB Δχ 2 ). In the first competing model, all 20 indicators loaded onto a single factor, χ 2 (170)=732.30, p<0.001; RMSEA=0.05; CFI=0.62; SRMR=0.08. In this model, six standardized loadings fell below Additionally, the hypothesized model fit significantly better, SB Δχ 2 (10)=250.78, p< In the second competing model, social manipulation and social rebuff were merged into one factor due to their conceptual similarity. This model fit the data relatively well, χ 2 (164)= Descriptive statistics are provided for all variables in Table 2. Means and standard deviations are presented at both levels for the daily peer victimization and negative emotion variables. To compute these descriptive statistics at Level 2, we first aggregated the available daily data to yield one average daily score for each variable for every child. For instance, we aggregated children s daily reports of sadness to find their average daily sadness scores, from which we computed Level-2 descriptive statistics. Next, we explored sex differences by conducting independent-sample t tests for variables at Level 2. There were no significant differences for peer victimization, but two significant differences emerged for negative emotions. Girls reported higher levels of sadness (girls M=1.42; boys M= 1.26; t(179) = 2.23, p<0.05) and nervousness (girls M= 1.42; boys M=1.26; t(179)= 2.15, p<0.05). Boys were nominated by peers as significantly higher in peer rejection (boys M=0.16; girls M= 0.30; t(179)=3.15, p<0.01). A one-way ANOVA was conducted to explore racial/ethnic differences for all of the variables at Level 2. Eight children were excluded from this analysis; seven were missing data for race/ethnicity, and one was removed because he was the only Native American child. No significant racial/ethnic differences appeared in any of the daily variables or peer rejection. Finally, we computed bivariate correlations between peer rejection and all of the victimization and emotion variables aggregated at Level 2. Peer rejection evidenced positive correlations with just three other variables: embarrassment (r=0.21, p<0.01), nervousness (r=0.18, p<0.05), and verbal victimization (r=0.16, p<0.05). Daily Peer Victimization and Negative Emotions For the remainder of our analyses, we utilized multilevel regression via HLM 7 (Raudenbush et al. 2011). Multilevel regression accounts for interdependence in nested data by partitioning the variance of a dependent variable across each level (Raudenbush and Bryk 2002). This approach allowed us to model the unique effect of peer victimization on negative emotion for each child and then obtain an overall estimate of the degree to which daily negative emotion covaries with daily victimization.

8 1096 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2014) 42: First, we tested a series of two-level models to estimate the relations of each type of victimization with every emotion (Table 3). In each model, one negative emotion was specified as the dependent variable, and one type of victimization was entered as a Level-1 predictor with a random slope. Based on our previous findings, sex was included as a Level-2 covariate predicting sadness and nervousness, and peer rejection was included as a Level-2 covariate predicting embarrassment and nervousness. Sex was dummy coded (boys = 0; girls = 1). All predictors were entered uncentered for all subsequent analyses. s and slopes were estimated for each model. represents the average level of negative emotion when no victimization experiences are reported and the two covariates (if included) equal zero. When sex is included, the intercept reflects the average level of negative emotion for boys (boys = 0). Because peer rejection was standardized, when it is included in a model, the intercept reflects the average level of negative emotion for children with the mean level of peer rejection for their class. The victimization slopes represent the average change in daily negative emotion for one additional victimization experience. Each peer victimization variable positively predicted every negative emotion. Therefore, children reported higher levels of each emotion on days they reported higher frequencies of each type of victimization. As an example, the estimated intercept for the model of physical victimization on anger suggests that children report an average anger level of 1.56 on days they report no instances of physical victimization. The slope estimate indicates that children report an average increase of 0.14 in daily anger for every additional experience of physical victimization. We then calculated the proportion of variance explained at Level 1 in each model (Raudenbush and Bryk 2002). The following percentages indicate the amount of variation in daily emotions explained by daily victimization. Physical victimization accounted for the following: sadness (7 %), anger (4 %), embarrassment (8 %), and nervousness (14 %). Verbal victimization accounted for the following: sadness (13 %), anger (8 %), embarrassment (20 %), and nervousness (7 %). Social manipulation accounted for the following: sadness (2 %), anger (6 %), embarrassment (18 %), and nervousness (7 %). Property attacks accounted for the following: sadness (5 %), anger (3 %), embarrassment (9 %), and nervousness (8 %). Social rebuff accounted for the Table 3 Multilevel regression: separate relations of daily peer victimization with daily negative emotions Sadness Anger Embarrassment Nervousness B SE B SE B SE B SE Physical victimization Verbal victimization Social manipulation Property attacks Social rebuff Average daily emotion 1.21** ** ** ** 0.05 Slope Average physical victimization slope 0.10** ** * ** 0.04 Average daily emotion 1.19** ** ** ** 0.04 Slope Average verbal victimization slope 0.07** ** ** * 0.02 Average daily emotion 1.23** ** ** ** 0.05 Slope Average social manipulation slope 0.07* ** ** ** 0.03 Average daily emotion 1.22** ** ** ** 0.04 Slope Average property attacks slope 0.07* ** * * 0.03 Average daily emotion 1.22** ** ** ** 0.04 Slope Average social rebuff slope 0.09** ** * ** 0.02 B unstandardized coefficient. SE standard error. Degrees of freedom ranged from 178 to 180. *p<0.01. **p< Sex was included as a covariate of the intercept for all models of sadness and nervousness. Peer rejection was included as a covariate of the intercept for all models of embarrassment and nervousness

9 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2014) 42: following: sadness (6 %), anger (5 %), embarrassment (6 %), and nervousness (5 %). Next, we examined the relative relations of the peer victimization variables with each negative emotion to determine whether certain victimization experiences account for greater variation in children s daily negative emotions (Table 4). We ran four two-level models, in which one negative emotion was specified as the dependent variable, and the five victimization variables were simultaneously entered as Level-1 predictors with random slopes. Again, sex was included as a Level-2 covariate predicting sadness and nervousness, while peer rejection was included as a Level-2 covariate predicting embarrassment and nervousness. The intercepts for these models represent the average level of negative emotion when none of the five types of victimization are reported and all covariates (if included) equal zero. Five slopes were estimated for each model, which reflect the average change in daily negative emotion for one additional victimization experience, while accounting for the effects of every other type of victimization. Physical victimization positively predicted all four negative emotions, verbal victimization positively predicted anger and embarrassment, and social rebuff positively predicted nervousness. For example, the estimated intercept for the anger model suggests that children report an average anger level of 1.47 on days they report no victimization of any type. The significant slope estimates indicate that children report an average increase of 0.08 in daily anger for every additional physical victimization experience and an average increase of 0.05 for every additional instance of verbal victimization. Finally, we calculated the proportion of variance explained at Level 1 by the five peer victimization variables in each model (Raudenbush and Bryk 2002). These percentages reflect the amount of Level-1 variance jointly accounted for by all five types of peer victimization. Together the victimization subtypes accounted for the following percentages of Level-1 variance: sadness (21 %), anger (15 %), embarrassment (39 %), and nervousness (30 %). Moderators of Emotional Reactivity to Daily Peer Victimization Finally, we examined whether sex or peer rejection moderated the relations of peer victimization with negative emotions (Table 5). That is, we tested whether sex or peer rejection moderated emotional reactivity to peer victimization. We tested two sets of two-level models. In the first set, we examined whether sex and peer rejection individually moderated emotional reactivity to victimization; in the second set, we explored whether sex and peer rejection interactively moderated emotional reactivity to victimization. By testing the models in this sequence, we sought to mirror the process of block-entry regression in HLM 7. Notably, we did not calculate proportions of variance explained for these models due to the complications that arise when testing complex multilevel models (Raudenbush and Bryk 2002). In the first set of models, one negative emotion was specified as the dependent variable, and one victimization subtype was entered as a Level-1 predictor with a random slope. Sex and peer rejection were included as Level-2 covariates of the negative emotion intercept and as Level-2 predictors of the Table 4 Multilevel regression: Relative relations of daily peer victimization with daily negative emotions Sadness Anger Embarrassment Nervousness B SE B SE B SE B SE Average daily emotion 1.16*** *** ** ** 0.03 Physical victimization slope Average physical victimization slope 0.08* ** * ** 0.04 Verbal victimization slope Average verbal victimization slope * * Social manipulation slope Average social manipulation slope Property attacks slope Average property attacks slope Social rebuff slope Average social rebuff slope * 0.03 B unstandardized coefficient. SE standard error. Degrees of freedom ranged from 178 to 180. *p<0.05. **p<0.01. ***p< Sex was included as a covariate of the intercept for all models of sadness and nervousness. Peer rejection was included as a covariate of the intercept for all models of embarrassment and nervousness

10 1098 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2014) 42: Table 5 Multilevel regression: Moderating effects of sex and peer rejection on the relations between peer victimization and negative emotions Sadness Anger Embarrassment Nervousness B SE B SE B SE B SE Average daily emotion 1.21*** *** *** *** 0.05 Physical victimization (PV) slope Average PV slope * ** 0.04 Effect of sex on PV slope ** Effect of peer rejection on PV slope 0.11** *** ** ** 0.03 Effect of sex X peer rejection on PV slope 0.14* *** * 0.04 Average daily emotion 1.18** *** *** *** 0.04 Verbal victimization (VV) slope Average VV slope *** Effect of sex on VV slope Effect of peer rejection on VV slope 0.08** *** * 0.02 Effect of sex X peer rejection on VV slope 0.12** ** 0.03 Average daily emotion 1.23*** *** *** *** 0.05 Social manipulation (SM) slope Average SM slope *** Effect of sex on SM slope 0.10* Effect of peer rejection on SM slope ** * 0.03 Effect of sex X peer rejection on SM slope Average daily emotion 1.22*** *** *** *** 0.04 Property attacks (PA) slope Average PA slope *** * 0.04 Effect of sex on PA slope Effect of peer rejection on PA slope 0.06* * *** 0.03 Effect of sex X peer rejection on PA slope Average daily emotion 1.21*** *** *** *** 0.04 Social rebuff (SR) slope Average SR slope ** * * 0.03 Effect of sex on SR slope Effect of peer rejection on SR slope 0.08* Effect of sex X peer rejection on SR slope 0.09* 0.05 B unstandardized coefficient. SE standard error. Degrees of freedom ranged from 177 to 178. *p<0.05. **p<0.01. ***p< Sex was dummy coded (boys = 0; girls = 1). Sex and peer rejection were included as a covariates of the daily emotion intercept in all models victimization slope. In contrast with the previous models, we included sex and peer rejection as Level-2 covariates in all models to permit us to explore significant interactions in the next set of models (Preacher et al. 2006). As Level-2 predictors of the victimization slopes, sex and peer rejection reflect two-way cross-level interaction terms (Bauer and Curran 2005). By including these interactions, we tested whether sex or peer rejection account for individual differences in the daily covariation of victimization and emotion. This allowed us to test our hypotheses that boys and girls would vary in their emotional reactivity to certain types of victimization, as would children who differ in their rejection by peers. We then tested a second set of models by adding the interaction of sex and peer rejection to the first set. Specifically, we entered this interaction (sex X peer rejection) as a Level-2 covariate of the negative emotion intercept and as Level-2 predictor of the victimization slope. As a Level-2 predictor of the victimization slopes, sex X peer rejection

11 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2014) 42: represents a three-way cross-level interaction term (Bauer and Curran 2005) that allowed us to explore whether peer rejection moderates emotional reactivity to victimization in distinct ways for boys and girls. In Table 5, we present results of models including the three-way interaction only when it was significant. Otherwise, we display the results of the first set of models. In these models, the intercepts reflect the average level of negative emotion for boys of average peer rejection on days when no peer victimization is reported. The victimization slopes represent the average change in negative emotion for every additional experience of victimization, while accounting for the effects of sex, peer rejection, and their interaction. These models also yield estimates of the effects of sex, peer rejection, and sex X peer rejection on the victimization slopes, which indicated whether these variables moderated children s emotional reactivity to victimization. We examined significant cross-level interactions with an online tool designed to probe interactive effects in multilevel regression (Preacher et al. 2006); we did not interpret significant two-way interactions when three-way interactions were significant. Sex Accounting for peer rejection, sex moderated children s emotional reactivity in just one model. Specifically, sex moderated the degree to which children s daily sadness covaried with their social manipulation experiences. An analysis of simple slopes indicated that the daily relation of social manipulation and sadness was positive and significant for girls (b=0.12, t(178)=3.45, p<0.001) but nonsignificant for boys (b=0.02, t(178)=0.79, p=0.43). Thus, girls displayed reactivity in daily sadness to social manipulation, whereas boys did not. Peer Rejection Accounting for sex, peer rejection moderated reactivity in nervousness to four types of victimization (physical victimization, verbal victimization, social manipulation, and property attacks), in embarrassment to two types (social manipulation and property attacks), and in sadness to property attacks. To probe these effects, we tested and graphed simple slopes for peer rejection values falling one standard deviation above and below the mean (high and low peer rejection, respectively). For all significant interactions, the victimization slope was positive and significant for high peer rejection (b= , t(178)= , p<0.01) but nonsignificant for low peer rejection (b= , t(178)= , p>0.50). Compared to low-rejected children, high-rejected children appear more reactive in several negative emotions to multiple types of victimization, particularly to property attacks. Sex by Peer Rejection Lastly, we explored whether sex and peer rejection interacted to moderate emotional reactivity to peer victimization. That is, we tested whether peer rejection is a stronger indicator of emotional reactivity to victimization for one sex than the other. Together, sex and peer rejection moderated reactivity in sadness to three types of victimization (physical victimization, verbal victimization, and social rebuff), in embarrassment to two types (physical and verbal victimization), and in anger to physical victimization. To probe the models with significant three-way interactions, we tested six corresponding models separately for boys and girls. In each model, one negative emotion was specified as the dependent variable, and one victimization variable was entered as a Level-1 predictor with a random slope. Peer rejection was included as a Level-2 covariate of the emotion intercept and as a Level-2 predictor of the victimization slope. These models allowed us to examine the moderating effect of peer rejection on emotional reactivity to victimization independently for boys and girls. Peer rejection was a significant moderator in each model for boys (B= , t(102)= , p<0.01) but nonsignificant for girls (B= , t(75)= , p>0.10). We then tested simple slopes for high and low peer rejection for boys using Preacher and colleagues (2006) tool. Post-hoc analyses revealed that peer rejection moderated the six victimization slopes for boys, such that every slope was positive and significant at high levels of peer rejection (b= , t(177)= , p<0.01) but nonsignificant at low levels (b= , t(177)= , p>0.29). Therefore, boys high in peer rejection evidenced emotional reactivity to victimization, whereas boys low in peer rejection did not. Conversely, peer rejection was not a significant moderator for girls; thus, high- and low-rejected girls did not vary in their emotional reactivity to victimization. Discussion The current study aimed to advance our understanding of preadolescents day-to-day encounters with peer victimization. First, we used daily-report methods to assess a wide range of peer victimization experiences and test the distinctiveness of multiple subtypes of victimization. Second, we examined the daily connections of peer victimization and emotional functioning by evaluating the degree to which children s daily negative emotions fluctuated with their daily victimization experiences. Third, we explored whether two child variables (sex and peer rejection) moderated children s daily emotional reactivity to peer victimization. Consistent with our hypothesis, the daily peer victimization checklist was reasonably represented by five factors: physical victimization, verbal victimization, social manipulation, property attacks, and social rebuff. By testing a series of competing models, we found support for distinction among the five victimization subtypes. In line with previous studies (e.g., Mynard and Joseph 2000), verbal victimization was reported most frequently. All five types of victimization were

Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, Volume 60, Number 3, July 2014, pp (Article)

Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, Volume 60, Number 3, July 2014, pp (Article) R l t n n lt pl T p f P r V t z t n, R t v t t P r V t z t n, nd d h v nt n F fth r d B nd rl h l T. rr, J l. H bb rd, L r n. ft Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, Volume 60, Number 3, July 2014, pp. 302-327 (Article)

More information

Seattle Personality Inventory Kindergarten-Grade 1 / Years 1-2 Fast Track Project Technical Report Mark Greenberg & Lili Lengua November 10, 1995

Seattle Personality Inventory Kindergarten-Grade 1 / Years 1-2 Fast Track Project Technical Report Mark Greenberg & Lili Lengua November 10, 1995 Seattle Personality Inventory Kindergarten-Grade 1 / Years 1-2 Fast Track Project Technical Report Mark Greenberg & Lili Lengua November 10, 1995 This technical report was written for both years one and

More information

FOR PROOFREADING ONLY

FOR PROOFREADING ONLY Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Vol. 32, No. 2, April 2004, pp. 159 173 ( C 2004) Negative Affect in Victimized Children: The Roles of Social Withdrawal, Peer Rejection, and Attitudes Toward Bullying

More information

Children Wave 1. Instruments Summary OVERVIEW

Children Wave 1. Instruments Summary OVERVIEW Universität Zürich Pädagogisches Institut University of Cambridge Institute of Criminology z-proso Zurich Project on the Social Development of Children Instruments Summary Children Wave 1 OVERVIEW DATA

More information

The Youth Experience Survey 2.0: Instrument Revisions and Validity Testing* David M. Hansen 1 University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign

The Youth Experience Survey 2.0: Instrument Revisions and Validity Testing* David M. Hansen 1 University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign The Youth Experience Survey 2.0: Instrument Revisions and Validity Testing* David M. Hansen 1 University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign Reed Larson 2 University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign February 28,

More information

peer relations; psychological adjustment; context; methodology; adolescent development

peer relations; psychological adjustment; context; methodology; adolescent development Contextual Variations in Negative Mood and State Self-Esteem What Role Do Peers Play? Bridget M. Reynolds Rena L. Repetti University of California, Los Angeles Journal of Early Adolescence Volume 28 Number

More information

Social Causes and Consequences of Rejection Sensitivity

Social Causes and Consequences of Rejection Sensitivity JOURNAL OF RESEARCH ON ADOLESCENCE, 17(3), 481 506 Copyright r 2007, Society for Research on Adolescence Social Causes and Consequences of Rejection Sensitivity Bonita London, Geraldine Downey, and Cheryl

More information

Early Behavior Problems as a Predictor of Later Peer Group Victimization: Moderators and Mediators in the Pathways of Social Risk

Early Behavior Problems as a Predictor of Later Peer Group Victimization: Moderators and Mediators in the Pathways of Social Risk Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Vol. 7, No. 3, 999, pp. 9 0 Early Behavior Problems as a Predictor of Later Peer Group Victimization: Moderators and Mediators in the Pathways of Social Risk David

More information

Running Head: SOCIAL COGNITION, AGGRESSION, AND FRIENDSHIP 1

Running Head: SOCIAL COGNITION, AGGRESSION, AND FRIENDSHIP 1 Running Head: SOCIAL COGNITION, AGGRESSION, AND FRIENDSHIP 1 Beyond Behavior: An Examination of the Associations Between Social Cognition, Aggression, and Friendship Quality Meghan Elizabeth Clifford Duke

More information

Assessment of Peer Rejection and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Preschool Boys: A Short-Term Longitudinal Study

Assessment of Peer Rejection and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Preschool Boys: A Short-Term Longitudinal Study Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Vol. 19, No. 4, 1991 Assessment of Peer Rejection and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Preschool Boys: A Short-Term Longitudinal Study Sheryl L. Olson 1,2 and Pearl

More information

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Preschool Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (1.5 5 yrs.) among Canadian children

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Preschool Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (1.5 5 yrs.) among Canadian children Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Preschool Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (1.5 5 yrs.) among Canadian children Dr. KAMALPREET RAKHRA MD MPH PhD(Candidate) No conflict of interest Child Behavioural Check

More information

Introduction to Multilevel Models for Longitudinal and Repeated Measures Data

Introduction to Multilevel Models for Longitudinal and Repeated Measures Data Introduction to Multilevel Models for Longitudinal and Repeated Measures Data Today s Class: Features of longitudinal data Features of longitudinal models What can MLM do for you? What to expect in this

More information

Introduction to Multilevel Models for Longitudinal and Repeated Measures Data

Introduction to Multilevel Models for Longitudinal and Repeated Measures Data Introduction to Multilevel Models for Longitudinal and Repeated Measures Data Today s Class: Features of longitudinal data Features of longitudinal models What can MLM do for you? What to expect in this

More information

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES Correlational Research Correlational Designs Correlational research is used to describe the relationship between two or more naturally occurring variables. Is age related to political conservativism? Are

More information

familiar peers during later middle childhood. Moreover, the cumulative effects of

familiar peers during later middle childhood. Moreover, the cumulative effects of WRIGHT, JONATHAN CLARK., M.A. Stress and Social Evaluative Concerns: Stability Across Later Middle Childhood. (2011) Directed by Dr. Heidi Gazelle. 58 pp. The present study aims to determine if two different

More information

On the Performance of Maximum Likelihood Versus Means and Variance Adjusted Weighted Least Squares Estimation in CFA

On the Performance of Maximum Likelihood Versus Means and Variance Adjusted Weighted Least Squares Estimation in CFA STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODELING, 13(2), 186 203 Copyright 2006, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. On the Performance of Maximum Likelihood Versus Means and Variance Adjusted Weighted Least Squares Estimation

More information

The expression and regulation of negative emotions: Risk factors for young children s peer victimization

The expression and regulation of negative emotions: Risk factors for young children s peer victimization Development and Psychopathology 16 ~2004!, 335 353 Copyright 2004 Cambridge University Press Printed in the United States of America DOI: 10.10170S0954579404044542 The expression and regulation of negative

More information

Political Science 15, Winter 2014 Final Review

Political Science 15, Winter 2014 Final Review Political Science 15, Winter 2014 Final Review The major topics covered in class are listed below. You should also take a look at the readings listed on the class website. Studying Politics Scientifically

More information

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Procrastination Assessment Scale for Students

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Procrastination Assessment Scale for Students 611456SGOXXX10.1177/2158244015611456SAGE OpenYockey and Kralowec research-article2015 Article Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Procrastination Assessment Scale for Students SAGE Open October-December

More information

The Regulating Role of Negative Emotions in Children s Coping with Peer Rejection

The Regulating Role of Negative Emotions in Children s Coping with Peer Rejection Child Psychiatry Hum Dev (2010) 41:515 534 DOI 10.1007/s10578-010-0185-2 ORIGINAL ARTICLE The Regulating Role of Negative Emotions in Children s Coping with Peer Rejection Kimberly L. Goodman Michael A.

More information

Al Said Abdul Khalik 1. Egypt Associate Professor, National Center for Examination and Educational Evaluation, Rd. 9 Mokattam, Cairo,

Al Said Abdul Khalik 1. Egypt Associate Professor, National Center for Examination and Educational Evaluation, Rd. 9 Mokattam, Cairo, The Effectiveness of Bibliotherapy as an Intervention on Improving Aggressive Behavior of Fifth Graders Children with Emotional and Behavioral Disorders Al Said Abdul Khalik 1 1 Associate Professor, National

More information

College Student Self-Assessment Survey (CSSAS)

College Student Self-Assessment Survey (CSSAS) 13 College Student Self-Assessment Survey (CSSAS) Development of College Student Self Assessment Survey (CSSAS) The collection and analysis of student achievement indicator data are of primary importance

More information

When Friends Disappoint: Boys and Girls Responses to Transgressions of Friendship Expectations

When Friends Disappoint: Boys and Girls Responses to Transgressions of Friendship Expectations Child Development, January/February 2012, Volume 83, Number 1, Pages 104 119 When Friends Disappoint: Boys and Girls Responses to Transgressions of Friendship Expectations Julie Paquette MacEvoy Boston

More information

11/24/2017. Do not imply a cause-and-effect relationship

11/24/2017. Do not imply a cause-and-effect relationship Correlational research is used to describe the relationship between two or more naturally occurring variables. Is age related to political conservativism? Are highly extraverted people less afraid of rejection

More information

By Lora A. Connor B.A., 2008, California State University, Long Beach

By Lora A. Connor B.A., 2008, California State University, Long Beach By Lora A. Connor B.A., 2008, California State University, Long Beach A Thesis Proposal December 2014 Committee Members: James Amirkhan, Ph.D. (Chair) Courtney Ahrens, Ph.D. Young-Hee Cho, Ph.D. Agenda

More information

3 CONCEPTUAL FOUNDATIONS OF STATISTICS

3 CONCEPTUAL FOUNDATIONS OF STATISTICS 3 CONCEPTUAL FOUNDATIONS OF STATISTICS In this chapter, we examine the conceptual foundations of statistics. The goal is to give you an appreciation and conceptual understanding of some basic statistical

More information

The Development of Scales to Measure QISA s Three Guiding Principles of Student Aspirations Using the My Voice TM Survey

The Development of Scales to Measure QISA s Three Guiding Principles of Student Aspirations Using the My Voice TM Survey The Development of Scales to Measure QISA s Three Guiding Principles of Student Aspirations Using the My Voice TM Survey Matthew J. Bundick, Ph.D. Director of Research February 2011 The Development of

More information

Analysis of the Reliability and Validity of an Edgenuity Algebra I Quiz

Analysis of the Reliability and Validity of an Edgenuity Algebra I Quiz Analysis of the Reliability and Validity of an Edgenuity Algebra I Quiz This study presents the steps Edgenuity uses to evaluate the reliability and validity of its quizzes, topic tests, and cumulative

More information

Published online: 18 Aug 2014.

Published online: 18 Aug 2014. This article was downloaded by: ["University at Buffalo Libraries"] On: 26 January 2015, At: 09:07 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered

More information

ABSTRACT. Tracey Meryl Potter, Ph.D., Professor Hedwig Teglasi, Department of Counseling and Personnel Services

ABSTRACT. Tracey Meryl Potter, Ph.D., Professor Hedwig Teglasi, Department of Counseling and Personnel Services ABSTRACT Title of Document: THE ASSESSMENT OF THE FORMS AND FUNCTIONS OF CHILDHOOD AGGRESSION RECONSIDERED Tracey Meryl Potter, Ph.D., 2009 Directed By: Professor Hedwig Teglasi, Department of Counseling

More information

Implicit Theories Relate to Youth Psychopathology, But How? A Longitudinal Test of Two Predictive Models

Implicit Theories Relate to Youth Psychopathology, But How? A Longitudinal Test of Two Predictive Models Child Psychiatry Hum Dev (2016) 47:603 617 DOI 10.1007/s10578-015-0595-2 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Implicit Theories Relate to Youth Psychopathology, But How? A Longitudinal Test of Two Predictive Models Jessica

More information

11 Validity. Content Validity and BIMAS Scale Structure. Overview of Results

11 Validity. Content Validity and BIMAS Scale Structure. Overview of Results 11 Validity The validity of a test refers to the quality of inferences that can be made by the test s scores. That is, how well does the test measure the construct(s) it was designed to measure, and how

More information

Data Analysis in Practice-Based Research. Stephen Zyzanski, PhD Department of Family Medicine Case Western Reserve University School of Medicine

Data Analysis in Practice-Based Research. Stephen Zyzanski, PhD Department of Family Medicine Case Western Reserve University School of Medicine Data Analysis in Practice-Based Research Stephen Zyzanski, PhD Department of Family Medicine Case Western Reserve University School of Medicine Multilevel Data Statistical analyses that fail to recognize

More information

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Area Principle Bar Chart Boxplot Conditional Distribution Dotplot Empirical Rule Five Number Summary Frequency Distribution Frequency Polygon Histogram Interquartile

More information

Differential Associations Between Relational and Overt Aggression and Children s Psychosocial Adjustment

Differential Associations Between Relational and Overt Aggression and Children s Psychosocial Adjustment J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2012) 34:182 190 DOI 10.1007/s10862-011-9274-1 Differential Associations Between Relational and Overt Aggression and Children s Psychosocial Adjustment Teresa M. Preddy & Paula

More information

Running head: CFA OF STICSA 1. Model-Based Factor Reliability and Replicability of the STICSA

Running head: CFA OF STICSA 1. Model-Based Factor Reliability and Replicability of the STICSA Running head: CFA OF STICSA 1 Model-Based Factor Reliability and Replicability of the STICSA The State-Trait Inventory of Cognitive and Somatic Anxiety (STICSA; Ree et al., 2008) is a new measure of anxiety

More information

The Psychometric Properties of Dispositional Flow Scale-2 in Internet Gaming

The Psychometric Properties of Dispositional Flow Scale-2 in Internet Gaming Curr Psychol (2009) 28:194 201 DOI 10.1007/s12144-009-9058-x The Psychometric Properties of Dispositional Flow Scale-2 in Internet Gaming C. K. John Wang & W. C. Liu & A. Khoo Published online: 27 May

More information

Desensitization Questionnaire Stuttering (DST) (Zückner 2016) Instructions on filling in the questionnaire, evaluation and statistical data

Desensitization Questionnaire Stuttering (DST) (Zückner 2016) Instructions on filling in the questionnaire, evaluation and statistical data Desensitization Questionnaire Stuttering (DST) (Zückner 2016) Instructions on filling in the questionnaire, evaluation and statistical data Contents Page 1. Field of application 1 2. Information on filling

More information

Structural Validation of the 3 X 2 Achievement Goal Model

Structural Validation of the 3 X 2 Achievement Goal Model 50 Educational Measurement and Evaluation Review (2012), Vol. 3, 50-59 2012 Philippine Educational Measurement and Evaluation Association Structural Validation of the 3 X 2 Achievement Goal Model Adonis

More information

Reliability and Validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory Generic Core Scales, Multidimensional Fatigue Scale, and Cancer Module

Reliability and Validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory Generic Core Scales, Multidimensional Fatigue Scale, and Cancer Module 2090 The PedsQL in Pediatric Cancer Reliability and Validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory Generic Core Scales, Multidimensional Fatigue Scale, and Cancer Module James W. Varni, Ph.D. 1,2

More information

Questionnaire on Anticipated Discrimination (QUAD)(1): is a self-complete measure comprising 14 items

Questionnaire on Anticipated Discrimination (QUAD)(1): is a self-complete measure comprising 14 items Online Supplement Data Supplement for Clement et al. (10.1176/appi.ps.201300448) Details of additional measures included in the analysis Questionnaire on Anticipated Discrimination (QUAD)(1): is a self-complete

More information

UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter. Lawrence Feinstein. Jyllian Call. University of California, Los Angeles

UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter. Lawrence Feinstein. Jyllian Call. University of California, Los Angeles UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter Lawrence Feinstein Jyllian Call University of California, Los Angeles December, 1986 Further information on the instrument can be obtained

More information

Emotional Development

Emotional Development Emotional Development How Children Develop Chapter 10 Emotional Intelligence A set of abilities that contribute to competent social functioning: Being able to motivate oneself and persist in the face of

More information

ABSTRACT. This study investigated the relations among victimization and psychological,

ABSTRACT. This study investigated the relations among victimization and psychological, ABSTRACT Title of Dissertation: MULTIPLE DIMENSIONS OF PEER VICTIMIZATION AND THEIR RELATIONS WITH CHILDREN S PSYCHOLOGICAL, SOCIAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND ACADEMIC FUNCTIONING Sarah K. Parsons, Doctor of Philosophy,

More information

Social Determinants and Consequences of Children s Non-Cognitive Skills: An Exploratory Analysis. Amy Hsin Yu Xie

Social Determinants and Consequences of Children s Non-Cognitive Skills: An Exploratory Analysis. Amy Hsin Yu Xie Social Determinants and Consequences of Children s Non-Cognitive Skills: An Exploratory Analysis Amy Hsin Yu Xie Abstract We assess the relative role of cognitive and non-cognitive skills in mediating

More information

Business Statistics Probability

Business Statistics Probability Business Statistics The following was provided by Dr. Suzanne Delaney, and is a comprehensive review of Business Statistics. The workshop instructor will provide relevant examples during the Skills Assessment

More information

Suicidal Ideation and School Bullying Experiences After Controlling for Depression and Delinquency

Suicidal Ideation and School Bullying Experiences After Controlling for Depression and Delinquency Journal of Adolescent Health 53 (2013) S27eS31 www.jahonline.org Original article Suicidal Ideation and School Bullying Experiences After Controlling for Depression and Delinquency Dorothy L. Espelage,

More information

Multifactor Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Multifactor Confirmatory Factor Analysis Multifactor Confirmatory Factor Analysis Latent Trait Measurement and Structural Equation Models Lecture #9 March 13, 2013 PSYC 948: Lecture #9 Today s Class Confirmatory Factor Analysis with more than

More information

Donna L. Coffman Joint Prevention Methodology Seminar

Donna L. Coffman Joint Prevention Methodology Seminar Donna L. Coffman Joint Prevention Methodology Seminar The purpose of this talk is to illustrate how to obtain propensity scores in multilevel data and use these to strengthen causal inferences about mediation.

More information

ABSTRACT. The Social Information Processing Patterns of Peer-Victimized Children

ABSTRACT. The Social Information Processing Patterns of Peer-Victimized Children ABSTRACT Title: The Social Information Processing Patterns of Peer-Victimized Children Sarah Katherine Groff, Master of Arts, 2005 Directed By: Professor Hedwig Teglasi, Department of Counseling and Personnel

More information

In this chapter we discuss validity issues for quantitative research and for qualitative research.

In this chapter we discuss validity issues for quantitative research and for qualitative research. Chapter 8 Validity of Research Results (Reminder: Don t forget to utilize the concept maps and study questions as you study this and the other chapters.) In this chapter we discuss validity issues for

More information

Perceived Norms and Classroom Ethnic Composition

Perceived Norms and Classroom Ethnic Composition University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 2014 Perceived Norms and Classroom Ethnic Composition Thomas Christopher O'Brien University of Massachusetts

More information

Supplementary Material. other ethnic backgrounds. All but six of the yoked pairs were matched on ethnicity. Results

Supplementary Material. other ethnic backgrounds. All but six of the yoked pairs were matched on ethnicity. Results Supplementary Material S1 Methodological Details Participants The sample was 80% Caucasian, 16.7% Asian or Asian American, and 3.3% from other ethnic backgrounds. All but six of the yoked pairs were matched

More information

Problem Situation Form for Parents

Problem Situation Form for Parents Problem Situation Form for Parents Please complete a form for each situation you notice causes your child social anxiety. 1. WHAT WAS THE SITUATION? Please describe what happened. Provide enough information

More information

CHAPTER 3 RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

CHAPTER 3 RESEARCH METHODOLOGY CHAPTER 3 RESEARCH METHODOLOGY 3.1 Introduction 3.1 Methodology 3.1.1 Research Design 3.1. Research Framework Design 3.1.3 Research Instrument 3.1.4 Validity of Questionnaire 3.1.5 Statistical Measurement

More information

Chapter 4. Outsiders and Defenders Bullying-related Behavioral Reputation Versus. Daily Self-reported Provictim Intervention Behavior

Chapter 4. Outsiders and Defenders Bullying-related Behavioral Reputation Versus. Daily Self-reported Provictim Intervention Behavior Outsiders and Defenders Bullying-related Behavioral Reputation Versus Daily Self-reported Provictim Intervention Behavior Submitted for Publication as: Pronk, J., Nishina, A., Olthof, T., & Goossens, F.

More information

Self-Oriented and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism in the Eating Disorder Inventory Perfectionism Subscale

Self-Oriented and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism in the Eating Disorder Inventory Perfectionism Subscale Self-Oriented and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism in the Eating Disorder Inventory Perfectionism Subscale Simon B. Sherry, 1 Paul L. Hewitt, 1 * Avi Besser, 2 Brandy J. McGee, 1 and Gordon L. Flett 3

More information

Available from Deakin Research Online:

Available from Deakin Research Online: This is the published version: Richardson, Ben and Fuller Tyszkiewicz, Matthew 2014, The application of non linear multilevel models to experience sampling data, European health psychologist, vol. 16,

More information

It Takes Two to Fight: A Test of Relational Factors and a Method for Assessing Aggressive Dyads

It Takes Two to Fight: A Test of Relational Factors and a Method for Assessing Aggressive Dyads Developmental Psychology 1999, Vol. 35, No. 5, 1179-1188 Copyright 1999 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0012-1649/99/S3.00 It Takes Two to Fight: A Test of Relational Factors and a Method

More information

Reactive and Proactive Aggression: Stability of Constructs and Relations to Correlates

Reactive and Proactive Aggression: Stability of Constructs and Relations to Correlates The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 2007, 167(4), 365 382 Copyright 2007 Heldref Publications Reactive and Proactive Aggression: Stability of Constructs and Relations to Correlates MEGHAN D. MCAULIFFE JULIE

More information

STRONG EMOTIONAL RESPONSES

STRONG EMOTIONAL RESPONSES STRONG EMOTIONAL RESPONSES A strong emotional response is created when an individual says or does something that makes us, as members of social groups, feel diminished, offended, threatened, stereotyped,

More information

MENTAL HEALTH 2011 SURVEY RESULTS REPORT. and Related Behaviors. Figure 1 n Trends in mental health indicators, Grades 9 12, New Mexico,

MENTAL HEALTH 2011 SURVEY RESULTS REPORT. and Related Behaviors. Figure 1 n Trends in mental health indicators, Grades 9 12, New Mexico, 2011 SURVEY RESULTS REPORT MENTAL HEALTH and Related Behaviors 50 40 30 20 Persistent sadness or hopelessness Percent (%) 10 0 2003 2005 2007 2009 2011 31.9 28.7 30.8 29.7 29.1 Seriously considered suicide

More information

Instrument equivalence across ethnic groups. Antonio Olmos (MHCD) Susan R. Hutchinson (UNC)

Instrument equivalence across ethnic groups. Antonio Olmos (MHCD) Susan R. Hutchinson (UNC) Instrument equivalence across ethnic groups Antonio Olmos (MHCD) Susan R. Hutchinson (UNC) Overview Instrument Equivalence Measurement Invariance Invariance in Reliability Scores Factorial Invariance Item

More information

OLS Regression with Clustered Data

OLS Regression with Clustered Data OLS Regression with Clustered Data Analyzing Clustered Data with OLS Regression: The Effect of a Hierarchical Data Structure Daniel M. McNeish University of Maryland, College Park A previous study by Mundfrom

More information

Running head: CFA OF TDI AND STICSA 1. p Factor or Negative Emotionality? Joint CFA of Internalizing Symptomology

Running head: CFA OF TDI AND STICSA 1. p Factor or Negative Emotionality? Joint CFA of Internalizing Symptomology Running head: CFA OF TDI AND STICSA 1 p Factor or Negative Emotionality? Joint CFA of Internalizing Symptomology Caspi et al. (2014) reported that CFA results supported a general psychopathology factor,

More information

CHAPTER 3 DATA ANALYSIS: DESCRIBING DATA

CHAPTER 3 DATA ANALYSIS: DESCRIBING DATA Data Analysis: Describing Data CHAPTER 3 DATA ANALYSIS: DESCRIBING DATA In the analysis process, the researcher tries to evaluate the data collected both from written documents and from other sources such

More information

Development of a New Fear of Hypoglycemia Scale: Preliminary Results

Development of a New Fear of Hypoglycemia Scale: Preliminary Results Development of a New Fear of Hypoglycemia Scale: Preliminary Results Jodi L. Kamps, 1 PHD, Michael C. Roberts, 2 PHD, ABPP, and R. Enrique Varela, 3 PHD 1 Children s Hospital of New Orleans, 2 University

More information

The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose. Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX

The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose. Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX 2 Web Appendix A: Panel data estimation approach As noted in the main

More information

Stability and Change of Adolescent. Coping Styles and Mental Health: An Intervention Study. Bernd Heubeck & James T. Neill. Division of Psychology

Stability and Change of Adolescent. Coping Styles and Mental Health: An Intervention Study. Bernd Heubeck & James T. Neill. Division of Psychology Stability and Change of Adolescent Coping Styles and Mental Health: An Intervention Study Bernd Heubeck & James T. Neill Division of Psychology The Australian National University Paper presented to the

More information

All reverse-worded items were scored accordingly and are in the appropriate direction in the data set.

All reverse-worded items were scored accordingly and are in the appropriate direction in the data set. PSYC 948: Latent Trait Measurement and Structural Equation Modeling Homework #7 (Total 10 Points) Due: Wednesday, April 10, 2013 at 11:59pm. Homework Problems: The homework problems are based a simulated

More information

Examining the efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) to understand pre-service teachers intention to use technology*

Examining the efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) to understand pre-service teachers intention to use technology* Examining the efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) to understand pre-service teachers intention to use technology* Timothy Teo & Chwee Beng Lee Nanyang Technology University Singapore This

More information

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity Behav. Sci. 2013, 3, 170 191; doi:10.3390/bs3010170 Article OPEN ACCESS behavioral sciences ISSN 2076-328X www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living:

More information

Healing Trauma Evaluation Year 1 Findings

Healing Trauma Evaluation Year 1 Findings 2551 Galena Avenue #1774 Simi Valley, CA 93065 310-801-8996 Envisioning Justice Solutions, Inc. Determining the Programs, Policies, and Services Needed to Rebuild the Lives of Criminal Justice Involved

More information

Personal Well-being Among Medical Students: Findings from a Pilot Survey

Personal Well-being Among Medical Students: Findings from a Pilot Survey Analysis IN BRIEF Volume 14, Number 4 April 2014 Association of American Medical Colleges Personal Well-being Among Medical Students: Findings from a Pilot Survey Supplemental Information References 1.

More information

HTS Report EPS. Emotional Processing Scale. Technical Report. Anna Patient ID Date 29/09/2016. Hogrefe Ltd, Oxford

HTS Report EPS. Emotional Processing Scale. Technical Report. Anna Patient ID Date 29/09/2016. Hogrefe Ltd, Oxford EPS Emotional Processing Scale Technical Report HTS Report ID 28641-2 Date 29/09/2016 OVERVIEW EPS 2/8 Overview Summary profile Total Emotional Processing Score Structure of this report Profile sheet Table

More information

bivariate analysis: The statistical analysis of the relationship between two variables.

bivariate analysis: The statistical analysis of the relationship between two variables. bivariate analysis: The statistical analysis of the relationship between two variables. cell frequency: The number of cases in a cell of a cross-tabulation (contingency table). chi-square (χ 2 ) test for

More information

Self Determination Theory, COACHE, and Faculty Outcomes in Higher Education. Lisa M. Larson Mack C. Shelley Sandra W. Gahn Matthew Seipel

Self Determination Theory, COACHE, and Faculty Outcomes in Higher Education. Lisa M. Larson Mack C. Shelley Sandra W. Gahn Matthew Seipel Self Determination Theory, COACHE, and Faculty Outcomes in Higher Education Lisa M. Larson Mack C. Shelley Sandra W. Gahn Matthew Seipel Overview ISU COACHE survey through the lens of Self- Determination

More information

TWO WAYS OF THINKING ABOUT A RELATIONSHIP ISSUE

TWO WAYS OF THINKING ABOUT A RELATIONSHIP ISSUE TWO WAYS OF THINKING ABOUT A RELATIONSHIP ISSUE Here are two different ways of thinking about a relationship issue. Take a mild issue (4 on a scale of 10 in intensity) and view it through these two different

More information

Correspondence of Pediatric Inpatient Behavior Scale (PIBS) Scores with DSM Diagnosis and Problem Severity Ratings in a Referred Pediatric Sample

Correspondence of Pediatric Inpatient Behavior Scale (PIBS) Scores with DSM Diagnosis and Problem Severity Ratings in a Referred Pediatric Sample 1 1999 Florida Conference on Child Health Psychology Gainesville, FL Correspondence of Pediatric Inpatient Behavior Scale (PIBS) Scores with DSM Diagnosis and Problem Severity Ratings in a Referred Pediatric

More information

Dual Pathways from Reactive Aggression to Depressive Symptoms in Children: Further Examination of the Failure Model

Dual Pathways from Reactive Aggression to Depressive Symptoms in Children: Further Examination of the Failure Model Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0426-6 Dual Pathways from Reactive Aggression to Depressive Symptoms in Children: Further Examination of the Failure Model Spencer

More information

CHILD PTSD CHECKLIST CHILD VERSION (CPC C) TRAUMATIC EVENTS

CHILD PTSD CHECKLIST CHILD VERSION (CPC C) TRAUMATIC EVENTS CHILD PTSD CHECKLIST CHILD VERSION (CPC C) 7 18 years. (Version May 23, 2014.) Name ID Date TRAUMATIC EVENTS TO COUNT AN EVENT, YOU MUST HAVE FELT ONE OF THESE: (1) YOU FELT LIKE YOU MIGHT DIE, OR (2)

More information

The Impact of the Peaceful People Program on Elementary Students in an Urban School District Using Comparison Groups

The Impact of the Peaceful People Program on Elementary Students in an Urban School District Using Comparison Groups The Impact of the Peaceful People Program on Elementary Students in an Urban School District Using Comparison Groups Report written by Kimberly Williams, PhD. Associate Professor of Education State University

More information

Effects of School-Level Norms on Student Substance Use

Effects of School-Level Norms on Student Substance Use Prevention Science, Vol. 3, No. 2, June 2002 ( C 2002) Effects of School-Level Norms on Student Substance Use Revathy Kumar, 1,2,4 Patrick M. O Malley, 1 Lloyd D. Johnston, 1 John E. Schulenberg, 1,3 and

More information

Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible

Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible University of Connecticut DigitalCommons@UConn Honors Scholar Theses Honors Scholar Program Spring 5-1-2008 Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible Juemei Yang

More information

Peer and family support as moderators of the relationship between stress and symptoms in lowincome

Peer and family support as moderators of the relationship between stress and symptoms in lowincome Via Sapientiae: The Institutional Repository at DePaul University College of Liberal Arts & Social Sciences Theses and Dissertations College of Liberal Arts and Social Sciences 6-2010 Peer and family support

More information

Michael Armey David M. Fresco. Jon Rottenberg. James J. Gross Ian H. Gotlib. Kent State University. Stanford University. University of South Florida

Michael Armey David M. Fresco. Jon Rottenberg. James J. Gross Ian H. Gotlib. Kent State University. Stanford University. University of South Florida Further psychometric refinement of depressive rumination: Support for the Brooding and Pondering factor solution in a diverse community sample with clinician-assessed psychopathology Michael Armey David

More information

Confirmatory Factor Analysis and Reliability of the Mental Health Inventory for Australian Adolescents 1

Confirmatory Factor Analysis and Reliability of the Mental Health Inventory for Australian Adolescents 1 Confirmatory Factor Analysis and Reliability of the Mental Health Inventory for Australian Adolescents 1 Bernd G. Heubeck and James T. Neill The Australian National University Key words: Mental Health

More information

Electronic Supplementary Material. for. Pride, Personality, and the Evolutionary Foundations of Human Social Status

Electronic Supplementary Material. for. Pride, Personality, and the Evolutionary Foundations of Human Social Status 1 Electronic Supplementary Material for Pride, Personality, and the Evolutionary Foundations of Human Social Status Joey T. Cheng, Jessica L. Tracy, & Joseph Henrich 2 Dominance and Prestige Scale Construction

More information

C h a p t e r 1 1. Psychologists. John B. Nezlek

C h a p t e r 1 1. Psychologists. John B. Nezlek C h a p t e r 1 1 Multilevel Modeling for Psychologists John B. Nezlek Multilevel analyses have become increasingly common in psychological research, although unfortunately, many researchers understanding

More information

Students Perceptions of School Climate During the Middle School Years: Associations with Trajectories of Psychological and Behavioral Adjustment

Students Perceptions of School Climate During the Middle School Years: Associations with Trajectories of Psychological and Behavioral Adjustment Am J Community Psychol (2007) 40:194 213 DOI 10.1007/s10464-007-9143-y ORIGINAL PAPER Students Perceptions of School Climate During the Middle School Years: Associations with Trajectories of Psychological

More information

Tobacco Use Percent (%)

Tobacco Use Percent (%) Tobacco Use 1 8 6 2 23 25 27 Lifetime cigarette use 64.8 62. 59.9 Current cigarette smoker 3.2 25.7 24.2 Current cigar smoker 19.4 21.3 18.9 First cigarette before age 13 24.7 2. 18. Current spit tobacco

More information

Collecting & Making Sense of

Collecting & Making Sense of Collecting & Making Sense of Quantitative Data Deborah Eldredge, PhD, RN Director, Quality, Research & Magnet Recognition i Oregon Health & Science University Margo A. Halm, RN, PhD, ACNS-BC, FAHA Director,

More information

Depression among human care service professionals and positive/negative work life events: a structural equation modeling analysis

Depression among human care service professionals and positive/negative work life events: a structural equation modeling analysis Kawasaki Medical Journal ( ) doi. /KMJ-E ( ) 53 Regular Article Depression among human care service professionals and positive/negative work life events: a structural equation modeling analysis Hiromichi

More information

RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SOCIOMETRIC STATUS AND ANXIETY1. Nara Gakugei University

RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SOCIOMETRIC STATUS AND ANXIETY1. Nara Gakugei University Japanese Psychological Research19 64, Vol.6, No.2, 67-71 RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SOCIOMETRIC STATUS AND ANXIETY1 TOSHIMI UEDA2 Nara Gakugei University An attempt was made to explore intensively the relationship

More information

CHILD PTSD CHECKLIST PARENT VERSION (CPC P) TRAUMATIC EVENTS

CHILD PTSD CHECKLIST PARENT VERSION (CPC P) TRAUMATIC EVENTS CHILD PTSD CHECKLIST PARENT VERSION (CPC P) 7 18 years. Version May 23, 2014. Name ID Date TRAUMATIC EVENTS TO COUNT AN EVENT, YOUR CHILD MUST HAVE FELT ONE OF THESE: (1) FELT LIKE HE/SHE MIGHT DIE, OR

More information

Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment

Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment Marshall University Marshall Digital Scholar Management Faculty Research Management, Marketing and MIS Fall 11-14-2009 Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment Wai Kwan

More information

Life Satisfaction and Student Engagement in Adolescents

Life Satisfaction and Student Engagement in Adolescents J Youth Adolescence (2011) 40:249 262 DOI 10.1007/s10964-010-9517-6 EMPIRICAL RESEARCH Life Satisfaction and Student Engagement in Adolescents Ashley D. Lewis E. Scott Huebner Patrick S. Malone Robert

More information

Title: Reliability and validity of the adolescent stress questionnaire in a sample of European adolescents - the HELENA study

Title: Reliability and validity of the adolescent stress questionnaire in a sample of European adolescents - the HELENA study Author's response to reviews Title: Reliability and validity of the adolescent stress questionnaire in a sample of European adolescents - the HELENA study Authors: Tineke De Vriendt (tineke.devriendt@ugent.be)

More information

Threat Assessment in Schools (2002). Washington, D.C.: U.S. Secret Service & U.S. Dept. of Education.

Threat Assessment in Schools (2002). Washington, D.C.: U.S. Secret Service & U.S. Dept. of Education. Six Principles of the Threat Assessment Process Threat assessment is a central component in preventing targeted school violence. The threat assessment process involves identifying, assessing, and managing

More information

The Bilevel Structure of the Outcome Questionnaire 45

The Bilevel Structure of the Outcome Questionnaire 45 Psychological Assessment 2010 American Psychological Association 2010, Vol. 22, No. 2, 350 355 1040-3590/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0019187 The Bilevel Structure of the Outcome Questionnaire 45 Jamie L. Bludworth,

More information