MODIFYING daily activities may be among the simplest

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "MODIFYING daily activities may be among the simplest"

Transcription

1 Journal of Gerontology: PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCES 2005, Vol. 60B, No. 5, P251 P258 Copyright 2005 by The Gerontological Society of America Complexity of Work and Risk of Alzheimer s Disease: A Population-Based Study of Swedish Twins Ross Andel, 1 Michael Crowe, 2 Nancy L. Pedersen, 3,7 James Mortimer, 4 Eileen Crimmins, 5 Boo Johansson, 6 and Margaret Gatz 3,7 1 School of Aging Studies, University of South Florida, Tampa. 2 Department of Psychology, University of Alabama at Birmingham. 3 Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, The Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden. 4 Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, University of South Florida, Tampa. 5 Andrus Gerontology Center, University of Southern California, Los Angeles. 6 Department of Psychology, Göteborg University, Sweden. 7 Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles. We examined the association between risk of dementia or Alzheimer s disease (AD) and occupation by using measures of complexity of work with data, people, and things. The study included 10,079 members of the population-based Swedish Twin Registry who were participants in the HARMONY study. We diagnosed dementia by means of a two-stage procedure cognitive impairment screening followed by full clinical evaluation. We analyzed data with case-control and cotwin control designs. The cotwin control design provides control over genetic and familial factors. In the case-control study, controlling for age, gender, and level of education, we found that more complex work with people was associated with reduced risk of AD. Greater complexity of work with people and data was protective in twin pairs discordant for AD. Findings suggest that greater complexity of work, and particularly complex work with people, may reduce the risk of AD. MODIFYING daily activities may be among the simplest ways to affect the course of human aging. Behavioral factors associated with lifestyle choices such as smoking, drinking alcohol, or exercising have long been known to influence the onset of diseases, including cancer or heart disease. Recently, more attention has been paid to the possibility that lifestyle may also play a role in reducing the risk of dementia and Alzheimer s disease (AD). Because dementia is among the most salient health concerns of older adults (Henig, 1994), knowing whether behavioral factors may modify risk is of great practical interest. Factors that cannot be amended, such as family history and increasing age, are among the most reliable predictors of dementia (Farrer et al., 1997; Hendrie, 1998). However, there is some evidence that an intellectually stimulating lifestyle may help modify late-life cognitive outcomes (Fratiglioni, Paillard- Borg, & Winblad, 2004; Katzman, 1995; Orrell & Sahakian, 1995; Scarmeas & Stern, 2003). Some researchers have found factors that seem to reflect levels of intellectual stimulation over the course of adulthood, such as education (Gatz et al., 2001; Launer et al., 1999), leisure activity (Crowe, Andel, Gatz, Pedersen, & Johansson, 2003; Fratiglioni, Wang, Ericsson, Maytan, Winblad, 2000; Wilson et al., 2002), and occupation (Smyth et al., 2004; Stern et al., 1995), to be related to reduced risk of dementia. Other researchers (e.g., Salthouse, Berish, & Miles, 2002) have cautioned that empirical support for the possibility that cognitive stimulation affects cognition is still only tentative at best. Occupation may be particularly interesting with respect to examining the potential association between intellectual stimulation and subsequent dementia, because people generally spend a substantial portion of their adult years at work. Along the lines of the use it or lose it hypothesis (Katzman, 1995; Orrell & Sahakian, 1995), occupations with high mental demands may provide a form of mental exercise that supports brain function further into older adulthood. Mental exercise provided by frequent engagement in intellectually demanding activity at work may facilitate the maintenance of inherent cognitive reserve, leading to more sophisticated cerebral networks in old age (Churchill et al., 2002; Kolb & Whishaw, 1998) and allowing aging individuals to tolerate dementia neuropathology longer into the progression of the disease (Scarmeas & Stern, 2003; Stern et al., 1995). A similar hypothesis was articulated by Schooler (1984) to explain the effect of complexity of work on cognitive functioning in nondemented older adults. Schooler posited that complex environments that reward cognitive effort and require making decisions motivate individuals to continue to develop their intellectual capacities. To substantiate the environmental complexity hypothesis, Kohn and Schooler (1983) and Schooler, Mulatu, and Oates (1999) derived a factor score from self-reports about complexity of work with data, people, and things, modeled after the Dictionary of Occupational Titles (U.S. Department of Labor, 1965). Results based on this measure suggested that substantive complexity of work facilitates intellectual flexibility and promotes stable cognitive function. The link between occupation and dementia has generally been explored by use of occupational classifications that reflect occupational status. Lifetime occupations characterized by low socioeconomic status have been found to increase the risk of P251

2 P252 ANDEL ET AL. dementia in case-control studies such as the Canadian Study of Health and Aging (1994), studies based in Sicily (Azzimondi, D Alessandro, Pandolfo, & Feruglio, 1998) and Kungsholmen, Sweden (Fratiglioni, Ahlbom, Viitanen, & Winblad, 1993), and studies of incident dementia conducted in East Boston (Evans et al., 1997), New York (Stern et al., 1994), and Kungsholmen (Qui et al., 2003). Potential explanations for this relationship include greater mental demands of high-status occupations (Stern et al.) and a greater likelihood of adverse occupational exposures in low-status occupations (Canadian Study of Health and Aging; Fratiglioni et al.). However, no association between low-status versus high-status occupation and risk of dementia was reported in case-control studies such as the Conselice Study based in Northern Italy (Ravaglia et al., 2002) or in studies of incident dementia such as the French PAQUID project (Personnes Agées QUID project; Helmer et al., 2001) and a study conducted in the United Kingdom (Paykel et al., 1994), leaving the link between occupation and dementia unclear. Two studies conducted by Stern and colleagues (1995) and Smyth and colleagues (2004) have used indicators reflecting occupational demands. In a study of AD patients, Stern and colleagues used factor scores reflecting substantive complexity, interpersonal demands, management requirements, and physical demands derived from occupational characteristics based on the Dictionary of Occupational Titles (U.S. Department of Labor, 1977) to explore the association between occupational factors and current cerebral blood flow. They found that, when dementia severity, age, and education were controlled for, patients who had held jobs with high interpersonal and physical demands had relatively greater deficits in cerebral blood flow in the parietal area compared with patients who had held jobs with low demands. High physical and interpersonal demands also accounted for a significant amount of variance when these variables were entered into a covariate-adjusted model in a stepwise fashion. Stern and colleagues interpreted these results as showing a delay in clinical expression of AD that they attributed to the protective effect of occupational demands on the brain. The same study found no differences in cerebral blood flow related to substantive complexity or management requirements. In a case-control study, Smyth and associates derived scores from the Dictionary of Occupational Titles that were collapsed to reflect mental, social, physical, and motor demands. They analyzed types of occupational demands in separate models. They found that, after race, gender, year of birth, and education were controlled for, participants diagnosed with AD had held jobs with lower mental and higher physical occupational demands compared with controls. Moreover, the difference in occupational demands between demented and nondemented participants appeared to be greater for occupations held closer to retirement. In both the Stern and the Smyth studies, as one potential explanation of their findings the authors offered that differences in occupational status probably reflected differences in occupational complexity. Using data from a population-based twin registry, we explored the association between occupation and risk for all types of dementia and for AD only. We coded each occupation to reflect three types of intellectual demands at work complexity of work with data, with people, and with things. These measures, called worker functions, have been used as basic indicators of complexity (Miller, Treiman, Cain, & Roos, 1980). We expected that high complexity of work, particularly on the dimensions of work with data and people, would be associated with reduced risk of dementia and AD. We expected to find less relevance of complexity of work with things to dementia and AD, because this complexity dimension appears to provide a relatively poor representation of general complexity of work (Kohn & Schooler, 1983; Schooler et al., 1999) and is characterized by lower reliability of measurement (Miller et al.). We analyzed the data within two designs case control and cotwin control. The cotwin-control design estimates disease risk in twins who are discordant for the disease (i.e., one twin has the disease whereas the cotwin does not). A cotwin-control study can account for genetic and familial factors better than other types of case-control designs (see Lichtenstein et al., 2002). For example, because twins share their age and gender (for same-sex pairs), control over these covariates is inherent in the design. Because twins are generally reared together, they share their early-life environment, including childhood socioeconomic status. Finally, twins tend to be similar in terms of cognitive abilities (Pedersen, Plomin, Nesselroade, & McClearn, 1992), thus providing some control over cognitive functioning earlier in life. The case-control design compared demented with nondemented individuals while statistically controlling for covariates. METHODS Participants Participants in this study were members of the Swedish Twin Registry (Lichtenstein et al., 2002) a population-based registry of all twins residing in Sweden who were 65 years of age or older in The Swedish Twin Registry was established in the 1960s when all twins in Sweden were identified. In 1961, 1963, and 1967, all same-sex twin pairs born before 1926 were mailed a questionnaire. Nonresponders in 1967 were sent another questionnaire in All samesex pairs born between 1926 and 1958 were sent a questionnaire in Opposite-sex pairs were registered but were not sent questionnaires. In 1998, the HARMONY study began a follow-up of all twins from both same- and opposite-sex twin pairs who were at least 65 years of age. A random sample of twins was selected for contact by telephone each month. The cognitive screening used the previously validated TELE instrument (Gatz et al., 1995; Gatz, Reynolds, Nikolic, Lowe, & Karel, 2002). Individuals who screened positive for cognitive dysfunction and their cotwins were contacted for an in-person clinical diagnostic evaluation for dementia. Clinical diagnoses of dementia followed the diagnostic criteria set forth in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Differential diagnoses were made according to National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke Alzheimer s Disease and Related Disorders Association (NINCDS ADRDA) criteria for AD (McKhann et al., 1984), National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Association Internationale pour la et l Enseignement en Neurosciences (NINDS AIREN) criteria for

3 COMPLEXITY OF WORK AND ALZHEIMER S DISEASE P253 Variable Table 1. Demographic Characteristics for Participants in the Case-Control Study Cases All Dementias AD Only Controls Crude OR (95% CI) All Dementias N ,854 Women (%) ( ) Age at screening M (SD) 82.0 (6.7) 82.9 (6.2) 72.5 (6.0) 1.21 ( ) Range Education More than basic (%) ( ) AD Only 1.57 ( ) 1.23 ( ) 0.41 ( ) Notes: AD ¼ Alzheimer s disease; OR ¼ odds ratio; CI ¼ confidence interval. CIs correspond to p,.05. If the CI does not include 1.00, then the result is significant. vascular dementia (Roman et al., 1993), Lund and Manchester criteria for frontal temporal dementia (Lund and Manchester Groups, 1994; Neary et al., 1998), and consensus criteria for dementia with Lewy bodies (McKeith et al., 1996). A complete discussion of the study design can be found in Gatz and colleagues (2005). In the HARMONY study, 20,269 participants were eligible for screening. The response rate for telephone screening was 71.5%; the response rate for those eligible for the clinical phase was 70.2%. The same telephone screening also included questions about demographic factors, health and behavioral information, and main lifetime occupation (Lichtenstein et al., 2002). For those who responded to the telephone screening, 85.4% supplied data about lifetime occupation. This information was obtained from self-reports for 93% of the participants and from reports by knowledgeable informants for 7% of participants. Overall, 10,168 individuals aged 65 and older had both complete data for cognitive status and main lifetime occupation. Of these, 314 individuals were classified as dementia cases and 9,854 individuals were classified as controls. Among cases, informant reports about occupation were available for 225, while 89 had self-reports only, potentially introducing a recall bias. Therefore, we excluded these 89 cases from the analyses. Among controls, 325 had only informant reports as a result of various health reasons other than cognitive impairment and were kept in the dataset. Thus, we included 10,079 participants (4,835 individuals and 2,622 complete twin pairs) in the casecontrol analyses. Of the 225 dementia cases, 146 were diagnosed with AD. Demographic characteristics of cases and controls are presented in Table 1. We were able to identify 55 complete twin pairs discordant for dementia, of which 28 pairs were discordant for AD. The zygosity and gender of twins that we included in our cotwincontrol analyses are shown in Table 2. In analyses with twin pairs discordant for all types of dementia, twin cases included 29 men and 26 women, and cotwin controls included 26 men and 29 women. In analyses with twin pairs discordant for AD only, there were 14 men and 14 women among twin cases and 15 men and 13 women among cotwin controls. Table 2. Twin Pairs Discordant for All Types of Dementia and Alzheimer s Disease Only by Zygosity and Gender Pair Type No. of Pairs Male Female Monozygotic All dementias AD only Dizygotic All dementias AD only Opposite sex All dementias AD only Notes: AD¼ Alzheimer s disease. For dementia, N ¼ 55; for AD only, N ¼ 28. Measures Occupation. The main independent variable in the study was complexity of lifetime occupation. Information about occupation was collected in the 1998 telephone screening. Each participant, or a knowledgeable informant, was asked, What kind of occupation did you have during the major part of your working life? Occupational information from these telephone interviews was provided to Statistics Sweden for coding according to categories from the 1980 Swedish Population and Housing Census (Statistics Sweden, 1980). We created a grid that matched each occupational category from the 1980 Swedish Census to the corresponding category in the 1970 U.S. Census (U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1973). Two independent raters, one based in the United States and the other in Sweden, performed the code matching between the 1980 Swedish Census and the 1970 U.S. Census for all occupational categories. The two raters initially agreed in matching 90% of the categories. Differences in matching the remaining 10% were discussed by the two raters until consensus was reached. The high initial agreement is not surprising given that the two classification structures are very similar. Next, we applied occupational complexity scores developed by Roos and Treiman (1980) for each occupational category. Because complexity scores were already assigned to each U.S. Census category, no judgment was required in this step. The scores reflect those from the fourth edition of the Dictionary of Occupational Titles (U.S. Department of Labor, 1977), where over 12,000 occupations were rated by means of extensive onsite observations by qualified job analysts at multiple sites across the United States (Miller et al., 1980). The ratings for each occupation included complexity of work with data, people, and things. (Definitions of the characteristics of work performed at each level of complexity can be found at These measures are intended to reflect the levels of complexity at which a worker in a particular occupation functions in relation to data, people, and things (Miller et al., p. 19). A study using a large sample of the U.S. labor force in 1971 was conducted to assess interrater agreement of the initial ratings of different occupations (Cain & Treiman, 1981). The study yielded reliability estimates around.85 for complexity of work with data,.87 for

4 P254 ANDEL ET AL. Table 3. Proportion of Participants With Main Lifetime Occupations in Each Level of Occupational Complexity (N ¼ 10,079) Dimension % Data Synthesizing 0.5 Coordinating 6.9 Analyzing 25.5 Compiling 16.0 Computing 27.7 Copying 6.2 Comparing 17.2 M 3.0 SD 1.6 People Mentoring 0.4 Negotiating 0.7 Instructing 3.0 Supervising 3.3 Diverting 0.7 Persuading 2.5 Speaking or signaling 22.6 Serving 34.6 Taking instructions 32.2 M 6.3 SD 1.6 Things Setting up 0.0 Precision work 1.7 Operating 23.1 Driving and operating 11.8 Manipulating 16.5 Tending 11.0 Feeding or offbearing 3.8 Handling 32.1 M 4.1 SD 2.2 complexity of work with people, and.46 for complexity of work with things. The scores range from 0.0 to 6.0 for complexity of work with data, 0.0 to 8.0 for the complexity of work with people, and 0.0 to 7.0 for the complexity of work with things. The codes are constructed so that lower scores reflect higher complexity and higher scores reflect lower complexity. Using one occupation as an example, Radio and TV Mechanic is assigned scores of 2.0 for complexity of work with data, 7.7 for people, and 1.2 for things, indicating that the occupation mostly involves analyzing data, taking instructions helping people, and precision working with things. Occupation Telephone Operator is assigned scores of 4.8 for complexity of work with data, 5.8 for people, and 2.3 for things, indicating that the occupation involves copying data, speaking signaling people, and operating controlling things. Table 3 summarizes the distribution of participants in the present study across complexity levels. Average complexity was 3.0 (6 1.6) for complexity of work with data, 6.3 (6 1.6) for people, and 4.1 (6 2.2) for things. In comparison, the averages for the same variables were 3.3 (6 1.9), 6.2 (6 2.0), and 4.5 (6 2.6) in a sample of the U.S. labor force in 1971 (Cain & Treiman, 1981). Score distribution for complexity of work with people was more skewed than score distributions for complexity of work with data or things, which corresponds with findings reported in Miller and associates (1980) and Cain and Treiman (1981). In our study, skewness was.19 for complexity of work with data, 1.77 for complexity of work with people, and.09 for complexity of work with things. We observed significant intercorrelations for all three measures of occupational complexity. Complexity of work with data was positively correlated with complexity of work with people (r ¼.54, p,.05) and things (r ¼.19, p,.05). There was an inverse correlation between complexity of work with people and things (r ¼.34, p,.05). The correlations for the U.S. labor force in 1971 were.56 for complexity of work with data and with people,.04 for complexity of work with data and with things, and.32 for complexity of work with people and with things (Cain & Treiman, 1981). Other measures. We considered age, gender, and level of education to be the main covariates in this study, as these have been identified as risk factors for dementia in a number of research reviews (e.g., Cummings, Vinters, Cole, & Khachaturian, 1998; Hendrie, 1998; Launer et al., 1999). In casecontrol analyses, participants were on average 72.7 years old (range years). Women accounted for 52% of the sample. Table 1 shows tests of the association of each covariate with dementia and with AD. Higher age, female gender, and low educational attainment were risk factors for all types of dementia and for AD. Correlation coefficients between level of education and occupational complexity were.34 for data,.36 people, and.04 for things ( p,.05 for all reported correlations). In our cotwin control analyses, the average age of participants was 80 years (range years), and 50% of the sample were female. Differences between twin cases and cotwin controls on gender and on education were not significant. For the purposes of data analysis, we measured education as basic versus more than basic. Participants described their highest level of education attained, that is, basic education, gymnasium, vocational high school, or university. Basic education corresponded to 6 years or, later, 7 years of school, and it reflected the level of education that was mandatory in Sweden during the first part of the 20th century. In this sample, 55% of the individuals attained this level of basic education, whereas 42% attained more than basic education. Statistical Analyses In our case-control analyses, we used logistic regression models to assess the association between complexity of lifetime occupation and risk of AD and all dementias. We used the LOGISTIC procedure in SAS (version 8.2; SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, NC). Because we included members of complete twin pairs in the analyses in the same way as individual participants, we corrected confidence intervals with robust standard errors for having two members from the same family included, using a program developed for this purpose and described by Moradi and associates (2002). Zygosity did not explain a significant amount of variance in any of the models ( p..30 in all models), and it did not influence the relationship between occupational complexity and risk of dementia or AD only. Therefore, we excluded it from final models. Results show the odds that participants with high levels of occupational complexity in their

5 COMPLEXITY OF WORK AND ALZHEIMER S DISEASE P255 Table 4. Case-Control Analyses of Association Between Occupational Complexity and Risk of Dementia and AD Only Variable OR (95% CI) OR Adjusted for Education (95% CI) Complexity with data All dementias 0.95 ( ) 1.00 ( ) AD only 0.94 ( ) 0.98 ( ) Complexity with people All dementias 0.82 ( ) 0.86 ( ) AD only 0.80 ( ) 0.83 ( ) Complexity with things All dementias 1.07 ( ) 1.07 ( ) AD only 1.06 ( ) 1.06 ( ) Notes: AD¼ Alzheimer s disease; OR ¼ odds ratio; CI ¼ confidence interval. Table data consisted of 225 cases of dementia (9,854 controls) and 146 cases of AD (9,854 controls). CIs correspond to p,.05. ORs were adjusted for age and gender. Table 5. Cotwin Control Analyses of the Association Between Occupational Complexity and Risk of All Types of Dementia and AD Only Variable Pairs Discordant for Complexity No. of Cases Higher No. of Cotwins Higher Crude OR (95% CI) Complexity with data All dementias ( ) AD only ( ) Complexity with people All dementia ( ) AD only ( ) Complexity with things All dementia ( ) AD only ( ) Notes: AD¼ Alzheimer s disease; OR ¼ odds ratio; CI ¼ confidence interval. For dementia, N ¼ 55 pairs; for AD only, N ¼ 28 pairs. CIs correspond to p,.05. main lifetime occupation would be at a reduced risk of all types of dementia and AD only later in life. We calculated the odds while sequentially controlling for age and gender, level of education, and other types of complexity. In our cotwin control analyses, we used conditional logistic regression models in the SAS procedure PHREG. Results show the odds that the twin with a relatively higher complexity of main lifetime occupation is at a reduced risk of dementia or AD compared with the cotwin with a relatively lower complexity. We explored the associations between occupational complexity and risk of dementia in all discordant twin pairs and in discordant monozygotic twin pairs alone. We calculated crude odds ratios and odds ratios controlling for other complexity dimensions. We did not control gender and level of education within these analyses because we had established that twins did not differ significantly on these variables. Most twin pairs were of the same sex. Among opposite-sex pairs, the chances of being demented were equivalent for men and women. Twins also did not differ on age, by definition, and therefore no control for age was warranted. In all analyses, we used a two-tailed level of significance set at p ¼.05. RESULTS Case-Control Analyses Results for the association between dimensions of occupational complexity and risk of dementia and AD only are summarized in Table 4. We found that, when we took age, gender, and level of education into account, higher complexity of work with people in main lifetime occupation was associated with a reduced risk of all types of dementia and AD only. Controlling for age, gender, and level of education, we found that the association between lower complexity of work with things and a reduced risk of dementia was marginally significant ( p ¼.05) but not significant for AD only. When we added complexity of work with data and things into the same model, higher complexity of work with people was protective against AD only (odds ratio or OR ¼ 0.78; 95% confidence interval or CI ¼ 0.63, 0.98). The result for complexity of work with people and risk of all types of dementia was only marginal (OR ¼ 0.84; 95% CI ¼ 0.71, 1.00; p ¼.05), and other complexity dimensions showed no significant associations. No significant findings for complexity of work with data emerged in the case-control analyses. Because the measure of complexity of work with people was skewed, we also conducted the same analyses by using log-transformed complexity measures. The results remained unchanged. Cotwin Control Analyses Results of conditional logistic regression analyses for twins discordant for dementia and those discordant for AD only are summarized in Table 5. Twins with higher scores for complexity of work with people were at a lower risk of dementia and AD only than their cotwins, and twins with higher scores for complexity of work with data were at a lower risk of AD. When we entered all complexity dimensions into one model, higher complexity of work with people between twins was associated with lower risk dementia (OR ¼ 0.44; 95% CI ¼ 0.20, 0.97). We could not perform the parallel analyses with twins discordant for AD because of the small sample size. No other significant results emerged in these analyses. When we considered only monozygotic twin pairs, twins with higher complexity of work with people were at a lower risk of dementia (crude OR ¼ 0.27; 95% CI ¼ 0.08, 0.98). No other results were significant. Results remained unchanged when we added level of education into the models. DISCUSSION Our aim in this study was to examine the association between specific types of occupational complexity and risk of all types of dementia and AD only in a population-based sample of twins. Although most adults spend substantial portions of their lives in activities commonly performed at work, not enough is known about how these work-related activities influence risk of

6 P256 ANDEL ET AL. dementia later in life. We examined the association between three specific types of intellectual demands encountered at work complexity of work with data, people, and things and the risk of dementia and AD. To our knowledge, this is the first study to explore these specific types of occupational complexity in relation to risk of dementia and AD. As far as we know, it is also the first study to use two designs within one study case control and cotwin control to examine the occupation dementia association. Finding similar results in the cotwincontrol study suggests that results from the case-control study cannot be explained by genetic or familial confounding. Our principal finding was that a higher complexity of work with people in the main lifetime occupation was associated with a reduced risk of AD and all types of dementia combined later in life, independent of age, gender, and level of education. The effect was sustained when we also controlled complexity of work with data and things, and when we analyzed data in a cotwin-control design that partially accounted for unmeasured genetic and familial factors shared by twins. In addition, a higher complexity of work with things was associated with a marginal increase in risk of dementia in case-control analyses; furthermore, in cotwin-control analyses, twins with a higher complexity of work with data were at a lower risk of AD than their cotwins. Our findings for complexity of work with people are consistent with those of Stern and associates (1995), who found that interpersonal demands of main lifetime occupation may delay the onset of AD. Smyth and colleagues (2004) did not find significant results for social demands. However, they found significantly higher scores for AD cases compared with control individuals on a factor representing mental occupational demands. Our finding that twins with greater complexity of work with data than their cotwins were at a reduced risk of AD complements this finding. Comparing across studies, however, is somewhat constrained by the use of different occupational variables by different research groups. For example, complexity of work with people in our study includes mental demands related to organizing tasks as well as social demands related to negotiating and supervising. The finding that occupation may influence risk of dementia and AD complements findings from studies with nondemented older adults. Schooler and colleagues (1999, 2004) suggested that exposure to complex environments at work affects the level of intellectual flexibility across the life course. The findings are also consistent with the use it or lose it hypothesis, in which frequent engagement in mental practice is hypothesized to reduce risk of AD and other types of dementia (Katzman, 1995; Orrell & Sahakian, 1995). Further research is needed to discern why occupations that require complex work with people appear to provide a buffer against dementia. Stern and colleagues (1995) attributed their finding of delayed AD onset for occupations with high demands on interpersonal skills to some unknown, unique life experience associated with working at such a job. Another possibility is that work that involves a person socially, regardless of how complex the work is in other respects, may be protective. In the Kungsholmen study, with a case-control design and using incident dementia cases and social network measured at baseline, Fratiglioni and associates (2000) found that having a larger social network was associated with a lower risk of dementia. In a Japanese case-control sample, Kondo, Niino, and Shido (1994) found that reported low social engagement was a risk factor for dementia. Crowe and colleagues (2003) found that intellectual or cultural leisure activities, such as social visits and going to the theater or cinema, reduced risk of dementia in a cotwin-control study. Another possibility may be that greater occupational complexity represents higher occupational status, which would be related to higher income, less exposure to workplace toxins, and better health-related resources. However, it is unlikely that differences among jobs that differ on complexity of work with people reflect differences in exposure to workplace toxins. Our finding that higher complexity of work with things may increase risk of dementia may be more conducive to such an inference. Several limitations should be noted. First, this study is a study of prevalent dementia cases. The use of prevalent dementia cases exposes the results to a potential confounding effect of differential survival as a competing explanation for the association between occupational complexity and the risk of dementia. For example, occupations with a lower degree of substantive complexity (Moore & Hayward, 1990) have been found to be associated with higher mortality. However, if less complex occupations are associated with mortality, odds ratios observed in this study would probably underestimate the strength of the association between complex occupations and the risk of dementia. Second, the sample of twins discordant for dementia included only 17 monozygotic twin pairs, limiting our ability to observe within-twin pair differences. The statistical power to detect significant findings at a p ¼.05 level of significance was 60% or less for the cotwin-control analyses. The fact that we did find significant results in the cotwin-control sample gives credence to the possibility that complex work influences risk of dementia and AD. A post hoc analysis found that the control groups from the case-control analyses and the cotwin-control analyses did not differ significantly on any of the three complexity measures ( p..05), adding confidence to the conclusion that results were parallel across the two designs. Third, even the case-control sample had a relatively small number of cases, which can result in insufficient power and greater risk of Type II error. The power to observe significant results was between 70% and 90%. Fourth, the use of complexity scores from the 1970 U.S. Census for occupations in the 1980 Swedish Census may have resulted in some inaccuracies caused by cross-national differences in occupations. Fifth, informants were used for participants who could not provide information about main lifetime occupation because they were cognitively impaired. Although the use of informant data for cases and some controls (3%) may lead to bias, the bias of using informant-report data has been found to be minimal in previous studies (Demissie et al., 2001; Rocca et al., 1986). This may be particularly true for a very objective variable such as main lifetime occupation. Finally, some members of the HARMONY study could not be interviewed as a result of illness and absence of an informed proxy. Therefore, information about main lifetime occupation of these participants could not be obtained during the telephone screening. Although these individuals were later reached in person or a proxy was interviewed for purposes of clinical diagnosis, they were not included in this study.

7 COMPLEXITY OF WORK AND ALZHEIMER S DISEASE P257 In conclusion, using a population-based sample of twins within case-control and cotwin-control study designs, we found that complexity of work, and particularly complex work with people, can influence risk of dementia and AD later in life, even when age, gender, and level of education were controlled in case-control analyses and when unmeasured factors shared by twins were controlled in cotwin-control analyses. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This research was supported by the National Institute on Aging under Grants R01 AG08724 and P30 AG17265, and by an Alzheimer s Association/Zenith Fellows Award. Address correspondence to Ross Andel, PhD, School of Aging Studies MHC 1321, University of South Florida, 4202 E. Fowler Avenue, Tampa, FL randel@cas.usf.edu REFERENCES American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Azzimondi, G., D Alessandro, R., Pandolfo, G., & Feruglio, F. S. (1998). Comparative study of the prevalence of dementia in two Sicilian communities with different psychological backgrounds. Neuroepidemiology, 17, Cain, P. S., & Treiman, D. J. (1981). The Dictionary of Occupational Titles as a source of occupational data. American Sociological Review, 46, Canadian Study of Health and Aging. (1994). The Canadian Study of Health and Aging: Risk factors for Alzheimer s disease in Canada. Neurology, 44, Churchill, J. D., Galvez, R., Colcombe, S., Swain, R. A., Kramer, A. F., & Greenough, W. T. (2002). Exercise, experience and the aging brain. Neurobiology of Aging, 23, Crowe, M., Andel, R., Gatz, M., Pedersen, N. L., & Johansson, B. (2003). Does participation in leisure activities lead to a reduced risk of dementia? Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 58B, P249 P255. Cummings, J. L., Vinters, H. V., Cole, G. M., & Khachaturian, Z. S. (1998). Alzheimer s disease: Etiologies, pathophysiology, cognitive reserve, and treatment opportunities. Neurology, 51(Suppl. 1), S2 S17. Demissie, S., Green, R. C., Mucci, L., Tziavas, S., Martelli, K., Bang, K., et al. (2001). Reliability of information collected by proxy in family studies of Alzheimer s disease. Neuroepidemiology, 20, Evans, D. A., Hebert, L. E., Beckett, L. A., Scherr, P. A., Albert, M. S., Chown, M. J., et al. (1997). Education and other measures of socioeconomic status and risk of incident Alzheimer disease in a defined population of older persons. Archives of Neurology, 54, Farrer, L. A., Cupples, L. A., Haines, J. L., Hyman, B., Kukull, W. A., Mayeux, R., et al. (1997). Effects of age, sex, and ethnicity on the association between Apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer disease: A meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association, 278, Fratiglioni, L., Ahlbom, A., Viitanen, M., & Winblad, B. (1993). Risk factors for late-onset Alzheimer s disease: A population-based, casecontrol study. Annals of Neurology, 33, Fratiglioni, L., Paillard-Borg, S., & Winblad, B. (2004). An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurology, 3, Fratiglioni, L., Wang, H.-X., Ericsson, K., Maytan, M., & Winblad, B. (2000). Influence of social network on occurrence of dementia: A community-based longitudinal study. The Lancet, 355, Gatz, M., Fratiglioni, L., Johansson, B., Berg, S., Mortimer, J. A., Reynolds, C.A., et al. (2005). Complete ascertainment of dementia in the Swedish Twin Registry: the HARMONY study. Neurobiology of Aging, 26, Gatz, M., Reynolds, C., John, R., Johansson, B., Mortimer, J., & Pedersen, N. L. (2002). Telephone screening to identify potential dementia cases in a population-based sample of older adults. International Psychogeriatrics, 14, Gatz, M., Reynolds, C., Nikolic, J., Lowe, B., & Karel, M. (1995). An empirical test of telephone screening to identify potential dementia cases. International Psychogeriatrics, 9, Gatz, M., Svedberg, P., Pedersen, N. L., Mortimer, J. A., Berg, S., Johansson, B. (2001). Education and the risk of Alzheimer s Disease: Findings from the Study of Dementia in Swedish Twins. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 56B, P292 P300. Helmer, C., Letenneur, L., Rouch, I., Richard-Harston, S., Barberger- Gateau, P., Fabrigoule, C., et al. (2001). Occupation during life and risk of dementia in French elderly community residents. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 71, Hendrie, H. C. (1998). Epidemiology of dementia and Alzheimer s disease. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 6, S3 S18. Henig, R. M. (1994, April, 24). Is misplacing your glasses Alzheimer s? New York Times Sunday Magazine, Katzman, R. (1995). Can late life social and leisure activities delay the onset of dementia? Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 43, Kohn, M. L., & Schooler, C. (1983). Work and personality: An inquiry into the impact of social stratification. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. Q. (1998). Brain plasticity and behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, Kondo, K., Niino, M., & Shido, K. (1994). A case-control study of Alzheimer s disease in Japan Significance of life-styles. Dementia, 5, Launer, L. J., Andersen, K., Dewey, M. E., Letenneur, L., Ott, A., Amaducci, L. A., et al. (1999). Rates and risk factors for dementia and Alzheimer s disease: Results from EURODEM pooled analyses. Neurology, 52, Lichtenstein, P., De Faire, U., Floderus, B., Svartengren, M., Svedberg, P., & Pedersen, N. L. (2002). The Swedish Twin Registry: A unique source for clinical, epidemiological, and genetic studies. Journal of Internal Medicine, 252, Lund and Manchester Groups. (1994). Clinical and neuropathological criteria for frontotemporal dementia. Journal of Neurological and Neurosurgical Psychiatry, 57, McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., & Stadlan, E. M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer s disease. Neurology, 34, McKeith, I. G., Galasko, D., Kosaka, K., Perry, E. K., Dickson, D. W., Hansen, L. A., et al. (1996). Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathologic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): Report of the Consortium on DLB International Workshop. Neurology, 47, Miller, A. R., Treiman, D. J., Cain, P. S., & Roos, P. A. (1980). Work, jobs, and occupations: A critical review of occupational titles. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Moradi, T., Adami, H.-O., Ekbom, A., Wedrén, S., Terry, P., Floderus, B., et al. (2002). Physical activity and risk for breast cancer a prospective cohort study among Swedish twins. International Journal of Cancer, 100, Moore, D. E., & Hayward, M. D. (1990). Occupational careers and mortality of elderly men. Demography, 27, Neary, D., Snowdon, J. S., Gustafson, L., Passant, U., Stuss, D., Black, S., et al. (1998). Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology, 51, Orrell, M., & Sahakian, B. (1995). Education and dementia. British Journal of Medicine, 310, Paykel, E. S., Brayne, C., Huppert, F. A., Gill, C., Barkley, C., Gehlhaar, E., et al. (1994). Incidence of dementia in a population older than 75 years in the United Kingdom. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, Pedersen, N. L., Plomin, R., Nesselroade, J. R., & McClearn, G. E. (1992). A quantitative genetic analysis of cognitive abilities during the second half of the life span. Psychological Science, 3, Qui, C., Karp, A., von Strauss, E., Winblad, B., Fratiglioni, L., & Bellander, T. (2003). Lifetime principal occupation and risk of Alzheimer s disease in the Kungsholmen project. American Journal of Industrial Medicine, 43, Ravaglia, G., Forti, P., Maioli, F., Sacchetti, L., Mariani, E., Nativio, V., et al. (2002). Education, occupation, and prevalence of dementia: Findings from the Conselice Study. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 14,

8 P258 ANDEL ET AL. Rocca, W. A., Fratiglioni, L., Bracco, L., Pedone, D., Groppi, C., & Schoenberg, B. S. (1986). The use of surrogate respondents to obtain questionnaire data in case-control studies of neurologic diseases. Journal of Chronic Diseases, 39, Roman, G. C., Tatemichi, T. K., Erkinjuntti, T., Cummings, J. L., Masdeu, J. C., Garcia, J. H., et al. (1993). Vascular dementia: Diagnostic criteria for research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop. Neurology, 43, Roos, P. A., & Treiman, D. J. (1980). DOT scales for the 1970 Census classification. In A. R. Miller, D. J. Treiman, P. S Cain, & P. A. Roos (Eds.), Work, jobs, and occupations: A critical review of occupational titles (pp ). Washington, DC: National Academy Press. SAS Institute. (2003). SAS System for Microsoft Windows (Version 9) [Computer software]. Cary, NC: SAS Institute, Inc. Salthouse, T. A., Berish, B. E., & Miles, J. D. (2002). The role of cognitive stimulation on the relations between age and cognitive functioning. Psychology and Aging, 17, Scarmeas, N., & Stern, Y. (2003). Cognitive reserve and lifestyle. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 54, Schooler, C. (1984). Psychological effects of complex environment during the life span: A review and theory. Intelligence, 8, Schooler, C., Mulatu, M. S., & Oates, G. (1999). The continuing effects of substantively complex work on the intellectual functioning of older workers. Psychology and Aging, 14, Schooler, C., Mulatu, M. S., & Oates, G. (2004). Occupational selfdirection, intellectual functioning, and self-directed orientation in older workers: Findings and implications for individuals and societies. American Journal of Sociology, 110, 161. Smyth, K. A., Fritsch, T., Cook, T. B., McClendon, M. J., Santillan, C. E., & Friedland, R. P. (2004). Worker functions and traits associated with occupations and the development of AD. Neurology, 63, Statistics Sweden. (1980). Folk och Bostadsräkningen 1980 [Population and housing census 1980]. Stockholm, Sweden: Statistiska centralbyrån. Stern, Y., Alexander, G. E., Prohovnik, I., Stricks, L., Link, B., Lennon, M. C., et al. (1995). Relationship between lifetime occupation and parietal flow: Implications for a reserve against Alzheimer s disease pathology. Neurology, 45, Stern, Y., Gurland, B., Tatemichi, T. K., Tang, M. X., Wilder, D., & Mayeux, R. (1994). Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer s disease. Journal of the American Medical Association, 271, U.S. Bureau of the Census. (1973). Census of population 1970 (Subject Reports, Final Report PC2-7A: Occupational characteristics). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. U.S. Department of Labor. (1965). Dictionary of occupational titles (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. U.S. Department of Labor. (1977). Dictionary of occupational titles (4th ed.). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Wilson, R. S., Mendes de Leon, C. F., Barnes, L. L., Schneider, J. A., Bienias, J. L., & Evans, D. A., et al. (2002). Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. Journal of the American Medical Association, 287, Received November 29, 2004 Accepted April 22, 2005 Decision Editor: Thomas M. Hess, PhD

The effect of education and occupational complexity on rate of cognitive decline in Alzheimer s patients

The effect of education and occupational complexity on rate of cognitive decline in Alzheimer s patients Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society (2006), 12, 147 152. Copyright 2006 INS. Published by Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. DOI: 10.10170S1355617706060206 BRIEF COMMUNICATION

More information

Mid- and Late-life Diabetes in Relation to the Risk of Dementia: A Population-based Twin Study

Mid- and Late-life Diabetes in Relation to the Risk of Dementia: A Population-based Twin Study Diabetes Publish Ahead of Print, published online October 24, 2008 Mid- and Late-life Diabetes in Relation to the Risk of Dementia: A Population-based Twin Study Weili Xu, 1 MD, PhD; Chengxuan Qiu, 1 MD,

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Metab Brain Dis. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 October 24.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Metab Brain Dis. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 October 24. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Metab Brain Dis. 2006 September ; 21(2-3): 235 240. doi:10.1007/s11011-006-9017-2. Risk factors for incident Alzheimer s disease in

More information

Research Article Playing a Musical Instrument as a Protective Factor against Dementia and Cognitive Impairment: A Population-Based Twin Study

Research Article Playing a Musical Instrument as a Protective Factor against Dementia and Cognitive Impairment: A Population-Based Twin Study Hindawi Publishing Corporation International Journal of Alzheimer s Disease Volume 2014, Article ID 836748, 6 pages http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2014/836748 Research Article Playing a Musical Instrument as

More information

Relation of Education and Occupation-based Socioeconomic Status to Incident Alzheimer s Disease

Relation of Education and Occupation-based Socioeconomic Status to Incident Alzheimer s Disease American Journal of Epidemiology Copyright 2004 by the Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health All rights reserved Vol. 159, No. 2 Printed in U.S.A. DOI: 10.1093/aje/kwh018 Relation of Education

More information

Detection of Dementia Cases in Two Swedish Health Registers: A Validation Study

Detection of Dementia Cases in Two Swedish Health Registers: A Validation Study Journal of Alzheimer s Disease 61 (2018) 1301 1310 DOI 10.3233/JAD-170572 IOS Press 1301 Detection of Dementia Cases in Two Swedish Health Registers: A Validation Study Debora Rizzuto a,, Adina L. Feldman

More information

Baseline Characteristics of Patients Attending the Memory Clinic Serving the South Shore of Boston

Baseline Characteristics of Patients Attending the   Memory Clinic Serving the South Shore of Boston Article ID: ISSN 2046-1690 Baseline Characteristics of Patients Attending the www.thealzcenter.org Memory Clinic Serving the South Shore of Boston Corresponding Author: Dr. Anil K Nair, Chief of Neurology,

More information

With increased life expectancy, the incidence of

With increased life expectancy, the incidence of Eriksson Sörman, D., Sundström, A., Rönnlund, M., Adolfsson, R., & Nilsson, L.G. (2014). Leisure activity in old age and risk of dementia: a 15-year prospective study. Journals of Gerontology, Series B:

More information

Examining the influence of gender, education, social class and birth cohort on MMSE tracking over time: a population-based prospective cohort study

Examining the influence of gender, education, social class and birth cohort on MMSE tracking over time: a population-based prospective cohort study Matthews et al. BMC Geriatrics 2012, 12:45 RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access Examining the influence of gender, education, social class and birth cohort on MMSE tracking over time: a population-based prospective

More information

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. APOE Genotype, Family History of Dementia, and Alzheimer Disease Risk

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. APOE Genotype, Family History of Dementia, and Alzheimer Disease Risk ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION APOE Genotype, Family History of Dementia, and Alzheimer Disease Risk A 6-Year Follow-up Study Wenyong Huang, MD; Chengxuan Qiu, MD, PhD; Eva von Strauss, PhD; Bengt Winblad, MD,

More information

Clinical Study Depressive Symptom Clusters and Neuropsychological Performance in Mild Alzheimer s and Cognitively Normal Elderly

Clinical Study Depressive Symptom Clusters and Neuropsychological Performance in Mild Alzheimer s and Cognitively Normal Elderly Hindawi Publishing Corporation Depression Research and Treatment Volume 2011, Article ID 396958, 6 pages doi:10.1155/2011/396958 Clinical Study Depressive Symptom Clusters and Neuropsychological Performance

More information

Validity of Family History for the Diagnosis of Dementia Among Siblings of Patients With Late-onset Alzheimer s Disease

Validity of Family History for the Diagnosis of Dementia Among Siblings of Patients With Late-onset Alzheimer s Disease Genetic Epidemiology 15:215 223 (1998) Validity of Family History for the Diagnosis of Dementia Among Siblings of Patients With Late-onset Alzheimer s Disease G. Devi, 1,3 * K. Marder, 1,3 P.W. Schofield,

More information

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Diagnostic Validity of the Dementia Questionnaire for Alzheimer Disease

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Diagnostic Validity of the Dementia Questionnaire for Alzheimer Disease ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION Diagnostic Validity of the Dementia Questionnaire for Alzheimer Disease Ronald J. Ellis, MD, PhD; Kaining Jan, MD; Claudia Kawas, MD; William C. Koller, MD; Kelly E. Lyons, PhD; Dilip

More information

Citation for the original published paper (version of record):

Citation for the original published paper (version of record): http://www.diva-portal.org This is the published version of a paper published in European Journal of Ageing. Citation for the original published paper (version of record): Ericsson, M C., Gatz, M., Kåreholt,

More information

Risk Factors for Alzheimer s Disease: A Prospective Analysis from the Canadian Study of Health and Aging

Risk Factors for Alzheimer s Disease: A Prospective Analysis from the Canadian Study of Health and Aging American Journal of Epidemiology Copyright 2002 by the Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health All rights reserved Vol. 156, No. 5 Printed in U.S.A. DOI: 10.1093/aje/kwf074 Risk Factors for Alzheimer

More information

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Change in Cognitive Function in Older Persons From a Community Population

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Change in Cognitive Function in Older Persons From a Community Population Change in Cognitive Function in Older Persons From a Community Population Relation to Age and Alzheimer Disease ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION Robert S. Wilson, PhD; Laurel A. Beckett, PhD; David A. Bennett, MD;

More information

Following in Your Father s Footsteps: A Note on the Intergenerational Transmission of Income between Twin Fathers and their Sons

Following in Your Father s Footsteps: A Note on the Intergenerational Transmission of Income between Twin Fathers and their Sons D I S C U S S I O N P A P E R S E R I E S IZA DP No. 5990 Following in Your Father s Footsteps: A Note on the Intergenerational Transmission of Income between Twin Fathers and their Sons Vikesh Amin Petter

More information

Vascular dementia (VaD) is preceded by several years of

Vascular dementia (VaD) is preceded by several years of Cognitive Functioning in Preclinical Vascular Dementia A 6-Year Follow-Up Erika Jonsson Laukka, MSc; Sari Jones, MSc; Laura Fratiglioni, MD, PhD; Lars Bäckman, PhD Background and Purpose Recent studies

More information

Social Capital as a Protective Factor Against Cognitive Decline and Dementia

Social Capital as a Protective Factor Against Cognitive Decline and Dementia Social Capital as a Protective Factor Against Cognitive Decline and Dementia Nicole D. Anderson, PhD, CPsych Senior Scientist, Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Associate Professor, Psychiatry & Psychology,

More information

Challenges in design and analysis of large register-based epidemiological studies

Challenges in design and analysis of large register-based epidemiological studies FMS/DSBS autumn meeting 2014 Challenges in design and analysis of large register-based epidemiological studies Caroline Weibull & Anna Johansson Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics (MEB)

More information

Risk Factors for Vascular Dementia: A Hospital-Based Study in Taiwan

Risk Factors for Vascular Dementia: A Hospital-Based Study in Taiwan 22 Risk Factors for Vascular Dementia: A Hospital-Based Study in Taiwan Jun-Cheng Lin 1,2, Wen-Chuin Hsu 1, Hai-Pei Hsu 1,2, Hon-Chung Fung 1, and Sien-Tsong Chen 1 Abstract- Background: In Taiwan, next

More information

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity Behav. Sci. 2013, 3, 170 191; doi:10.3390/bs3010170 Article OPEN ACCESS behavioral sciences ISSN 2076-328X www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living:

More information

Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia

Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia Neuropsychologia 45 (2007) 459 464 Note Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia Ellen Bialystok a,b,, Fergus I.M. Craik b,c, Morris Freedman b,d,e a Department of Psychology,

More information

Childhood school performance, education and occupational complexity: a life-course study of dementia in the Kungsholmen Project

Childhood school performance, education and occupational complexity: a life-course study of dementia in the Kungsholmen Project International Journal of Epidemiology, 2016, 1207 1215 doi: 10.1093/ije/dyw008 Advance Access Publication Date: 10 March 2016 Original article Lifetime determinants of cognitive ageing, dementia and early

More information

The validity of national hospital discharge register data on dementia: a comparative analysis using clinical data from a university medical centre

The validity of national hospital discharge register data on dementia: a comparative analysis using clinical data from a university medical centre ORIGINAL ARTICLE The validity of national hospital discharge register data on dementia: a comparative analysis using clinical data from a university medical centre I.E. van de Vorst,, *, I. Vaartjes, L.F.

More information

EFFECT OF DIFFERENT DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA ON THE PREVALENCE OF DEMENTIA. Special Article

EFFECT OF DIFFERENT DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA ON THE PREVALENCE OF DEMENTIA. Special Article EFFECT OF DIFFERENT DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA ON THE PREVALENCE OF DEMENTIA Special Article THE EFFECT OF DIFFERENT DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA ON THE PREVALENCE OF DEMENTIA TIMO ERKINJUNTTI, M.D., PH.D., TRULS ØSTBYE,

More information

eappendix S1. Studies and participants

eappendix S1. Studies and participants eappendix S1. Studies and participants Eligible population from 11 cohort studies N = 96,211 Excluded: Missing data on exposure or outcome N = 6047 Analytic sample for study of minimally adjusted ERI-

More information

The Author Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of The Gerontological Society of America.

The Author Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of The Gerontological Society of America. Journal of Gerontology: MEDICAL SCIENCES The Author 2009. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of The Gerontological Society of America. Cite journal as: J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci All rights

More information

Heritability for Alzheimer's Disease: The Study of Dementia in Swedish Twins

Heritability for Alzheimer's Disease: The Study of Dementia in Swedish Twins Journal of Gerontology: MEDICAL SCIENCES 1997, Vol. 2A, No. 2, M117-M12 Copyright 1997 by The Cerontological Society of America Heritability for Alzheimer's Disease: The Study of Dementia in Swedish Twins

More information

Leisure Activities and the Risk of Dementia in the Elderly

Leisure Activities and the Risk of Dementia in the Elderly The new england journal of medicine original article Leisure Activities and the Risk of Dementia in the Elderly Joe Verghese, M.D., Richard B. Lipton, M.D., Mindy J. Katz, M.P.H., Charles B. Hall, Ph.D.,

More information

Incidence and Risk of Dementia

Incidence and Risk of Dementia American Journal of Epidemiology Copyright O 1998 by The Johns Hopkins University School of Hygiene and Public Health All rights reserved Vol. 147, No. 6 Printed In USA. Incidence and Risk of Dementia

More information

Influences of Preclinical Dementia and Impending Death on the Magnitude of Age-Related Cognitive Deficits

Influences of Preclinical Dementia and Impending Death on the Magnitude of Age-Related Cognitive Deficits Psychology and Aging Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2002, Vol. 17, No. 3, 435 442 0882-7974/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0882-7974.17.3.435 Influences of Preclinical Dementia

More information

Incidence of Dementia and Alzheimer Disease in 2 Communities

Incidence of Dementia and Alzheimer Disease in 2 Communities ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION Incidence of Dementia and Alzheimer Disease in 2 Communities Residing in Ibadan, Nigeria, and Residing in Indianapolis, Indiana Hugh C. Hendrie, MB, ChB Adesola Ogunniyi, MD Kathleen

More information

IT IS WELL known that individuals with dementia. Classifying eating-related problems among institutionalized people with dementia.

IT IS WELL known that individuals with dementia. Classifying eating-related problems among institutionalized people with dementia. doi:10.1111/pcn.12375 Regular Article Classifying eating-related problems among institutionalized people with dementia Shunichiro Shinagawa, MD, PhD, 1 * Kazuki Honda, MD, PhD, 2 Tetsuo Kashibayashi, MD,

More information

EMPIRICAL ISSUES IN THE STUDY OF COGNITIVE AGING THROUGH POPULATION-BASED STUDIES

EMPIRICAL ISSUES IN THE STUDY OF COGNITIVE AGING THROUGH POPULATION-BASED STUDIES EMPIRICAL ISSUES IN THE STUDY OF COGNITIVE AGING THROUGH POPULATION-BASED STUDIES Silvia Mejía-Arango* and Brian Downer** *El Colegio de la Frontera Norte, Tijuana, Mexico **University of Texas Medical

More information

Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study

Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study Diabetologia (2013) 56:1031 1035 DOI 10.1007/s00125-013-2859-3 SHORT COMMUNICATION Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study S. Carlsson & A. Ahlbom & P. Lichtenstein

More information

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Apolipoprotein E 4 and Age at Onset of Sporadic and Familial Alzheimer Disease in Caribbean Hispanics

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Apolipoprotein E 4 and Age at Onset of Sporadic and Familial Alzheimer Disease in Caribbean Hispanics ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION Apolipoprotein E 4 and Age at Onset of Sporadic and Familial Alzheimer Disease in Caribbean Hispanics Lucia Olarte, BS; Nicole Schupf, PhD; Joseph H. Lee, DPH; Ming-Xin Tang, PhD;

More information

THERE has been considerable interest in the ability to

THERE has been considerable interest in the ability to Journal of Gerontology: PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCES 2007, Vol. 62B, No. 6, 000 000 Copyright 2007 by The Gerontological Society of America The Power of Personality in Discriminating Between Healthy Aging and

More information

Diversity and Dementia

Diversity and Dementia Diversity and Dementia Kala M. Mehta, DSc, MPH January 17, 2012 Overview Background Incidence and Prevalence of Dementia Why are these differences found? What s important for diverse dementia patients

More information

The 9 year cognitive decline before dementia of the Alzheimer type: a prospective population-based study

The 9 year cognitive decline before dementia of the Alzheimer type: a prospective population-based study doi:10.103/brain/awh41 Brain (200), 12, 103 1101 The year cognitive decline before dementia of the Alzheimer type: a prospective population-based study Hélène Amieva, 1 Hélène Jacqmin-Gadda, 2 Jean-Marc

More information

Early-onset dementia. Chapter 2

Early-onset dementia. Chapter 2 Early-onset dementia Chapter 2 21 22 Early-onset dementia is associated with higher mortality Chapter 2.1 E.L.G.E. Koedam Y.A.L. Pijnenburg D.J.H. Deeg M.M.E. Baak A.E. van der Vlies P. Scheltens W.M.

More information

Mini-Mental State Examination Item Scores as Predictors of Alzheimer's Disease: Incidence Data From the Kungsholmen Project, Stockholm

Mini-Mental State Examination Item Scores as Predictors of Alzheimer's Disease: Incidence Data From the Kungsholmen Project, Stockholm Journal of Gerontology: MEDICAL SCIENCES 1997, Vol. 52A. No. 5, M299-M304 Copyright 1997 by The Gemntological Society of America Mini-Mental State Examination Item Scores as Predictors of Alzheimer's Disease:

More information

I n the past three decades various cognitive screening

I n the past three decades various cognitive screening 700 PAPER The seven minute screen: a neurocognitive screening test highly sensitive to various types of dementia E F J Meulen, B Schmand, J P van Campen, S J de Koning, R W Ponds, P Scheltens, F R Verhey...

More information

Dr Veenu Gupta MD MRCPsych Consultant, Child Psychiatrist Stockton on Tees, UK

Dr Veenu Gupta MD MRCPsych Consultant, Child Psychiatrist Stockton on Tees, UK Dr Veenu Gupta MD MRCPsych Consultant, Child Psychiatrist Stockton on Tees, UK Extremely Preterm-EP Very Preterm-VP Preterm-P Late Preterm-LP There is greater improvement of survival at extremely low

More information

Brain Bank of the Brazilian Aging Brain Study Group a Collection Focused in Human Control Cases

Brain Bank of the Brazilian Aging Brain Study Group a Collection Focused in Human Control Cases Brain Bank of the Brazilian Aging Brain Study Group a Collection Focused in Human Control Cases Lea Tenenholz Grinberg, M.D, Ph.D Associate Professor of Pathology University of the City of São Paulo Medical

More information

Clinical Diagnosis. Step 1: Dementia or not? Diagnostic criteria for dementia (DSM-IV)

Clinical Diagnosis. Step 1: Dementia or not? Diagnostic criteria for dementia (DSM-IV) Step 1: Dementia or not? Diagnostic criteria for dementia (DSM-IV) A. The development of multiple cognitive deficits manifested by both 1 and 2 1 1. Memory impairment 2. One (or more) of the following

More information

The importance of cognitive ageing for understanding dementia

The importance of cognitive ageing for understanding dementia The importance of cognitive ageing for understanding dementia Archana Singh-Manoux, PhD 1,2,3 Mika Kivimäki, PhD 2 * Corresponding author & address: 1 Centre for Research in Epidemiology & Population Health,

More information

Gambling and gambling problems in Sweden Results from a one-year follow-up

Gambling and gambling problems in Sweden Results from a one-year follow-up Gambling and gambling problems in Sweden 2009-2010 Results from a one-year follow-up Summary Results from a one-year follow-up Since the end of the 1990s, prevalence studies have become increasingly common.

More information

Moderate alcohol consumption and risk of developing dementia in the elderly: the contribution of prospective studies.

Moderate alcohol consumption and risk of developing dementia in the elderly: the contribution of prospective studies. Moderate alcohol consumption and risk of developing dementia in the elderly: the contribution of prospective studies. Luc Letenneur To cite this version: Luc Letenneur. Moderate alcohol consumption and

More information

The Swedish Twin Registry in the Third Millennium: An Update

The Swedish Twin Registry in the Third Millennium: An Update The Swedish Twin Registry in the Third Millennium: An Update Paul Lichtenstein 1, Patrick F. Sullivan 1,2, Sven Cnattingius 1, Margaret Gatz 3,1, Sofie Johansson 1, Eva Carlström 1, Camilla Björk 1, Magnus

More information

Exploring the Neural Basis of Cognitive Reserve

Exploring the Neural Basis of Cognitive Reserve Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 1380-3395/03/2505-691$16.00 2003, Vol. 25, No. 5, pp. 691 701 # Swets & Zeitlinger Exploring the Neural Basis of Cognitive Reserve Yaakov Stern, Eric

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY APPENDIX

SUPPLEMENTARY APPENDIX Type 2 diabetes as a risk factor for dementia in women compared with men: a pooled analysis of 23 million people and more than 100,000 cases of dementia SUPPLEMENTARY APPENDIX Supplementary Methods Newcastle

More information

Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised*

Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised* The Clinical Neuropsychologist -//-$., Vol., No., pp. - Swets & Zeitlinger Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised* Rodney D. Vanderploeg, John A. Schinka, Tatyana Jones, Brent J. Small,

More information

Estimating the Validity of the Korean Version of Expanded Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) Scale

Estimating the Validity of the Korean Version of Expanded Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) Scale Estimating the Validity of the Korean Version of Expanded Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) Scale Seong Hye Choi, M.D.*, Duk L. Na, M.D., Byung Hwa Lee, M.A., Dong-Seog Hahm, M.D., Jee Hyang Jeong, M.D.,

More information

Complexity of lifetime occupation and cognitive performance in old age

Complexity of lifetime occupation and cognitive performance in old age Occupational Medicine 2013;63:556 562 Advance Access publication 9 November 2013 doi:10.1093/occmed/kqt115 Complexity of lifetime occupation and cognitive performance in old age P. C. Correa Ribeiro 1,

More information

Frontotemporal dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies in a case-control study of Alzheimer s disease

Frontotemporal dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies in a case-control study of Alzheimer s disease International Psychogeriatrics: page 1 of 8 C 2009 International Psychogeriatric Association doi:10.1017/s1041610209009454 Frontotemporal dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies in a case-control study

More information

Diabetes and Risk of Dementia Luchsinger et al. Diabetes Mellitus and Risk of Alzheimer s Disease and Dementia with Stroke in a Multiethnic Cohort

Diabetes and Risk of Dementia Luchsinger et al. Diabetes Mellitus and Risk of Alzheimer s Disease and Dementia with Stroke in a Multiethnic Cohort American Journal of Epidemiology Copyright 2001 by the Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health All rights reserved Vol. 154, No. 7 Printed in U.S.A. Diabetes and Risk of Dementia Luchsinger

More information

T H E B E T T E R H E A L T H N E W S

T H E B E T T E R H E A L T H N E W S Dr. Paul G. Varnas & WholeHealthAmerica.com present V O L U M E 7, I S S U E 2 T H E B E T T E R H E A L T H N E W S F E B R U A R Y, 2 0 1 0 D E M E N T I A A N D E X E R C I S E L E T S H E A R I T F

More information

REGULAR RESEARCH ARTICLES

REGULAR RESEARCH ARTICLES REGULAR RESEARCH ARTICLES Total Scores of the CERAD Neuropsychological Assessment Battery: Validation for Mild Cognitive Impairment and Dementia Patients With Diverse Etiologies Eun Hyun Seo, M.A., Dong

More information

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY 6.1 Research Design Research is an organized, systematic, data based, critical, objective, scientific inquiry or investigation into a specific problem, undertaken with the

More information

Papers. Smoking and dementia in male British doctors: prospective study. Abstract. Subjects and methods. Introduction

Papers. Smoking and dementia in male British doctors: prospective study. Abstract. Subjects and methods. Introduction Smoking and dementia in male British doctors: prospective study Richard Doll, Richard Peto, Jillian Boreham, Isabelle Sutherland Abstract Objective To assess the possible association between smoking and

More information

The Cross-sectional Association Between Blood Pressure and Alzheimer s Disease in a Biracial Community Population of Older Persons

The Cross-sectional Association Between Blood Pressure and Alzheimer s Disease in a Biracial Community Population of Older Persons Journal of Gerontology: MEDICAL SCIENCES 2000, Vol. 55A, No. 3, M130 M136 Copyright 2000 by The Gerontological Society of America The Cross-sectional Association Between Blood Pressure and Alzheimer s

More information

Buy full version here - for $ 7.00

Buy full version here - for $ 7.00 This is a Sample version of the Apathy Evaluation Scale (AES) The full version of watermark.. the AES comes without sample The full complete 40 page version includes AES Overview information AES Scoring/

More information

Papers. Detection of Alzheimer s disease and dementia in the preclinical phase: population based cohort study. Abstract.

Papers. Detection of Alzheimer s disease and dementia in the preclinical phase: population based cohort study. Abstract. Detection of Alzheimer s disease and dementia in the preclinical phase: population based cohort study Katie Palmer, Lars Bäckman, Bengt Winblad, Laura Fratiglioni Abstract Objectives To evaluate a simple

More information

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Comparison of the Short Test of Mental Status and the Mini-Mental State Examination in Mild Cognitive Impairment

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Comparison of the Short Test of Mental Status and the Mini-Mental State Examination in Mild Cognitive Impairment ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION Comparison of the Short Test of Mental Status and the Mini-Mental State Examination in Mild Cognitive Impairment David F. Tang-Wai, MDCM; David S. Knopman, MD; Yonas E. Geda, MD;

More information

Heterogeneity in Risk Factors for Cognitive Impairment, No Dementia: Population- Based Longitudinal Study From the Kungsholmen Project

Heterogeneity in Risk Factors for Cognitive Impairment, No Dementia: Population- Based Longitudinal Study From the Kungsholmen Project Heterogeneity in Risk Factors for Cognitive Impairment, No Dementia: Population- Based Longitudinal Study From the Kungsholmen Project Roberto Monastero, M.D., Ph.D., Katie Palmer, M.P.H., Ph.D., Chengxuan

More information

Incidence of Dementia over Three Decades in the Framingham Heart Study

Incidence of Dementia over Three Decades in the Framingham Heart Study The new england journal of medicine Original Article Incidence of Dementia over Three Decades in the Framingham Heart Study Claudia L. Satizabal, Ph.D., Alexa S. Beiser, Ph.D., Vincent Chouraki, M.D.,

More information

Cognitive ageing and dementia: The Whitehall II Study

Cognitive ageing and dementia: The Whitehall II Study Cognitive ageing and dementia: The Whitehall II Study Archana SINGH-MANOUX NIH: R01AG013196; R01AG034454; R01AG056477 MRC: K013351, MR/R024227 BHF: RG/13/2/30098 H2020: #643576 #633666 Outline Lifecourse

More information

Research Article Hypertension and Dementia in the Elderly: The Leisure World Cohort Study

Research Article Hypertension and Dementia in the Elderly: The Leisure World Cohort Study International Hypertension Volume 2012, Article ID 205350, 5 pages doi:10.1155/2012/205350 Research Article Hypertension and Dementia in the Elderly: The Leisure World Cohort Study Annlia Paganini-Hill

More information

Alzheimer s Disease. Clinical characteristics of late-onset Alzheimer s disease (LOAD) A/Prof David Darby

Alzheimer s Disease. Clinical characteristics of late-onset Alzheimer s disease (LOAD) A/Prof David Darby Alzheimer s Disease Clinical characteristics of late-onset Alzheimer s disease (LOAD) A/Prof David Darby Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health 28-6-2013 The burden of late-onset Alzheimer

More information

R esults from several recent studies point to an association

R esults from several recent studies point to an association 962 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Psychosocial work factors and dementia A Seidler, A Nienhaus, T Bernhardt, T Kauppinen, A-L Elo, L Frölich... See end of article for authors affiliations... Correspondence to: Dr A

More information

Predicting Offspring Conduct Disorder Using Parental Alcohol and Drug Dependence

Predicting Offspring Conduct Disorder Using Parental Alcohol and Drug Dependence Predicting Offspring Conduct Disorder Using Parental Alcohol and Drug Dependence Paul T. Korte, B.A. J. Randolph Haber, Ph.D. Abstract Introduction: Previous research has shown that the offspring of parents

More information

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION Patient s Rating of Cognitive Ability Using the AD8, a Brief Informant Interview, as a Self-rating Tool to Detect Dementia James E. Galvin, MD, MPH; Catherine M. Roe, PhD; Mary A.

More information

Fatal primary malignancy of brain. Glioblasatoma, histologically

Fatal primary malignancy of brain. Glioblasatoma, histologically TABLE 10.2 TBI and Brain Tumors Reference Study Design Population Type of TBI Health s or Annegers et al., 1979 Burch et al., 1987 Carpenter et al., 1987 Hochberg et al., 1984 Double cohort All TBI in

More information

Quality ID #282: Dementia: Functional Status Assessment National Quality Strategy Domain: Effective Clinical Care

Quality ID #282: Dementia: Functional Status Assessment National Quality Strategy Domain: Effective Clinical Care Quality ID #282: Dementia: Functional Status Assessment National Quality Strategy Domain: Effective Clinical Care 2018 OPTIONS FOR INDIVIDUAL MEASURES: REGISTRY ONLY MEASURE TYPE: Process DESCRIPTION:

More information

The course of neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia. Part II: relationships among behavioural sub-syndromes and the influence of clinical variables

The course of neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia. Part II: relationships among behavioural sub-syndromes and the influence of clinical variables INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF GERIATRIC PSYCHIATRY Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2005; 20: 531 536. Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/gps.1317 The course of neuropsychiatric

More information

Studies have shown that familial aggregation is of

Studies have shown that familial aggregation is of No Evidence of Sex Differences in Heritability of Irritable Bowel Syndrome in Swedish Twins Pia Svedberg, 1 Saga Johansson, 3,4 Mari-Ann Wallander, 3,6 and Nancy L. Pedersen 2,5 1 Section of Personal Injury

More information

The frequency of stroke and dementia increases with

The frequency of stroke and dementia increases with Stroke in 85-Year-Olds Prevalence, Incidence, Risk Factors, and Relation to Mortality and Dementia Martin Liebetrau, MD; Bertil Steen, MD, PhD; Ingmar Skoog, MD, PhD Background and Purpose Stroke and dementia

More information

Measure #282: Dementia: Functional Status Assessment National Quality Strategy Domain: Effective Clinical Care

Measure #282: Dementia: Functional Status Assessment National Quality Strategy Domain: Effective Clinical Care Measure #282: Dementia: Functional Status Assessment National Quality Strategy Domain: Effective Clinical Care 2017 OPTIONS FOR INDIVIDUAL MEASURES: REGISTRY ONLY MEASURE TYPE: Process DESCRIPTION: Percentage

More information

Depressive Symptoms Among Colorado Farmers 1

Depressive Symptoms Among Colorado Farmers 1 February 1995 Depressive Symptoms Among Colorado Farmers 1 L. Stallones, M. Leff, C. Garrett, L. Criswell, T. Gillan 2 ARTICLE ABSTRACT Previous studies have reported farmers to be at higher risk of suicide

More information

National estimates of the prevalence of Alzheimer s disease in the United States

National estimates of the prevalence of Alzheimer s disease in the United States Alzheimer s & Dementia 7 (2011) 61 73 National estimates of the prevalence of Alzheimer s disease in the United States Ron Brookmeyer a, *, Denis A. Evans b,c, Liesi Hebert b, Kenneth M. Langa d,e,f, Steven

More information

11/1/2013. Depression affects approximately 350 million people worldwide, and is the leading cause of disability globally (WHO, 2012)

11/1/2013. Depression affects approximately 350 million people worldwide, and is the leading cause of disability globally (WHO, 2012) Depression affects approximately 350 million people worldwide, and is the leading cause of disability globally (WHO, 2012) College of Arts & Sciences Department of Sociology State University Of New York

More information

The prevalence of dementia in the People s Republic of China: a systematic analysis of studies

The prevalence of dementia in the People s Republic of China: a systematic analysis of studies Age and Ageing 2007; 36: 619 624 The Author 2007. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the British Geriatrics Society. doi:10.1093/ageing/afm128 All rights reserved. For Permissions, please

More information

Trial Designs. Professor Peter Cameron

Trial Designs. Professor Peter Cameron Trial Designs Professor Peter Cameron OVERVIEW Review of Observational methods Principles of experimental design applied to observational studies Population Selection Looking for bias Inference Analysis

More information

Published & in Press

Published & in Press Published & in Press 1. Anstey, K. J., Bielak, A., Birrell, C., Byles, J., Burns, R. A., Kiely, K., and the DYNOPTA Investigators, (accepted 07/02/2011). Understanding ageing in older Australians: The

More information

Investigating Motivation for Physical Activity among Minority College Females Using the BREQ-2

Investigating Motivation for Physical Activity among Minority College Females Using the BREQ-2 Investigating Motivation for Physical Activity among Minority College Females Using the BREQ-2 Gherdai Hassel a, Jeffrey John Milroy a, and Muhsin Michael Orsini a Adolescents who engage in regular physical

More information

Citation for the original published paper (version of record):

Citation for the original published paper (version of record): http://www.diva-portal.org Postprint This is the accepted version of a paper published in Journal of Aging and Health. This paper has been peer-reviewed but does not include the final publisher proof-corrections

More information

Brain reserve and dementia: a systematic review

Brain reserve and dementia: a systematic review Psychological Medicine, 2006, 36, 441 454. f 2005 Cambridge University Press doi:10.1017/s0033291705006264 First published online 6 October 2005. Printed in the United Kingdom REVIEW ARTICLE Brain reserve

More information

Natural Progression Model of Cognition and Physical Functioning among People with Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer s Disease

Natural Progression Model of Cognition and Physical Functioning among People with Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer s Disease Journal of Alzheimer s Disease 37 (2013) 357 365 DOI 10.3233/JAD-130296 IOS Press 357 Natural Progression Model of Cognition and Physical Functioning among People with Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer

More information

Online Cognitive Training Improves Cognitive Performance.

Online Cognitive Training Improves Cognitive Performance. Online Cognitive Training Improves Cognitive Performance. Bernard Croisile (1), Diana Miner (2), Sandrine Bélier (2), Michel Noir (2), Franck Tarpin-Bernard (2) 2007 (1) Centre Mémoire de Ressources et

More information

Mild Cognitive Impairment in the General Population: Occurrence and progression to Alzheimer s disease

Mild Cognitive Impairment in the General Population: Occurrence and progression to Alzheimer s disease Mild Cognitive Impairment in the General Population: Occurrence and progression to Alzheimer s disease, Marie Curie Fellow- EU Aging Research Center Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society

More information

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Detecting Dementia With the Mini-Mental State Examination in Highly Educated Individuals

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Detecting Dementia With the Mini-Mental State Examination in Highly Educated Individuals ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION Detecting Dementia With the Mini-Mental State Examination in Highly Educated Individuals Sid E. O Bryant, PhD; Joy D. Humphreys, MA; Glenn E. Smith, PhD; Robert J. Ivnik, PhD; Neill

More information

Preclinical Symptoms of Major Depression in Very Old Age: A Prospective Longitudinal Study

Preclinical Symptoms of Major Depression in Very Old Age: A Prospective Longitudinal Study BERGER, PRECLINICAL Am J Psychiatry SMALL, SYMPTOMS FORSELL, 155:8, August OF ET MAJOR AL. 1998 DEPRESSION Preclinical Symptoms of Major Depression in Very Old Age: A Prospective Longitudinal Study Anna-Karin

More information

ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION

ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Heart Failure and Risk of Dementia and Alzheimer Disease A Population-Based Cohort Study Chengxuan Qiu, MD, PhD; Bengt Winblad, MD, PhD; Alessandra Marengoni, MD; Inga Klarin, MD;

More information

Behavioral and Psychologic Symptoms in Different Types of Dementia

Behavioral and Psychologic Symptoms in Different Types of Dementia ORIGINAL ARTICLE Behavioral and Psychologic Symptoms in Different Types of Dementia Ming-Jang Chiu,* Ta-Fu Chen, Ping-Keung Yip, Mau-Sun Hua, 1 Li-Yu Tang 2 Background/Purpose: Behavioral and psychologic

More information

Familial Mental Retardation

Familial Mental Retardation Behavior Genetics, Vol. 14, No. 3, 1984 Familial Mental Retardation Paul L. Nichols ~ Received 18 Aug. 1983--Final 2 Feb. 1984 Familial patterns of mental retardation were examined among white and black

More information

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Telephone-Based Identification of Mild Cognitive Impairment and Dementia in a Multicultural Cohort

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Telephone-Based Identification of Mild Cognitive Impairment and Dementia in a Multicultural Cohort ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION Telephone-Based Identification of Mild Cognitive Impairment and Dementia in a Multicultural Cohort Jennifer J. Manly, PhD; Nicole Schupf, PhD; Yaakov Stern, PhD; Adam M. Brickman,

More information

Evaluation of the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria in the diverentiation of Alzheimer s disease and frontotemporal dementia

Evaluation of the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria in the diverentiation of Alzheimer s disease and frontotemporal dementia 184 Neurology A R Varma J S Snowden P R Talbot D Neary Medical Physics, Manchester Royal Infirmary, Manchester, UK J J Lloyd Pathological Sciences, Manchester Medical School, Manchester, UK D M A Mann

More information

Classification of Persons by Dementia Status in the National Health and Aging Trends Study

Classification of Persons by Dementia Status in the National Health and Aging Trends Study Classification of Persons by Dementia Status in the National Health and Aging Trends Study Judith D. Kasper, PhD Johns Hopkins University Vicki A. Freedman, PhD University of Michigan Brenda C. Spillman,

More information

Intelligence 40 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Intelligence

Intelligence 40 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Intelligence Intelligence 40 (2012) 352 361 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Intelligence Does the direction and magnitude of cognitive change depend on initial level of ability? Timothy A. Salthouse

More information