Comparison between subjective and actigraphic measurement of sleep and sleep rhythms

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Comparison between subjective and actigraphic measurement of sleep and sleep rhythms"

Transcription

1 J. Sleep Res. (1999) 8, Comparison between subjective and actigraphic measurement of sleep and sleep rhythms STEVEN W. LOCKLEY, DEBRA J. SKENE andjosephine ARENDT School of Biological Sciences, University of Surrey, Guildford, UK Accepted in revised form 27 January 1999; received 28 September 1998 SUMMARY Sleep is often assessed in circadian rhythm studies and long-term monitoring is required to detect any changes in sleep over time. The present study aims to investigate the ability of the two most commonly employed methods, actigraphy and sleep logs, to identify circadian sleep/wake disorders and measure changes in sleep patterns over time. In addition, the study assesses whether sleep measured by both methods shows the same relationship with an established circadian phase marker, urinary 6- sulphatoxymelatonin. A total of 49 registered blind subjects with different types of circadian rhythms were studied daily for at least four weeks. Grouped analysis of all study days for all subjects was performed for all sleep parameters ( days data per sleep parameter). Good correlations were observed when comparing the measurement of sleep timing and duration (sleep onset, sleep offset, night sleep duration, day-time nap duration). However, the methods were poorly correlated in their assessment of transitions between sleep and wake states (sleep latency, number and duration of night awakenings, number of day-time naps). There were also large and inconsistent differences in the measurement of the absolute sleep parameters. Overall, actigraphs recorded a shorter sleep latency, advanced onset time, increased number and duration of night awakenings, delayed offset, increased night sleep duration and increased number and duration of naps compared with the subjective sleep logs. Despite this, there was good agreement between the methods for measuring changes in sleep patterns over time. In particular, the methods agreed when assessing changes in sleep in relation to a circadian phase marker (the 6-sulphatoxymelatonin (amt6s) rhythm) in both entrained (n = 30) and free-running (n = 4) subjects. KEYWORDS actigraphy, blindness, circadian, naps, sleep INTRODUCTION The assessment of sleep is often performed in circadian rhythm studies. Changes in sleep due to circadian rhythm disorders may take a long time to become apparent and thus require long-term monitoring to detect any changes. Polysomnography (PSG) is considered to be the gold standard method for assessing sleep but it is not always practical for longitudinal studies or field use. Two principal methods are used to measure sleep in field studies, as they are convenient for the subject, require minimal supervision and can easily be maintained. The first technique, subjective sleep assessment, Correspondence: Steven W. Lockley, School of Biological Sciences, University of Surrey, Guildford, GU2 5XH, UK. Tel.: ; fax: ; s.lockley@surrey.ac.uk 1999 European Sleep Research Society consists of daily sleep diaries or logs. Alternatively, actigraphy, using wrist-worn activity monitors, indirectly assesses when sleep occurs from the amount of physical activity detected. Both methods, however, have shortcomings. For subjective measurement of sleep, there is evidence to suggest that people have difficulties assessing their own sleep especially when suffering from insomnia (e.g. Carskadon et al. 1976) and that there may be gender- and age-related effects on subjective sleep assessment (Reyner and Horne 1995). Although there is obviously some difficulty in recollecting exact sleep times or duration, in long-term studies it should be easy for subjects to distinguish between major differences in sleep parameters (e.g. between 5, 30 and 60 min for sleep latency). However, the limitations of subjective sleep assessments need to be kept in mind when interpreting the results. Activity monitors have been widely used in a variety of 175

2 176 S.W. Lockley et al. clinical investigations, including identification of sleep disorders marker. The first question our study addresses is whether and drug treatments (for review see Sadeh et al. 1995). They subjective and actigraphic methods give the same absolute do not depend on subjective measurement but difficulty may values for night and day sleep parameters. As has been noted arise in the interpretation of activity levels. For example, the previously (Carskadon et al. 1976; Hauri and Wisbey 1992; monitor cannot distinguish sleep from inactivity or night- Sadeh et al. 1995), there may be discrepancies in night sleep time restlessness during sleep from night-time awakening. This measured by subjective and objective (actigraphic and/or PSG) problem may be accentuated in individuals with sleep disorders methods. To our knowledge, there has been no systematic, longwho spend time lying still but awake in bed, or those that may term comparison of actigraphic and subjective measurement of sleep restlessly (Sadeh et al. 1995). spontaneous napping under normal conditions although the There are also some problems associated with PSG. This Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT) has been assessed technique usually requires individuals to sleep in research simultaneously by PSG and actigraphy (Levine et al. 1986). laboratories which are known to change habitual sleep patterns The second question is whether actigraphically assessed sleep (Reynolds et al. 1992) and induce a first-night effect (Riedel and sleep logs are able to assess similar changes in sleep et al. 1998). There may also be inter-individual differences in patterns, thus helping identification of a circadian sleep-wake the scoring of results. Even though all of these methods attempt disorder. Thirdly, the study investigates whether the established to measure sleep, they measure different things, i.e. subjective relationship between sleep timing and circadian phase is recollection of sleep, motor activity or electrical activity of the observed in both subjective and actigraphic data. brain. Previous work, using primarily subjective sleep measurement, has shown that blind individuals are likely to have circadian METHOD sleep/wake disorders (Miles et al. 1977; Okawa et al. 1987; Subjects Arendt et al. 1988; Klein et al. 1993; Lockley et al. 1997a, 1999). They are caused by insufficient light input from the A total of 49 subjects were studied (36 males, 13 females; mean retina to the central circadian oscillator, situated in the age±sd = 46.6±12.2 years). All subjects were registered as suprachiasmatic nuclei, for entrainment of rhythms to a solar blind and suffered from a range of pathologies (Lockley et al. 24-h day. Longitudinal measurement of the sleep/wake pattern, 1997b). Of these 49 subjects, 19 had light perception or better with simultaneous assessment of a reliable phase-marker of the and 30 had no conscious light perception. They were recruited circadian clock, is required before accurate identification of a from a database compiled at the Moorfields Eye Hospital, UK disorder can be made. The three major circadian sleep/wake as part of an epidemiological study of sleep disorders in the disorders, defined by the (ICSD 1990) and known to occur in blind (Tabandeh et al. 1998). Ethical permission for the study blind people, are advanced sleep phase syndrome (ASPS), was granted by the University of Surrey Advisory Committee delayed sleep phase syndrome (DSPS) and non 24-h circadian on Ethics and the Moorfields Eye Hospital Ethics Committee. sleep/wake syndrome (or free-running sleep/wake rhythm). In Subjects were excluded if they were taking any medication ASPS, the subject s sleep/wake cycle is advanced relative to a known to affect sleep and/or melatonin production (tricyclic conventional sleep time and in DSPS, the sleep/wake cycle is antidepressants, monoamine oxidase inhibitors, serotonin relatively delayed (ICSD 1990). In some blind subjects, ASPS reuptake inhibitors, benzodiazepines, neuroleptics, β-blockers and DSPS are associated with advanced and delayed amt6s or sleeping medication). Informed consent was obtained from rhythms, respectively (Lockley et al. 1999). In non 24-h sleep/ all subjects and included consent for the assessment of urine wake syndrome, the subject s clock assumes it s own for a number of drugs (opiates, amphetamine, barbiturates, endogenous period which is different from, and usually longer benzodiazepine, cocaine, cannabis, tricyclic antidepressants). than, 24 h. The sleep/wake cycle is characterised by a gradual Prior to the study, subjects underwent a full ophthalmological delay in sleep onset and offset times and frequent naps of a long examination and interview and completed the Pittsburgh Sleep duration when the circadian clock is completely desynchronised Quality Index (PSQI) questionnaire (Buysse et al. 1989). The from the 24-h day (Miles et al. 1977; Lockley et al. 1999). majority of subjects (86%) complained of a sleep disorder as Actigraphy has been used to assess free-running activity and assessed by the PSQI (score 5 indicative of a sleep disorder). sleep/wake rhythms in sighted humans in laboratory based The subjects were studied for at least four weeks. They were isolation experiments (Middleton et al. 1996). Blind children, asked to keep daily sleep and nap diaries recording sleep for whom sleep diaries may be less reliable, have also been latency, onset, offset, number and duration of night awakenings investigated by actigraphy to reveal disordered sleep and and the number and duration of daytime naps. Naps were daytime sleepiness (Tzischinsky et al. 1991). Improvement in defined as any sleep that occurred outside the subjects bedtime actigraphically derived sleep has been shown following period. The diaries were recorded in several ways according to melatonin treatment (Tzischinsky et al. 1992). the subjects preference and visual ability. The methods used The present study investigates the ability of actigraphy and were large print forms, audiotape, computer disk, and sleep logs to identify sleep/wake disorders in a group of blind Braille. Data were transcribed onto audiotape or computer subjects and whether sleep measured by both methods shows disk or dictated to the researcher. Subjects were also asked to the same relationship with an established circadian phase wear activity monitors (Motionloggers or Minimotionloggers,

3 Comparison of subjective and actigraphic sleep 177 Ambulatory Monitoring Inc., New York, USA) on the wrist range ). The same analysis was then repeated for continuously for 24 h a day for the duration of the study. The each individual. For each test, the mean difference between the monitors were preset to the zero crossing mode so that the methods was calculated by subtracting the actigraphic score signal voltage from the motion transducer is compared with a from the subjective score. reference voltage for a change in state, effectively measuring To assess whether similar trends were observed in the data activity frequency (User s Guide for the Motionlogger using the subjective and actigraphic methods, daily sleep onset Actigraph, Ambulatory Monitoring). The subjects were only and offset times were fitted with best-fit regression lines for allowed to remove the activity monitor if there was a risk of each individual. The period (tau) of any free-running rhythm it getting wet or if it interfered with any usual activity. This was calculated by adding the slope of the regression line to 24 was recorded in an activity diary. The actigraphs were [tau = slope (h)] and was considered significant if the programmed to group the data automatically into one minute 95% confidence limits of the regression line did not cross 0 (i.e. epochs (the largest epoch that can be used for automatic sleep h). Free-running periods derived by the two methods scoring). Raw data were downloaded onto computer disk at were considered non-significantly different if the 95% regular intervals and the activity monitors reset. confidence limits of the two regression lines overlapped. On For 24 or 48 h each week, subjects collected sequential 4- four occasions there was a clear jump in the sleep plots, and hourly urine samples (8 h overnight) for analysis of the major in these instances the longest continuous sequence of days urinary metabolite of melatonin. Full details of the methods without abrupt shifts was fitted with the regression line. used to determine circadian rhythm type from the 6- Correlation analysis was performed on the rhythm periods for sulphatoxymelatonin (amt6s) rhythms are described in sleep onset and offset between the two methods. Lockley et al. (1997b). Data analysis Calculation of sleep parameters Statistical analysis Using all the data for all the subjects as a group, Pearson correlations and Students t-tests were performed between subjective and actigraphic results for sleep latency, number and duration of night awakenings, onset, offset, total night sleep duration, and the number and duration of day-time naps (n For the subjective data, the sleep parameters were calculated as follows. Sleep onset was the sum of time at which sleep was attempted and estimated sleep latency. Sleep offset was taken as the wake up time. Total night sleep duration was calculated by subtracting the minutes of night sleep lost from the time between sleep onset and offset. Activity data were edited using the subjects activity diary and analysed by the Action3 software (Ambulatory Monitoring Inc., New York, USA). Days with more than 33% of data points missing were considered incomplete and excluded from the analysis. Only full nights with no data missing were used. To calculate night sleep parameters, automatic sleep/ wake scoring was performed on the activity data between bedtime and get up time derived from the subjective sleep diaries. Similarly, day sleep parameters were automatically scored for sleep between get up time and bedtime derived from the sleep logs. The sleep scoring algorithm assesses the activity of the preceding four min and the subsequent two min of each minute before defining whether each minute is awake, asleep or both. Automatic sleep scoring gave measures of night-time sleep latency, onset, offset, number and mean duration of night awakenings. The automatically derived day- time parameters were total day sleep duration and number of separate sleep episodes. amt6s analysis Urinary amt6s concentrations were measured and analysed by cosinor analysis to reveal the peak or acrophase (phi) of the amt6s rhythm. The results of these measurements are discussed in detail in Lockley et al. 1997b. Using the acrophase times, subjects were classified into four circadian rhythm categories; normally entrained to 24 h (NE; phi range = h, n = 21), abnormally entrained to 24 h (AE; phi range = h, n = 9), free-running (FR; tau range = h, n = 17) or unclassified with no discernible rhythm (UN; n = 2). One of the subjects clearly had a free-running amt6s rhythm but, due to lack of urine samples, an estimation of tau was not possible and this subject was excluded from analysis that required a tau estimation. Unclassified subjects were omitted from any further analysis involving amt6s rhythms. Sleep in relation to circadian phase In subjects that did not show free-running amt6s rhythms, the mean amt6s acrophase time (phi) was used to define circadian phase. Multiple regression analysis (MANOVA) was performed between this value, the mean subjective and actigraphic sleep parameters and age of the subject. In subjects with free-running amt6s rhythms, the regression line fitted through the amt6s acrophase times was used to calculate the daily amt6s acrophase for each person. Mean subjective and actigraphic sleep parameters were compared during the subjects normal (amt6s acrophase h) and abnormal (amt6s acrophase h) circadian phase. For those subjects that passed through a full circadian cycle during the study, sleep and nap data were firstly expressed in relation to each individual s overall mean for each parameter and then grouped into hourly amt6s acrophase times. One- way ANOVA was used to calculate whether there was an overall

4 178 S.W. Lockley et al. significant change in these parameters with circadian phase (amt6s acrophase). For details of the analysis and results of the subjective sleep data see Lockley et al. (1999). RESULTS Sleep parameters The mean (±SD) number of nights analysed per subject was 23±7 (range 6 35). At least 1062 nights of data were compared for all six night-sleep parameters (range days per sleep parameter). Two subjects failed to record the minutes of sleep lost per night in their sleep logs and therefore a subjective estimate of the total night sleep could not be calculated for these individuals. Two subjects also failed to record sleep latency but did record an estimated sleep onset time. The mean (±SD) number of days analysed per subject was 23±7 (range 6 36). In total, 1112 days of data were compared for both nap parameters. Figure 1 shows the correlation analyses for all the raw data for all subjects (n range ). Good correlations were observed when comparing the measurement of sleep timing and duration, i.e. sleep onset [r = 0.77, n = 1150 (Fig. 1C)] and sleep offset [r = 0.88, n = 1150 (Fig. 1D)] and to a lesser extent, night-time sleep duration [r = 0.57, n = 1109 (Fig. 1B)] and day-time nap duration [r = 0.48, n = 1112 (Fig. 1H)]. In contrast, the methods were poorly correlated in their assessment of transitions between sleep and wake states, i.e. sleep latency [r = 0.12, n = 1062 (Fig. 1A)], number of night awakenings [r = 0.06, n = 1122 (Fig. 1E)], duration of night awakenings [r = 0.22, n = 1072 (Fig. 1F)] and the number of day-time naps [r = 0.05, n = 1112 (Fig. 1G)]. The correlation coefficients between the two methods varied considerably between individuals and between sleep parameters. Sleep onset, offset and night sleep duration were generally highly positively correlated although the large ranges indicate the extent of the Figure 1. The correlation of all raw data for all subjects over all variation between subjects (mean r±sd = 0.68±0.37, nights (n range = ). The vertical axes show actigraphic values and the horizontal axes show subjective values. 0.59±0.43 and 0.64±0.26, respectively). The other parameters were generally less well correlated with a similar large variation between individuals (mean r±sd = 0.45±0.30 for the proportion of subjects that showed a significant difference duration of naps; 0.29±0.26 for the duration of night between the two methods than did not (χ 2 = 34.5, P < ). awakenings; 0.22±0.25 for the number of naps; 0.19±0.32 for The incidence of significant differences was greatest in the the number of night awakenings and 0.07±0.29 for sleep number of naps (41/49 subjects, 84%) and least for sleep onset latency). (19/49, 39%) (Fig. 3B). The discrepancy between the two When analysed as a group, there were statistically significant methods was largest when measuring the number or duration differences between the absolute measures of all sleep of sleep or wake episodes (duration and number of night parameters (Fig. 2). Actigraphy showed a longer sleep period awakenings, night sleep duration, number and duration of day- (i.e. shorter sleep latency, advanced sleep onset, delayed sleep time naps) and smallest when comparing sleep timing (sleep offset and longer night sleep duration), greater sleep disturbance onset, offset). To illustrate this point, Figure 4 shows plots of (increased number and duration of night awakenings) and all the sleep parameters measured by both methods in one greater day-time sleepiness (increased number and duration of subject. In all the analyses, the two methods gave results that naps) compared with subjective sleep assessments. differed significantly. Actigraphy measured shorter sleep latency Analysing individuals, there were positive and negative (Fig. 4A), increased night sleep duration (Fig. 4B), an advanced relationships between the two methods (Fig. 3A). Figure 3B sleep onset (Fig. 4C), a delayed sleep offset (Fig. 4D), increased depicts only those subjects that showed significant differences number and duration of night awakenings (Fig. 4E,F) and between the methods. Overall, there was a significantly higher increased number and duration of day-time naps (Fig. 4G,H).

5 Comparison of subjective and actigraphic sleep 179 Figure 2. The mean (±sem) values for all sleep parameters for all subjects as assessed by subjective diaries (white columns) and actigraphic automatic sleep-scoring (black columns) ( P< 0.05; paired Student s t-test). Sleep rhythms Free-running sleep onset and offset rhythms, calculated by regression analysis, were observed in some subjects. The subjective and actigraphic methods agreed in determining the period of sleep rhythms in 42 subjects for sleep onset, 38 of whom had taus that were not significantly different from h. The other four subjects had free-running taus that were statistically indistinguishable by the two methods. Of the remaining seven subjects, five had a free-running tau according to the subjective data whereas the tau derived from the actigraphic data was h. The other two subjects had the reverse relationship. Analysis of the subjective and actigraphic measures of sleep offset rhythms also agreed in 42 subjects; 40 had rhythms not significantly different from h and two subjects had freerunning taus that were statistically indistinguishable. The two methods did not agree in seven subjects, four of whom had a free-running rhythm according to the subjective data but were entrained according to the actigraphic data. In the other three subjects this relationship was reversed. When the taus assessed by both methods were compared in Figure 3. The mean difference (±SD) between subjective and actigraphic measurements of sleep. The positive direction (black bars) refers to subjective values > actigraphic values and the negative direction (grey bars) refers to subjective values < actigraphic values. Figure 3(A) shows the mean difference for all subjects. Figure 3(B) shows the mean difference for only those subjects that had a significant difference between the two methods (P< 0.05, Student s t- test). The double line delineates the left axis (minutes) from the right axis (number). The numbers above each panel show the number of subjects. all 49 subjects, there were significant correlations (P < 0.01) for sleep onset and offset, correlation coefficients (r) being 0.35 and 0.42, respectively. Sleep in relation to circadian rhythm type In order to test whether sleep was grossly affected by circadian rhythm type the mean sleep parameters were compared between NE, AE and FR subjects. There were significant differences in the subjective number of naps between the subgroups and in the subjective and actigraphic duration of day-time naps (P < 0.05, one-way ANOVA). In all cases, NE subjects had the fewest naps of the shortest duration. Both subjective and actigraphic measures of night-time sleep duration showed significantly greater sleep duration in NE subjects compared to AE and FR subjects. Sleep in relation to circadian phase Entrained subjects Thirty subjects had amt6s rhythms which were entrained to the 24 h day (n = 21 NE; n = 9 AE). When controlled for age, subjective sleep onset showed a significant relationship with amt6s phase (P < 0.05, MANOVA) with a correlation coefficient

6 180 S.W. Lockley et al. Figure 4. The plots of eight sleep parameters simultaneously measured by subjective sleep diaries (Ε Ε) and automatic actigraphy (Χ Χ) in one subject (S48) for 32 days. (r) of When the amt6s rhythm was advanced, sleep onset (subjective data, P = 0.07; actigraphic data P = 0.46) (Fig. 5A) was advanced and when the amt6s rhythm was delayed, sleep and a significant change in both measures of sleep offset onset was delayed. Actigraphic sleep onset data agreed with (P < 0.05) (Fig. 5B). Both methods confirmed that sleep onset the subjective data showing a similar but non-significant effect and offset were relatively advanced when the amt6s acrophase with amt6s acrophase (r = 0.35, P = 0.06, MANOVA). No other was advanced and delayed when the amt6s acrophase was sleep parameters showed a significant relationship with amt6s delayed. phase when controlled for age. Both methods also showed significant changes in the duration of night sleep and day-time naps in relation to Free-running subjects circadian phase (P< 0.05) (Fig. 5C,D, respectively). Night sleep duration was minimal when the amt6s rhythm was There were nine FR subjects which had both subjective out of phase and maximal when the amt6s rhythm was in and actigraphic data and passed in (amt6s acrophase phase. Conversely, day-time nap duration was maximal when h) and out ( h) of a normal circadian amt6s was out of phase and minimal when amt6s was in phase. Only subjective assessment of the number and duration phase. Only in the subjective data did the number of naps of naps showed a significant change with phase, there being change significantly with phase (Fig. 5E) reflecting the more naps of a longer duration when the amt6s phase was changes in nap duration. Of the other sleep parameters, only abnormal ( h). The actigraphic data showed the actigraphic duration of night awakenings showed a significant same trend but it was not statistically significant. change with phase (data not shown) due to a large duration Four subjects had free-running amt6s rhythms that passed of night awakenings when the amt6s acrophase was between through most (> 75%) of a full circadian cycle during the study and h. As Fig. 5 shows, there is a remarkable and had sufficient activity data for assessment of sleep across degree of similarity in the change of sleep with circadian all phases. Figure 5 shows the grouped analysis of these phase between data assessed by the two methods. However, subjective and actigraphic sleep data, plotted in relation to it should be stressed that these data are expressed in relation circadian phase (amt6s acrophase time). Both methods showed to each individual s own mean before grouping which will similar changes in sleep timing with circadian phase. There mask large variations in absolute values both between subjects was a non-significant change in both measures of sleep onset and methods.

7 Comparison of subjective and actigraphic sleep 181 Figure 5. The mean (±sem) sleep parameters for four subjects (calculated with respect to each subjects own mean) plotted against amt6s acrophase time using subjective (Ε Ε) and actigraphic (Β Β) data. The grey areas represent the mean (±2SD) amt6s acrophase time for normally entrained sighted subjects (n = 80, English and Arendt, unpublished results). DISCUSSION duration, nap duration, sleep offset, latency, duration of night awakenings and sleep onset. However, the study also demonstrates that in general, actigraphic and subjective sleep data agree when determining the rhythms of sleep and both detect similar changes in sleep in relation to circadian phase. The greatest inconsistencies between the two methods were found in the absolute measurement of the duration and incidence of sleep or wake episodes (number and duration of night awakenings, duration of night sleep and the number and duration of day-time naps). Actigraphy overestimated sleep periods (night- and day-time) and night-time disturbance compared to sleep diaries. One reason for these results could be the way in which actigraphs assess sleep, namely by measurement of motor activity. Activity monitors appear to be misinterpreting inactivity during wakefulness, both prior to and after the actual night sleep period and during the day, as sleep. In addition, they interpret night-time activity during sleep as waking periods. There is less inconsistency between the measurements of sleep timing (onset and offset) between the two methods, although in some individuals there were still large differences in either direction (even the sleep parameter with the least differences (sleep onset) showed a significant difference in 39% of individuals). We believe that sleep and nap diaries are useful tools to interpret the actigraphic measure. For example, in our experience, blind people often go to bed and listen to the radio or audiotapes before going to sleep. By asking individuals to note their bedtime and the time of trying to fall asleep separately, we can determine whether the time spent in bed is spent awake or asleep. Our results show that activity-derived sleep latency is short and sleep onsets are advanced compared to the sleep logs and therefore actigraphs are likely to be interpreting the whole in bed period as sleep. Similarly, subjective sleep offset times are likely to be accurate and again, activity-derived sleep offsets were delayed relative to the subjective measure. The nap diary is designed so that subjects only make an entry in the nap diary when a nap has occurred and therefore the periods of day-time sleeps recorded subjectively are reliable. Actigraphically-derived naps were much more frequent and of a longer duration than the subjective nap data. These results highlight the need for simultaneous use of sleep diaries with actigraphy wherever possible, in order that sleep diaries can be used to edit actigraphy data and provide points of reference for analysis. Accurate use of the subjective event marker available on some actigraphs may help reduce the inconsistency. In general, actigraphs recorded a shorter sleep latency, advanced onset time, increased number and duration of night awakenings, delayed offset, increased night sleep duration and increased number and duration of naps compared with the subjective sleep logs. The extent and magnitude of these discrepancies makes sole use of activity monitors inappropriate to assess night or day sleep in studies where absolute values for sleep periods, duration and disturbance are required. The higher degree of significant error in the assessment of day- time naps indicates that the activity monitor is unsuitable for This study compared actigraphic and subjective measurement of sleep in field conditions in 49 blind individuals living in their normal environment. The results demonstrate a large variation between the methods in the measurement of absolute sleep parameters. This difference is greatest in the number of naps, followed by the number of night awakenings, night sleep

8 182 S.W. Lockley et al. measurement of day-time sleep using the present algorithm. Unfortunately, the direction and magnitude of the differences between the two methods is not predictable and does not appear to be strongly related to any variable in these subjects. Even within a subject, the direction and significance of differences was not consistent in all the sleep parameters measured. It is important to present the direction of error because a simple average would tend towards zero as the negative and positive values cancel each other out (Chambers 1994). There is no clear consensus on the differences between subjective, PSG and actigraphic sleep data. Most previous studies have validated actigraphy against PSG recordings in an attempt to substitute actigraph-derived sleep measurement when PSG is not possible (for review, see Sadeh et al. 1995). Our conclusion that actigraphy overestimates sleep duration compared to subjective logs supports previous studies that have shown actigraphy to overestimate sleep relative to sleep diaries and/or PSG despite good correlations (Hauri and Wisbey 1992; Chambers 1994; Schmidt-Nowara et al. 1992; Cole et al. 1992) although not in all cases (Levine et al. 1986; Driver et al. 1998). The main finding of these reports was that actigraphs were only marginally acceptable when attempting to measure the exact time an individual was asleep (especially for subjects who may lie awake motionless, i.e. insomniacs) but were appropriate for documenting longitudinal changes in sleep patterns or schedules. Overall, actigraphy tended to be less accurate at times when sleep was less likely to occur. Our results confirm and extend these general conclusions. It appears that actigraphs overestimated sleep duration compared to PSG (Hauri and Wisbey 1992) to a similar extent that we have found comparing actigraphs with sleep logs. Our study found that in 66% of individuals, actigraphs overestimated night sleep duration compared to 56% reported by Hauri and Wisbey (1992). Similarly, the overall error in night sleep duration, regardless of direction, was 60 min in our study compared to 49 min (Hauri and Wisbey 1992). These comparisons suggest that sleep logs may estimate sleep more accurately than actigraphs and provide results closer to the expected PSG data. However, further work is required to assess sleep measurement (using PSG, actigraphy and sleep diaries) over prolonged periods in the field or a home environment (Broughton et al. 1996). One way to minimise the difference may be to modify the sleep/wake scoring algorithm to make it less sensitive to switches between sleep and wake states. However, this new algorithm would require extensive validation in a variety of populations prior to its routine use. There are other factors which may account for these differences. Most previous work has been undertaken in a laboratory setting which is known to affect sleep, usually by advancing the sleep offset time (Reynolds et al. 1992). The duration of the studies may also be important given that the short assessment periods used in most studies (2 3 nights) are prone to first-night effects (Riedel et al. 1998) and that reliability between subjective and actigraphic data has been shown to improve with increased number of study days (Acebo and Carskadon 1997). Comparisons between data should be interpreted with caution. Many authors report > 80% agreement between activity-derived sleep and PSG when compared on an epochepoch basis (e.g. Mullaney et al. 1980; Webster et al. 1982; Levine et al. 1986; Sadeh et al. 1989; Dunham et al. 1991; Cole et al. 1992; Hauri and Wisbey 1992; Reid and Dawson 1998). Although this suggests an acceptable level of agreement between the methods, there is no indication as to where the 20% of inaccurate comparisons occur. If, for example, the inaccuracy occurs close to the onset and offset of sleep, which our results suggest, it could give misleading results about sleep timing and duration. Similarly, studies often report a high correlation between sleep parameters assessed by different methods and conclude high degrees of consistency between the methods (e.g. Cole et al. 1992; Sadeh et al. 1995). However, whilst such a correlation may suggest a strong directional relationship between the data, it gives no indication of how similar the actual raw data are. Our study shows that despite good correlations between the data, there may still be a significant difference between the absolute sleep measurements. This discrepancy must be kept in mind if absolute, rather than relative sleep data are required. This study has also investigated the ability of both methods to detect sleep rhythms and changes in sleep in relation to circadian phase. Blind subjects with disordered circadian rhythms (Lockley et al. 1997b) are ideal to study the relationship between circadian rhythms disorders and sleep (Lockley et al. 1999). Changes in sleep were assessed by both methods in subjects with different types of circadian rhythms. Overall, there appears to be little difference in the assessment of sleep rhythms by regression analysis between subjective and actigraphic measures. In addition, the influence of permanent (in AE subjects) or transient (in FR subjects) circadian disorders on sleep is measured well by either method and could be confidently used longitudinally to assess changes in sleep in relation to phase. The choice of method may be important depending on the study population; for example, sleep diaries may be inappropriate for subjects that are not compliant or motivated, such as children or the elderly, and actigraphy may be more suitable. Subjects should always be used as their own controls to minimise the large difference in absolute values measured by subjective and actigraphic methods. In conclusion, this study demonstrates large and inconsistent errors between actigraphically-determined automatic sleep scoring and daily subjective sleep logs in a longitudinal home- based field study of sleep patterns in blind individuals. The greatest disagreement between the two methods is found in the assessment of absolute values of night and day sleep parameters with the largest error occurring in day sleep assessment. This is most likely due to misinterpretation by automatic sleep- scoring of low levels of motor activity as sleep episodes. Thus if absolute values of sleep parameters are required, the present results suggest that actigraphs are not appropriate for sole use in sleep studies, especially in determining day and night-time

9 Comparison of subjective and actigraphic sleep 183 sleep disturbance. Both methods should be employed in order Levine, B., Moyles, T., Roehrs, T., Fortier, J. and Roth, T. Actigraphic to allow cross-referencing. Despite this, our study showed good monitoring and polygraphic recording in determination of sleep and wake. Sleep Res., 1986, 15: 247. agreement between actigraphy and subjective assessment in Lockley, S. W., Skene, D. J., Arendt, J., Tabandeh, H., Bird, A. C. measuring changes in sleep patterns over time. Subjective and Defrance, R. Relationship between melatonin rhythms and diaries and actigraphs are therefore equally suited to assess visual loss in the blind. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab., 1997b, 82: relative alterations of sleep where absolute values are not required. Lockley, S. W., Skene, D. J., Butler, L. J. and Arendt, J. Sleep and activity rhythms are related to circadian phase in the blind. Sleep, 1999, in press. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Lockley, S. W., Skene, D. J., Tabandeh, H., Bird, A. C., Defrance, R. and Arendt, J. Relationship between napping and melatonin in the This work was supported by South Thames Regional Health blind. J. Biol. Rhythms, 1997a, 12: Authority, UK; Stockgrand Ltd, University of Surrey, UK; Middleton, B., Arendt, J. and Stone, B. M. Human circadian rhythms and Servier Research and Development, France. S.W. Lockley in constant dim light (8 lux) with knowledge of clock time. J. Sleep is supported by the Wellcome Trust grant /Z/96/Z. Res., 1996, 5: Miles, L. E. M., Raynal, D. M. and Wilson, M. A. Blind man living in normal society has circadian rhythms of 24.9 hours. Sci., 1977, REFERENCES 198: Mullaney, D. J., Kripke, D. F. and Messin, S. Wrist-actigraphic Acebo, C. and Carskadon, M. A. Reliability of actigraph measures in estimation of sleep time. Sleep, 1980, 3: children and adolescents. Proceedings of the Associated Professional Okawa, M., Nanami, T., Wada, S., Shimizu, T., Hishikawa, Y., Sasaki, Sleep Societies (APSS), 1997, 347. H. et al. Four congenitally blind children with circadian sleep-wake Arendt, J., Aldhous, M. and Wright, J. Synchronisation of a disturbed rhythm disorder. Sleep, 1987, 10: sleep-wake cycle in a blind man by melatonin treatment. Lancet, Reid, K. and Dawson, D. Correlation between actigraphic and EEG 1988, i: measurements of sleep in younger and older subjects during a Broughton, R., Fleming, J. and Fleetham, J. Home assessment of sleep simulated shiftwork schedule. Sleep, 1998, 21 (Suppl.): 206. disorders by portable monitoring. J. Clin. Neurophysiol., 1996, 13: Riedel, B. W., Winfield, C. and Lichstein, K. L. Anxiety and first night effect in older insomniacs. Sleep, 1998, 21 (Suppl.): 131. Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, C. F., III Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R. and Reyner, A. and Horne, J. A. Gender- and age-related differences in Kupfer, D. J. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument sleep determined by home-recorded sleep logs and actimetry from for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res., 1989, 28: 400 adults. Sleep, 1995, 18: Reynolds, C. F., III Grochocinski, V. J., Monk, T. H., Buysse, D. J., Carskadon, M. A., Dement, W. C., Mitler, M. M., Guilleminault, C. Giles, D. E., Coble, P. A. et al. Concordance between habitual sleep G., Zarcone, V. P. and Spiegel, R. S. Self-reports versus sleep laboratory findings in 122 drug-free subjects with complaints of times and laboratory recording schedules. Sleep, 1992, 15: chronic insomnia. Am. J. Psychiatry, 1976, 133: Sadeh, A., Alster, J., Urbach, D. and Lavie, P. Actigraphically based Chambers, M. J. Actigraphy and insomnia: a closer look; part 1. Sleep, automatic bedtime sleep-wake scoring: validity and clinical 1994, 17: applications. J. Amb. Monitoring, 1989, 2: Cole, R. J., Kripke, D. F., Gruen, W., Mullaney, D. J. and Gillin, J. Sadeh, A., Hauri, P. J., Kripke, D. F. and Lavie, P. The role of C. Automatic sleep-wake identification from wrist activity. Sleep, actigraphy in the evaluation of sleep disorders. Sleep, 1995, 18: 1992, 15: Driver, H., Baker, F., Maloney, S. and Mitchell, D. A comparison of Schmidt-Nowara, W. W., Beck, A. A. and Jessop, C. A. Actigraphic objective EEG measures of sleep onset and awakenings with assessment of a treatment trial of sleep restriction in chronic subjective data in healthy young men and women. Sleep, 1998, 21 insomnia. Sleep Res., 1992, 21: 259. (Suppl.): 292. Tabandeh, H., Lockley, S. W., Buttery, R., Skene, D. J., Defrance, R., Dunham, D. W., Hoffman, R. F. and Broughton, R. J. Wrist actigraphy Arendt, J. and Bird, A. C. Disturbances of sleep in blindness. Am. and sleep-wake estimation revisited. Sleep Res., 1991, 20A: 491. J. Ophthalmol., 1998, 126: Hauri, P. J. and Wisbey, J. Wrist actigraphy in insomnia. Sleep, 1992, Tzischinsky, O., Pal, I., Epstein, R., Dagan, Y. and Lavie, P. The 15: importance of timing in melatonin administration in a blind man. ICSD-International Classification of Sleep Disorders. In: M. J. Thorpy J. Pineal Res., 1992, 12: (Ed.) Diagnostic and coding manual. Allen Press, Lawrence, Tzischinsky, O., Skene, D. J., Epstein, R. and Lavie, P. Circadian Klein, T., Martens, H., Dijk, D.-J., Kronauer, R. E., Seely, E. W. and rhythms in 6-sulphatoxymelatonin and nocturnal sleep in blind Czeisler, C. A. Circadian sleep regulation in the absence of light children. Chronobiol. Internat : perception: Chronic non-24-hour circadian rhythm sleep disorder in Webster, J. B., Kripke, D. J., Messin, S., Mullaney, D. J. and Wyborney, a blind man with a regular 24-hour sleep-wake schedule. Sleep, 1993, G. An activity-based sleep monitor system for ambulatory use. Sleep, 16: , 5:

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep Sleep, 6(4):319-325 1983 Raven Press, New York Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep T. Roehrs, F. Zorick, J. Sicklesteel, R. Wittig, and T. Roth Sleep Disorders and Research

More information

The Effects of Short Daytime Naps for Five Consecutive Days

The Effects of Short Daytime Naps for Five Consecutive Days Sleep Research Online 5(1): 13-17, 2003 http://www.sro.org/2003/hayashi/13/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 96-214X 2003 WebSciences The Effects of Short Daytime s for Five Consecutive Mitsuo

More information

The Relationships between Sleep-Wake Cycle and Academic Performance in Medical Students

The Relationships between Sleep-Wake Cycle and Academic Performance in Medical Students Biological Rhythm Research 0165-0424/01/3202-263$16.00 2001, Vol. 32, No. 2, pp. 263 270 Swets & Zeitlinger The Relationships between Sleep-Wake Cycle and Academic Performance in Medical Students Ana Ligia

More information

Light treatment for sleep disorders: consensus report. IV. Sleep phase and duration disturbances.

Light treatment for sleep disorders: consensus report. IV. Sleep phase and duration disturbances. J Biol Rhythms 1995 Jun;10(2):135-47 Related Articles, Books, LinkOut Light treatment for sleep disorders: consensus report. IV. Sleep phase and duration disturbances. Terman M, Lewy AJ, Dijk DJ, Boulos

More information

INSOMNIAS. Stephan Eisenschenk, MD Department of Neurology

INSOMNIAS. Stephan Eisenschenk, MD Department of Neurology INSOMNIAS INSOMNIAS General criteria for insomnia A. Repeated difficulty with sleep initiation, duration, consolidation or quality. B. Adequate sleep opportunity, persistent sleep difficulty and associated

More information

Hetlioz (tasimelteon)

Hetlioz (tasimelteon) Hetlioz (tasimelteon) Policy Number: 5.01.687 Last Review: 01/2019 Origination: 01/2019 Next Review: 01/2020 Policy Blue Cross and Blue Shield of Kansas City (Blue KC) will provide coverage for Hetlioz

More information

Chapter 2. Development of a portable device for tele-monitoring. of physical activities during sleep

Chapter 2. Development of a portable device for tele-monitoring. of physical activities during sleep Author: Chih-Ming Cheng (2007-07-11); recommended: Yeh-Liang Hsu (2007-07-). Note: This article is Chapter 2 of Chih-Ming Cheng s PhD thesis Development of a portable system for tele-monitoring of sleep

More information

Actigraphy Correctly Predicts Sleep Behavior in Infants Who Are Younger than Six Months, When Compared with Polysomnography

Actigraphy Correctly Predicts Sleep Behavior in Infants Who Are Younger than Six Months, When Compared with Polysomnography 0031-3998/05/5804-0761 PEDIATRIC RESEARCH Vol. 58, No. 4, 2005 Copyright 2005 International Pediatric Research Foundation, Inc. Printed in U.S.A. Actigraphy Correctly Predicts Sleep Behavior in Infants

More information

Comparison of arbitrary definitions of circadian time periods with those determined by wrist actigraphy in analysis of ABPM data

Comparison of arbitrary definitions of circadian time periods with those determined by wrist actigraphy in analysis of ABPM data Journal of Human Hypertension (1999) 13, 449 453 1999 Stockton Press. All rights reserved 0950-9240/99 $12.00 http://www.stockton-press.co.uk/jhh ORIGINAL ARTICLE Comparison of arbitrary definitions of

More information

Circadian Rhythms in Children and Adolescents

Circadian Rhythms in Children and Adolescents Circadian Rhythms in Children and Adolescents Sarah Morsbach Honaker, Ph.D., CBSM Assistant Professor of Pediatrics IU School of Medicine Society for Behavioral Sleep Medicine Practice and Consultation

More information

Patterns of Sleepiness in Various Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence

Patterns of Sleepiness in Various Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence Sleep, 5:S165S174 1982 Raven Press, New York Patterns of Sleepiness in Various Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence F. Zorick, T. Roehrs, G. Koshorek, J. Sicklesteel, *K. Hartse, R. Wittig, and T.

More information

Waiting Time Distributions of Actigraphy Measured Sleep

Waiting Time Distributions of Actigraphy Measured Sleep The Open Sleep Journal, 2008, 1, 1-5 1 Waiting Time Distributions of Actigraphy Measured Sleep J.E. Slaven*,1, A. Mnatsakanova 1, S. Li 1,2, J.M. Violanti 3, C.M. Burchfiel 1, B.J. Vila 4 and M.E. Andrew

More information

Correcting distorted perception of sleep in insomnia: a novel behavioural experiment?

Correcting distorted perception of sleep in insomnia: a novel behavioural experiment? Behaviour Research and Therapy 42 (2004) 27 39 www.elsevier.com/locate/brat Correcting distorted perception of sleep in insomnia: a novel behavioural experiment? Nicole K.Y. Tang, Allison G. Harvey Department

More information

The Use of Bright Light in the Treatment of Insomnia

The Use of Bright Light in the Treatment of Insomnia Chapter e39 The Use of Bright Light in the Treatment of Insomnia Leon Lack and Helen Wright Department of Psychology, Flinders University, Adelaide, South Australia PROTOCOL NAME The use of bright light

More information

What Is the Moment of Sleep Onset for Insomniacs?

What Is the Moment of Sleep Onset for Insomniacs? Sleep, 6(1): 10-\5 1983 Raven Press, New York What Is the Moment of Sleep Onset for Insomniacs? Peter Rauri and Elaine Olmstead Dartmouth Medical School, Hanover, New Hampshire, U,S,A, Summary: Subjective

More information

THE PINEAL hormone melatonin is considered to be one

THE PINEAL hormone melatonin is considered to be one 0021-972X/97/$03.00/0 Vol. 82, No. 11 Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism Printed in U.S.A. Copyright 1997 by The Endocrine Society Relationship between Melatonin Rhythms and Visual Loss in

More information

Index. sleep.theclinics.com. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type.

Index. sleep.theclinics.com. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. A Actigraphy, 475, 485, 496 Adolescents, sleep disorders in, 576 578 Adults, sleep disorders in, 578 580 Advanced sleep phase disorder, 482 Age,

More information

September 26 28, 2013 Westin Tampa Harbour Island. Co-sponsored by

September 26 28, 2013 Westin Tampa Harbour Island. Co-sponsored by September 26 28, 2013 Westin Tampa Harbour Island Co-sponsored by Insights Into Non-24-Hour Disorder Thomas Roth, PhD Wayne State University Detroit, MI University of Michigan School of Medicine Ann Arbor,

More information

The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: Individual Variability and Time of Day Effect in Normal Young Adults

The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: Individual Variability and Time of Day Effect in Normal Young Adults Sleep 13(5):385-394, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 1990 Association of Professional Sleep Societies The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: Individual Variability and Time of Day Effect in Normal Young Adults M.

More information

Wrist Actigraph Versus Self-Report in Normal Sleepers: Sleep Schedule Adherence and Self-Report Validity

Wrist Actigraph Versus Self-Report in Normal Sleepers: Sleep Schedule Adherence and Self-Report Validity BEHAVIORAL SLEEP MEDICINE, 2(3), 134 143 Copyright 2004, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Wrist Actigraph Versus Self-Report in Normal Sleepers: Sleep Schedule Adherence and Self-Report Validity Colleen

More information

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms People typically sleep about 8 hours per day, and spend 16 hours awake. Most people sleep over 175,000 hours in their lifetime. The vast amount of time spent sleeping

More information

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle Sleep 10(1):62-68, Raven Press, New York 1987, Association of Professional Sleep Societies Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components

More information

Drug Review Rozerem (ramelteon)

Drug Review Rozerem (ramelteon) Drug Review Rozerem (ramelteon) Introduction 1 Ramelteon is a melatonin receptor agonist with affinity for MT 1 and MT 2 and selectivity over the MT 3 receptor. The activity at the MT 1 and MT 2 receptors

More information

The New England Journal of Medicine ENTRAINMENT OF FREE-RUNNING CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS BY MELATONIN IN BLIND PEOPLE. Study Design

The New England Journal of Medicine ENTRAINMENT OF FREE-RUNNING CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS BY MELATONIN IN BLIND PEOPLE. Study Design ENTRAINMENT OF FREE-RUNNING CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS BY MELATONIN IN BLIND PEOPLE ROBERT L. SACK, M.D., RICHARD W. BRANDES, B.S., ADAM R. KENDALL, B.S., AND ALFRED J. LEWY, M.D., PH.D. ABSTRACT Background Most

More information

Sleepiness: Its Measurement and Determinants

Sleepiness: Its Measurement and Determinants Sleep, 5:S128-S134 1982 Raven Press, New York Sleepiness: Its Measurement and Determinants T. Roth, T. Roehrs, and F. Zorick Sleep Disorders and Research Center, Henry Ford Hospital, Detroit, Michigan

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION METHODS Participants. All participants were mentally and physically healthy. The average (± SD) body mass index was 21.4 ± 2.7 kg/m 2 in women and 22.8 ± 2.4 kg/m 2 in men (p=0.18).

More information

Utilizing Actigraphy Data and Multi-Dimensional Sleep Domains

Utilizing Actigraphy Data and Multi-Dimensional Sleep Domains Utilizing Actigraphy Data and Multi-Dimensional Sleep Domains Katie L. Stone, Ph.D. For: WHI Investigator Meeting May 3-4, 2018 Chicago, Illinois Objectives Brief overview of sleep actigraphy data collection

More information

INTRINSIC SLEEP DISORDERS. Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is a common complaint. Causes of EDS are numerous and include:

INTRINSIC SLEEP DISORDERS. Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is a common complaint. Causes of EDS are numerous and include: INTRINSIC SLEEP DISORDERS Introduction Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is a common complaint. Causes of EDS are numerous and include: Intrinsic sleep disorders (e.g. narcolepsy, obstructive sleep apnoea/hypopnea

More information

Brief Report: Sleep in Parents of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders

Brief Report: Sleep in Parents of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Brief Report: Sleep in Parents of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Lisa J. Meltzer, PHD Department of Pediatrics and Division of Pulmonary Medicine, University of Pennsylvania and The Children s

More information

Circadian Rhythms and Sleep. Rapid Shift in Sleep Time and Acrophase of Melatonin Secretion in Short Shift Work Schedule

Circadian Rhythms and Sleep. Rapid Shift in Sleep Time and Acrophase of Melatonin Secretion in Short Shift Work Schedule Sleep, 19(7):539-543 1996 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Circadian Rhythms and Sleep Fast Track Publication Rapid Shift in Sleep Time and Acrophase of Melatonin Secretion

More information

Chronobiology and Sleep. Prolonged Interval From Body Temperature Nadir to Sleep Offset in Patients With Delayed Sleep Phase Syndrome

Chronobiology and Sleep. Prolonged Interval From Body Temperature Nadir to Sleep Offset in Patients With Delayed Sleep Phase Syndrome Sleep, 19(1):36-40 1996 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Chronobiology and Sleep Prolonged Interval From Body Temperature Nadir to Sleep Offset in Patients With Delayed Sleep

More information

Definition 1: A fixed point iteration scheme to approximate the fixed point, p, of a function g, = for all n 1 given a starting approximation, p.

Definition 1: A fixed point iteration scheme to approximate the fixed point, p, of a function g, = for all n 1 given a starting approximation, p. Supplemental Material: A. Proof of Convergence In this Appendix, we provide a computational proof that the circadian adjustment method (CAM) belongs to the class of fixed-point iteration schemes (FPIS)

More information

Apps and fitness trackers that measure sleep: Are they useful?

Apps and fitness trackers that measure sleep: Are they useful? REVIEW MEGHNA P. MANSUKHANI, MD Center for Sleep Medicine, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN BHANU PRAKASH KOLLA, MD, MRCPsych Center for Sleep Medicine and Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic,

More information

MORNINGNESS-EVENINGNES05S QUESTIONNAIRE Self-Assessment Version (MEQ-SA) 1. Name: Date:

MORNINGNESS-EVENINGNES05S QUESTIONNAIRE Self-Assessment Version (MEQ-SA) 1. Name: Date: MORNINGNESS-EVENINGNES05S QUESTIONNAIRE Self-Assessment Version (MEQ-SA) 1 Name: Date: For each question, please select the answer that best describes you by circling the point value that best indicates

More information

Chronic Insomnia: DSM - V. Insomnia DSM - V. Patient Symptoms. Insomnia: Assessment and Overview of Management. Insomnia Management in the Digital Age

Chronic Insomnia: DSM - V. Insomnia DSM - V. Patient Symptoms. Insomnia: Assessment and Overview of Management. Insomnia Management in the Digital Age Insomnia Management in the Digital Age Dr Anup Desai Sleep & Respiratory Medicine MBBS (syd), PhD (syd), FRACP Senior Staff Specialist, POW Hospital Medical Director, Sydney Sleep Centre Senior Lecturer,

More information

SLEEP PROBLEMS DURING ADOLESCENCE: LINKS

SLEEP PROBLEMS DURING ADOLESCENCE: LINKS In: The Dance of Sleeping and Eating Among Adolescents ISBN: 978-1-61209-710-7 Editors: Yael Latzer and Orna Tzischinsky 2012 Nova Science Publishers, Inc. Chapter 9 SLEEP PROBLEMS DURING ADOLESCENCE:

More information

Medications that are not FDA approved for children will be discussed. NAPNAP National Conference 2018

Medications that are not FDA approved for children will be discussed. NAPNAP National Conference 2018 Medications that are not FDA approved for children will be discussed NAPNAP National Conference 2018 (Honaker & Meltzer, 2016; Keyes, Maslowsky, Hamilton & Schulenberg, 2015) Chronically disrupted sleep

More information

Effect of individualized social activity on sleep in nursing home residents with dementia Richards K C, Beck C, O'Sullivan P S, Shue V M

Effect of individualized social activity on sleep in nursing home residents with dementia Richards K C, Beck C, O'Sullivan P S, Shue V M Effect of individualized social activity on sleep in nursing home residents with dementia Richards K C, Beck C, O'Sullivan P S, Shue V M Record Status This is a critical abstract of an economic evaluation

More information

Measuring Sleep Habits Without Using a Diary: The Sleep Timing Questionnaire

Measuring Sleep Habits Without Using a Diary: The Sleep Timing Questionnaire INSTRUMENTATION AND METHODOLOGY Measuring Sleep Habits Without Using a Diary: The Sleep Timing Questionnaire Timothy H. Monk, Ph.D., D.Sc., Daniel J. Buysse, M.D., Kathy S. Kennedy, B.A., Jaime M. Potts,

More information

Hypnotic Activity of Melatonin

Hypnotic Activity of Melatonin HYPNOTIC ACTIVITY OF MELATONIN Hypnotic Activity of Melatonin Barbara M. Stone, Claire Turner, Sue L. Mills, and A. N. Nicholson Defence Evaluation and Research Agency, Centre for Human Sciences, Farnborough,

More information

Beyond Sleep Hygiene: Behavioral Approaches to Insomnia

Beyond Sleep Hygiene: Behavioral Approaches to Insomnia Beyond Sleep Hygiene: Behavioral Approaches to Insomnia Rocky Garrison, PhD, CBSM Damon Michael Williams, RN, PMHNP-BC In House Counseling Laughing Heart LLC 10201 SE Main St. 12 SE 14 th Ave. Suite 10

More information

Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Modeling of Actigraphy Derived Sleep Variables

Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Modeling of Actigraphy Derived Sleep Variables 6 The Open Sleep Journal, 2008,, 6-0 Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Modeling of Actigraphy Derived s J.E. Slaven*,, M.E. Andrew, J.M. Violanti 2, C.M. Burchfiel and B.J. Vila 3 Biostatistics and

More information

Study «CLIMSOM & Sleep Quality»

Study «CLIMSOM & Sleep Quality» Study «CLIMSOM & Sleep Quality» 211 Actigraphic Measures 1. Contextof the Study 1 out of 5 suffer from insomnia. The environment is one of the most influential factors on sleep (noise, temperature and

More information

Effects of Shift and Night Work in the Offshore Petroleum Industry: A Systematic Review

Effects of Shift and Night Work in the Offshore Petroleum Industry: A Systematic Review Industrial Health 2013, 51, 530 544 Review Article Effects of Shift and Night Work in the Offshore Petroleum Industry: A Systematic Review Ingrid Nesdal FOSSUM 1 *, Bjørn BJORVATN 2, 3, Siri WAAGE 2, 3

More information

Does melatonin improve sleep in older people? A randomised crossover trial

Does melatonin improve sleep in older people? A randomised crossover trial Age and Ageing 2003; 32: 164 170 Age and Ageing Vol. 32 No. 2 # British Geriatrics Society 2003; all rights reserved Does melatonin improve sleep in older people? A randomised crossover trial JONATHAN

More information

Overview of the Biology of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms

Overview of the Biology of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms Overview of the Biology of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms Daniel J. Buysse, MD UPMC Professor of Sleep Medicine Professor of Psychiatry and Clinical and Translational Science University of Pittsburgh School

More information

Circadian photoreception in humans: More than meets the eye

Circadian photoreception in humans: More than meets the eye DAYLIGHTING (4.430) MIT Architecture Circadian photoreception in humans: More than meets the eye Steven W. Lockley, Ph.D. Division of Sleep Medicine, Brigham and Women s Hospital, Boston, MA Division of

More information

EVALUATION OF SLEEP IN INDIVIDUALS WITH GLAUCOMA By: Dana Bakir M.Sc. Candidate Supervised by: Dr. Olga Overbury and Dr.

EVALUATION OF SLEEP IN INDIVIDUALS WITH GLAUCOMA By: Dana Bakir M.Sc. Candidate Supervised by: Dr. Olga Overbury and Dr. EVALUATION OF SLEEP IN INDIVIDUALS WITH GLAUCOMA By: Dana Bakir M.Sc. Candidate Supervised by: Dr. Olga Overbury and Dr. Caitlin Murphy SCHOOL OF OPTOMETRY, UNIVERSITY OF MONTREAL Sleep Sleep is controlled

More information

Nocturnal road traffic noise and sleep: Day-by-day variability assessed by actigraphy and sleep logs during a one week sampling. Preliminary findings

Nocturnal road traffic noise and sleep: Day-by-day variability assessed by actigraphy and sleep logs during a one week sampling. Preliminary findings Nocturnal road traffic noise and sleep: Day-by-day variability assessed by actigraphy and sleep logs during a one week sampling. Preliminary findings Sandra Pirrera¹*, Elke De Valck¹, Raymond Cluydts¹

More information

Discrepancy between subjective and objective sleep in patients with depression

Discrepancy between subjective and objective sleep in patients with depression PCN Psychiatric and Clinical Neurosciences 1323-13162003 Blackwell Science Pty Ltd 573June 2003 1114 Subjective and objective sleep K. Tsuchiyama et al. 10.1046/j.1323-1316.2003.01114.x Original Article259264BEES

More information

Practice Parameters for the Use of Actigraphy in the Assessment of Sleep and Sleep Disorders: An Update for 2007

Practice Parameters for the Use of Actigraphy in the Assessment of Sleep and Sleep Disorders: An Update for 2007 PRACTICE PARAMETER Practice Parameters for the Use of in the Assessment of Sleep and Sleep Disorders: An Update for 2007 Standards of Practice Committee, American Academy of Sleep Medicine 1 Timothy Morgenthaler,

More information

Clinical Trial Synopsis TL , NCT#

Clinical Trial Synopsis TL , NCT# Clinical Trial Synopsis, NCT#00492011 Title of Study: A Randomized, Double-Blind, Placebo-Controlled, Parallel-Group Study to Evaluate the Ability of Ramelteon 1 mg, 4 mg, and 8 mg to Alleviate the Insomnia

More information

The Forty-Eight Hour Day

The Forty-Eight Hour Day Sleep, 1(2):191197 1978 Raven Press, New York The FortyEight Hour Day Wilse B. Webb Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida Summary: Four normal young adult male subjects

More information

Circadian Period and the Melatonin Rhythm in Men and Women: Predictors of Sleep during the Weekend and in the Laboratory

Circadian Period and the Melatonin Rhythm in Men and Women: Predictors of Sleep during the Weekend and in the Laboratory Circadian Period and the Melatonin Rhythm in Men and Women: Predictors of Sleep during the Weekend and in the Laboratory Running title: Circadian period and weekend sleep JOSR-12-134-R1 Alpar S Lazar*

More information

Non-24-Hour Sleep-Wake Syndrome in a Sighted Man: Circadian Rhythm Studies and Efficacy of Melatonin Treatment

Non-24-Hour Sleep-Wake Syndrome in a Sighted Man: Circadian Rhythm Studies and Efficacy of Melatonin Treatment Sleep, 19(7):544-553 1996 American' Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Non-24-Hour Sleep-Wake Syndrome in a Sighted Man: Circadian Rhythm Studies and Efficacy of Melatonin Treatment

More information

Collaborating to Develop Digital Biomarkers with Passive Data Collection

Collaborating to Develop Digital Biomarkers with Passive Data Collection Collaborating to Develop Digital Biomarkers with Passive Data Collection Iain Simpson IXICO June 2018 1 Setting of Data Collection Market evolution: biosensors and digital biomarkers Clinic Home Digital

More information

SLEEP AND MELATONIN SECRETION ABNORMALITIES IN CHILDREN & ADOLESCENTS WITH FASD DR. S. GORIL DR. D. ZALAI DR. C. SHAPIRO DR. L. A.

SLEEP AND MELATONIN SECRETION ABNORMALITIES IN CHILDREN & ADOLESCENTS WITH FASD DR. S. GORIL DR. D. ZALAI DR. C. SHAPIRO DR. L. A. SLEEP AND MELATONIN SECRETION ABNORMALITIES IN CHILDREN & ADOLESCENTS WITH FASD DR. S. GORIL DR. D. ZALAI DR. C. SHAPIRO DR. L. A. SCOTT SLEEP Pivotal role in brain development during maturation Sleep

More information

Circadian period and the timing of melatonin onset in men and women: predictors of sleep during the weekend and in the laboratory

Circadian period and the timing of melatonin onset in men and women: predictors of sleep during the weekend and in the laboratory J Sleep Res. (213) 22, 155 159 Circadian rhythms Circadian period and the timing of melatonin onset in men and women: predictors of sleep during the weekend and in the laboratory ALPAR S. LAZAR 1 *, NAYANTARA

More information

The Treatment of Early-Morning Awakening Insomnia With 2 Evenings of Bright Light

The Treatment of Early-Morning Awakening Insomnia With 2 Evenings of Bright Light The Treatment of Early-Morning Awakening Insomnia With 2 Evenings of Bright Light Leon Lack, PhD; 1 Helen Wright, PhD; 1,2 Kristyn Kemp, PhD; 2 Samantha Gibbon, MPSYC(Clin); 3 1 School of Psychology, Flinders

More information

Facts about Sleep. Circadian rhythms are important in determining human sleep patterns/ sleep-waking cycle

Facts about Sleep. Circadian rhythms are important in determining human sleep patterns/ sleep-waking cycle Sleep Sleep is described as a state of unconsciousness or partial consciousness from which a person can be roused by stimulation Period of rest and recovery People spend about a third of their lives sleeping

More information

A COMPARISON OF THE PITTSBURGH SLEEP QUALITY INDEX, A NEW SLEEP QUESTIONNAIRE, AND SLEEP DIARIES. Kevin J. Sethi, B.S.

A COMPARISON OF THE PITTSBURGH SLEEP QUALITY INDEX, A NEW SLEEP QUESTIONNAIRE, AND SLEEP DIARIES. Kevin J. Sethi, B.S. A COMPARISON OF THE PITTSBURGH SLEEP QUALITY INDEX, A NEW SLEEP QUESTIONNAIRE, AND SLEEP DIARIES Kevin J. Sethi, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS August

More information

Managing Insomnia: an example sequence of CBT-based sessions for sleep treatment

Managing Insomnia: an example sequence of CBT-based sessions for sleep treatment Managing Insomnia: an example sequence of CBT-based sessions for sleep treatment Session 1: Introduction and sleep assessment -Assess sleep problem (option: have client complete 20-item sleep questionnaire).

More information

Volitional Lifestyle and Nocturnal Sleep in the Healthy Elderly

Volitional Lifestyle and Nocturnal Sleep in the Healthy Elderly Sleep Research Online 4(3): 91-96, 2001 http://www.sro.org/2001/shirota/91/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 1096-214X 2001 WebSciences Volitional Lifestyle and Nocturnal Sleep in the Healthy Elderly

More information

Melatonin treatment and light therapy for chronic sleep onset insomnia in children van Maanen, A.

Melatonin treatment and light therapy for chronic sleep onset insomnia in children van Maanen, A. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Melatonin treatment and light therapy for chronic sleep onset insomnia in children van Maanen, A. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): van Maanen,

More information

Actigraphy. Description. Section: Medicine Effective Date: April 15, 2017

Actigraphy. Description. Section: Medicine Effective Date: April 15, 2017 Subject: Actigraphy Page: 1 of 11 Last Review Status/Date: March 2017 Actigraphy Description Actigraphy refers to the assessment of activity patterns by devices typically placed on the wrist or ankle that

More information

EFFICACY OF MODAFINIL IN 10 TAIWANESE PATIENTS WITH NARCOLEPSY: FINDINGS USING THE MULTIPLE SLEEP LATENCY TEST AND EPWORTH SLEEPINESS SCALE

EFFICACY OF MODAFINIL IN 10 TAIWANESE PATIENTS WITH NARCOLEPSY: FINDINGS USING THE MULTIPLE SLEEP LATENCY TEST AND EPWORTH SLEEPINESS SCALE EFFICACY OF MODAFINIL IN 10 TAIWANESE PATIENTS WITH NARCOLEPSY: FINDINGS USING THE MULTIPLE SLEEP LATENCY TEST AND EPWORTH SLEEPINESS SCALE Shih-Bin Yeh 1 and Carlos Hugh Schenck 2,3 1 Department of Neurology

More information

Dr Alex Bartle. Medical Director Sleep Well Clinic Christchurch

Dr Alex Bartle. Medical Director Sleep Well Clinic Christchurch Dr Alex Bartle Medical Director Sleep Well Clinic Christchurch 11:00-11:55 WS #113: Circadian Sleep Disorders 12:05-13:00 WS #125: Circadian Sleep Disorders (Repeated) Overview The Structure of Sleep

More information

Daytime Alertness in Patients with Chronic Insomnia Compared with Asymptomatic Control Subjects

Daytime Alertness in Patients with Chronic Insomnia Compared with Asymptomatic Control Subjects Sleep I ():54-60, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 988 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Daytime Alertness in Patients with Chronic Insomnia Compared with Asymptomatic Control Subjects Edward Stepanski,

More information

Sleep Management in Parkinson s

Sleep Management in Parkinson s Sleep Management in Parkinson s Booklet 1 Introduction An introduction to Sleep Management in Parkinson s Sleep disturbances are commonly experienced by those with Parkinson s, and by the relatives and

More information

ATHLETES & PRESCRIBING PHYSICIANS PLEASE READ

ATHLETES & PRESCRIBING PHYSICIANS PLEASE READ ATHLETES & PRESCRIBING PHYSICIANS PLEASE READ USADA can grant a Therapeutic Use Exemption (TUE) in compliance with the World Anti- Doping Agency International Standard for TUEs. The TUE application process

More information

Sleep, Fatigue, and Performance. Gregory Belenky, M.D. Sleep and Performance Research Center

Sleep, Fatigue, and Performance. Gregory Belenky, M.D. Sleep and Performance Research Center Sleep, Fatigue, and Performance Gregory Belenky, M.D. The Earth at Night: The Problem of 24/7 Operations The 24-Hour Sleep/Wake Cycle Waking 0000 Slow Wave 1800 0600 REM 1200 Sleep-Related Factors Affecting

More information

SLEEP DISORDERS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children

SLEEP DISORDERS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children SLEEP DISORDERS Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children Distinctive Features of Pediatric Sleep Daytime sleepiness uncommon

More information

TOP 10 LIST OF SLEEP QUESTIONS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Sleep Disorders Unit Massachusetts General Hospital for Children

TOP 10 LIST OF SLEEP QUESTIONS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Sleep Disorders Unit Massachusetts General Hospital for Children TOP 10 LIST OF SLEEP QUESTIONS Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Sleep Disorders Unit Massachusetts General Hospital for Children QUESTION #1: ARE SLEEP ISSUES IN CHILDREN THE SAME AS IN ADULTS? Distinctive Features

More information

Actigraphy. Policy Number: Last Review: 7/2018 Origination: 7/2008 Next Review: 1/2019

Actigraphy. Policy Number: Last Review: 7/2018 Origination: 7/2008 Next Review: 1/2019 Actigraphy Policy Number: 2.01.73 Last Review: 7/2018 Origination: 7/2008 Next Review: 1/2019 Policy Blue Cross and Blue Shield of Kansas City (Blue KC) will not provide coverage for actigraphy. This is

More information

Guidelines for the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT): A Standard Measure of Sleepiness *

Guidelines for the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT): A Standard Measure of Sleepiness * Sleep 9(4):519-524, Raven Press, New York 1986, Association of Professional Sleep Societies Guidelines for the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT): A Standard Measure of Sleepiness * Chairman: Mary A. Carskadon

More information

Overview. Surviving shift work. What is the circadian rhythm? Components of a Generic Biological Timing System 31/10/2017

Overview. Surviving shift work. What is the circadian rhythm? Components of a Generic Biological Timing System 31/10/2017 Overview Surviving shift work Dr Claire M. Ellender Respiratory and Sleep Physician Princess Alexandra Hospital Conflicts nil relevant Circadian rhythm Impacts of shift work on health Case example Circadian

More information

Medical Policy. MP Actigraphy. Related Policies None

Medical Policy. MP Actigraphy. Related Policies None Medical Policy MP 2.01.73 BCBSA Ref. Policy: 2.01.73 Last Review: 09/28/2017 Effective Date: 09/28/2017 Section: Medicine End Date: 06/26/2018 Related Policies None DISCLAIMER Our medical policies are

More information

Ageing and the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep during forced desynchrony of rest, melatonin and temperature rhythms

Ageing and the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep during forced desynchrony of rest, melatonin and temperature rhythms Keywords: 8853 Journal of Physiology (1999), 516.2, pp. 611 627 611 Ageing and the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep during forced desynchrony of rest, melatonin and temperature rhythms

More information

Defining and determining the properties of the human sleep homeostat Zavada, Andrei

Defining and determining the properties of the human sleep homeostat Zavada, Andrei University of Groningen Defining and determining the properties of the human sleep homeostat Zavada, Andrei IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish

More information

Medical Policy An independent licensee of the Blue Cross Blue Shield Association

Medical Policy An independent licensee of the Blue Cross Blue Shield Association Actigraphy Page 1 of 12 Medical Policy An independent licensee of the Blue Cross Blue Shield Association Title: Actigraphy Professional Institutional Original Effective Date: January 1, 2009 Original Effective

More information

Measuring sleep and sleepiness with mobile devices

Measuring sleep and sleepiness with mobile devices Measuring sleep and sleepiness with mobile devices Rebecca M. C. Spencer, PhD Personal Health Monitoring University of Massachusetts, Amherst Sleep IALS Why measure sleep? Memory Decisionmaking Stress

More information

Milena Pavlova, M.D., FAASM Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital Assistant Professor of Neurology, Harvard Medical School Medical

Milena Pavlova, M.D., FAASM Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital Assistant Professor of Neurology, Harvard Medical School Medical Milena Pavlova, M.D., FAASM Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital Assistant Professor of Neurology, Harvard Medical School Medical Director, Faulkner EEG and Sleep Testing Center Course

More information

Articles. Reliability of the Children s Sleep Habits Questionnaire Hebrew Translation and Cross Cultural Comparison of the Psychometric Properties

Articles. Reliability of the Children s Sleep Habits Questionnaire Hebrew Translation and Cross Cultural Comparison of the Psychometric Properties Reliability of the Children s Sleep Habits Questionnaire Hebrew Translation and Cross Cultural Comparison of the Psychometric Properties Orna Tzchishinsky, D.Sc., 1 Dubi Lufi, Ph.D. 1 and Tamar Shochat,

More information

Sleep disorders in visually impaired children. Annotations

Sleep disorders in visually impaired children. Annotations Sleep disorders in visually impaired children Gregory Stores* MA MD FRCP FRCPsych; Paul Ramchandani BM MRCPsych, University of Oxford Section of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Park Hospital for Children,

More information

Individual Planning: A Treatment Plan Overview for Individuals Sleep Disorder Problems.

Individual Planning: A Treatment Plan Overview for Individuals Sleep Disorder Problems. COURSES ARTICLE - THERAPYTOOLS.US Individual Planning: A Treatment Plan Overview for Individuals Sleep Disorder Problems. Individual Planning: A Treatment Plan Overview for Individuals Sleep Disorder Problems.

More information

Exploring daytime sleepiness during migraine

Exploring daytime sleepiness during migraine Exploring daytime sleepiness during migraine J.H.M. TULEN 1, D.L. STRONKS 1,2, L. PEPPLINKHUIZEN 1, J. PASSCHIER 2 DEPARTMENTS OF PSYCHIATRY 1 AND MEDICAL PSYCHOLOGY & PSYCHOTHERAPY 2, UNIVERSITY HOSPITAL

More information

Sleep and School Readiness Sleep Habits and Cognitive Development! An important developmental transition!

Sleep and School Readiness Sleep Habits and Cognitive Development! An important developmental transition! // Sleep and School Readiness Sleep Habits and Cognitive Development An important developmental transition The point when many children are first evaluated for school readiness. School Readiness Victoria

More information

Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics November 2009, Volume 11, Number 11:

Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics November 2009, Volume 11, Number 11: Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics November 2009, Volume 11, Number 11: 876-881. CLINICAL PEARL Managing the Effects of Shift Work in Medicine Holger Link, MD, and Robert Sack,

More information

Effects of a rotating-shift schedule on nurses vigilance as measured by the Psychomotor Vigilance Task

Effects of a rotating-shift schedule on nurses vigilance as measured by the Psychomotor Vigilance Task Originals Effects of a rotating-shift schedule on nurses vigilance as measured by the Psychomotor Vigilance Task Naoko Kiriyama-Suga 1,2), Kouichi Fukunaga 2), Yuko Oguma 1,3) 1) Graduate School of Health

More information

sleep latencies Arousal and sleepiness in insomnia patients the tendency to fall asleep

sleep latencies Arousal and sleepiness in insomnia patients the tendency to fall asleep Arousal and sleepiness in insomnia patients Sleepiness the tendency to fall asleep Elke De Valck, Raymond Cluydts, Sandra Pirrera Department of Cognitive and Physiological Psychology, Vrije Universiteit

More information

Note: Diagnosis and Management of Obstructive Sleep Apnea Syndrome is addressed separately in medical policy

Note: Diagnosis and Management of Obstructive Sleep Apnea Syndrome is addressed separately in medical policy Applies to all products administered or underwritten by Blue Cross and Blue Shield of Louisiana and its subsidiary, HMO Louisiana, Inc.(collectively referred to as the Company ), unless otherwise provided

More information

Actigraphy refers to the assessment of activity patterns by devices typically placed on the wrist or ankle

Actigraphy refers to the assessment of activity patterns by devices typically placed on the wrist or ankle Subject: Actigraphy Page: 1 of 13 Last Review Status/Date: June 2015 Actigraphy Description Actigraphy refers to the assessment of activity patterns by devices typically placed on the wrist or ankle that

More information

Citation Hong Kong Practitioner, 1997, v. 19 n. 12, p

Citation Hong Kong Practitioner, 1997, v. 19 n. 12, p Title Melatonin use in sleep disorders Author(s) Chung, KF Citation Hong Kong Practitioner, 1997, v. 19 n. 12, p. 669-672 Issued Date 1997 URL http://hdl.handle.net/10722/45309 Rights This work is licensed

More information

November 24, External Advisory Board Members:

November 24, External Advisory Board Members: November 24, 2010 To: Fred W. Turek, Ph.D. Charles E. & Emma H. Morrison Professor of Biology Director, Center for Sleep and Circadian Biology Northwestern University RE: External Advisory Board Report

More information

Consciousness. Mind-body Problem. Cartesian Substance Dualism 2/2/11. Fundamental issue addressed by psychologists Dualism. Monism

Consciousness. Mind-body Problem. Cartesian Substance Dualism 2/2/11. Fundamental issue addressed by psychologists Dualism. Monism Consciousness Mind-body Problem Fundamental issue addressed by psychologists Dualism Mind is immaterial Mind can exist separate from the body Monism Mind and body are different aspects of the same thing

More information

Glasgow, 1055 Great Western Road, Glasgow G12 0XH, UK

Glasgow, 1055 Great Western Road, Glasgow G12 0XH, UK Title and running title: Conscientiousness predicts diurnal preference Alexandra L. Hogben* 1 Jason Ellis* 2 Simon N. Archer 1 Malcolm von Schantz 1 * These authors contributed equally to this work 1 Surrey

More information

Recent Advances in Understanding Sleep and Sleep Disturbances in Older Adults Growing Older Does Not Mean Sleeping Poorly

Recent Advances in Understanding Sleep and Sleep Disturbances in Older Adults Growing Older Does Not Mean Sleeping Poorly CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Recent Advances in Understanding Sleep and Sleep Disturbances in Older Adults Growing Older Does Not Mean Sleeping Poorly Michael V. Vitiello Departments of

More information

Sleep This factsheet is available in a downloadable PDF here. Table of Contents Introduction Insomnia How Long does Insomnia Last? How much Sleep should You get? Symptoms of Insomnia Causes of Insomnia

More information

Time-of-Day Effect on Academic Achievement of Adolescents

Time-of-Day Effect on Academic Achievement of Adolescents Time-of-Day Effect on Academic Achievement of Adolescents Phuc Thanh Luong Advisor: Professor Giovanni Peri University of California, Davis Department of Economics March 2017 ECN 194H: A-B ABSTRACT This

More information