Effects of Nicotine Deprivation on Alcohol-Related Information Processing and Drinking Behavior

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Effects of Nicotine Deprivation on Alcohol-Related Information Processing and Drinking Behavior"

Transcription

1 Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2000, Vol. 109, No. 1, Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc X/00/S5.00 DOI: // X Effects of Nicotine Deprivation on Alcohol-Related Information Processing and Drinking Behavior Tibor P. Palfai Boston University Peter M. Monti Providence Veterans Affairs Medical Center and Brown University Brian Ostafm Boston University Kent Hutchison University of Colorado at Boulder This study examined the influence of smoking cues and nicotine deprivation on responses to alcohol among hazardous drinkers. Fifty-six daily smoking, hazardous drinkers were exposed to either smoking cues or control cues after either 6 hr of nicotine deprivation or no deprivation. Urges to drink alcohol, alcohol-related cognitive processing, and alcohol consumption were assessed after cue exposure. Results indicated that nicotine deprivation increased urges to drink, the accessibility of alcohol outcome expectancies, and the volume of alcohol consumed. There was little influence of the smoking cue manipulation on these processes. Implications for understanding the mechanisms underlying alcoholtobacco interactions are discussed. The association between smoking and alcohol consumption has been well established in both clinical and nonclinical populations (Henningfield, Clayton, & Pollin, 1990; Istvan & Matazarro, 1984). Alcohol use is positively correlated with a number of indices of smoking, including number of cigarettes smoked per day and nicotine dependence (Monti, Rohsenow, Colby, & Abrams, 1995). Over 80% of alcoholics smoke, and they tend to be heavier smokers than individuals in the normal population (Hughes, 1993). Similarly, smokers are more likely to drink alcohol and drink larger quantities than nonsmokers (Zacny, 1990). Both alcohol and nicotine dependence place an individual at higher risk for dependence on the other substance (Henningfield et al., 1990; Kozlowski et al., 1993), and there is some evidence that use patterns of one substance may jeopardize efforts to reduce use of the other substance (Murray, Istvan, Voelker, Rigdon, & Wallace, 1995; Sobell, Sobell, Kozlowski, & Toneatto, 1990; Zimmerman, Warheit, Ulbrich, & Auth, 1990). The co-occurrence of alcohol and tobacco use presents a dilemma to health professionals who are trying to promote change in individuals who use both of these substances. For clinicians treating nicotine-dependent problem drinkers, a primary question is Tibor P. Palfai, Department of Psychology, Boston University; Peter M. Monti, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Providence Veterans Affairs Medical Center, and Center for Alcohol and Addiction Studies, Brown University; Brian Ostafln, Department of Psychology, Boston University; Kent Hutchison, Department of Psychology, University of Colorado at Boulder. This research was funded by Grant R03AA from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism and by a Department of Veterans Affairs Career Research Scientist Award. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Tibor P. Palfai, 64 Cummington Street, Boston, Massachusetts Electronic mail may be sent to palfai@bu.edu. whether continued use of tobacco puts patients at risk for relapse to problem drinking patterns or whether nicotine abstinence may jeopardize efforts to change drinking behavior (Monti et al., 1995; Zacny, 1990). The ability to answer this question and to design optimal interventions will come from a better understanding of the mechanisms by which nicotine deprivation and nicotine use are associated with alcohol consumption. There is evidence to suggest that the use of alcohol or nicotine may increase motivation for the other substance. Studies of tobacco and alcohol use have demonstrated that the amount of alcohol or tobacco used by an individual within different contexts tends to covary with the use of the other substance. Laboratory studies have shown that heavy drinkers tend to show higher rates of smoking when they are given higher doses of alcohol (see Henningfield, 1984; Mintz, Boyd, Rose, Charuvastra, & Jarvik, 1985; Nil, Buzzi, & Battig, 1984), and studies have reported more anticipatory reward and postsmoking satisfaction after alcohol consumption (Glautier, Clements, White, Taylor. & Stolerman, 1996). Alcohol consumption has been shown to increase urges to smoke independent of exposure to explicit smoking cues (Burton & Tiffany, 1997). Increased self-administration of alcohol has been shown to be associated with increased rates of smoking in both laboratory (Mello, Mendelson, Sellers, & Kuehnle, 1980) and naturalistic studies (Shiffman et al., 1994). Considerably less is known about the cross-substance effects of nicotine deprivation. However, there is strong theoretical reason to believe that nicotine deprivation may increase motivation for alcohol. Behavioral choice models of alcohol use (Vuchinich & Tucker, 1988) are based on the well-established finding that individuals respond to restrictions on reinforcers with increased pursuit of alternate available reinforcers. Naturalistic studies of problem drinkers (Tucker, Vuchinich, & Pukish, 1995; Vuchinich & Tucker, 1996) and laboratory studies with social drinkers (Vuchinich & Simpson, 1998; Vuchinich & Tucker, 1983) have shown

2 NICOTINE DEPRIVATION AND RESPONSES TO ALCOHOL 97 that the way that individuals respond to alcohol varies with the value of alternative reinforcers and the constraints placed on them. Because these processes appear to be important across multiple substances (Higgins, 1997; Vuchinich & Tucker, 1988), restrictions on access to nicotine may result in an increase in alcohol use for those who use both substances for reinforcement. In addition to its role as an alternative reinforcer, alcohol use may provide a means of managing the aversive subjective consequences of nicotine deprivation. Two of the more frequently observed responses to nicotine deprivation are increased urges to smoke and negative affect. Not surprisingly, daily smokers who are deprived of nicotine report higher urges to smoke in laboratory studies (Maude-Griffin & Tiffany, 1996; Sayette & Hufford, 1994). They also exhibit higher levels of negative affect than those who are not deprived (Drobes & Tiffany, 1997; Zinser, Baker, Sherman, & Cannon, 1992), and longer periods of deprivation have been associated with higher levels of negative affect in some studies (Payne, Smith, Sturges, & Holleran, 1996). Urges to smoke in conjunction with restrictions on smoking behavior are likely to be aversive to most smokers (Tiffany, 1990). Indeed, negative affect and urges to smoke appear to be highly correlated for those who are deprived of nicotine (Zinser et al., 1992) or those who are exposed to smoking cues without being permitted access to cigarettes (Brandon, Wetter, & Baker, 1996; Drobes & Tiffany, 1997; Elash, Tiffany, & Vrana, 1995). Nicotine deprivation may result in enhanced motivation to drink alcohol because of a desire to reduce these subjective states. Social learning (Niaura et al., 1988; Marlatt, 1985), motivational (Cox & Klinger, 1988), and tension reduction theories (Cappell & Greeley, 1987) have clarified the important function of alcohol as a means of coping with aversive subjective states. Research has identified the central role of negative affect as a motive for drinking (Cooper, Russell, Skinner, & Windle, 1992) and a trigger for relapse (Marlatt, 1985). Moreover, individuals who have stronger expectancies of tension reduction from alcohol appear to drink more frequently and consume greater quantities of alcohol (Hittner, 1995; Mann, Chassin, & Sher, 1987). According to this coping hypothesis, individuals may show heightened urges to drink and/or increased alcohol consumption after nicotine deprivation in an effort to reduce negative affect and urges to smoke. Although coping processes may mediate the link between nicotine deprivation and alcohol motivation, a number of investigators have suggested that nicotine deprivation may have a much broader influence on drug motivational systems through the activation of substance use schema (Baker, Morse, & Sherman, 1987; Goldman & Rather, 1993; Tiffany, 1990). Adopting Lang's (1979) bioinformational model, it has been suggested that drug motivation may be understood as the activation of information schema concerning drug effects and drug use. These substance use schema contain information about stimulus cues; verbal, physiological, and behavioral responses; and interpretive information about the meaning of stimulus and response elements (Niaura, Goldstein, & Abrams, 1991). According to this perspective, the presentation of drug cues, whether internal (e.g., a memory of a typical smoking context) or external (e.g., observing someone smoke a cigarette), activates specific types of information in memory and modifies cognitive processing that guides substance use behavior. Theoretical models used to explain cross-substance effects have largely been influenced by research on use patterns of single substances. Motivation to use a given drug may be enhanced by appetitive stimuli, such as drug administration (Stewart, dewit, & Eikelboom, 1984; Wise, 1988), and withdrawal-eliciting stimuli, such as drug deprivation (Wilder, 1973). In an effort to integrate appetitive and withdrawal-based mechanisms of addiction, Baker et al. (1987) proposed that the motivation to use substances was mediated by two distinct urge networks, a positive affect urge network and a negative affect urge network. The positive affect network includes information on signals of drug availability, appetitive stimuli, cognitions about drug outcomes, and positive affect. According to this model, the use of a substance stimulates the positive affect network, a motivational system that mediates the pursuit of appetitive consequences. The negative affect urge network, on the other hand, contains information on withdrawalassociated physiology and behavior, stimuli signaling drug unavailability, negative affect, and expectations of the drugs' desirable effects. The activation of this network is due to drug withdrawal and is hypothesized to increase urges, increase negative affect, and increase expectations of the drugs' desirable effects. Baker et al. (1987) suggest that individuals are more likely to pursue drugs because the incentive salience of the drug is increased by deprivation. Information-processing models not only provide a framework for understanding the association between types of substancerelated responses and triggers, but they also elucidate specific changes in cognitive processing that may take place during a heightened drug motivational state. Two processes that appear to be important in motivation for alcohol and nicotine are substancespecific perceptual processing and substance use outcome expectancy accessibility. There have been a number of studies that have provided support for the view that chronic substance use and cue-induced drug motivational states may be associated with heightened processing of substance-specific cues. Studies using a variety of cognitive interference paradigms, such as the Stroop task (Johnsen, Laberg, Cox, Vaksdal, & Hugdahl, 1994) and dual-processing vigilance tasks (Sayette et al., 1994), have suggested that alcoholics exhibit greater interference in cognitive processing than social drinkers when the distraction information is alcohol related. Moreover, alcohol-specific Stroop interference among alcoholic patients appears to be associated with daily alcohol intake (Stetter, Ackermann, Bizer, Straube, & Mann, 1995). Research has supported the view that nicotine deprivation may cause changes in the speed with which smokers process smoking-related cues. Smokers who are deprived of nicotine appear to exhibit cue-specific interference from smoking-related information on the modified Stroop (Gross, Jarvik, & Rosenblatt, 1993) and dual-processing vigilance tasks (Sayette & Hufford, 1994). Thus, external smoking cues impair processing on tasks that require that the participant ignore substance cues. Conversely, on tasks that require attention to substance cues, nicotine deprivation appears to increase the speed at which individuals process smoking-related information as measured by lexical decision tasks (Jarvik, Gross, Rosenblatt, & Stein, 1995). In addition to changes in the way that external information is processed, substance-related cues may modify the processing of information about drug effects (Cox & Klinger, 1988; Goldman & Rather, 1993). Both smoking (Brandon et al., 1996) and alcohol researchers (Goldman & Rather, 1993) have pointed to the impor-

3 98 PALFAI, MONTI, OSTAFIN, AND HUTCHISON tance of reinforcement expectancies as predictors of drug motivation. Positive expectancies about smoking have been shown to predict smoking frequency and relapse (Brandon et al., 1996). Similarly, alcohol outcome expectancies have been shown to be important correlates of the frequency and intensity of alcohol use across a number of studies (e.g., Goldman, Brown, Christiansen, & Smith, 1991; Rather, Goldman, Roehrich, & Brannick, 1992) and to predict subsequent drinking patterns among nonclinical (Christiansen, Smith, Roehling, & Goldman, 1989; Stacy, 1997) and clinical populations (Brown, 1985; Jones & McMahon, 1994). Although there are different models of how alcohol expectancies may influence alcohol motivation (Goldman & Rather, 1993; Marlatt, 1985; Niaura et al., 1988; Oei & Baldwin, 1994), they share the view that drug motivational states are influenced by the activation of these expectancies in memory. Research has shown that the presentation of alcohol cues in the environment increases the accessibility of these expectancies as measured by performance on implicit memory tasks with alcohol expectancy words (Goldman & Rather, 1993). Moreover, direct manipulations of expectancy accessibility have been shown to increase alcohol volume consumed (Roehrich & Goldman, 1995). Motivation for substances appears to be characterized by a broad range of behavioral, physiological, cognitive, and subjective responses. There is also suggestive evidence that cues for one substance may elicit cross-substance responses. Urges to smoke appear to be increased upon presentation of alcohol cues for alcoholics in treatment (Rohsenow et al., 1997), and the association between urges to drink and smoke are stronger when alcoholics are presented with alcohol cues (Gulliver et al., 1995). The propositional network memory model may help explain the effects of cues on cross-substance use (Baker et al., 1987). Through repeated co-occurrent use, and use in response to similar triggers (e.g., negative affect), smoking-related and drinking-related structures become associated in memory. Thus, the presentation of cues for smoking may automatically prime alcohol-related information. Cross-priming in memory may serve to increase speed and selectivity of alcohol cue processing, enhance the accessibility of alcohol-related outcomes, increase urges to drink, and intensify drinking behavior. The present study focused on providing a clearer understanding of how smoking-related cues in the environment and efforts to refrain from smoking influence processes related to alcohol consumption among daily smoking hazardous drinkers, nondependent alcohol users whose drinking patterns put them at risk for alcoholrelated harm (Higgins-Biddle & Babor, 1996). Using a factorial design, the independent and combined effects of mild nicotine deprivation and cigarette cues on alcohol-related information processing, urges to drink, and alcohol consumption were examined. It was hypothesized that the presence of smoking cues and nicotine withdrawal would intensify urges to drink, increase the accessibility of alcohol expectancies, and increase the speed of lexical processing of alcohol stimulus words. Moreover, it was predicted that these smoking triggers would increase urges to drink alcohol in anticipation of consumption and increase beer consumption during a taste-test procedure (Marlatt, Demling, & Reid, 1973). Through correlational and mediational analyses, this study examined the nature of the association between responses to smoking triggers and measures of motivation to consume alcohol. Participants Method Fifty-six participants were recruited from posted advertisements and college and local newspaper advertisements that requested social drinkers who were smokers. Inclusion criteria were: (a) hazardous drinkers (Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test [AUDIT; Babor & Grant, 1992] scores of 8 or above) whose most frequently consumed beverage was beer, (b) age between 21 and 45 years, and (c) daily smokers of over 10 cigarettes per day. Participants were excluded if they had been hospitalized for psychiatric treatment, had been treated for alcohol abuse or dependence, were currently taking medications that could interact with alcohol, or were known to be pregnant. The final sample was evenly divided between males and females,' was predominantly Caucasian (93%), and had a mean age of 26.4 (SO = 6.34). Participants scored a mean of (SD = 5.65) on the AUDIT and consumed an average of (SD = 82.47) standard alcoholic drinks over the preceding month. They smoked an average of 17.6 cigarettes per day (SD = 6.33), scored a mean of 6.04 (SD = 1.8) on the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire (Fagerstrom, 1978), and reported regular smoking for a mean of 9.39 years (SD 5.9). Design The study was a 2 X 2 factorial design, with nicotine deprivation status (deprived and nondeprived) and cue (smoking cue vs. control cue) as the between-subjects variables. Participants were randomly assigned to one of four conditions stratified by gender. Additional analyses were conducted on urge and affect ratings that were taken at a number of points during the protocol. Three conceptually distinct phases for each participant were analyzed for these ratings in a 2 (nicotine deprivation) X 2 (cue) X 3 (trial) mixed factorial design. Measures AUDIT. The AUDIT (Babor & Grant, 1992) is a 10-item screening instrument used to identify persons whose alcohol consumption may be harmful to their health. Hazardous drinking scores are based on frequency of heavy consumption, frequency of intoxication, number of alcoholrelated problems, and concern about drinking. It has been cross-culturally validated and has been shown to have a adequate reliability. A cutoff score of 8 has been shown to detect 92% of those identified within primary care settings as being hazardous drinkers, with 94% of nonhazardous drinkers falling below this cutoff (Saunders et al., 1993). Time-line follow back (Sobell, Maisto, Sobell, & Cooper, This calendar-based instrument is used to gain information regarding patterns of drinking including frequency, intensity, and type of beverage consumed. It demonstrates high test-retest reliability and has been validated across a variety of alcohol-using populations. Fagerstrom tolerance questionnaire (Fagerstrom, 1978). This questionnaire is an eight-item, 11-point index that assesses smoking dependence. This highly utilized instrument provides information about number of cigarettes per day, nicotine yield, and contextual and temporal conditions of smoking (Fagerstrom & Schneider, 1989). Withdrawal symptoms questionnaire (Shijfinan & Jarvik, I976/, This is a 25-item, Likert-scale measure that is used to assess nicotine withdrawal symptoms. Four subscales have been shown to be reliable and valid 1 Post hoc analyses were also conducted with gender as an additional independent variable. There was no evidence that gender interacted with cue or deprivation for any analysis. Consequently, gender effects are not discussed further.

4 NICOTINE DEPRIVATION AND RESPONSES TO ALCOHOL 99 indicators of nicotine withdrawal: craving, physical symptoms, psychological discomfort, and stimulation-sedation. Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). This 20-item measure provides assessment of positive (10 items) and negative affect (10 items). Participants rate positive and negative adjectives on a 7-point Likert scale. The subscales have good internal consistency and test retest reliability (Watson et al., 1988). This measure has been used extensively in studies of mood including studies of mood variability among smokers (Zinser et al., 1992). Urge rating forms. Urge to drink and urge to smoke measures were taken after each experimental trial. Participants rated their urge on two 11-point Likert scales. These ratings have been used in several previous studies as well validated and reliable self-report measures of craving (Rohsenow et al., 1997). Taste test. To measure alcohol consumption, a modified taste-test procedure (Marlatt et al., 1973) was used. Participants were informed that they would be tasting three different brands of beer. Three glasses were presented to each participant, each of which contained 360 ml of beer. Each glass contained 240 ml of nonalcoholic beer, which has effectively been used as placebo (Martin, Earleywine, & Young, 1990), and 120 ml of one of three different alcoholic beers. Participants were provided with a tastetest rating form and were told that a series of adjectives would be presented on the computer screen one at a time. They were informed that they would not be expected to complete the ratings of all the adjectives during the taste test. For each adjective, participants were asked to rate each of the three beverages. Participants were instructed that they could take as much time as needed to make each rating and sample as much as they liked to make each judgment. A computer key was pressed to move from one adjective to the next. They were given 10 min to sample the beer and make their ratings of each adjective, although they were not explicitly informed about the duration of the task. As in Marlatt et al. (1973), the task was designed so that participants would continue tasting throughout the taste-test period. Consequently, no participant finished the rating task before the end of the 10-min period, Computerized expectancy task. This task was utilized to assess the accessibility of positive and negative alcohol outcome expectancies {Palfai, Monti, Colby, & Rohsenow, 1997). The task was adapted from work on attitude accessibility (Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986) and involves the use of response times to assess momentary changes in the degree to which expectancy information is accessible to the participant. Alcohol expectancy items were adapted from the Alcohol Expectancies Inventory (Rather & Goldman, 1994). For the alcohol-related items, participants were presented with the phrase "Alcohol makes me" followed by a stimulus word. Using a computer key, participants were asked to respond as quickly as possible to indicate whether or not the expectancy item applied to their own experience with alcohol. Participants also made timed responses to a series of traits that they judged as either applicable or not applicable to themselves. These self-items served as an internal control for response speed. These items followed the phrase, "I am generally." Expectancy accessibility was calculated as the difference between response time to endorse self-items versus alcohol-related items. Lexical decision task. To measure the processing of alcohol-related information, participants performed a lexical decision task after exposure to either control or cigarette cues. The lexical decision task consisted of a series of 80 letter strings presented on an IBM microcomputer one at a time. Upon presentation of each letter string, participants had to judge by means of a response key whether the string was a word or a nonword. Of these letter strings, 20 items were alcohol-related words (e.g., drink, bottle), 20 were control items that were matched for length and frequency in the English language (Francis & KuCera, 1982), and 40 were nonsense strings. Presentation of letter strings was randomized for each participant, and lexical decision response times were recorded using Micro Experimental Laboratory (Schneider, 1988) computer software. Response times to control and alcohol-related items provided information regarding the influence of smoking cues on information-processing speed and assessed the relative speed with which participants processed alcohol-related information. Procedure Participants were screened by phone to determine eligibility for the study. During the initial phone interview, the study was briefly described and participants were informed that they might be asked to abstain from smoking for 6 hr before the study. Those who expressed interest were then screened. After the screening process, all participants were instructed not to consume alcohol within 12 hr of the study and were asked to either not smoke for one half hour (nondeprived) or 6 hr before the study (nicotine deprived). No participant declined participation after the assignment to condition. To increase compliance, participants were informed that they would be asked to sign an affidavit and provide breath samples to ensure that they met these conditions before participating. They were informed that if they did not meet these criteria at the time of testing they would be excluded without compensation. (No participant was actually excluded on this basis.) Carbon monoxide (CO) samples were taken upon the participants' arrival at the laboratory. Because this study involved alcohol consumption, all female participants were asked to take a pregnancy test before participation, and all participants had to present proof of age (e.g., driver's license). After completing consent and preexperimental procedures, participants in the nondeprived condition were asked to smoke one of their usual cigarettes. This was done to ensure similar levels of nicotine deprivation for those in the nondeprived condition (Payne et al., 1996). Participants then completed a questionnaire battery that included a variety of measures of smoking and drinking history. Thirty minutes after participants began these measures, the baseline procedure began. Participants were instructed lo sit quietly for 5 min while heart rate and skin conductance level (SCL) were taken (results of these measures are discussed elsewhere). After the baseline procedure, participants were asked lo rate their urges to drink, urges to smoke, and affect. Participants were then presented with the smoking cue exposure task modified from Sayette and Hufford (1994). In this task, participants were presented with either smoking or control cues in two 1-min cue exposure periods. In the smoking cue condition, participants were presented with a lighter, an ashtray, a cigarette, and a packet of their favorite cigarette- At the beginning of each trial participants were asked to light the cigarette and hold it comfortably in their dominant hand. Participants in the control cue condition were presented with a pencil and a packet of pencils. They were asked to lift the pencil and hold it comfortably in their dominant hand in a writing posture for 1 min. After each trial, participants were asked to rate their urge to smoke, urge to drink, and affect. After each of the cue exposure trials, participants completed one of two computer tasks, the alcohol expectancy task or the lexical decision task, in counterbalanced order. The second computer task was followed by a 1-min relaxation period followed by a 1-min anticipation period. During this phase, the beer for the taste-test procedure was brought before the participants, and they were reminded about the upcoming drinking task that was to take place in the next few minutes. After this 1-min anticipation phase, participants again filled out subjective rating forms. Upon completion of these measures, participants were instructed on the taste-test procedure and were allowed to begin tasting for the 10-min period. After rating the three types of beer on a variety of dimensions, participants filled out a final rating of urge to smoke, urge to drink, and affect. The cue exposure protocol is summarized in Figure I. Manipulation Check Results Although the experimental protocol precluded multiple assessment of CO levels, breath samples were taken to compare CO

5 100 PALFAI, MONTI, OSTAFIN, AND HUTCHISON Questionnaires» Baseline * [Trial 1: Baseline Ratings] * Cue Exposure #1 Cognitive Task #1 > Cue Exposure #2» [Trial 2: Cue Exposure Ratings] * Cognitive Task #2 > [Trial 3: Anticipation Ratings] * Taste Test Figure 1. Experimental procedure and measurement. levels between deprived and nondeprived groups. Results indicated that CO levels were significantly greater for the nondeprived smokers (M = 22.3, SD 7.8) than for deprived smokers (M = 12.1, SD = 10.3), F(l, 55) = 5.66, p <.01. Multivariate analysis of the Withdrawal Symptoms Questionnaire subscales demonstrated a significant difference between groups, F(4, 48) = 7.39, p <.001. Subsequent univariate tests showed that deprived participants reported significantly greater craving and psychological discomfort than nondeprived participants. Baseline Data To compare experimental characteristics across conditions, we conducted a 2 (nicotine deprivation) X 2 (cue) aualysis of variance (ANOVA) on AUDIT scores, standard drinks per month, and average number of cigarettes per day. No group differences were observed. Urge to Smoke A 2 X 2 X 3 (trial) ANOVA was conducted to examine the influence of nicotine deprivation and cigarette cue on urge to smoke at the different time periods: after baseline, after the cue exposure trials, and after the anticipation of alcohol consumption. Greenhouse-Geiser corrections were used in this and subsequent within-subject analyses to protect against within-trial covariance heterogeneity. Mean urge to smoke ratings by condition are presented in Table 1. Results indicated a main effect for nicotine deprivation only. F(l, 51) = 13.25, p <.05. Post-cue exposure ratings of urge to smoke were not influenced by cue condition, and there was no interaction with nicotine deprivation condition. Thus, the presentation of the cigarette cue did not appear to intensify urge to smoke ratings for either nicotine-deprived or nondeprived participants. Affect Ratings Ratings of positive and negative affect were subjected to separate 2 X 2 X 3 repeated-measures ANOVAs. As expected, participants in the deprived condition exhibited significantly higher levels of negative affect, F(l, 51) = 8.93, p <.05, than those in the nondeprived condition. This difference did not interact with trial or cue type. Negative affect ratings remained stable from baseline (for deprived, M = 18.7, SD = 7.3; for nondeprived, M 13.6, SD 5.0) to post-cue exposure (for deprived, M = 18.5, SD = 7.0; for nondeprived, M = 13.9, SD = 5.2). Ratings of positive affect, on the other hand, were not influenced by nicotine deprivation or cue. Urge to Drink A 2 X 2 X 3 (trial) ANOVA was conducted to examine the influence of nicotine deprivation and cigarette cue on urge to drink at baseline, after cue exposure, and in anticipation of alcohol consumption. Results indicated a main effect for nicotine deprivation, F(l, 51) = 6.79, p <.05, that was qualified by a Nicotine Deprivation X Trial interaction, F(2, 102) = 4.48, p <.05. Mean urge ratings by nicotine deprivation condition are presented in Figure 2. Simple effects analyses demonstrated a significant difference between baseline ratings and anticipation ratings of urge to drink for participants in the deprived condition. This difference Table 1 Urge to Smoke Ratings by Deprivation and Cue Condition Baseline Cue exposure Anticipation Variable M SD M SD M SD NONDEP-CTRL NONDEP-CIG DEP-CTRL DEP-CIG Baseline Cue Exposure Anticipation Note. NONDEP-CTRL = nondeprived/control exposure; NONDEP- CIG = nondeprived/cigarelte exposure; DEP-CTRL - nicotine deprived/ control exposure; DEP-CIG = nicotine deprived/cigarette exposure. Figure 2. Trial Urge to drink ratings by nicotine deprivation across trials.

6 NICOTINE DEPRIVATION AND RESPONSES TO ALCOHOL 101 was not shown for participants in the nondeprived condition. This finding is consistent with the view that deprivation may increase incentive motivation for alcohol. Expectancy Accessibility Because the shape of the data distribution was positively skewed, response latencies to alcohol expectancy words and selfrelated items were subjected to a negative reciprocal transformation. Mean scores were calculated for each participant on items that were endorsed (i.e., "yes" responses) for both alcohol expectancies and self-related items. Participants endorsed more positive expectancies (M 8.21, SD = 3.87) than negative expectancies (M = 5.98, SD = 3.36), and 2 participants did not endorse any negative expectancies. There were no effects of deprivation or cue on response latencies to alcohol expectancies or self-related items; however, participants endorsed self-related items more quickly than alcohol-related items. To assess accessibility (Fazio et al., 1986), difference scores were calculated between response times to positive expectancies and self-related items and between response times to negative expectancies and self-related items. Because there was no significant interaction between expectancy type and the independent variables, a 2 (cue) X 2 (nicotine deprivation) ANOVA was conducted with mean response differences between alcohol expectancy and self-related items. Smaller differences between expectancy responses and self-responses were indicative of greater accessibility of alcohol outcome expectancies. These analyses showed a significant main effect for nicotine deprivation, F(\, 52) = 5.47, p <.05, suggesting that participants who were deprived responded relatively faster to alcohol expectancy items than those who were not (for deprived group, M.10, SD =.11; for nondeprived group, M =.18, SD =.12). There was no effect for cue, however, F(l, 52) =.27, p - ns. Lexical Decision Performance Because of the distributional characteristics of these data, response latencies to the lexical decision words were normalized using a logarithmic transformation. Two separate mean scores were then calculated for each participant, one for alcohol and one for control words. Response times to identify alcohol words were subtracted from control items to produce a measure of alcoholrelated processing speed. A one-way ANOVA did not demonstrate an effect of cue, F(\, 52) = 1.47, p = ns, or nicotine deprivation, F(l, 52) = 2.46,p = ns, on alcohol-related processing. Thus, these analyses did not support an effect of smoking cue or nicotine deprivation on the relative speed with which individuals process alcohol-related external cues. Taste Test A modified taste-test methodology was utilized to examine the influence of nicotine deprivation and external smoking cues on alcohol consumption. It was hypothesized that those in the cigarette cue and deprived conditions would consume more alcohol during the taste-test procedure. A 2 X 2 ANOVA revealed a main effect for nicotine deprivation status on amount of alcohol consumed, F(l, 52) = 5.02, p <.05. Participants who were in the nicotine-deprived condition (M = , SO = ) consumed more beer than those in the nondeprived condition (M = 250.6, SD = ). However, there were no differences between the cue groups (cigarette vs. pencil) on consumption, F(1, 52) =.09, nor was there a Cue X Deprivation interaction, F(\, 52) =.55. Mediations! analyses were conducted to examine whether the effects of nicotine deprivation on alcohol consumption could be explained by changes in negative affect, urge to smoke, urge to drink, or the accessibility of alcohol expectancies. Ratings after the cue exposure were used in these mediational analyses. Separate regression analyses were conducted to examine whether nicotine deprivation was significantly associated with the mediational variable, whether the mediational variable predicted volume consumed, and whether the effect of nicotine deprivation on drinking volume was reduced by regressing volume on nicotine deprivation and the mediator simultaneously (Baron & Kenney, 1986; Holmbeck, 1997). These analyses suggested that urge to smoke, urge to drink, and alcohol expectancies each served as a partial mediators for the relation between nicotine deprivation and drinking. Nicotine deprivation significantly predicted volume (j8 =.29, p <.05), post-cue exposure ratings of urge to drink (j9.30, p <.05), alcohol expectancy activation ()3 =.31, p <.05), and urges to smoke (/3 =.39, p <.05). Moreover, urges to smoke (0 =.28, p <.05), urges to drink (/3 =.46, p <.05), and alcohol expectancy latencies ( 3 =.34, p <.05) each predicted volume consumed. Post-cue exposure negative affect was not significantly associated with drinking volume (/3 =.15, p = ns). When nicotine deprivation and the predictor variables were entered simultaneously, the effect of nicotine deprivation on drinking volume was reduced by alcohol expectancies ((3 =.21), urges to drink (fi =.17), and urges to smoke ( =.22), suggesting partial mediation. Finally, to examine the association between urge to drink in anticipation of drinking and actual consumption, urge ratings during anticipation were regressed on volume. Again, urges to drink before alcohol consumption was also strong predictors of volume (13 =.59, p <.05). When urges to drink and nicotine deprivation were entered simultaneously, nicotine deprivation status no longer predicted drinking behavior ( 3 =.05, p = ns). Although urges to smoke after cue exposure was also positively correlated with alcohol consumption (r.28, p <.05), this relation was no longer, significant when urge to drink was controlled (pr.15). Conversely, urge to drink was still a significant predictor of drinking behavior even when urge to smoke was controlled (pr.43). Correlations Among Post-Cue Exposure Measures, Urge to Drink in Anticipation of Drinking, and Alcohol Consumption Correlations between alcohol expectancy response times, subjective ratings after the cue exposure phase, urge to drink during anticipation, and drinking volume are presented in Table 2. Significant associations were observed between each of these measures after cue exposure. Moreover, these post-cue exposure ratings were correlated with urge to drink when participants anticipated drinking. Most notably, although post-cue exposure negative affect was not significantly associated with drinking behavior, it was strongly associated with urge to drink just prior to drinking. Relative latencies to endorse alcohol expectancies were negatively correlated with urge to drink, urge to smoke, and

7 102 PALFAI, MONTI, OSTAFIN, AND HUTCHISON Table 2 Correlations Between Urge to Drink, Urge to Smoke, Alcohol Expectancy Accessibility, Affect, and Alcohol Consumption Variable UD EXP NA PA US UDA Volume.46*.34* *.59* UDA.85*.35*.47*.13.43* US.33.33*.66*.12 PA NA.45*.41* EXP.28* Note. Volume = beer volume consumed; UDA = urge to drink alcohol after the anticipation phase; US = urge to smoke after the cue exposure trial; PA = positive affect after the cue exposure trial; NA = negative affect after the cue exposure trial; EXP = alcohol expectancy accessibility (difference between reciprocal response time to endorse alcohol expectancy items and self-items); UD = urge to drink alcohol after the cue exposure trial. * p <.05. negative affect after cue exposure. This suggests that heightened accessibility of positive alcohol expectancy information was associated with stronger urges to drink and negative affect. There was no association between negative outcome latencies and these measures. Discussion The present study sought to examine the effects of mild nicotine deprivation and cigarette cues on alcohol-related responses. Results suggest that nicotine deprivation may intensify subjective, cognitive, and behavioral responses to alcohol. Daily smokers who were asked to refrain from smoking for 6 hr before the study reported more intense urges for alcohol before drinking, demonstrated heightened accessibility of alcohol expectancies, and consumed more beer during a taste test compared to smokers who were not deprived. Moreover, urge to drink and the accessibility of alcohol expectancy information partially mediated the effects of nicotine deprivation on alcohol consumption. Nicotine deprivation also appeared to increase the incentive value of alcohol (Baker et al., 1987; Cox & Klinger, 1988; Stewart, dewit, & Eikelboom, 1984). The interaction between nicotine deprivation and trial showed that participants who were deprived demonstrated a significantly greater increase in urges to drink from baseline to anticipation than those who were not deprived. Taken together, these results are consistent with the view that nicotine deprivation may increase drinking behavior by activating alcoholrelated information in memory. These findings suggest that crosssubstance use may be mediated by changes across multiple information-processing systems that reflect altered incentive motivation for a highly associated substance (Baker et al., 1987). This study provided less evidence for the view that changes in drinking behavior due to nicotine deprivation are a result of efforts to cope with negative affect. Consistent with previous research (Drobes & Tiffany, 1997; Maude-Griffin & Tiffany, 1996), nicotine deprivation influenced negative affect and urges to smoke. Participants who were deprived of nicotine experienced higher levels of negative affect whether or not they were exposed to smoking cues. However, negative affect had a different relation to urges to drink in anticipation of alcohol consumption than to alcohol consumption itself. Post-cue exposure negative affect was highly correlated with urges for alcohol and predicted anticipatory urges for alcohol. It was not, however, associated with drinking behavior during the taste test. A number of studies have shown that negative affect may differentially influence urges and substance use behavior (Herman, 1974; Payne, Schare, Levis, & Colletti, 1991). These findings are consistent with the view that information-processing systems that mediate conscious elements of drinking motivation may operate independently from those that direct routinized behavioral components of drinking (Tiffany, 1990). Certainly, the similarity in assessment methods for verbal compared to behavioral components of drinking motivation may also account for these differences. Another consideration is the low level of negative affect exhibited by both groups. It appears that, although participants in the deprived condition experienced more negative affect, the effects of 6 hr of nicotine deprivation on negative affect for this sample of smokers was relatively mild. Negative affect may be a stronger predictor of alcohol-related behavior under conditions of longer periods of nicotine abstinence (Baker et al., 1987). Although negative affect differentially predicted alcohol consumption and urges to drink, urges to drink after cue exposure were highly correlated with both anticipatory urges to drink and drinking behavior. Indeed, anticipatory urges to drink fully mediated the effects of nicotine deprivation on drinking volume. These findings appear to suggest a strong concordance between alcoholrelated subjective experience and behavior, a finding that has not been observed consistently across laboratory studies (Tiffany, 1990). This may be due, in part, to the nature of the drinking task. Participants are asked to inhibit nicotine use (in the nicotinedeprived condition) and delay alcohol consumption before the taste test (during the anticipation phase). Such impediments to the use of substances may increase the likelihood that nonautomatic processes (e.g., urge processing) are activated during the drug use sequence (Tiffany, 1990). These results also provide support for the view that the accessibility of alcohol outcome expectancies may mediate increased drinking behavior. Alcohol outcome expectancy accessibility was associated with both anticipatory urges to drink and increased alcohol consumption. This cue-elicited activation of expectancy information in memory is theorized to be a central mechanism in the link between drinking triggers and alcohol use (Goldman & Rather, 1993). Previous work has shown that cues influence the accessibility of alcohol outcome expectancies (Goldman & Rather, 1993) and that the direct activation of alcohol outcome expectancies leads to increased alcohol consumption (Roehrich & Goldman, 1995). The present study has extended this work to show the link between cues, expectancy accessibility, and drinking behavior in a single study. These results provide suggestive evidence that alcohol expectancy activation may partially mediate the link between smoking and drinking patterns for those whose access to nicotine is restricted. Nicotine deprivation did not, however, result in enhanced processing across both information-processing tasks. Neither cue nor nicotine-deprivation status was associated with facilitated processing of alcohol-related information on the lexical decision task. Clearly, a number of differences in the nature of the task may have lead to this discrepancy. However, the differential effects of nic-

8 NICOTINE DEPRIVATION AND RESPONSES TO ALCOHOL 103 otine deprivation on lexical decision performance and the alcohol expectancy task speak to the importance of distinguishing between cognitive-processing systems in future work on the effects of cues on alcohol-related processing. A large body of research has shown that depression and anxiety may have mood-congruent effects on specific cognitive processes (Mineka & Sutton, 1992). Early stage cognitive-processing systems that identify the presence of alcoholrelated cues (as measured by the lexical decision task) may be differentially affected by mild nicotine deprivation than processes that mediate higher order information about alcohol outcome expectancies. Similarly, our study found little evidence for the influence of nicotine deprivation on enhanced reactivity to smoking cues. Smoking cues did not differentially increase urges to smoke among smokers who were deprived of nicotine. This may be partially due to the fact that urges to smoke were very high after the baseline phase for smokers who were deprived. Consequently, a number of smokers may have already rated their urges as extremely high before the cue manipulation. This absence of differential reactivity to smoking cues among smokers with longer nicotine deprivation is consistent with other studies on urge processing (Maude-Griffin & Tiffany, 1996). The motivational context of the smokers' nicotine deprivation must also be considered when interpreting these results. In this study, smokers are motivated by an external contingency (i.e., participation in a study) to abstain from smoking. These results may be different among participants who abstain due to a desire to quit (Maude-Griffin & Tiffany, 1996) Another factor that may have influenced the impact of the external smoking cue is the availability of nicotine in the cue exposure protocol (Droungas, Ehrman, Childress, & O'Brien, 1995; Juliano & Brandon, 1998). Participants in our study did not expect to smoke during the experimental procedure. Recent work has suggested that the influence of cigarette cues on urge reactivity may be increased by perceived availability of the cigarette, whereas their influence on cognitive processing may be increased by perceived unavailability (Juliano & Brandon, 1998). Finally, one must consider the nature of the specific procedure used in this study. Participants in the nondeprived condition did not begin the cue reactivity assessment until 30 min after their cigarette, and they were required to complete a computer task before the post-cue exposure assessment of urge to smoke. This time period without smoking and the demands of a computer task may have lead to mild increases in urges for those in the nondeprived condition that may have obscured differences between smoking cue and control cue conditions (Sayette & Hufford, 1994). Although the presentation of cigarette cues did increase urges to smoke among nondeprived smokers over baseline, cigarette cues appeared to have little differential effect on smokingrelated or alcohol-related responses. It will be important to clarify in future studies how factors such as the presence and availability of cigarettes, abstinence duration, and the motivational context of nicotine abstinence interact to influence smoking-related and alcohol-related responses. This study examined the effects of nicotine deprivation on distinct responses believed to relate to alcohol use. A number of investigators have suggested that these responses are interconnected as part of an information-processing subsystem that is believed to underlie drug motivation (Baker et al., 1987; Niaura et al., 1991). Correlational analyses from our study provide some support for this view. Consistent with studies that have examined urge responses to alcohol cues (Gulliver et al., 1995; Rohsenow et al., 1997), our study found that urges to drink and urges to smoke were positively correlated. Correlations between expectancy and subjective measures revealed that the accessibility of alcohol expectancy information (which includes information about positive and negative reinforcement from alcohol) was positively correlated with urges for alcohol and negative affect across smoking conditions. This suggests that for participants who are deprived and participants who are nondeprived, urges for alcohol may be associated with negative affect. Future work may profit from the examination of how nicotine use influences these response associations. In sum, our study found that nicotine deprivation influenced a number of processes that are associated with increased motivation for alcohol, including urges to drink, negative affect, and the accessibility of alcohol outcome expectancies. Although these findings provide support for an information-processing perspective, they are also consistent with other models of cross-substance use, including behavioral choice theories (Vuchinich & Tucker, 1988), which view preferences for reinforcers to be influenced by both access to a given substance and access to alternative reinforcers. Daily smoking hazardous drinkers responded to restrictions on nicotine with increased use of an alternative available substance. These results suggest that nicotine deprivation may place hazardous drinkers at risk for heightened alcohol consumption and may indicate that efforts to restrict smoking could lead to higher levels of alcohol use among this population, at least during early attempts to quit. Interventions for hazardous drinking smokers will be advanced by a more refined understanding of the mechanisms that underlie concurrent alcohol-tobacco use patterns and through specification of how attempts to regulate the use of one substance is influenced by the use of the other (Palfai, Colby, Monti, & Rohsenow, 1997; Sobell et al., 1990). References Babor, T. F., & Grant, M. (1992). Programme on substance abuse. Project on identification and management of alcohol-related problems. Report on Phase 11: A randomized clinical trial of brief interventions in primary health care. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization. Baker, T. B., Morse, E., & Sherman, J. E. (1987). The motivation to use drugs: A psychobiological analysis of urges. In P. C. Rivers (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 34. Alcohol and addictive behavior (pp ). Lincoln, ME: University of Nebraska Press. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, Brandon, T. H., Wetter, D. W., & Baker, T. B. (1996). Affect, expectancies, urges, and smoking: Do they conform to models of drug motivation and relapse? Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 4, Brown, S. A. (1985). Reinforcement expectancies and alcoholism treatment outcome after a one year follow-up. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 46, Burton, S. M., & Tiffany, S. T. (1997). The effect of alcohol consumption on craving to smoke. Addiction, 92, Cappell, H., & Greeley, J. (1987). Alcohol and tension reduction: An update on research and theory. In K. E. Leonard. & H. T. Blane et al.

9 104 PALFAI, MONTI, OSTAFIN, AND HUTCHISON (Eds.), Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism (2nd ed., pp ). New York: Guilford Press. Christiansen, B. A., Smith, G. T., Roehling, P. V., & Goldman, M. S. (1989). Using alcohol expectancies to predict drinking behavior after one year. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, Cooper, M. L., Russell, M., Skinner, J. B., & Windle, M. (1992). Development and validation of a three-dimensional measure of drinking motives. Psychological Assessment, 4, Cox, W. M., & Klinger, E. (1988). A motivational model of alcohol use. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, Drobes, D. J. 5 & Tiffany, S. T. (1997). Induction of smoking urge through imaginal and in Vivo procedures: Physiological and self-report manifestations. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, Droungas, A., Ehrman, R. N., Childress, A. R., & O'Brien, C. P. (1995). Effect of smoking cues and cigarette availability on craving and smoking behavior. Addictive Behaviors, 20, Elash, C. A., Tiffany, S. T., & Vrana, S. R. (1995). Manipulation of smoking urges and affect through a brief-imagery procedure: Self-report, psychophysiological, and startle probe responses. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 3, Fagerstrom, K. O. (1978). Measuring degree of physical dependence to tobacco smoking with reference to individualization of treatment. Addictive Behaviors, 3, Fagerstrom, K. 0., & Schneider, N. G. (1989). Measuring nicotine dependence: A review of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 12, Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, Francis, W. N., & Kucera, H, (1982). Frequency analysis of English usage: Lexicon and grammar. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Glautier, S., Clements, K,, White, J. A. W., Taylor, C., & Stolerman, I. P. (1996). Alcohol and the reward value of cigarette smoking. Behavioral Pharmacology, 7, Goldman, M. S., Brown, S. A., Christiansen, B. A., & Smith. G. T. (1991). Alcoholism and memory: Broadening the scope of alcohol expectancy research. Psychological Bulletin, 110, Goldman, M. S., & Rather, B. C. (1993). Substance use disorders: Cognitive models and architecture. In K. Dobson & P. C. Kendall (Eds.), Psychopathology and cognition (pp ). New York: Academic Press. Gross, T. M., Jarvik, M. E., & Rosenblatt, M. R. (1993). Nicotine abstinence produces content-specific Stroop interference. Psychopharmacology, UO, Gulliver, S. B., Rohsenow, D. J., Colby, S. M., Dey, A. N., Abrams, D. B., Niaura, R., & Monti, P. M. (1995). Interrelationship of smoking and alcohol dependence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 56, Henningfield, J. E. (1984). Behavioral pharmacology of cigarette smoking. In T. Thompson, P. B. Davis, & J. E. Barrett (Eds.), Advances in behavioral pharmacology (Vol. 4, pp ). New York: Academic Press. Henningfield, J. E., Clayton, R., & Pollin, W. (1990). Involvement of tobacco in alcoholism and illicit drug use. British Journal of Addiction, 85, Herman, C. P. (1974). External and internal cues as determinants of the smoking behavior of light and heavy smokers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, Higgins, S. T. (1997). The influence of alternative reinforcers on cocaine use and abuse: A brief review. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 57, Higgins-Biddle, J. C., & Babor, T. F. (1996). Reducing risky drinking: A report on the early identification and management of alcohol problems through screening and brief interventions. Farmington: University of Connecticut Health Center. Hittner, J. B. (1995). Tension-reduction expectancies and alcohol beverage preferences revisited: Associations to drinking frequency and gender. International Journal of the Addictions, 30, Holmbeck, G. N. (1997). Toward terminological, conceptual, and statistical clarity in the study of mediators and moderators: Examples from child-clinical and pediatric psychology literatures. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, Hughes, J. R. (1993). Treatment of smoking cessation in smokers with past alcohol/drug problems. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 10, Istvan, J., & Matazarro, J. D. (1984). Tobacco, alcohol, and caffeine use: A review of their interrelationships. Psychological Bulletin, 95, Jarvik, M. E., Gross, T. M., Rosenblatt, M. R., & Stein, R. E. (1995). Enhanced lexical processing of smoking stimuli during smoking abstinence. Psychopharmacology, 118, Johnsen, B. H., Laberg, J. C., Cox, W. M., Vaksdal, A., & Hugdahl, K. (1994). Alcoholic subjects' attentional bias in the processing of alcoholrelated words. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 8, Jones, B. T., & McMahon, J. (1994). Negative alcohol expectancy predicts post-treatment abstinence survivorship. The whether, when, and why of relapse to a first drink. Addiction, 89, Juliano, L. M,, & Brandon, T. H. (1998). Reactivity to instructed smoking availability and environmental cues: Evidence with urge and reaction time. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 6, Kozlowski, L. T., Henningfield, J. E., Keenan, R. M., Lei, H., Leigh, G., Jelinel, L. C., Pope, M. A., & Haertzen, C. A. (1993). Patterns of alcohol, cigarette, and caffeine and other drug use in two drug abusing populations. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 10, Lang, P. J. (1979). A bio-informational theory of emotional imagery. Psychophysiology, 16, Mann, L. M., Chassin, L., & Sher, K. J. (1987). Alcohol expectancies and risk for alcoholism. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, Marlatt, G. A. (1985). Relapse prevention: Theoretical rationale and overview of the model. In G. A. Marian & J. R. Gordon (Eds.), Relapse prevention (pp. 3-70). New York: Guilford Press. Marlatt, G. A., Demming, B., & Reid, J. B. (1973). Loss of control drinking in alcoholics: An experimental analogue. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 81, Martin, C. S., Earleywine, M., & Young, R. D. (1990). Identification of nonalcoholic and alcoholic beers: Effects of consumption practices and beer brand. Addictive Behaviors, 14, Maude-Griffin, P. M., & Tiffany, S. T. (1996). The production of smoking urges through imagery: The impact of affect and smoking abstinence. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 4, Mello, N. K., Mendelson, J. H., Sellers, M. L., & Kuehnle, J. C. (1980). Effect of alcohol and marijuana on tobacco smoking. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 27, Mineka, S., & Sutton, S. K. (1992). Cognitive biases and the emotional disorders. Psychological Science, 3, Mintz, J., Boyd, G., Rose, J. E., Charuvastra, V. C., & Jarvik, M. E. (1985). Alcohol increases cigarette smoking: A laboratory demonstration. Addictive Behaviors, 10, Monti, P. M., Rohsenow, D. J., Colby, S. M., & Abrams, D. B. (1995). Smoking among alcoholics during and after treatment: Implications for models, treatment strategies and policy. In J. B. Fertig & J. P. Allen (Eds.), Alcohol and tobacco: From basic science to policy (NIAAA Research Monograph No. 30). Washington DC: US Department of Health and Human Services. Murray, R. P., Istvan, J. A., Voelker, H. T., Rigdon. M. A., & Wallace, M. D. (1995). Level of involvement with alcohol and success at smoking cessation in the lung health study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 56,

10 NICOTINE DEPRIVATION AND RESPONSES TO ALCOHOL 105 Niaura, R. S., Goldstein, M., & Abrams, D. (1991). A bioinformational systems perspective on tobacco dependence [Special issue: Future directions in tobacco research]. British Journal of Addiction, 86, Niaura, R. S., Rohsenow, D. J., Binkoff, J. A., Monti, P. M., Pedrazza, M., & Abrams, D. B. (1988). Relevance of cue reactivity to understanding alcobol and smoking relapse. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, Nil, R., Buzzi, R., & Battig, K. (1984). Effects of single doses of alcohol and caffeine on cigarette smoke puffing behavior. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 20, Oei, T. P. S., & Baldwin, A. R. (1994). Expectancy theory: A two process model of alcohol use and abuse. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 55, Palfai, T. P., Colby, S. M., Monti, P. M., & Rohsenow, D. J. (1997). Effects of suppressing the urge to drink on smoking topography: A preliminary study. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 11, Palfai, T. P., Monti, P. M., Colby, S. M., & Rohsenow, D. J. (1997). Effects of suppressing the urge to drink on the accessibility of alcohol outcome expectancies. Behavior Research and Therapy, 35, Payne, T. J., Schare, M. L., Levis, D. J., & Colletti, G. (1991). Exposure to smoking-relevant cues: Effects on desire to smoke and topographical components of smoking behavior. Addictive Behaviors, 16, Payne, T. J., Smith, P. O., Sturges, L. V., & Holleran, S. (1996). Reactivity to smoking cues: Mediating roles of nicotine deprivation and duration of deprivation. Addictive Behaviors, 21, Rather. B. C., & Goldman, M. S. (1994). Drinking-related differences in the memory organization of alcohol expectancies. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 2, Rather. B. C., Goldman, M. S., Roehrich, L., & Brannick, M. (1992). Empirical modeling of an alcohol expectancy network using multidimensional scaling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, Roehrich, L., & Goldman, M. S. (1995). Implicit priming of alcohol expectancy memory processes and subsequent drinking behavior. Experimental, and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 4, Rohsenow, D. J., Monti, P. M., Colby, S. M., Gulliver, S. B., Sirota, A. D., Niaura, R. S., & Abrams, D. B. (1997). Effects of alcohol cues on smoking urges and topography among alcoholic men. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 21, Saunders, J. B., Aasland, O. G., Babor, T. F., de la Fuente, J. R., & Grant, M. (1993). Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption-h. Addiction, 88, Sayette, M. A., & Hufford, M. R. (1994). Effects of cue exposure and deprivation on cognitive resources in smokers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, Sayette, M. A., Monti, P. M., Rohsenow, D. J., Gulliver, S. B., Colby, S., Sirota, A., Niaura, R. S., & Abrams, D. B. (1994). The effects of cue exposure on reaction time in male alcoholics. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 55, Schneider, W. (1988). Micro Experimental Laboratory: An integrated system for IBM PC compatibles. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 20, Shiffman, S. M., Fischer, L. A., Paty, J., Gnys, M., Hickcox, M., & Kassel, J. D. (1994). Drinking and smoking: A field study of their association. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 16, Shiffman, S. M., & Jarvik, M. E. (1976). Smoking withdrawal symptoms in two weeks of abstinence. Psychopharmacology, 50, Sobell, L. C., Maisto, S. A., Sobell, M. B., & Cooper, A. M. (1979). Reliability of alcohol abusers' self-reports of drinking behavior. Behavior Research and Therapy, 17, Sobell, L. C., Sobell, M. B., Kozlowski, L. T., & Toneatto, T. (1990). Alcohol or tobacco research versus alcohol and tobacco research. British Journal of Addiction, Stacy, A. W. (1997). Memory activation and expectancy as prospective predictors of alcohol and marijuana use. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, Stetter, P., Ackermann, K., Bizer, A., Straube, E. R., & Mann, K. (1995). Effects of disease-related cues in alcoholic inpatients: Results of a controlled "alcohol Stroop" study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 19, Stewart, J., dewit, H., & Eikelboom, R. (1984). Role of unconditioned and conditioned drug effects in the self-administration of opiates and stimulants. Psychological Review, 91, Tiffany, S. T. (1990). A cognitive model of drug urges and drug use behavior: Role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychological Review, 97, Tucker, J. A., Vuchinich, R. E., & Pukish, M. A. (1995). Molar environmental contexts surrounding recovery from alcohol problems by treated and untreated problem drinkers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, J, Vuchinich, R. E., & Simpson, C. A. (1998). Hyperbolic temporal discounting in social drinkers and problem drinkers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 6, Vuchinich, R. E., & Tucker, J. A. (1983). Behavioral theories of choice as a framework for studying drinking behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92, Vuchinich, R. E., & Tucker, J. A. (1988). Contributions from behavioral theories of choice to an analysis of alcohol abuse. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, Vuchinich, R. E., & Tucker, J. A. (1996). Alcoholic relapse, life events, and behavioral theories of choice: A prospective analysis. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 4, Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, Wikler, A. (1973). Dynamics of drug dependence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 28, Wise, R. A. (1988). The neurobiology of craving: Implications for the understanding and treatment of addiction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, Zacny, J. P. (1990). Behavioral aspects of alcohol-tobacco interactions. In M. Galanter (Ed.), Recent developments in alcoholism (Vol. 8, pp ). New York: Plenum. Zimmerman, R. S., Warheit, G. J., Ulbrich, P. M., & Auth, J. B. (1990). The relationship between alcohol use and attempts and success at smoking cessation. Addictive Behaviors, 15, Zinser, M. C., Baker, T. B., Sherman, J. E., & Cannon, D. S. (1992). Relation between self-reported affect and drug urges and cravings in continuing and withdrawing smokers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 4, Received August 17, 1998 Revision received May 24, 1999 Accepted May 24, 1999

Negative affect words prime beer consumption in young drinkers

Negative affect words prime beer consumption in young drinkers Addictive Behaviors 31 (2006) 169 173 Short communication Negative affect words prime beer consumption in young drinkers Martin Zack a, *, Constantine X. Poulos a, Fofo Fragopoulos a, Tracy M. Woodford

More information

DOES THE SELF-CONTROL OF ALCOHOL USE become

DOES THE SELF-CONTROL OF ALCOHOL USE become 926 JOURNAL OF STUDIES ON ALCOHOL / NOVEMBER 2006 Activating Action Tendencies: The Influence of Action Priming on Alcohol Consumption Among Male Hazardous Drinkers* TIBOR P. PALFAI, PH.D. Department of

More information

The journal of Nicotine & Tobacco Research would like to apologize for an error that occurred in the publication of-

The journal of Nicotine & Tobacco Research would like to apologize for an error that occurred in the publication of- Nicotine & Tobacco Research Volume 10, Number 5 (May 2008) 933 Erratum The journal of Nicotine & Tobacco Research would like to apologize for an error that occurred in the publication of- Nicotine & Tobacco

More information

Context-dependency of cue-elicited urge to smoke

Context-dependency of cue-elicited urge to smoke RESEARCH REPORT Blackwell Science, LtdOxford, UKADDAddiction0965-2140 2005 Society for the Study of Addiction 100 Original Article Context-dependency of cue-elicited urge to smoke Roy Thewissen et al.

More information

Reactivity to Alcohol Cues: Isolating the Role of Perceived Availability

Reactivity to Alcohol Cues: Isolating the Role of Perceived Availability Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 13, No. 3, 229 237 1064-1297/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1064-1297.13.3.229 Reactivity to Alcohol

More information

Addiction and Dependence

Addiction and Dependence Addiction and Dependence Michael C. Fiore MD, MPH, MBA Professor of Medicine Director, Center for Tobacco Research and Intervention University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health FDA TPSAC

More information

Mood and Implicit Alcohol Expectancy Processes: Predicting Alcohol Consumption in the Laboratory

Mood and Implicit Alcohol Expectancy Processes: Predicting Alcohol Consumption in the Laboratory Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research Vol. 36, No. 1 January 2012 Mood and Implicit Alcohol Expectancy Processes: Predicting Alcohol Consumption in the Laboratory Jeffrey D. Wardell, Jennifer

More information

Effects of naltrexone with nicotine replacement on smoking cue reactivity: preliminary results

Effects of naltrexone with nicotine replacement on smoking cue reactivity: preliminary results Psychopharmacology (1999) 142 : 139 143 Springer-Verlag 1999 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Kent E. Hutchison Peter M. Monti Damaris J. Rohsenow Robert M. Swift Suzanne M. Colby Maryann Gnys Raymond S. Niaura

More information

Comparing attentional bias to smoking cues in current smokers, former smokers, and non-smokers using a dot-probe task

Comparing attentional bias to smoking cues in current smokers, former smokers, and non-smokers using a dot-probe task University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs Staff Publications U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs 2002 Comparing attentional bias

More information

Thank you Dr. XXXX; I am going to be talking briefly about my EMA study of attention training in cigarette smokers.

Thank you Dr. XXXX; I am going to be talking briefly about my EMA study of attention training in cigarette smokers. Thank you Dr. XXXX; I am going to be talking briefly about my EMA study of attention training in cigarette smokers. 1 This work is a result of the combined efforts of myself and my research advisor, Dr.

More information

Relation Between Self-Reported Affect and Drug Urges and Cravings in Continuing and Withdrawing Smokers

Relation Between Self-Reported Affect and Drug Urges and Cravings in Continuing and Withdrawing Smokers Journal of Abnormal Psychology 1992, Vol. 101, No. 4,617-629 Copyright 1992 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0021-843X/92/S3.00 Relation Between Self-Reported Affect and Drug Urges and Cravings

More information

The Craving to Smoke in Flight Attendants: Relations With Smoking Deprivation, Anticipation of Smoking, and Actual Smoking

The Craving to Smoke in Flight Attendants: Relations With Smoking Deprivation, Anticipation of Smoking, and Actual Smoking Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2010 American Psychological Association 2010, Vol. 119, No. 1, 248 253 0021-843X/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0017778 The Craving to Smoke in Flight Attendants: Relations With

More information

The 5A's are practice guidelines on tobacco use prevention and cessation treatment (4):

The 5A's are practice guidelines on tobacco use prevention and cessation treatment (4): Smoking Cessation Module Tobacco use is the single greatest preventable cause of chronic diseases and premature deaths worldwide. The Canadian Cancer Society reports that tobacco product use is responsible

More information

ALCOHOLISM: CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH Vol. 30, No. 2 February 2006

ALCOHOLISM: CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH Vol. 30, No. 2 February 2006 ALCOHOLISM: CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH Vol. 30, No. 2 February 2006 The Search for New Ways to Change Implicit Alcohol-Related Cognitions in Heavy Drinkers Reinout W. Wiers, W. Miles Cox, Matt

More information

Practical ways of reducing cigarette cravings. Robert West Cancer Research UK and UCL UK National Smoking Cessation Conference June 2005

Practical ways of reducing cigarette cravings. Robert West Cancer Research UK and UCL UK National Smoking Cessation Conference June 2005 Practical ways of reducing cigarette cravings Robert West Cancer Research UK and UCL UK National Smoking Cessation Conference June 2005 Outline What are cravings? The role of cravings in addiction What

More information

FROM THE FRAMEWORK OF Social Learning Theory

FROM THE FRAMEWORK OF Social Learning Theory READ AND CURTIN 759 Contextual Influences on Alcohol Expectancy Processes* JENNIFER P. READ, PH.D., AND JOHN J. CURTIN, PH.D. Department of Psychology, Park Hall, University at Buffalo, State University

More information

Applying Behavioral Theories of Choice to Substance Use in a Sample of Psychiatric Outpatients

Applying Behavioral Theories of Choice to Substance Use in a Sample of Psychiatric Outpatients Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 1999, Vol. 13, No. 3,207-212 Copyright 1999 by the Educational Publishing Foundation 0893-164X/99/S3.00 Applying Behavioral Theories of Choice to Substance Use in a Sample

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by:[brown University] [Brown University] On: 7 June 2007 Access Details: [subscription number 769425690] Publisher: Informa Healthcare Informa Ltd Registered in England and

More information

Evaluation of the brief questionnaire of smoking urges (QSU-brief) in laboratory and clinical settings

Evaluation of the brief questionnaire of smoking urges (QSU-brief) in laboratory and clinical settings Nicotine & Tobacco Research (2001) 3, 7 16 Evaluation of the brief questionnaire of smoking urges (QSU-brief) in laboratory and clinical settings Lisa Sanderson Cox, Stephen T. Tiffany, Arden G. Christen

More information

Implicit Affective Responses to Smoking Cues: A Comparison of Smoking Groups and Withdrawal States

Implicit Affective Responses to Smoking Cues: A Comparison of Smoking Groups and Withdrawal States College of William and Mary W&M Publish Undergraduate Honors Theses Theses, Dissertations, & Master Projects 5-2011 Implicit Affective Responses to Smoking Cues: A Comparison of Smoking Groups and Withdrawal

More information

Management of Tobacco Dependence. Dr. Lokesh Kumar Singh Associate Professor Department of Psychiatry AIIMS, Raipur

Management of Tobacco Dependence. Dr. Lokesh Kumar Singh Associate Professor Department of Psychiatry AIIMS, Raipur Management of Tobacco Dependence Dr. Lokesh Kumar Singh Associate Professor Department of Psychiatry AIIMS, Raipur Difficult to identify any other condition that presents such a mix of lethality, prevalence,

More information

Virtual Reality Cue Reactivity Assessment in Cigarette Smokers ABSTRACT

Virtual Reality Cue Reactivity Assessment in Cigarette Smokers ABSTRACT CYBERPSYCHOLOGY & BEHAVIOR Volume 8, Number 5, 2005 Mary Ann Liebert, Inc. Virtual Reality Cue Reactivity Assessment in Cigarette Smokers PATRICK S. BORDNICK, M.S.W., M.P.H., Ph.D., 1 KEN M. GRAAP, M.Ed.,

More information

Using the Balanced-Placebo Design for Parsing Nicotine Administration Effects

Using the Balanced-Placebo Design for Parsing Nicotine Administration Effects Using the Balanced-Placebo Design for Parsing Nicotine Administration Effects Thomas H. Brandon, Ph.D. & Amanda M. Palmer, M.A. Moffitt Cancer Center University of South Florida Tampa, Florida, USA Global

More information

Motivational enhancement therapy for high-risk adolescent smokers

Motivational enhancement therapy for high-risk adolescent smokers Addictive Behaviors 32 (2007) 2404 2410 Short communication Motivational enhancement therapy for high-risk adolescent smokers Amy Helstrom a,, Kent Hutchison b, Angela Bryan b a VA Boston Healthcare System,

More information

Smoking Cues Decrease Prepulse Inhibition of the Startle Response and Increase Subjective Craving in Humans

Smoking Cues Decrease Prepulse Inhibition of the Startle Response and Increase Subjective Craving in Humans Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 1999, Vol. 7, No. 3, 250-256 Copyright 1999 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1064-1297/99/S3.00 Smoking Cues Decrease Prepulse Inhibition of

More information

Randomized controlled trial of physical activity counseling as an aid to smoking cessation: 12 month follow-up

Randomized controlled trial of physical activity counseling as an aid to smoking cessation: 12 month follow-up Addictive Behaviors 32 (2007) 3060 3064 Short communication Randomized controlled trial of physical activity counseling as an aid to smoking cessation: 12 month follow-up Michael Ussher a,, Robert West

More information

Poor impulse control and heightened attraction to alcohol-related imagery in repeat DUI offenders

Poor impulse control and heightened attraction to alcohol-related imagery in repeat DUI offenders Poor impulse control and heightened attraction to alcohol-related imagery in repeat DUI offenders Abstract Melissa A. Miller, M.S. and Mark T. Fillmore, Ph.D. Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky

More information

Session 1: Days 1-3. Session 4: Days Session 2: Days 4-7. Session 5: Days Session 3: Days Day 21: Quit Day!

Session 1: Days 1-3. Session 4: Days Session 2: Days 4-7. Session 5: Days Session 3: Days Day 21: Quit Day! Tobacco cessation overview calendar 21-Day Countdown to Quitting Session 1: Days 1-3 List health benefits of quitting. List expectations of overcoming your habits and addictions. List your top three Schedule

More information

Eye movements to smoking-related pictures in smokers: relationship between attentional biases and implicit and explicit measures of stimulus valence

Eye movements to smoking-related pictures in smokers: relationship between attentional biases and implicit and explicit measures of stimulus valence Blackwell Science, LtdOxford, UKADDAddiction1360-0443 2003 Society for the Study of Addiction to Alcohol and Other Drugs98Original ArticleBiases in eye movements in smokerskarin Mogg et al. RESEARCH REPORT

More information

Automatic Affective Responses to Smoking Cues

Automatic Affective Responses to Smoking Cues Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 15, No. 4, 400 409 1064-1297/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1064-1297.15.4.400 Automatic Affective

More information

Introduction ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION

Introduction ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Psychopharmacology (2001) 155:27 34 DOI 10.1007/s002130000629 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Kent E. Hutchison Robert Swift Damaris J. Rohsenow Peter M. Monti Dena Davidson Alissa Almeida Olanzapine reduces urge

More information

Alcohol Expectancy Priming and Drinking Behavior: The Role of Compatibility Between Prime and Expectancy Content

Alcohol Expectancy Priming and Drinking Behavior: The Role of Compatibility Between Prime and Expectancy Content Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 2009 American Psychological Association 2009, Vol. 23, No. 2, 329 333 0893-164X/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0015704 Alcohol Expectancy Priming and Drinking Behavior: The Role

More information

Attentional bias for cannabis and nicotine related words in cannabis users

Attentional bias for cannabis and nicotine related words in cannabis users Attentional bias for cannabis and nicotine related words in cannabis users Word count abstract: 263 Word count text: 8893 Tables and Figures: 10 Pages: 27 Björn van Vlokhoven b.j.h.a.vanvlokhoven@uu.students.nl

More information

Tobacco dependence: Implications for service provision

Tobacco dependence: Implications for service provision Tobacco dependence: Implications for service provision Andy McEwen, PhD University College London & National Centre for Smoking Cessation and Training (NCSCT) Tuesday 29th January 2013 Cardiff Conflict

More information

Chege Kimani Gabriel. Department of Sociology & Psychology, Moi University, Kenya. Accepted 26 October 2014

Chege Kimani Gabriel. Department of Sociology & Psychology, Moi University, Kenya.   Accepted 26 October 2014 Vol. 1(5), pp. 54-58, October 2014 ISSN: 2384-6178 2014 Academic Research Journals http://www.academicresearchjournals.org/arjpc/index.htm Academic Research Journal of Psychology and Counselling Full Length

More information

EVIDENCE-BASED INTERVENTIONS TO HELP PATIENTS QUIT TOBACCO

EVIDENCE-BASED INTERVENTIONS TO HELP PATIENTS QUIT TOBACCO EVIDENCE-BASED INTERVENTIONS TO HELP PATIENTS QUIT TOBACCO Lena Matthias Gray, MSA, CTTS-M University of Michigan MHealthy Tobacco Consultation Service Overview of Tobacco Use The World Health Organization

More information

An experimental test of an alcohol expectancy challenge in mixed gender groups of young heavy drinkers

An experimental test of an alcohol expectancy challenge in mixed gender groups of young heavy drinkers Addictive Behaviors 29 (2004) 215 220 Short communication An experimental test of an alcohol expectancy challenge in mixed gender groups of young heavy drinkers Reinout W. Wiers*, Remco H.C. Kummeling

More information

Craving to smoke in orthodox Jewish smokers who abstain on the Sabbath: a comparison to a baseline and a forced abstinence workday

Craving to smoke in orthodox Jewish smokers who abstain on the Sabbath: a comparison to a baseline and a forced abstinence workday Psychopharmacology (2005) 183: 294 299 DOI 10.1007/s00213-005-0192-3 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Reuven Dar. Florencia Stronguin. Roni Marouani. Meir Krupsky. Hanan Frenk Craving to smoke in orthodox Jewish

More information

Addictive Behaviors 39 (2014) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Addictive Behaviors

Addictive Behaviors 39 (2014) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Addictive Behaviors Addictive Behaviors 39 (2014) 1516 1521 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Addictive Behaviors Effect of exercise on cigarette cravings and ad libitum smoking following concurrent stressors Angela

More information

Cognitive Effects of Cigarette Deprivation and Quit Status. Rachel Phillips. David J. Kavanagh. Jon May and Jackie Andrade

Cognitive Effects of Cigarette Deprivation and Quit Status. Rachel Phillips. David J. Kavanagh. Jon May and Jackie Andrade Cognitive Effects of Cigarette Deprivation and Quit Status Rachel Phillips School of Psychology, University of Queensland David J. Kavanagh School of Medicine, University of Queensland Jon May and Jackie

More information

AN ECOLOGICAL MOMENTARY ASSESSMENT ANALYSIS OF RELATIONS AMONG COPING, AFFECT, AND SMOKING LAPSE HARUKA MINAMI. A thesis submitted to the

AN ECOLOGICAL MOMENTARY ASSESSMENT ANALYSIS OF RELATIONS AMONG COPING, AFFECT, AND SMOKING LAPSE HARUKA MINAMI. A thesis submitted to the AN ECOLOGICAL MOMENTARY ASSESSMENT ANALYSIS OF RELATIONS AMONG COPING, AFFECT, AND SMOKING LAPSE by HARUKA MINAMI A thesis submitted to the Graduate School-New Brunswick Rutgers, The State University of

More information

Identifying effective behaviour change techniques in brief interventions to reduce alcohol consumption. Susan Michie

Identifying effective behaviour change techniques in brief interventions to reduce alcohol consumption. Susan Michie Identifying effective behaviour change techniques in brief interventions to reduce alcohol consumption Susan Michie Professor of Health Psychology University College London Society for the Study of Addiction:

More information

Comparison of cannabis and tobacco withdrawal: Severity and contribution to relapse

Comparison of cannabis and tobacco withdrawal: Severity and contribution to relapse Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 35 (2008) 362 368 Regular article Comparison of cannabis and tobacco withdrawal: Severity and contribution to relapse Alan J. Budney, (Ph.D.) a,, Ryan G. Vandrey, (Ph.D.)

More information

Cognitive biases and addiction: an evolution in theory and method

Cognitive biases and addiction: an evolution in theory and method Addiction (2001) 96, 47 56 CONCEPTUALIZING ADDICTION Cognitive biases and addiction: an evolution in theory and method CHRISTOPHER G. MCCUSKER Department of Clinical Psychology, The Royal Hospitals Trust,

More information

ALCOHOL SENSITIVITY AND ALCOHOL CRAVING

ALCOHOL SENSITIVITY AND ALCOHOL CRAVING RAY, MESKEW-STACER, AND HUTCHISON 379 The Relationship Between Prospective Self-Rating of Alcohol Sensitivity and Craving and Experimental Results From Two Alcohol Challenge Studies* LARA A. RAY, M.A.,

More information

ALCOHOL CRAVING IN PROBLEM AND OCCASIONAL ALCOHOL DRINKERS

ALCOHOL CRAVING IN PROBLEM AND OCCASIONAL ALCOHOL DRINKERS Alcohol & Alcoholism Vol. 41, No. 4, pp. 421 425, 2006 Advance Access publication 24 April 2006 doi:10.1093/alcalc/agl035 ALCOHOL CRAVING IN PROBLEM AND OCCASIONAL ALCOHOL DRINKERS SABINE M. GRÜSSER*,

More information

Working Memory in Nonsmokers and in Cigarette Smokers During. Abstinence and Relative Satiety

Working Memory in Nonsmokers and in Cigarette Smokers During. Abstinence and Relative Satiety Working Memory in Nonsmokers and in Cigarette Smokers During Abstinence and Relative Satiety A. Mendrek 1,, J. Monterosso 1, S.L. Simon 1, M Jarvik 1, A. Brody 1, R. Olmstead 1, M.S. Cohen 1, M. Ernst

More information

Drug and Alcohol Dependence

Drug and Alcohol Dependence Drug and Alcohol Dependence 106 (2010) 204 211 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Drug and Alcohol Dependence journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/drugalcdep Seeing the forest through the trees:

More information

Brief Intervention (BI) for Adolescents

Brief Intervention (BI) for Adolescents Brief Intervention (BI) for Adolescents Sharon Levy, MD, MPH Director, Adolescent Substance Abuse Program Boston Children s Hospital Associate Professor of Pediatrics Harvard Medical School What is BI?

More information

Evaluation of the mood and physical symptoms scale (MPSS) to assess cigarette withdrawal

Evaluation of the mood and physical symptoms scale (MPSS) to assess cigarette withdrawal Psychopharmacology (2004) 177: 195 199 DOI 10.1007/s00213-004-1923-6 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Robert West. Peter Hajek Evaluation of the mood and physical symptoms scale (MPSS) to assess cigarette withdrawal

More information

Subliminal Processing of Smoking-Related and Affective Stimuli in Tobacco Addiction

Subliminal Processing of Smoking-Related and Affective Stimuli in Tobacco Addiction Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 2008, Vol. 16, No. 4, 301 312 1064-1297/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0012640 Subliminal Processing of Smoking-Related

More information

Relations of Alcohol Consumption With Smoking Cessation Milestones and Tobacco Dependence

Relations of Alcohol Consumption With Smoking Cessation Milestones and Tobacco Dependence Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 2012 American Psychological Association 2012, Vol. 80, No. 6, 1075 1085 0022-006X/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0029931 Relations of Alcohol Consumption With Smoking

More information

Chapter 7. Screening and Assessment

Chapter 7. Screening and Assessment Chapter 7 Screening and Assessment Screening And Assessment Starting the dialogue and begin relationship Each are sizing each other up Information gathering Listening to their story Asking the questions

More information

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN THE ATTENTIONAL BLINK TO ALCOHOL RELATED CUES. Francesco M. DePalma, B.A.

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN THE ATTENTIONAL BLINK TO ALCOHOL RELATED CUES. Francesco M. DePalma, B.A. INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN THE ATTENTIONAL BLINK TO ALCOHOL RELATED CUES by Francesco M. DePalma, B.A. A thesis submitted to the Graduate Council of Texas State University in partial fulfillment of the

More information

Reliability of lifetime drinking history among alcoholic men

Reliability of lifetime drinking history among alcoholic men Washington University School of Medicine Digital Commons@Becker Posters 2005: Alcoholism and Comorbidity 2005 Reliability of lifetime drinking history among alcoholic men T. Jacob R. A. Seilhamer K. Bargiel

More information

What am I Smoking/ Vaping?

What am I Smoking/ Vaping? What am I Smoking/ Vaping? What s in a cigarette? What s in an e cigarette? Page Image source: http://www.huffingtonpost.com/0//0/effects of quitting smoking_n_.html Page Support for Changing Your Tobacco

More information

Selective cognitive processing of drug cues in heroin dependence

Selective cognitive processing of drug cues in heroin dependence Journal of Psychopharmacology 14(4) (2000) 395 400 2000 British Association for Psychopharmacology (ISSN 0269-8811) SAGE Publications, London, Thousand Oaks, CA and New Delhi 0269 8811[2000012]14:4; 395

More information

Jennifer W. Tidey Publication List 09/15/11

Jennifer W. Tidey Publication List 09/15/11 PUBLICATIONS LIST ORIGINAL PUBLICATIONS IN PEER-REVIEWED JOURNALS 1. Miczek KA, Haney M, Tidey J, Vatne T, Weerts E, DeBold JF (1989). Temporal and sequential patterns of agonistic behavior: Effects of

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Addict Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 March 01.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Addict Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 March 01. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Addict Behav. 2013 March ; 38(3): 1840 1843. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.09.010. Cue-induced cigarette and food craving: A common effect?

More information

Examining the distinction and concordance between implicit measures of alcohol expectancies: Toward agreement on their meaning and use

Examining the distinction and concordance between implicit measures of alcohol expectancies: Toward agreement on their meaning and use University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School 2007 Examining the distinction and concordance between implicit measures of alcohol expectancies: Toward agreement

More information

Background and Analysis Objectives Methods and Approach

Background and Analysis Objectives Methods and Approach Cigarettes, Tobacco Dependence, and Smoking Cessation: Project MOM Final Report Lorraine R. Reitzel, Ph.D., The University of Texas, MD Anderson Cancer Center Background and Analysis Objectives Background.

More information

Emotional Responses to Goal Attainment: Strength of Regulatory Focus as Moderator

Emotional Responses to Goal Attainment: Strength of Regulatory Focus as Moderator Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1997, Vol. 72, No. 3, 515-525 0022-3514/97/$3.00 Emotional Responses to Goal Attainment: Strength

More information

Treatment Outcomes from the TDC: A Look at Smoking Cessation Among Patients with Co- Occurring Disorders

Treatment Outcomes from the TDC: A Look at Smoking Cessation Among Patients with Co- Occurring Disorders University of Kentucky From the SelectedWorks of Chizimuzo T.C. Okoli December, 2010 Treatment Outcomes from the TDC: A Look at Smoking Cessation Among Patients with Co- Occurring Disorders Chizimuzo T.C.

More information

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants Supplementary experiment: neutral faces This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants would automatically shift their attention towards to objects the seen

More information

Carry-over effects of smoking cue exposure on working memory performance

Carry-over effects of smoking cue exposure on working memory performance Nicotine & Tobacco Research Volume 9, Number 5 (May 2007) 613 619 Carry-over effects of smoking cue exposure on working memory performance Stephen J. Wilson, Michael A. Sayette, Julie A. Fiez, Elizabeth

More information

Foundations of Addictions

Foundations of Addictions Foundations of Addictions Week 8 Glenn Maynard, LPC Addiction as a maladaptive behavior This approach is grounded in psychology Model comes from classical conditioning, operative conditioning and social

More information

Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect. David K. Sherman

Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect. David K. Sherman 1 Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect David K. Sherman University of California, Santa Barbara Traci Mann University of California,

More information

NICOTINE PHARMACOLOGY and PRINCIPLES of ADDICTION. 3 rd of 3 Prep for Session 1

NICOTINE PHARMACOLOGY and PRINCIPLES of ADDICTION. 3 rd of 3 Prep for Session 1 NICOTINE PHARMACOLOGY and PRINCIPLES of ADDICTION 3 rd of 3 Prep for Session 1 CHEMISTRY of NICOTINE Pyridine ring N H N CH 3 Pyrrolidine ring Nicotiana tabacum Natural liquid alkaloid Colorless, volatile

More information

Despite substantial declines over the past decade,

Despite substantial declines over the past decade, 19 The journey to quitting smoking Margot Shields Abstract Objectives This article outlines smoking trends over the past 10 years among the population aged 18 or older. Factors associated with smoking

More information

The effectiveness of attentional bias modification for substance use disorder symptoms in adults: a systematic review

The effectiveness of attentional bias modification for substance use disorder symptoms in adults: a systematic review Heitmann et al. Systematic Reviews (2018) 7:160 https://doi.org/10.1186/s13643-018-0822-6 RESEARCH Open Access The effectiveness of attentional bias modification for substance use disorder symptoms in

More information

Tobacco Withdrawal and Negative Affect: An Analysis of Initial Emotional Response Intensity and Voluntary Emotion Regulation

Tobacco Withdrawal and Negative Affect: An Analysis of Initial Emotional Response Intensity and Voluntary Emotion Regulation Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 115, No. 1, 96 102 0021-843X/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.1.96 Tobacco Withdrawal and Negative

More information

Concurrent Brief versus Intensive Smoking Intervention during Alcohol Dependence Treatment

Concurrent Brief versus Intensive Smoking Intervention during Alcohol Dependence Treatment University of Connecticut DigitalCommons@UConn Articles - Research University of Connecticut Health Center Research 11-2007 Concurrent Brief versus Intensive Smoking Intervention during Alcohol Dependence

More information

Author: Marvin D. Krank, Ph.D. Author s Affiliation: UBC Okangan. Second Author: Aarin P. Frigon Second Author s Affiliation: UBC Okanagan

Author: Marvin D. Krank, Ph.D. Author s Affiliation: UBC Okangan. Second Author: Aarin P. Frigon Second Author s Affiliation: UBC Okanagan Individual Presentation Title: Preventing Substance Use Transitions in Adolescents Using Feedback from Implicit Cognition Session Title: Youth Treatment Initiatives Thread & Session Number: Treatment Approaches

More information

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes.

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes. University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 1995 The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the

More information

Clinical Evaluation: Assessment Goals

Clinical Evaluation: Assessment Goals Clinical Evaluation: Assessment Goals 1. Define Assessment Process 2. Identify Assessment Instruments 3. Define DSM-5 criteria for Substance Abuse and Dependence, specifiers and multi-axial assessment

More information

Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: The elaborated intrusion theory of desire

Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: The elaborated intrusion theory of desire University of Plymouth PEARL Faculty of Health and Human Sciences https://pearl.plymouth.ac.uk School of Psychology 2005-04 Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: The elaborated intrusion theory of desire

More information

Application of Virtual Reality-Cue Exposure Therapy for Reducing Alcohol Craving

Application of Virtual Reality-Cue Exposure Therapy for Reducing Alcohol Craving CYBERTHERAPY CONFERENCE 2006 Application of Virtual Reality-Cue Exposure Therapy for Reducing Alcohol Craving Hyoseok Kwon, B.A., Jang-Han Lee, Ph.D. Department of Psychology, Chung-Ang University Sungwon

More information

Relapse Prevention. Jennifer S. B. Moran, MA, TTS MFMER slide-1

Relapse Prevention. Jennifer S. B. Moran, MA, TTS MFMER slide-1 Relapse Prevention Jennifer S. B. Moran, MA, TTS 2013 MFMER slide-1 Learning Objectives Describe cognitive, behavioral, and pharmacologic relapse prevention concepts that may be introduced early in a tobacco

More information

Understanding the Impact of Cigarette Smoking Using Brain Imaging

Understanding the Impact of Cigarette Smoking Using Brain Imaging Understanding the Impact of Cigarette Smoking Using Brain Imaging Angelica Morales, PhD Postdoctoral Fellow Developmental Brain Imaging Lab Oregon Health & Science University No Financial Disclosures The

More information

FACTORS PREDICTING SMOKING IN A LABORATORY-BASED SMOKING- CHOICE TASK KRYSTEN WILLIAMS BOLD. A thesis submitted to the Graduate School-New Brunswick

FACTORS PREDICTING SMOKING IN A LABORATORY-BASED SMOKING- CHOICE TASK KRYSTEN WILLIAMS BOLD. A thesis submitted to the Graduate School-New Brunswick FACTORS PREDICTING SMOKING IN A LABORATORY-BASED SMOKING- CHOICE TASK by KRYSTEN WILLIAMS BOLD A thesis submitted to the Graduate School-New Brunswick Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey In partial

More information

THE EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL CUE EXPOSURE ON NON-DEPENDENT DRINKERS ATTENTIONAL BIAS FOR ALCOHOL-RELATED STIMULI

THE EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL CUE EXPOSURE ON NON-DEPENDENT DRINKERS ATTENTIONAL BIAS FOR ALCOHOL-RELATED STIMULI Alcohol & Alcoholism Vol. 38, No. 1, pp. 45 49, 2003 doi:10.1093/alcalc/agg010, available online at www.alcalc.oupjournals.org THE EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL CUE EXPOSURE ON NON-DEPENDENT DRINKERS ATTENTIONAL

More information

OPIOID ANTAGONISTS IN THE TREATMENT OF ALCOHOL DEPENDENCE: CLINICAL EFFICACY AND PREVENTION OF RELAPSE

OPIOID ANTAGONISTS IN THE TREATMENT OF ALCOHOL DEPENDENCE: CLINICAL EFFICACY AND PREVENTION OF RELAPSE Alcohol & Alcoholism, Vol. 31, Suppl. 1, pp. 77-81, 1996 OPIOID ANTAGONISTS IN THE TREATMENT OF ALCOHOL DEPENDENCE: CLINICAL EFFICACY AND PREVENTION OF RELAPSE STEPHANIE S. O'MALLEY Department of Psychiatry.

More information

Tailoring Tobacco Treatment Services based on Psychological Factors

Tailoring Tobacco Treatment Services based on Psychological Factors Tailoring Tobacco Treatment Services based on Psychological Factors Thomas J Payne, PhD Professor and Director of Research Department of Otolaryngology and Communicative Sciences Associate Director, The

More information

Chapter 7. Screening and Assessment

Chapter 7. Screening and Assessment Chapter 7 Screening and Assessment Screening And Assessment Starting the dialogue and begin relationship Each are sizing each other up Information gathering Listening to their story Asking the questions

More information

EDRS. trends. bulletin. Alcohol use disorders amongst a group of regular ecstasy users. Key findings. july Introduction.

EDRS. trends. bulletin. Alcohol use disorders amongst a group of regular ecstasy users. Key findings. july Introduction. ecstasy and related drug trends july 2011 bulletin Authors: Sheena Arora and Lucy Burns, National Drug and Alcohol Research Centre, University of New South Wales Suggested citation: Arora, S. & Burns,

More information

Effects of a low dose of alcohol on cognitive biases and craving in heavy drinkers

Effects of a low dose of alcohol on cognitive biases and craving in heavy drinkers DOI 10.1007/s00213-007-1023-5 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Effects of a low dose of alcohol on cognitive biases and craving in heavy drinkers Tim Schoenmakers & Reinout W. Wiers & Matt Field Received: 17 August

More information

Tests of the elaborated intrusion theory of craving and desire: Features of alcohol craving during treatment for an alcohol disorder

Tests of the elaborated intrusion theory of craving and desire: Features of alcohol craving during treatment for an alcohol disorder University of Plymouth PEARL Faculty of Health and Human Sciences https://pearl.plymouth.ac.uk School of Psychology 2010-12-24 Tests of the elaborated intrusion theory of craving and desire: Features of

More information

DUI SERVICE PROVIDER ORIENTATION DAY 2 AFTERNOON: DUI ASSESSMENT TOOLS

DUI SERVICE PROVIDER ORIENTATION DAY 2 AFTERNOON: DUI ASSESSMENT TOOLS DUI SERVICE PROVIDER ORIENTATION DAY 2 AFTERNOON: DUI ASSESSMENT TOOLS 2017-2018 IDHS DUI ORIENTATION ASSESSMENT TOOLS My Training Room Rules richard.krajewski@dupageco.org updated 10/2017 Assessment Tools

More information

5. Offer pharmacotherapy to all smokers who are attempting to quit, unless contraindicated.

5. Offer pharmacotherapy to all smokers who are attempting to quit, unless contraindicated. 0 11 Key Messages 1. Ask and document smoking status for all patients. 2. Provide brief advice on quit smoking at every visit to all smokers. 3. Use individual, group and telephone counselling approaches,

More information

Nicotine Replacement Therapy and Brief Motivational Interview for Emergency Department Smokers with Asthma

Nicotine Replacement Therapy and Brief Motivational Interview for Emergency Department Smokers with Asthma Yale University EliScholar A Digital Platform for Scholarly Publishing at Yale Yale Medicine Thesis Digital Library School of Medicine 3-4-2008 Nicotine Replacement Therapy and Brief Motivational Interview

More information

Stage of Change of Cigarette Smoking in Alcohol-Dependent Patients

Stage of Change of Cigarette Smoking in Alcohol-Dependent Patients Special Topic Section Interdisciplinary Alcohol Research in Europe Editor: M. Krausz (Hamburg) Eur Addict Res 2000;6:84 90 Stage of Change of Cigarette Smoking in Alcohol-Dependent Patients Daniele Zullino

More information

Identify the benefits of using a Brief Negotiated Intervention (BNI) to screen for alcohol and drug disorders. Review a four step model of Screening,

Identify the benefits of using a Brief Negotiated Intervention (BNI) to screen for alcohol and drug disorders. Review a four step model of Screening, Kate Speck, PhD Identify the benefits of using a Brief Negotiated Intervention (BNI) to screen for alcohol and drug disorders. Review a four step model of Screening, Brief Intervention and Referral to

More information

General and smoking cessation related weight concerns in veterans.

General and smoking cessation related weight concerns in veterans. University of Texas at El Paso From the SelectedWorks of Theodore V. Cooper 2006 General and smoking cessation related weight concerns in veterans. Theodore V. Cooper, University of Texas at El Paso M.

More information

Individual differences in difficulty quitting smoking

Individual differences in difficulty quitting smoking British Joumal of Addiction (1991) 86, 555-558 Individual differences in difficulty quitting smoking PETR HAJEK ICRF Health Behaviour Unit, Institute of Psychiatry, 101 Denmark Hill, London SES 8AP, UK

More information

Mindfulness as a Mediator of Psychological Wellbeing in a Stress Reduction Intervention for Cancer Patients - a randomized study

Mindfulness as a Mediator of Psychological Wellbeing in a Stress Reduction Intervention for Cancer Patients - a randomized study Mindfulness as a Mediator of Psychological Wellbeing in a Stress Reduction Intervention for Cancer Patients - a randomized study Richard Bränström Department of oncology-pathology Karolinska Institute

More information

DUI SERVICE PROVIDER ORIENTATION

DUI SERVICE PROVIDER ORIENTATION DUI SERVICE PROVIDER ORIENTATION Illinois Department of Human Services Division of Alcoholism and Substance Abuse Provided by the Institute for Legal, Legislative & Policy Studies Center for State Policy

More information

Trait anxiety and nicotine dependence in adolescents A report from the DANDY study

Trait anxiety and nicotine dependence in adolescents A report from the DANDY study Addictive Behaviors 29 (2004) 911 919 Short communication Trait anxiety and nicotine dependence in adolescents A report from the DANDY study Joseph R. DiFranza a, *, Judith A. Savageau a, Nancy A. Rigotti

More information

Nicotine Craving and Cue Exposure Therapy by Using Virtual Environments ABSTRACT

Nicotine Craving and Cue Exposure Therapy by Using Virtual Environments ABSTRACT CYBERPSYCHOLOGY & BEHAVIOR Volume 7, Number 6, 2004 Mary Ann Liebert, Inc. Nicotine Craving and Cue Exposure Therapy by Using Virtual Environments JANGHAN LEE, Ph.D., 1,4 YUNGSIK LIM, Ph.D., 2 SIMON J.

More information

Putative Mechanisms Underlying Risky Decision-Making in High Risk Drivers

Putative Mechanisms Underlying Risky Decision-Making in High Risk Drivers Putative Mechanisms Underlying Risky Decision-Making in High Risk Drivers Samantha Ashley Wells 1,2, Thomas Gordon Brown 1,2 Psychiatry Department, McGill University Psychiatry Department, Douglas Hospital

More information

Trigger. Myths About the Use of Medication in Recovery BUPRENORPHINE TREATMENT: A TRAINING FOR MULTIDISCIPLINARY ADDICTION PROFESSIONALS

Trigger. Myths About the Use of Medication in Recovery BUPRENORPHINE TREATMENT: A TRAINING FOR MULTIDISCIPLINARY ADDICTION PROFESSIONALS BUPRENORPHINE TREATMENT: A TRAINING FOR MULTIDISCIPLINARY ADDICTION PROFESSIONALS Module VI Counseling Buprenorphine Patients Myths About the Use of Medication in Recovery! Patients are still addicted!

More information