Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Language Sciences. journal homepage:

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Language Sciences. journal homepage:"

Transcription

1 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx ARTICLE Article history: Available online xxx INFO Keywords: Direct Object Scrambling High-Functioning Autism Specific language impairment Pragmatics Grammar Interface ABSTRACT Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Language Sciences journal homepage: Unravelling the complexity of Direct Object Scrambling Jeannette Schaeffer Amsterdam Center for Language and Communication, Dept. of Cultural Analysis & Linguistics, Faculty of Humanities, University of Amsterdam, Spuistraat 210, 1012 VT Amsterdam, The Netherlands 1. Introduction This study investigates the complexity of Direct Object Scrambling (DOS) by testing this phenomenon experimentally in three Dutch-speaking groups of children between the ages of 6 and 14: SLI, HFA, and TD (Specific Language Impairment, High Functioning Autism, and Typically Developing). First, the results show that, despite the failure to scramble in both pathological groups, the children with HFA do not display any morpho-syntactic deficits, whereas the children with SLI do. This suggests that the children with SLI and the children with HFA fail to scramble for different reasons. It is argued that children with SLI do this because of a problem with syntactic object placement, whereas children with HFA leave referential direct objects unscrambled because they fail to consistently integrate the different components of DOS. These results from two different pathological groups uncover different components in the complex phenomenon of DOS, at the interface of grammar (including (morpho-)syntax and semantics) and pragmatics: syntactic object placement, definiteness/referentiality, speaker/hearer beliefs, and information structure. Earlier work on DOS in younger, TD Dutch-acquiring children identifies another component of DOS, namely the pragmatic knowledge that speaker and hearer are always independent (Concept of Non-Shared Assumptions, Schaeffer, 2000). As such, the current study demonstrates the important contribution of acquisition research in TD as well as pathological populations to the unravelling and understanding of complex linguistic phenomena Published by Elsevier Ltd. This study investigates the complexity of Direct Object Scrambling (DOS) in Dutch from the perspective of impaired language acquisition. DOS concerns the placement of a direct object before or after an adverb or negation, as exemplified in (1) (4): address: j.c.schaeffer@uva.nl (J. Schaeffer) / 2016 Published by Elsevier Ltd.

2 2 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx Referential objects tend to be scrambled, as illustrated in (1) (3). In contrast, non-referential objects must remain unscrambled, as shown in (4). This will be further elaborated in section 2. It is argued that children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI) and children with High Functioning Autism (HFA) both have problems with DOS, but for different reasons: children with SLI fail to scramble because of grammatical difficulties; children with HFA fail to scramble because of problems with the integration of information from different levels. This illuminates the complexity of DOS: it is a phenomenon at the interface of grammar and pragmatics, thus requiring knowledge of the relevant pragmatic and grammatical operations, plus the interaction between these different types of knowledge. I therefore propose a definition of complexity in terms of interface: the more interaction between language components the phenomenon involves, the more complex it is. 2. Background 2.1. The phenomenon of Direct Object Scrambling in adult Dutch The examples in (1) (3) suggest that it is the referentiality of direct objects that drives scrambling, i.e., the placement in a position preceding negation or an adverb. Before elaborating on the issue of scrambling it is necessary to define referentiality. For Fodor and Sag (1982) the most crucial property of referential nominal expressions is their uniqueness : a referential nominal refers to a unique entity in the world, which can be identified by someone. This someone is usually, but not necessarily, the speaker. Schaeffer (2000) proposes a definition of referentiality that is close to Fodor and Sag's description, namely, a nominal expression is referential if it has a fixed referent, implying that it is known to the speaker and/or to someone whose propositional attitudes are being reported. This is stated in (5): Notice that referentiality is a semantic notion, i.e. it refers to the way a nominal expression is interpreted, or understood. As hinted at in the definition in (5), and further elaborated on in Schaeffer and Matthewson (2005), like definiteness, referentiality is tied to the different states of speaker and hearer beliefs. Beliefs which are shared by all interlocutors in a discourse are said to be in the so-called common ground of the discourse (see e.g. Stalnaker, 1974, 1978; Heim, 1982). The use of the definite article the requires the existence of a referent that is part of the shared beliefs between speaker and hearer, i.e. the common ground. As such, noun phrases that have a referent in the common ground are referential. As Schaeffer & Matthewson show, there are various ways for the existence of referents to become part of the common ground, two of which are illustrated in (6). 1 1 Note that there is another use of the definite article, namely the generic use, as in (i)(i) The American citizen knows every insurance policy on the market. As this use is not tested in the current study, it is not discussed here.

3 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx 3 In (6a), the existence of a unique entity corresponding to the definite noun phrase the girl is part of the shared beliefs between speaker and hearer, i.e. the common ground, because it was established in the previous discourse, namely by the indefinite noun phrase a girl. In (6b), the existence of a unique entity corresponding to the definite nominal expression the sun is likewise part of the common ground, but for a different reason, namely that it is part of the long-term shared beliefs between speaker and hearer. The existence of the sun is always entailed by the common ground and it need not be explicitly introduced in prior discourse. Sometimes, the speaker but not the hearer believes in the existence of an entity corresponding to the noun phrase. For example, in (7), the speaker has grounds for an existential assertion about movies, while the hearer does not. The sentence in (7) would be felicitous even if the hearer did not previously believe in the existence of any movies at all. An indefinite as in (7) is referred to as a referential indefinite. It is also possible for neither the speaker nor the hearer to have grounds for an existential assertion. Examples of this are given in (8). Indefinites such as the ones in (8) are referred to as non-referential indefinites. As Schaeffer and Matthewson (2005) propose, the three possible belief states can be schematized as in Table 1 (see also Ionin, 2006 for a similar, but four-way distinction). Believed by X is shorthand for X has grounds for an existential assertion. The description of referentiality provided above suggests that it requires both semantic knowledge (uniqueness, existentiality), and pragmatic knowledge (speaker/hearer beliefs). As such, it can be considered a phenomenon at the interface of semantics and pragmatics, and therefore complex. Now, let's return to the sentences in (1) (4) in section 1. These examples suggest that referential direct objects tend to be scrambled, thus suggesting that a position preceding an adverb or negation renders a referential interpretation. However, it is possible to come up with sentences with referential direct objects in which both orders (scrambled and non-scrambled) seem fine. Consider, for example, the sentences in (10B), with a definite, referential object and a sentential adverb, as answers to the question by A (capitals indicate stress): Native speakers of Dutch judge both sentences correct (cf. De Hoop, 2003, a.o.), 2 whereas they usually disfavor the non- 2 Nonetheless, in an experimental setting native Dutch speakers clearly prefer a definite direct object to precede high adverbs such as waarschijnlijk, as shown by Schaeffer (1997, 2000): in an Elicited Production Task the 23 Dutch-speaking adults scrambled referential objects over high adverbs 91% of the time.

4 4 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx Table 1 Belief states of interlocutors in a discourse. A Believed by speaker and hearer Part of common ground Definite Referential the/de B Believed by speaker only Not part of common ground Indefinite Referential a/een C Believed by neither speaker nor hearer Not part of common ground Indefinite Non-referential a/een English/Dutch scrambled order in (2) above. In Schaeffer (1997, 2000, 2012) I argue that this difference is due to the different types of adverbs being used: goed ( well ) is a so-called low adverb, or VP adverb, while waarschijnlijk ( probably ) or vaak ( often ) is a high, or sentential (IP) adverb (see also Cinque, 1999). In case of a high adverb such as waarschijnlijk/vaak, the direct object possibly occupies a position higher than its base-generated sister-of-v position (i.e., scrambled), but still lower than the high adverb, which is assumed to be adjoined to IP. This way, the surface word order in (10Ba) resembles a non-scrambled order, but it is reasonable to assume that, underlyingly, the direct object occupies a higher, scrambled position, providing the direct object het boek with its referential interpretation. This implies that there are different positions to scramble to. For a detailed and extended analysis of these different scrambling positions the reader is referred to Schaeffer (2000, 2012), who relates different types of referentiality to distinct scrambling positions. Finally, consider the dialogues in (11) and (12), adopted from Schaeffer (2012). If the referent of a definite direct object is directly available in the preceding discourse, as in (11), scrambling (even over a high adverb such as waarschijnlijk) is strongly preferred, while this scrambled word order is disfavored if the referent of the object is not available, as illustrated in (12Ba). The question arises as to which position the direct object de krant occupies in (12Bb), which could be either the non-scrambled sister-of-v position, or a position slightly higher than that, but still lower than the adverb waarschijnlijk. I argue that in this case, the definite direct object de krant ( the newspaper ) in (12Bb) remains truly unscrambled. This is confirmed by the sentence in (13), in which the high adverb waarschijnlijk from (12B) is replaced by the low adverb zorgvuldig ( carefully ), which is assumed to occupy a position just above VP (cf. Cinque, 1999):

5 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx 5 It is possible for the definite direct object de krant ( the newspaper ) to remain unscrambled, because its referent is part of the long-term shared knowledge between the speaker and the hearer (Schaeffer, 1997, 2000, 2012), or, as Ionin (2006) argues, it is a non-specific definite, or, as Erteschik-Shir (2007) puts it, it concerns a so-called permanently available topic. Similar to noun phrases such as the queen, the sun and the Bible, de krant ( the newspaper ) can be definite without being mentioned in the preceding discourse, because of its status as a fixture in the (model of the) world. Now, notice that, despite its permanently-available-topic status, the definite direct object de krant in (12Bb) as an answer to the question in (12A) does require focus: it is part of the new information requested by speaker A. Interestingly, this naturally follows from Cinque's (1993) Nuclear Stress Rule (NSR), which states that main sentential stress is assigned to the most deeply embedded constituent. If de krant in (12Bb) is truly unscrambled, i.e., occupies the sister-of-v position, it is the most deeply embedded constituent in the sentence, providing it with the required stress to mark focus. If it occupied a higher, scrambled position, it would not be the most deeply embedded constituent, and thus not automatically receive the required stress (through the NSR) to mark focus. Thus, while referential direct objects such as het boek in (10) are assumed to reside in a scrambled position (whether they precede or follow the high adverb waarschijnlijk or vaak), I argue that (non-specific, in Ionin's 2006 terms) definite direct objects such as de krant in (12Bb) remain in sister-of-v position, which is compatible with both their permanently-available-topic status and with their focus. Summarizing the relationship between (different types of) direct objects and (non-)scrambled positions we can say that definite direct objects that are referential because their referent is available in the preceding discourse tend to be scrambled, whereas (non-specific in Ionin's, 2006 terms) definite direct objects with a permanently-available-topic status can occupy a non-scrambled position. In contrast, non-referential objects must always occur in non-scrambled position. The discussion above suggests that DOS is an interface operation involving pragmatic, semantic and syntactic knowledge: First, consideration of both speaker and hearer beliefs (pragmatics) is necessary to establish definiteness and referentiality (semantics). Second, definiteness and referentiality need to be mapped onto the information structural notions of topic and focus (pragmatics), which, in turn, have certain positions in the sentence (syntax). In the next section I discuss some previous studies on the acquisition of referentiality and DOS by typically developing (TD) children. The aim is to show that it is difficult to tease apart the different contributions of syntax, semantics and pragmatics to the development of DOS, because typical development of these phenomena is rapid, and syntactic, semantic and pragmatic development go hand-in-hand, which is unsurprising, given the analysis of DOS provided above Previous acquisition studies on referentiality and Direct Object Scrambling It is well-known that young children overgenerate the definite article in indefinite contexts (English: Brown, 1973; Maratsos, 1974, 1976; Zehler and Brewer, 1982; French: Karmiloff-Smith, 1979; Hebrew: Armon-Lotem and Avram, 2005). However, most of these studies do not systematically distinguish between the two types of indefinite articles (see schema in (8)), namely believed by speaker only (the B contexts) and believed by neither speaker nor hearer (the C contexts). Schaeffer and Matthewson (2005) investigated the phenomenon of article choice in 26 monolingual English-acquiring children between the ages of 2 and 4 and a control group of 36 adults. Their Elicited Production Task tested all three belief states schematized in Table 1. Their results show that the children produce the definite article the 25% of the time in indefinite B-contexts, i.e., when the referent is believed to exist by the speaker only, whereas the adults virtually never did this (2%). In contrast, the was hardly ever overgenerated in C-contexts, i.e., when the referent is believed to exist by neither the speaker nor the hearer (children: 5%; adults: 3%). Finally, there was virtually no overgeneration of the indefinite article a in A-contexts (definite): both children and adults did this 2% of the time. In order to explain their results Schaeffer & Matthewson propose that young children under age 4 lack the pragmatic Concept of Non-Shared Assumptions (CNSA), which states that speaker and hearer assumptions are always independent (cf. Schaeffer, 1999, 2000). If the CNSA is absent, speaker and hearer assumptions are not always independent, implying that there are situations in which the speaker automatically attributes her/his own assumptions to the hearer. If a

6 6 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx child attributes her own (speaker-) beliefs to the hearer, she will not distinguish environment B (believed by speaker only) from environment A (believed by speaker and hearer). In other words, environment B becomes environment A. In these cases, she will use the article appropriate for environment A, which is the in English. Thus, in the cases in which the child does not distinguish speaker and hearer beliefs, she groups together environment A and B, as opposed to C. The CNSA reminds of Theory of Mind, the cognitive ability to attribute mental states to one self and others (Premack and Woodruff, 1978). As such, the CNSA may well be a precursor or component of Theory of Mind. Concluding, the overgeneration of the in B-contexts by young TD children can be attributed to pragmatic, or even cognitive immaturity. Turning now to the acquisition of DOS, Schaeffer (1997, 2000) carried out an Elicited Production Task on DOS over negation and different types of adverbs with 49 monolingual TD Dutch-acquiring children between the ages of 2 and 7. She found that 2- (and to a lesser extent) 3-year old TD Dutch-acquiring children often fail to scramble over negation in referential contexts (70% and 28%, respectively for definite DPs and 69% and 27%, respectively for proper names). Schaeffer attributes this to the failure to distinguish between different types of referentiality, namely between so-called discourse-related referentiality, in which referentiality is established on the basis of the preceding discourse, (as the girl in 6a), and non-discourse-related referentiality, in which referentiality is established on the basis of long-term shared knowledge, as the sun in (6b). Recall from example (12Bb) and the discussion following it that definite nouns such as the newspaper, the sun, the queen etc. have a special status in that they are part of long-term shared knowledge, or that they are a permanently available topic. As such, they can remain unscrambled. Assuming a movement analysis of DOS, Schaeffer claims that as long as children do not distinguish between definites such as the girl and the sun, the correct syntactic [referential] feature (discourse-related or non-discourse-related) motivating this movement cannot be established, and scrambling of the referential direct object takes place optionally. As the relevant distinction relates to speaker/hearer assumptions and what they are based on, it is concluded that it is the children's underdeveloped pragmatics that causes the failure to scramble referential direct objects in young Dutch-speaking children. In other words, Schaeffer claims that young Dutch-acquiring children who fail to scramble have immature pragmatics, rather than immature syntax or semantics. Nevertheless, it is not always clear from the language acquisition of TD children what the underlying causes of their language errors are. Syntactic, semantic, pragmatic and cognitive development takes place rapidly, and often at similar rates. Therefore, it is not easy to tease apart the relative contributions of these different components to language development in typically developing children. Moreover, as noted in section 2.1, DOS is a complex phenomenon, at the interface of grammar and pragmatics, and many different theories have been proposed to account for it. In an attempt to unravel this complexity and the respective roles of grammar and pragmatics in the adult Dutch phenomenon of DOS and in its development in child Dutch I turned to two types of pathology that express themselves in different kinds of language impairment: children with High-Functioning Autism (HFA), who often show weaknesses in pragmatics, and have problems integrating information from different levels, and children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI), whose primary impairment is in grammar (including morphosyntax and phonology). As we will see in the discussion section of this paper, the study of language impairment can help differentiate between existing analyses of a certain linguistic phenomenon, and even refine such analyses. The next sections discuss the kinds of language impairment in both groups and their potentially overlapping etiologies (High-Functioning) Autism High Functioning Autism (HFA) is a subgroup of people with so-called Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.; DSM-V; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) defines ASD as a neurodevelopmental disorder, associated with deficits in social communication and social interaction on the one hand, and restrictive, repetitive patterns of behavior, interests or activities on the other hand. These symptoms occur early in life, impair everyday functioning, and cannot be explained by global developmental delay or intellectual disabilities, although the latter frequently co-occurs with ASD. The spectrum illustrates the varying severity of the various forms of autism, with Autistic Disorder at the more severe end of the spectrum, through Asperger's Disorder, Pervasive Developmental Disorder-Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS), and HFA at the least severe end of the spectrum (Taylor et al., 2012). Individuals with HFA are higher functioning than other individuals with autism: they attend regular schools and all acquire functional use of verbal language. However, the DSM-V states that even when formal linguistic skills such as vocabulary and grammar are intact, the use of language for communicative purposes is impaired in ASD. Language acquisition studies involving children with autism largely support this description of language and language development in ASD, although mixed results have been reported (Grammar: Tager-Flusberg et al., 1990; Bartolucci et al., 1980; Howlin, 1984; Eigsti et al., 2007, 2011; Phonology: Eigsti et al., 2011; Bartak et al., 1975; Kjelgaard and Tager-Flusberg, 2001; Pragmatics: Ghaziuddin and Gerstein, 1996; Adachi et al., 2004; Kelley et al., 2006; Baltaxe, 1977; Baltaxe and D'Angiola, 1992; Fine et al., 1994; Arnold et al., 2009). The results of these studies illustrate the problems individuals with autism have with discourse management, and discourse reference in particular. In a nutshell, children with autism have communicative impairments that are most evidently manifested in their pragmatic and discourse functioning. While children with ASD may have other linguistic deficits, e.g., in the domain of syntax, this is not necessarily the case (cf. Perovic et al., 2013a,b).

7 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx 7 Recent neuroimaging studies suggest that ASD is a disorder of brain underconnectivity (Minshew and Williams, 2007; Just et al., 2012). These studies have shown that synchronization of activation between different brain regions is lower in ASD. The patterns of neurological underconnectivity in ASD have been proposed as an explanation for the unrelatedness of the main symptoms of ASD, i.e., social and communicative impairments on the one hand, and restricted and repetitive patterns of behavior and interests on the other hand (Just et al., 2012). Importantly, according to the underconnectivity hypothesis, individuals with ASD are likely to experience difficulty in processes that involve high levels of information integration and require coordination of multiple neural systems, whereas cognitive abilities that involve local neural networks and that require low levels of information processing are intact (Minshew and Williams, 2007). In terms of linguistic capacities, these findings have been related to impaired higher order language abilities and intact formal language skills in adults with ASD (Minshew et al., 1997) and in children with HFA (Williams et al., 2006). The linguistic abilities that are categorized as higher order in these studies are almost exclusively pragmatic or discourse abilities, such as comprehension and creation of story themes, metaphors, inferences and idioms, while vocabulary and basic syntactic abilities, such as being able to parse a sentence structure, are considered formal language skills. What crucially differentiates these different types of linguistic capacities is that higher-order abilities require high levels of information integration and involve multiple neural systems, while formal abilities demand low levels of information integration and are processed locally (Minshew and Williams, 2007) Specific Language Impairment In contrast, the primary language disorder in Specific Language Impairment (SLI) is often claimed to reside in grammar, including morphosyntax and phonology. SLI is defined as a combination of normal intelligence (an IQ equal to or greater than 85) and impaired performance on a battery of standardized language assessments (a composite language score of more than 1.25 SD below the mean) (Leonard, 1998; Tomblin et al., 1997). Potential causes such as hearing loss, low general ability, or physical impairment of articulators are excluded. ASD is also excluded when making a diagnosis of SLI: the textbook example of a child with SLI is of a child with normal social interaction and nonverbal communication, but with specific difficulties in mastering structural aspects of language, especially morphosyntax (Bishop, 2010). Several researchers have identified an even more specific type of language impairment, namely Grammatical Specific Language Impairment, or GSLI, with impairment in only morphosyntax, while pragmatics and other cognitive skills are intact (Van der Lely and Stollwerck, 1997; Van der Lely et al., 2011; Van der Lely and Ullman, 2001; Friedmann and Novogrodsky, 2011; Schaeffer, 2012, among others). However, recent research suggests that many children diagnosed with SLI have several additional impairments in cognition, such as a weak working memory and other executive functions, and in pragmatics. This has challenged the claim that SLI is specific to language (Lum et al., 2007, 2012; Henry et al., 2012; Evans et al., 2009) Objectives of the study In summary, the traditional assumption is that the primary language impairment of children with autism is concentrated in pragmatics and information integration, while children with SLI are primarily impaired in morphosyntax. However, some recent studies challenge these claims by showing that children with SLI also have pragmatic and cognitive impairments, and that children with ASD have additional grammatical impairments. The objective of the current study on the acquisition of Direct Object Scrambling (DOS) in children with HFA and in children with SLI is not to resolve the debate on the exact characteristics of SLI and ASD. Rather, its goal is to show how the study of pathological populations can provide novel insights into the complexity of linguistic phenomena such as DOS. In the next section I lay out my hypotheses and predictions regarding DOS in HFA and SLI, based on the discussion provided in sections Hypotheses and predictions Recall that Dutch direct objects prefer to occupy a position preceding negation or an adverb when they have a referential reading, i.e., when either both the speaker and the hearer believe the referent to exist (A-context) or only the speaker believes the referent to exist (B-context). Nonetheless, certain definite direct objects, such as the sun, the queen, can easily remain unscrambled, because of their special status of permanently available topic. When a direct object is non-referential, it must follow negation or the adverb (= non-scrambled position). Based on these observations, I hypothesize that DOS is a complex phenomenon at the interface of pragmatics, semantics and syntax, as defined in (14):

8 8 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx Furthermore, following the traditional assumptions regarding language impairments in children with HFA and in children with SLI (see sections 2.2 and 2.3), I hypothesize that children with HFA have problems with the integration of information from different components, such as the ones described in Hypothesis 1, and that children with SLI have impaired syntax. This is formalized in (15) and (16): The predictions that follow from these three hypotheses are listed in (17): The next section describes what methods were used to test these predictions. 3. Methods 3.1. Participants For the recruitment of the children with HFA autism groups on Facebook and Dutch organizations for autism were asked to post an advertisement on their websites and in their magazines. HFA participants were also recruited through personal contacts. This resulted in a group of 28 Dutch-speaking children with HFA, aged 5 14 (mean: 10;5; SD 2.37). All these children were diagnosed with an Autism Spectrum Disorder by means of the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Table 2 below

9 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx 9 Table 2 Specific diagnoses for children with HFA. N Age range Mean age (SD) Classical Autism ;3 (3.31) Asperger's Syndrome ;6 (1.86) PDD-NOS ;6 (2.89) ASD not further specified ;3 (0.92) presents the specific diagnoses for all children and their ages (Classical Autism, Asperger's Syndrome or Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS)). Furthermore, 28 children with SLI aged 6 14 (mean: 10;1, SD 2.11) were selected from special schools for children with speech and language problems in The Netherlands. Children with an IQ < 85 and/or officially diagnosed with any additional disorder (such as autism in the SLI group or language impairment in the HFA group) or AD(H)D were not included. Nevertheless, we do not exclude the existence of comorbidity with other developmental disorders in both the SLI and the HFA group. 28 typically developing (TD) children aged 6 14 (mean: 10;4 SD 2.15) served as a control group. All child participants were individually matched with respect to age and gender. Age-normalized scores of expressive and receptive linguistic ability were obtained from the Dutch version of the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF-4-NL) (Semel et al., 2008). Whereas the SLI group performed far below the norm score of the 50th percentile (mean 7.9th percentile, SD 7.34), the HFA and TD groups performed around or above the norm score (HFA mean 53.7th percentile, SD 29.46, TD mean 73.4th percentile, SD 24.85). Furthermore, Non-Word Repetition (Rispens and Baker, 2012) scores obtained from each group confirm the specific impairment of the children with SLI, as opposed to that of the children with HFA 3 : The SLI group's NWR score was 33.2%, vs. 62.6% for the HFA group, and 70.2% for the TD group. Additionally, parents of all HFA and of 15 SLI participants completed the Dutch version of the Children's Communication Checklist (CCC-2-NL) (Geurts, 2007). The CCC is a parents' questionnaire, and one of its main goals is to give an impression of pragmatic and grammatical difficulties. Subtracting the sum of scores on the language areas (speech, syntax, semantics and coherence) from the sum of scores on pragmatic areas' (initiation, nonverbal communication, social relations and interests) derives the Social Interaction Difference Index (SIDI). This is a difference score that is given as a percentile. Children who score beneath the 10th percentile have more structural language problems than pragmatic difficulties, whereas the opposite is true for children scoring above the 90th percentile. As expected, the HFA group has a high SIS score (mean 81.6, SD = 19.8), whereas the SLI group has a low SIS score (mean = 14.9, SD = 12.9). This shows that the HFA group experiences more pragmatic difficulties, while the SLI group has more structural language problems. Finally, 16 adult native speakers of Dutch aged (mean: 34; SD 14.26) were tested to ensure the psychological reality of the expected target responses of all experiments Materials and procedure The Direct Object Scrambling (DOS) Task used for the current study is part of a much larger battery of 16 tests designed to investigate the grammatical, pragmatic and cognitive development of children with SLI and children with HFA, in collaboration with Iris Duinmeijer, at the University of Amsterdam. For the purposes of this study, we focus here on the description of the DOS materials. The tasks of some of the other phenomena to which DOS performance is compared are briefly mentioned in section 4.2. The DOS materials consist of an Elicited Production Task (Schaeffer, 1997, 2000; Unsworth, 2005) in which the participants are asked to describe an event in a picture displayed on a computer screen to an experimenter (A) who cannot see the screen while a second experimenter (B) is sitting next to the participant. While watching the picture on the screen the participant and experimenter A listen to a story told by experimenter B. After this, experimenter A (who cannot see the screen) asks a question or makes a comment. As part of the introduction to the experiment the participant is told that experimenter A is not always paying attention and that experimenter A needs to be corrected if s/he says something wrong. All experimental items concern Direct Object Scrambling with respect to sentential negation, rather than adverbs for the following reason: Sentences with sentential negation present the clearest cases for Direct Object Scrambling in Dutch, because negation occupies one and the same position in the sentence structure. As discussed in section 2.1, there are different types of adverbs (e.g., high vs. low) which occupy different positions in the sentence structure. Therefore, it is sometimes difficult to determine the exact position (scrambled or non-scrambled) of the direct object, especially when it is used in combination with a high adverb. Negation circumvents this problem by occurring in a fixed, and relatively low position, allowing us to observe whether the object is in a non-scrambled sister-of-v position, or in a scrambled position higher up. Following Schaeffer and Matthewson's (2005) schema on definiteness and referentiality as presented in section 2.1, three different conditions are distinguished, namely (i) definite (6 test items), (ii) indefinite-referential (6 test items), and (iii) indefinite- 3 Children with SLI often score significantly lower than TD children on the NWR (cf. Rispens and Baker, 2012; among others).

10 10 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx non-referential (6 test items). Furthermore, the experiment contains 18 fillers, eliciting utterances with a definite or an indefinite noun phrase (which were used for a different study cf. Schaeffer et al., 2014, 2015). The conditions are schematized in Table 3, and sample scenarios of each condition are provided in (18a through c). 4 4 Capitals indicate stress.

11 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx 11 Table 3 Experimental conditions Elicited Production Task on Direct Object Scrambling. Condition 1 Definite 6 2 Indefinite referential 6 3 Indefinite non-referential 6 Total experimental items 18 Fillers 18 Number of items Note that in the preamble by experimenter B there is a sentence containing both negation niet and a direct object (dat that ). Yet, the direct object is in neither scrambled nor non-scrambled position. Dutch allows for a third position for a direct object, namely the topicalized position at the beginning of the structure. By employing this topicalized position for the direct object giving away a (non-)scrambled order in the preamble is avoided. In the Indefinite referential condition a cartoon figure and three identical objects were shown on the screen, as illustrated in (18b):

12 12 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx The reason a plural rather than a singular direct object was chosen in this condition is the fact that in experimental situations like this singular referential indefinites often get changed into a definite, as witnessed by the scrambling study in Schaeffer (1997, 2000). Finally, in the indefinite non-referential condition, no objects are shown. Only a cartoon figure is presented. Here the participant is not asked to correct experimenter A, but rather, experimenter A is not paying attention and asks the participant for help. A sample item is given in (18c):

13 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx 13 A complete list of experimental items can be found in the Appendix.

14 14 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx 4. Results and discussion 4.1. Results on Direct Object Scrambling In this section I present the results according to the three experimental conditions of the Elicited Production task on Direct Object Scrambling: 1) definite; 2) indefinite referential; 3) indefinite non-referential. Recall that the first two conditions require scrambling of the direct object, while the third condition does not. As the graph in Fig. 1 shows, both the children with HFA and children with SLI fail to scramble the definite direct object at substantial rates in obligatory contexts: As expected, the adults and the TD children perform almost at ceiling, and virtually never produce non-scrambled structures in this condition. Recall that the youngest TD children are 6 years old, i.e., much beyond the age of acquisition of Direct Object Scrambling as proposed by Schaeffer (1997, 2000), which is the age of 4. However, examining the white bars of the HFA and the SLI groups, we see that the children with HFA produce only 70% (SD: 35%) scrambled structures, and the children with SLI a mere 49% (SD: 35%). Statistical comparison (Mann Whitney U-tests) with the results of the TD children reveals that both the HFA group and the SLI group perform significantly worse than the TD group (mean: 91%; SD: 20%). (HFA TD: U = 233, p <.005, SLI TD: U = 127.5, p <.001). Interestingly, the number of non-scrambled responses in the HFA group (16%) does not significantly differ from that of the TD children, while the number of non-scrambled responses in the SLI group (33%) does. This suggests a slightly lesser problem with DOS in HFA than in SLI. Irrelevant answers in the definite condition include the use of non-referential indefinite NPs and personal pronouns as shown in (19a and 19b). Fig. 1. Proportions of scrambled, non-scrambled and irrelevant responses in definite condition.

15 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx 15 The result pattern of the definite condition is similar to that of the indefinite referential condition, as illustrated in Fig. 2: The results for the adults in Fig. 2 indicate that indefinite referential contexts impose a slightly weaker obligation to scramble the direct object than the definite condition: 13% of the responses contained a non-scrambled direct object. The same pattern can be seen in the TD children (9% non-scrambling). There are no differences between the adult group and the TD child group (U = 186.5, p =.288). Nevertheless, the vast majority of indefinite referential direct objects in this condition get scrambled, indicating at least a strong preference for scrambling in typical populations. In contrast, both the HFA group and the SLI group leave indefinite referential direct objects unscrambled at a rate of 37% and 47%, respectively. In both cases this is significantly more than the TD children (HFA TD: U = 244, p <.01, SLI TD: U = 162, p <.001). Interestingly, the number of (correctly) scrambled responses in the HFA group (57%) is not significantly lower than in the TD group, while the number of (correctly) scrambled responses in the SLI group (41%) is. Again, this suggests more knowledge of scrambling in HFA than in SLI. Irrelevant answers in the indefinite conditions include topicalized direct objects (20a) or the lack of sentential negation (20b): Merging the two referential conditions together, a clear picture of the failure to scramble referential direct objects by children with HFA and SLI emerges: Fig. 3 shows that there is no difference between the proportions of scrambled items between the adult group (mean: 91%; SD: 17%) and the TD child group (mean: 84%; SD: 23%; U = 191, p =.378). Both groups show a clear preference for scrambling referential objects. However, the HFA group scrambles referential direct objects only at a rate of 63%, while the children with SLI do this even less often: 45%. In both cases this is significantly lower than the scrambling rate of the TD group (84%) (HFA TD: U = 231, p <.01, SLI TD: U = 116, p <.001). Looking at it from the perspective of non-scrambled structures, both the HFA and the SLI Fig. 2. Proportions of scrambled, non-scrambled and irrelevant responses in indefinite referential condition.

16 16 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx Fig. 3. Proportions of scrambled, non-scrambled and irrelevant responses in referential conditions (definite and indefinite referential conditions collapsed). group fail to scramble referential direct objects significantly more often than their TD peers: the HFA group does this at a rate of 27%, and the SLI group at a rate of 40% (HFA TD: U = 232.5, p <.01, SLI TD: U = 97.5, p <.001). Nonetheless, the analysis of the responses in the third condition the indefinite non-referential condition suggests that Direct Object Scrambling is not random in the HFA and SLI groups, as shown in Fig. 4: As indicated by the light grey bars for each group, virtually all indefinite non-referential direct objects remain unscrambled. In fact, a Kruskal Wallis test reveals that there are no significant differences between the proportion of direct objects correctly left unscrambled between the child groups and the adult group (Chi 2 = 7.611, p =.055). Additionally, there is no significant difference regarding the other types of responses (scrambled and irrelevant) between the four groups either (Kruskal Wallis test: Chi 2 = 3.716, p =.294 and Chi 2 = 6.608, p =.086 respectively). To summarize the results, both children with HFA and children with SLI often fail to scramble in referential conditions. The children with HFA do this 27% of the time, the children with SLI 40%. The lower DOS failure rate in the HFA group suggests that children with HFA may have more knowledge of DOS than children with SLI (to be elaborated on in section 4.3). Furthermore, the children with HFA and the children with SLI behave target-like in the non-referential contexts: they do not scramble. This suggests that DOS is not random in these populations. Given the usual heterogeneity of impaired populations, and the wide age-range investigated in this study, some obvious questions to arise are a) whether DOS failure in each group is perhaps due to just a few participants and b) whether the younger children perform worse than the older children on DOS. An individual analysis of so-called failers and passers on DOS reveals that the answer to both questions is no. Participants were assigned a pass if they left no more than 2 out of 12 referential items unscrambled, and a fail if they left at least 3 out of 12 referential items unscrambled. This resulted in 18/28 (64%) failers Fig. 4. Proportions of scrambled, non-scrambled and irrelevant responses in indefinite non-referential condition.

17 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx 17 in the SLI group, 15/28 (54%) failers in the HFA group, 1 failer in the TD group (the youngest child) and no failers in the adult group. Thus, each impaired group contains a fair amount of failers, and the numbers of failers in both impaired groups are comparable. Furthermore, the 18 SLI failers ranged in age between 6 and 13, with a mean age of 9;11, and the 15 HFA failers ranged in age between 5 and 14, with a mean age of 9;10. This indicates that, contrary to what is known about DOS in TD children (Schaeffer, 1997, 2000), failing on DOS is not associated with age in children with SLI or with HFA. In other words, the individual results replicate the group results. Now, recall the predictions that children with HFA fail to scramble because of problems with the integration of the different components involved in DOS, and that children with SLI fail to scramble because of deficient syntax. The prediction that both groups have problems with scrambling is borne out, yet, the results presented so far cannot tell us anything about the underlying reasons. To this end, I examined all the children's scores on some other pragmatic and grammatical tasks that were part of the larger test battery, as mentioned in section 3. The results for the children with SLI are presented and discussed in section 4.2, the ones for the children with HFA in section Performance of children with SLI on other pragmatics and morpho-syntax tests As discussed in section 2, the choice between a definite and an indefinite article is partially driven by the pragmatic knowledge that speaker and hearer assumptions are always independent Schaeffer and Matthewson's (2005) Concept of Non-Shared Assumptions (CNSA), which may be related to Theory of Mind. The results of an Elicited Production Task on Article Choice show that the same children with SLI as the ones participating in the DOS experiment never overgenerate the definite article in (referential or non-referential) indefinite contexts (Schaeffer et al., 2014, 2015). These results are corroborated by the scores on a non-verbal Theory of Mind task (Colle et al., 2007): a one-way ANOVA revealed no significant differences between SLI, HFA and TD child groups (F = 1.619, p =.19) on this task (for a further discussion of the HFA performance on Theory of Mind, see section 4.3). Thus, the failure to scramble by the children with SLI in this study cannot be attributed to problems with referentiality, due to the lack of the CNSA or Theory of Mind, as was suggested for younger Dutch-acquiring TD children by Schaeffer (1997, 2000). This is not surprising in light of the traditional hypothesis that children with SLI are mainly impaired in their grammar (including morphosyntax), and that pragmatics is relatively spared. So what causes this failure to scramble in children with SLI? An investigation of the children's morpho-syntactic skills provides a key to answering this question. As mentioned in section 3, the DOS task is part of a larger test battery, including morpho-syntax tests such as the Dutch version of the CELF-IV (Core Language Score, Footnote 5), a Picture Selection Task on the mass-count distinction, crucially depending on the nominal plural morpheme (Van Witteloostuijn, 2013; Van Witteloostuijn and Schaeffer, 2015), and an Elicited Production Task on subject-verb agreement (Duinmeijer, 2012a, to appear). Table 4 shows that the children with SLI perform much worse than their TD peers on all three morpho-syntax tests: Mann Whitney U-tests regarding the differences between the SLI group and the TD group reveal that these are significant for all tasks (CELF: U = 19, p <.001; Mass-count: U = 227.5, p <.01; SVP: U = 130, p <.001). These results are in line with the hypothesis that children with SLI have impairments in their morpho-syntax, and suggest that their difficulty with Direct Object Scrambling stems from a syntactic weakness, as indicated in section 2.6. Spearman's Rank Order Correlations further confirm this for some of the morpho-syntax tests in the SLI group: a significant correlation is found between DOS (% scrambled in referential conditions) and the CELF percentile:.409, p <.05; a strong trend is found between DOS (% scrambled in referential conditions) and Subject Verb Production:.383, p =.053. The correlation between DOS and Mass-count (accuracy flexible nouns) is non-significant:.150, p =.455. I therefore propose that children with SLI fail to scramble referential objects because they have problems with syntactic object placement. Of course, a pragmatic cause cannot be excluded until pragmatics in children with SLI has been independently tested. In addition, it could be that children with SLI have difficulty with the semantic notions of uniqueness and existentiality. As none of the tests in our test battery specifically tested uniqueness and existentiality, future research with children with SLI should point out whether this is true or not. Yet, direct syntactic support for this hypothesis could come from three other syntax tests on object placement included in our larger test battery, namely a Picture Selection Task, a Judgment Task and an Elicited Production Task on Subject and Object Relative Clauses (Duinmeijer, 2012b, to appear). As the data for SLI are currently being analyzed, no hard numbers can be provided here, but the strong impression is that the children with SLI have indeed larger problems with the comprehension, judgment and production of Object Relative clauses than their TD peers (Duinmeijer, to appear). In the next section I discuss the additional pragmatics and morpho-syntax results for the children with HFA. 5 It is clear from the numbers in Table 6 that neither the TD children nor the children with HFA are particularly good at Object Relative Clauses, which calls for an explanation in and of itself. Nevertheless, it is striking that the children with HFA are no different from their TD peers on Object Relative Clauses, while they are outperformed by their TD peers on Direct Object Scrambling.

18 18 Language Sciences xxx (2017) xxx-xxx Table 4 SLI scores on morpho-syntax tests. CELF-IV a Mass-count distinction b Subject verb agreement SLI 7.9th percentile* (SD: 7.3) 68%* (SD: 20%) 83%* (SD: 26%) TD 73.4th percentile (SD: 24.9) 93% (SD: 11%) 96% (SD: 18%) * = significantly worse than TD (α =.05). a The Core language score of the CELF-IV includes the following sub-tests on grammar (crucially: NOT pragmatics): Concepts and Following Directions, Word Structure, Recalling Sentences, Formulated Sentences. b The percentages in this column reflect accuracy in the flexible conditions of the mass-count experiment (e.g., pizza vs. pizzas) Performance of children with HFA on other pragmatics and morpho-syntax tests Examination of the HFA group's performance on the (pragmatic) Article Choice task reveals a pattern similar to that of the SLI group: the same children with HFA as the ones participating in the DOS experiment never overgenerate the definite article in (referential or non-referential) indefinite contexts (Schaeffer et al., 2014, 2015). As already mentioned in section 4.2 on SLI above, the HFA group does not differ from the TD and SLI groups in terms of Theory of Mind scores either. Although this may be surprising in light of the common assumption that children with autism lack Theory of Mind (Baron-Cohen, 1988) it is in line with the Article Choice findings, and indicates that not all children with autism have a Theory of Mind deficit. The results on Article Choice and Theory of Mind suggest that, like the children with SLI, the children with HFA do not lack the CNSA, and that they know that speaker and hearer assumptions are always independent. In other words, the children with HFA have no difficulty distinguishing referential indefinites from non-referential indefinites and from definites. The children with HFA in this study resemble the children with SLI in that their failure to scramble cannot be attributed to problems with referentiality, due to the lack of the pragmatic CNSA or Theory of Mind, as was suggested for younger Dutch-acquiring TD children by Schaeffer (1997, 2000). To obtain a picture of the HFA group's morpho-syntactic skills, I analyzed their performance on the same morpho-syntax tests as mentioned in section 4.2 on SLI: the Dutch version of the CELF-IV (Core Language Score), a Picture Selection Task on the mass-count distinction, and an Elicited Production Task on subject-verb agreement. The results are presented in Table 5: As Table 5 shows, the children with HFA perform well on the mass-count distinction (82% correct). Compared to the TD children (92% correct) there is no significant difference (p =.352). The scores on the Subject-verb Production task show the same pattern: 95% correct as compared to 96% for the TD children, and again, no significant difference between the groups (U = 297, p =.158). The CELF scores indicate percentiles, so 50 can be considered the norm. As the last column demonstrates, the TD children performed well on the CELF (73.4th percentile), and higher than the HFA group (53.7th percentile). This difference is significant (U = 220.5, p <.05). Nonetheless, the children with HFA performed around the norm, and thus well on this task. Thus, the children with HFA in the current study perform well on grammatical tasks, and do not seem to be grammatically impaired. Correlations between the DOS scores and the three grammatical tasks, respectively, were not found either (Spearman's Rank Order Correlation: DOS (% scrambled in referential conditions) CELF percentile:.326, p =.097; DOS (% scrambled in referential conditions) mass/count (accuracy flexible nouns):.141, p =.482; DOS (% scrambled in referential conditions) SVP:.309, p =.116). To summarize, the children with HFA do not lack the pragmatic CNSA, show no morpho-syntactic impairment, and can therefore be hypothesized to know the syntax of object placement. This hypothesis gets support from the preliminary results of the grammatical Relative Clause tests that were also part of the larger test battery as mentioned above (Schaeffer and Siekman, 2016). In terms of producing, judging and comprehending Object Relative Clauses, the children with HFA do not differ significantly from their TD matches, as shown in Table 6 5 : Mann Whitney U-tests reveal that children with HFA are not outperformed by their TD age-mates on relative clause comprehension (U = 224, p =.274), judgment (U = 263.5, p =.616) or production (U = 299, p =.774). Thus, despite their TD-like competence on the pragmatic (referential) and the syntactic components of DOS, children with HFA still underperform on DOS, but do slightly better than the children with SLI. These results are compatible with the hypothesis that children with HFA have problems with the integration of information from different levels and for the more specific hypothesis that children with HFA have problems to consistently map the different pragmatic, semantic and syntactic components of DOS: the establishment of referentiality and definiteness by considering speaker and hearer assumptions, the mapping of referentiality and definiteness to information structural notions such as topic and focus, and, in turn, the mapping of topic and focus Table 5 HFA scores on morpho-syntax tests. CELF-IV Mass-count distinction Subject-verb agreement HFA 53.7th percentile (SD: 29.5) 82% (SD: 19%) 95% (SD: 15%) TD 73.4th percentile (SD: 24.9) 92% (SD: 11%) 96% (SD: 18%)

Linguistic and Cognitive Abilities in Children with Specific Language Impairment as Compared to Children with High-Functioning Autism

Linguistic and Cognitive Abilities in Children with Specific Language Impairment as Compared to Children with High-Functioning Autism Language Acquisition ISSN: 1048-9223 (Print) 1532-7817 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hlac20 Linguistic and Cognitive Abilities in Children with Specific Language Impairment

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository)

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Linguistic and other cognitive abilities in children with Specific Language Impairment as compared to children with High-Functioning Autism Schaeffer, J.C. Published

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Parigger, E. M. (2012). Language and executive functioning in children with ADHD Den Bosch: Boxpress

Citation for published version (APA): Parigger, E. M. (2012). Language and executive functioning in children with ADHD Den Bosch: Boxpress UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Language and executive functioning in children with ADHD Parigger, E.M. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Parigger, E. M. (2012). Language

More information

Brooke DePoorter M.Cl.Sc. (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Brooke DePoorter M.Cl.Sc. (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders Critical Review: In school-aged children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), what oral narrative elements differ from their typically developing peers? Brooke DePoorter M.Cl.Sc. (SLP) Candidate University

More information

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) DSM-5 (Criteria and Major Changes for SLP-Related Conditions) Individuals meeting the criteria will be given a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder with three levels of severity based on degree of support

More information

Pragmatic language impairments

Pragmatic language impairments Pragmatic language impairments Dorothy Bishop Wellcome Principal Research Fellow Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford 1 Oral communication involves: COMPREHENSION decoding speech

More information

social communication disorder: identification with the CCC-2? Courtenay Frazier Norbury University College London

social communication disorder: identification with the CCC-2? Courtenay Frazier Norbury University College London social communication disorder: identification with the CCC-2? Courtenay Frazier Norbury University College London my PhD plan identify diagnostic criteria for pragmatic language impairment that would reliably

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs

Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs DSM-IV to DSM-5: Primary Changes Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs Laurie Swineford, PhD CCC-SLP Washington State University DSM-IV Previously we used the

More information

Theory of Mind in ASD, SLI, & Hearing Impairment Groups

Theory of Mind in ASD, SLI, & Hearing Impairment Groups Theory of Mind in ASD, SLI, & Hearing Impairment Groups Megan Gresholdt Allison M. Haskill, Ph.D Augustana College Megan-Gresholdt@augustana.edu AllisonHaskill@augustana.edu What is Theory of Mind (ToM)?

More information

! Introduction:! ! Prosodic abilities!! Prosody and Autism! !! Developmental profile of prosodic abilities for Portuguese speakers!

! Introduction:! ! Prosodic abilities!! Prosody and Autism! !! Developmental profile of prosodic abilities for Portuguese speakers! Marisa Filipe Dezembro de 2013 pdpsi10020@fpce.up.pt EXCL/MHC-LIN/0688/2012 Summary Introduction: Prosodic Abilities in Children and Young Adults with Typical & Non-Typical Development Prosodic abilities

More information

An investigation of oral narratives in children with High Functioning Autism: aspects of microstructure and macrostructure

An investigation of oral narratives in children with High Functioning Autism: aspects of microstructure and macrostructure An investigation of oral narratives in children with High Functioning Autism: aspects of microstructure and macrostructure E L E N I B A L D I M T S I 1, E L E N I P E R I S T E R I 1, & I A N T H I T

More information

Faculty Advisor: Dr. Sandra Gillam. Abstract. 1. Introduction

Faculty Advisor: Dr. Sandra Gillam. Abstract. 1. Introduction Proceedings of The National Conference On Undergraduate Research (NCUR) 2015 Eastern Washington University, Cheney, WA April 16-18, 2015 Improving The Use Of Mental State Verbs By Children With Autism

More information

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children Parkland College A with Honors Projects Honors Program 2015 Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children Hannah Li Parkland College Recommended Citation Li, Hannah, "Social and Pragmatic Language

More information

Autism Update: Classification & Treatment

Autism Update: Classification & Treatment Autism Update: Classification & Treatment Dana Battaglia, Ph.D., CCC-SLP NYSUT Professional Issues Forum on Healthcare April 26 th, 2013 10:30-12:30 1 Who is here today? Our Goals for This Morning Introduce

More information

Children s Communication Checklist - 2: a validation study

Children s Communication Checklist - 2: a validation study Publié dans Revue Tranel (Travaux neuchâtelois de linguistique) 42, 53-63, 2005 qui doit être utilisée pour toute référence à ce travail Children s Communication Checklist - 2: a validation study Courtenay

More information

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA. Lisa Joseph, Ph.D.

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA. Lisa Joseph, Ph.D. AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA Lisa Joseph, Ph.D. Autism Spectrum Disorder Neurodevelopmental disorder Reflects understanding of the etiology of disorder as related to alterations

More information

Comprehension and Story Retelling. Abilities among Children with ASD. Compared to Children with Typical. Development

Comprehension and Story Retelling. Abilities among Children with ASD. Compared to Children with Typical. Development BAR-ILAN UNIVERSITY Comprehension and Story Retelling Abilities among Children with ASD Compared to Children with Typical Development Liat Kadosh Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for

More information

JOHN C. THORNE, PHD, CCC-SLP UNIVERSITY OF WASHINGTON DEPARTMENT OF SPEECH & HEARING SCIENCE FETAL ALCOHOL SYNDROME DIAGNOSTIC AND PREVENTION NETWORK

JOHN C. THORNE, PHD, CCC-SLP UNIVERSITY OF WASHINGTON DEPARTMENT OF SPEECH & HEARING SCIENCE FETAL ALCOHOL SYNDROME DIAGNOSTIC AND PREVENTION NETWORK OPTIMIZING A CLINICAL LANGUAGE MEASURE FOR USE IN IDENTIFYING SIGNIFICANT NEURODEVELOPMENTAL IMPAIRMENT IN DIAGNOSIS OF FETAL ALCOHOL SPECTRUM DISORDERS (FASD) JOHN C. THORNE, PHD, CCC-SLP UNIVERSITY OF

More information

Pragmatics and Disordered Speech. Mackenzie Salt, M.A., H.B.A., D-TEIL

Pragmatics and Disordered Speech. Mackenzie Salt, M.A., H.B.A., D-TEIL Pragmatics and Disordered Speech Mackenzie Salt, M.A., H.B.A., D-TEIL Disordered Speech What is disordered speech? How is it studied? Why is it important to study disordered speech? Disordered Speech and

More information

Leneh Buckle, BSc, MA

Leneh Buckle, BSc, MA Redefining autism: recent developments in diagnostic criteria Leneh Buckle, BSc, MA Introduction to me Grew up in Canada, now in UK Autistic family Studies neuroscience and psychology bioethics (incl.

More information

Language Disorders in Greek 5, May 2014

Language Disorders in Greek 5, May 2014 Mental state terms and the role of working memory in high functioning autistic children s narrative production ELENI BALDIMTSI 1, ELENI PERISTERI 1 & IANTHI-MARIA TSIMPLI 1,2 1 ARISTOTLE UNIVERSITY OF

More information

From: What s the problem? Pathway to Empowerment. Objectives 12/8/2015

From:   What s the problem? Pathway to Empowerment. Objectives 12/8/2015 Overcoming Intellectual Disability and Autism to Achieve Vocational & Academic Success Pathway to Empowerment Objectives 1 2 4 Learn to distinguish between intellectual disability and autism spectrum disorders.

More information

World Languages American Sign Language (ASL) Subject Matter Requirements

World Languages American Sign Language (ASL) Subject Matter Requirements World Languages American Sign Language (ASL) Subject Matter Requirements Part I: Content Domains for Subject Matter Understanding and Skill in World Languages American Sign Language (ASL) Domain 1. General

More information

INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER

INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER What is the DSM-5? The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (the DSM) is developed by the

More information

Critical Review: Late Talkers : What Can We Expect?

Critical Review: Late Talkers : What Can We Expect? Critical Review: Late Talkers : What Can We Expect? Ian Gallant M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate Western University: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders This critical review examines two specific questions

More information

What is Autism? -Those with the most severe disability need a lot of help with their daily lives whereas those that are least affected may not.

What is Autism? -Those with the most severe disability need a lot of help with their daily lives whereas those that are least affected may not. Autism Summary Autism What is Autism? The Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a developmental disability that can have significant implications on a child's ability to function and interface with the world

More information

what criteria for communication disorders? Courtenay Frazier Norbury Royal Holloway University of London

what criteria for communication disorders? Courtenay Frazier Norbury Royal Holloway University of London what criteria for communication disorders? Courtenay Frazier Norbury Royal Holloway University of London communication disorder language impairment below expectations on one or more aspects of language

More information

California Subject Examinations for Teachers

California Subject Examinations for Teachers California Subject Examinations for Teachers TEST GUIDE AMERICAN SIGN LANGUAGE SUBTEST III Subtest Description This document contains the World Languages: American Sign Language (ASL) subject matter requirements

More information

4 Detailed Information Processing in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome: The Usefulness of Neuropsychological Tests and Self-reports

4 Detailed Information Processing in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome: The Usefulness of Neuropsychological Tests and Self-reports 4 Detailed Information Processing in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome: The Usefulness of Neuropsychological Tests and Self-reports Submitted Chapter 4 Abstract Detailed information processing in 42

More information

Research Article The Michigan Autism Spectrum Questionnaire: A Rating Scale for High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders

Research Article The Michigan Autism Spectrum Questionnaire: A Rating Scale for High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders Hindawi Publishing Corporation Autism Research and Treatment Volume 2013, Article ID 708273, 5 pages http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2013/708273 Research Article The Michigan Autism Spectrum Questionnaire: A

More information

COMMUNICATION ASSESSMENT AND INTERVENTION FOR BILINGUAL LATINOS WITH ASPERGER SYNDROME (AS) Nicolás Linares-Orama, Ph. D. FILIUS Research Institute

COMMUNICATION ASSESSMENT AND INTERVENTION FOR BILINGUAL LATINOS WITH ASPERGER SYNDROME (AS) Nicolás Linares-Orama, Ph. D. FILIUS Research Institute COMMUNICATION ASSESSMENT AND INTERVENTION FOR BILINGUAL LATINOS WITH ASPERGER SYNDROME (AS) Nicolás Linares-Orama, Ph. D. FILIUS Research Institute University of Puerto Rico PURPOSE IN THIS PRESENTATION

More information

5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome

5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome 5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome Published in: Neuropsychologia, 2008, 47 (3), 652-656. Chapter 5 Abstract The semantic and phonemic fluency performance of adults with high functioning

More information

Introduction Stories or narratives are a common discourse genre embedded in everyday conversation (Norrick, 2010). It has been proposed that a key

Introduction Stories or narratives are a common discourse genre embedded in everyday conversation (Norrick, 2010). It has been proposed that a key Introduction Stories or narratives are a common discourse genre embedded in everyday conversation (Norrick, 2010). It has been proposed that a key function of narrative perhaps the very driving force behind

More information

These slides may also be found within the Comprehensive Overview Training PowerPoint, which provides guidance on every eligibility category.

These slides may also be found within the Comprehensive Overview Training PowerPoint, which provides guidance on every eligibility category. The following slides provide guidance on the eligibility category, definition, and criteria for child with a Autism Spectrum Disorder in Colorado public schools. These slides may also be found within the

More information

Asperger Syndrome: Facilitating Social Thinking Across The School Day. Michelle Garcia Winner MA CCC SLP

Asperger Syndrome: Facilitating Social Thinking Across The School Day. Michelle Garcia Winner MA CCC SLP Asperger Syndrome: Facilitating Social Thinking Across The School Day Michelle Garcia Winner MA CCC SLP Michelle@socialthinking.co m 1 Many labels of social learning challenges High Functioning Autism

More information

Narrative as a clinical assessment

Narrative as a clinical assessment Narrative as a clinical assessment NICOLA BOTTING LANGUAGE AND COMMUNICATION SCIENCE CITY UNIVERSITY LONDON Overview Background about narrative Different types of narrative Narrative in relation to diagnosis

More information

Kent Academic Repository

Kent Academic Repository Kent Academic Repository Full text document (pdf) Citation for published version Williams, David M. and Botting, Nicola and Boucher, Jill (2008) Language in autism and specific language impairment: Where

More information

LING 419: Linguistics and Child Language Disorders. Introduction 4SEP08

LING 419: Linguistics and Child Language Disorders. Introduction 4SEP08 LING 419: Linguistics and Child Language Disorders Introduction 4SEP08 Topics 1. About this course 2. What is clinical linguistics? 3. What are neurodevelopmental disorders? 4. Group comparison design

More information

",! " ", " " " " "!!! " $.

,!  ,     !!!  $. 27 35 5,6, Theory of Mind,,!, #!! Theory of Mind, ToM Theory of Mind (ToM)! ToM!!! $ Baron-Cohen, Leslie, & Frith, 985; Astington &)! (Jenkins, 995; Wellman, Cross, & Watson, 2,, $ #,# (joint attention)

More information

Bringing Your A Game: Strategies to Support Students with Autism Communication Strategies. Ann N. Garfinkle, PhD Benjamin Chu, Doctoral Candidate

Bringing Your A Game: Strategies to Support Students with Autism Communication Strategies. Ann N. Garfinkle, PhD Benjamin Chu, Doctoral Candidate Bringing Your A Game: Strategies to Support Students with Autism Communication Strategies Ann N. Garfinkle, PhD Benjamin Chu, Doctoral Candidate Outcomes for this Session Have a basic understanding of

More information

Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi

Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi We ll cover Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) ASD in young adults Social Communication (definition, components, importance,

More information

How to Recognize and Reduce Challenges to Children s Comprehension of Books

How to Recognize and Reduce Challenges to Children s Comprehension of Books How to Recognize and Reduce Challenges to Children s Comprehension of Books Rebecca Landa, Ph.D., CCC-SLP Director, Center for Autism and Related Disorders April 3, 2013 Main points Have a look at children

More information

The role of nonverbal working memory in morphosyntactic processing by children with specific language impairment and autism spectrum disorders

The role of nonverbal working memory in morphosyntactic processing by children with specific language impairment and autism spectrum disorders Ellis Weismer et al. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders (2017) 9:28 DOI 10.1186/s11689-017-9209-6 RESEARCH Open Access The role of nonverbal working memory in morphosyntactic processing by children

More information

Lecture 6: The Whorfian Hypothesis (contd.); autism spectrum disorders and language

Lecture 6: The Whorfian Hypothesis (contd.); autism spectrum disorders and language Lecture 6: The Whorfian Hypothesis (contd.); autism spectrum disorders and language Learning goals: o Understand what is meant by the Whorfian Hypothesis, and be able to illustrate it with (controversial)

More information

Municipal Employee Guide to Autism Awareness

Municipal Employee Guide to Autism Awareness RISK Winter 2014 Municipal Employee Guide to Autism Awareness A D M M R M A V I S O A D M R Y C I N O I S T R A T M M I T T I V E E Michigan Municipal Risk Management Authority Administrative Advisory

More information

Comparison of Grammar in Neurodevelopmental Disorders: The Case of Binding in Williams Syndrome and Autism With and Without Language Impairment

Comparison of Grammar in Neurodevelopmental Disorders: The Case of Binding in Williams Syndrome and Autism With and Without Language Impairment Language Acquisition, 20: 133 154, 2013 Published with License by Taylor & Francis ISSN: 1048-9223 print / 1532-7817 online DOI: 10.1080/10489223.2013.766742 Comparison of Grammar in Neurodevelopmental

More information

Alternative Augmentative Communication. American Sign Language

Alternative Augmentative Communication. American Sign Language Alternative Augmentative Communication American Sign Language Autism Spectrum Disorder A variable neuro-developmental disorder with impairments in communication, social interaction, and restrictive and

More information

1/30/2018. Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Disclosures. Learning Objectives

1/30/2018. Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Disclosures. Learning Objectives Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders Celine A. Saulnier, PhD Associate Professor Emory University School of Medicine Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Third Edition 1 Disclosures As

More information

Is Asperger Syndrome The Same As Autism?

Is Asperger Syndrome The Same As Autism? Asperger Syndrome BY: Rosalynd Lord Asperger Syndrome is a developmental disorder falling within the autistic spectrum affecting two- way social interaction, verbal and nonverbal communication and a reluctance

More information

8/23/2017. Chapter 21 Autism Spectrum Disorders. Introduction. Diagnostic Categories within the Autism Spectrum

8/23/2017. Chapter 21 Autism Spectrum Disorders. Introduction. Diagnostic Categories within the Autism Spectrum Chapter 21 Overview Core features of autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) Studies seeking an etiology for ASDs Conditions associated with ASDs Interventions and outcomes Introduction ASDs Class of neurodevelopmental

More information

Language is the behavior of the brain; communication is the behavior of the body and brain. Financial Disclosure

Language is the behavior of the brain; communication is the behavior of the body and brain. Financial Disclosure Financial Disclosure Asperger Syndrome & HFA Informal Dynamic Social Thinking Assessment Michelle Garcia Winner www.socialthinking.com I am affiliated with Think Social Publishing. I publish multiple books

More information

Differential Autism Diagnosis The Role of an SLP in Evaluating Social Communication Differences

Differential Autism Diagnosis The Role of an SLP in Evaluating Social Communication Differences Differential Autism Diagnosis The Role of an SLP in Evaluating Social Communication Differences DATE: October 13, 2018 PRESENTED BY: Jill Dolata, PhD, CCC-SLP & Cynthia Green, MS, CCC-SLP Goals Describe

More information

Concerns and questions about eligibility and social (pragmatic) communication disorder

Concerns and questions about eligibility and social (pragmatic) communication disorder Concerns and questions about eligibility and social (pragmatic) communication disorder Nickola Wolf Nelson, Ph.D., CCC-SLP Western Michigan University Presentation at ASHA Convention Orlando Florida Nov

More information

Critical Review: What Presenting Speech and Language Characteristics of Late Talkers Distinguish Those Who Recover from Those Who Do Not?

Critical Review: What Presenting Speech and Language Characteristics of Late Talkers Distinguish Those Who Recover from Those Who Do Not? Critical Review: What Presenting Speech and Language Characteristics of Late Talkers Distinguish Those Who Recover from Those Who Do Not? Melissa Dumoulin M.Cl.Sc. Speech-Language Pathology Candidate University

More information

Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders Celine A. Saulnier, PhD Associate Professor Emory University School of Medicine Director of Research Operations Marcus Autism Center Vineland Adaptive

More information

Autism & intellectual disabilities. How to deal with confusing concepts

Autism & intellectual disabilities. How to deal with confusing concepts Autism & intellectual disabilities How to deal with confusing concepts dr. Gerard J. Nijhof Orthopedagogue / GZ-psychologist PhD, Free University Amsterdam Private practice contact@gerardnijhof.nl +31

More information

Autism Diagnosis and Management Update. Outline. History 11/1/2013. Autism Diagnosis. Management

Autism Diagnosis and Management Update. Outline. History 11/1/2013. Autism Diagnosis. Management Autism Diagnosis and Management Update Cathleen Small, PhD, BCBA-D Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics Maine Medical Partners Outline Autism Diagnosis Brief history New, DSM-5 diagnostic criteria Expressed

More information

Pragma-c Deficit. Pragma-c deficit characteris-cs. Pragma-cs deficit causes 01/08/2016

Pragma-c Deficit. Pragma-c deficit characteris-cs. Pragma-cs deficit causes 01/08/2016 Pragma-c Deficit Pragma-c deficit characteris-cs Idiosyncra-c style Persistence Lack of kindness Lack of empathy Lack of social knowledge Distrust Aggressiveness Torque in teasing Shyness Dependence Pragma-cs

More information

Language problems. Language Impairment. A note of skepticism. Hearing impairment 5/15/11

Language problems. Language Impairment. A note of skepticism. Hearing impairment 5/15/11 Language problems Language Impairment Several conditions are related to difficulties in spoken language acquisition: Hearing impairment Intellectual impairments Autism/PDD Specific language impairment

More information

NEW YORK STATE TEACHER CERTIFICATION EXAMINATIONS

NEW YORK STATE TEACHER CERTIFICATION EXAMINATIONS NEW YORK STATE TEACHER CERTIFICATION EXAMINATIONS TEST DESIGN AND FRAMEWORK April 2016 Authorized for Distribution by the New York State Education Department This test design and framework document is

More information

Evaluating Language and Communication Skills

Evaluating Language and Communication Skills Evaluating Language and Communication Skills Statewide Conference on Education of the Deaf San Marcos, Texas July, 2016 Kelley Watt, M. Ed Region 4 Education Service Center Houston, Texas kelley.watt@esc4.net

More information

Syntax and its interfaces at the low and high ends of the autistic spectrum

Syntax and its interfaces at the low and high ends of the autistic spectrum Syntax and its interfaces at the low and high ends of the autistic spectrum Book or Report Section Accepted Version Terzi, A., Marinis, T. and Francis, K. (2016) Syntax and its interfaces at the low and

More information

Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams

Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams Marci Laurel, MA, CCC-SLP mlaurel@salud.unm.edu and Services (UCEDD) 1 Training Objectives Participants will: 1. Name at least three communication

More information

Autism in the Wild. Examination

Autism in the Wild. Examination Autism in the Wild Examination Instructor: Results: Name: Class: Date: Circle the correct answer: 1. Autism is a complex physiological disorder that most markedly involves some degree of difficulty with:

More information

Coordinated Family Services Plan

Coordinated Family Services Plan Page 1 of 8 Dear family, Creating a (CFSP) is a family-centred process. The goal of the CFSP is to: 1. Document shared goals for your child/family in a written/visual format; 2. Support communication between

More information

Questions for Eric London: Alternative diagnoses for autism

Questions for Eric London: Alternative diagnoses for autism OPINION, Q&A Questions for Eric London: Alternative diagnoses for autism BY JESSICA WRIGHT 2 JANUARY 2015 1 / 7 Deep dive: Listing detailed symptoms rather than assigning a single diagnostic label may

More information

An objective of present research across various

An objective of present research across various JSLHR Article Finiteness Marking in Boys With Fragile X Syndrome Audra M. Sterling, a Mabel L. Rice, a and Steven F. Warren a Purpose: The current study investigated finiteness marking (e.g., he walks,

More information

Arts and Entertainment. Ecology. Technology. History and Deaf Culture

Arts and Entertainment. Ecology. Technology. History and Deaf Culture American Sign Language Level 3 (novice-high to intermediate-low) Course Description ASL Level 3 furthers the study of grammar, vocabulary, idioms, multiple meaning words, finger spelling, and classifiers

More information

Autism is best characterized as a spectrum of developmental

Autism is best characterized as a spectrum of developmental Research Article Measuring Pragmatic Language in Speakers With Autism Spectrum Disorders: Comparing the Children s Communication Checklist 2 and the Test of Pragmatic Language Joanne Volden Linda Phillips

More information

Autistic Disorder. Asperger s Disorder: Problems: Higher Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders: Revealing Communication Deficits. Therefore, we must:

Autistic Disorder. Asperger s Disorder: Problems: Higher Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders: Revealing Communication Deficits. Therefore, we must: Higher Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders: Revealing Communication Deficits Glenis Benson, Ph.D. American Speech and Hearing Association Chicago, IL November 20, 2008 Problems: Assumption that language

More information

Pragmatic abilities of high-functioning Greek-speaking children with autism

Pragmatic abilities of high-functioning Greek-speaking children with autism ΨΥΧΟΛΟΓΙΑ, 2013, 20 (3) 321-337 PSYCHOLOGY, 2013, 20 (3) 321-337 Pragmatic abilities of high-functioning Greek-speaking children with autism THEODOROS MARINIS 1, ARHONTO TERZI 2 ANGELIKI KOTSOPOULOU 3,

More information

Comprehension of reflexive and personal pronouns in children with autism: A syntactic or pragmatic deficit?

Comprehension of reflexive and personal pronouns in children with autism: A syntactic or pragmatic deficit? Applied Psycholinguistics 34 (2013), 813 835 doi:10.1017/s0142716412000033 Comprehension of reflexive and personal pronouns in children with autism: A syntactic or pragmatic deficit? ALEXANDRA PEROVIC

More information

Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King

Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King Assessment and diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder For this month s topic, I am excited to share my recent experience using the fifth edition of the Diagnostic

More information

AUTISM: THEORY OF MIND. Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD

AUTISM: THEORY OF MIND. Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD AUTISM: THEORY OF MIND Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD Autism is Defined by behavioral criteria Some biological markers Clinical presentation is varied: education, temperament,

More information

Education Options for Children with Autism

Education Options for Children with Autism Empowering children with Autism and their families through knowledge and support Education Options for Children with Autism Starting school is a major milestone in a child s life, and a big step for all

More information

.,, ...,,, Schmitt, 1968; )

.,, ...,,, Schmitt, 1968; ) ? 9 ; 7;7 5 & ( ) &? * Schmitt 968; ) ( ) Wh Power & Quigley 97; Quigley Wilbur & Montanelli 97; Quigley Smith & Wilbur 97; (Geers & Moog 978; Berent 988; de Villiers 988; de Villiers de Villiers & Hoban

More information

Strategies for Building ASL Literacy

Strategies for Building ASL Literacy Strategies for Building ASL Literacy Charlotte Enns CAEDHH-BC Annual Conference October 21, 2016 Outline Challenges to Understanding and Teaching ASL Grammar Overview of ASL Grammar and Narrative Structures

More information

Autism and Communication

Autism and Communication Autism and Communication Summary Autism affects communication and social interaction to varying degrees. Some people with autism do not use speech to communicate, whereas others may be very articulate

More information

English and Persian Apposition Markers in Written Discourse: A Case of Iranian EFL learners

English and Persian Apposition Markers in Written Discourse: A Case of Iranian EFL learners 7 English and Persian Apposition Markers in Written Discourse: A Case of Iranian EFL learners Samaneh Chamanaraeian M.A. Student in Islamic Azad University (Isfahan Branch) samanechaman@yahoo.com and (corresponding

More information

Language Comprehension Profiles of Young Adolescents With Fragile X Syndrome

Language Comprehension Profiles of Young Adolescents With Fragile X Syndrome AJSLP Article Language Comprehension Profiles of Young Adolescents With Fragile X Syndrome Ashley Oakes, a,b Sara T. Kover, c and Leonard Abbeduto a,b Purpose: In this study, the authors sought to characterize

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

CLASSROOM & PLAYGROUND

CLASSROOM & PLAYGROUND The information contained in this booklet was most generously provided to Autism South Africa by the UK National Autistic Society The production and printing of this and 12 other brochures was made possible

More information

Allen Independent School District Bundled LOTE Curriculum Beginning 2017 School Year ASL III

Allen Independent School District Bundled LOTE Curriculum Beginning 2017 School Year ASL III Allen Independent School District Bundled LOTE Curriculum Beginning 2017 School Year ASL III Page 1 of 19 Revised: 8/1/2017 114.36. American Sign Language, Level III (One Credit), Adopted 2014. (a) General

More information

DATA Model Skills Checklist: Curriculum Crosswalk

DATA Model Skills Checklist: Curriculum Crosswalk APPENDIX B DATA Model Skills Checklist: Curriculum Crosswalk This document is a crosswalk of the DATA Model Skills Checklist with the Assessment, Evaluation, Programming System (AEPS). The crosswalk with

More information

University of Groningen. Communication abilities of children with ASD and ADHD Kuijper, Sanne

University of Groningen. Communication abilities of children with ASD and ADHD Kuijper, Sanne University of Groningen Communication abilities of children with ASD and ADHD Kuijper, Sanne IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from

More information

Story Time Language Program Summary Child Language Intervention Program Vanderbilt University

Story Time Language Program Summary Child Language Intervention Program Vanderbilt University Story Time Language Program Summary 2012-2013 Principal Investigator: Stephen Camarata, Ph.D. Child Language Intervention Program Vanderbilt University Researcher Biography Stephen Camarata, PhD Dept of

More information

Holt McDougal Avancemos!, Level correlated to the. Crosswalk Alignment of the National Standards for Learning Languages

Holt McDougal Avancemos!, Level correlated to the. Crosswalk Alignment of the National Standards for Learning Languages Holt McDougal Avancemos!, Level 2 2013 correlated to the Crosswalk Alignment of the National Standards for Learning Languages with the Common Core State Standards READING 1. Read closely to determine what

More information

Object Lessons. The Questions

Object Lessons. The Questions Object Lessons Major findings from: Functional Problem Solving Skills for Children with Pervasive Development Disorders (U.S. Department of Education grant # 84.024B60120) Charity Rowland, Ph.D., Principal

More information

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION. This chapter encompasses the background of the study, research questions, scope of

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION. This chapter encompasses the background of the study, research questions, scope of CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION This chapter encompasses the background of the study, research questions, scope of the study, aims of the study, research method and the organization. It plays an important role

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorders

Autism Spectrum Disorders Autism Spectrum Disorders The gap between research and interventions Sara Schroer & Michael Goldstein The plan 1. What is autism? 2. How does it emerge across development? 3. What are the core impairments?

More information

Q: What can you tell us about the work you do and your involvement with children with autism?

Q: What can you tell us about the work you do and your involvement with children with autism? If you know one person with autism, you know one person with autism April is Autism Awareness & Acceptance month and in an attempt to further educate the public about autism, Catriona Monthy, a registered

More information

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation.

Cover Page. The handle   holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation. Cover Page The handle http://hdl.handle.net/1887/19149 holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation. Author: Maljaars, Janne Pieternella Wilhelmina Title: Communication problems in children

More information

Overview. Clinical Features

Overview. Clinical Features Jessica Greenson, Ph.D. Autism Center University of Washington Clinical Features Overview Diagnostic & Statistical Manual IV (DSM IV) Prevalence Course of Onset Etiology Early Recognition Early Recognition

More information

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Alex Rice M.Cl.Sc SLP Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication

More information

Improving Communication in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi Speech & Language Therapist

Improving Communication in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi Speech & Language Therapist Improving Communication in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi Speech & Language Therapist Communication in ASD We will cover Typical language development Language presentation in ASD Language

More information

CHAPTER FIVE METHODOLOGY

CHAPTER FIVE METHODOLOGY CHAPTER FIVE METHODOLOGY To hone in on characteristics that are universal among and specific to individuals with a particular syndrome or disability, researchers often compare the performance of a target

More information

Cognitive styles sex the brain, compete neurally, and quantify deficits in autism

Cognitive styles sex the brain, compete neurally, and quantify deficits in autism Cognitive styles sex the brain, compete neurally, and quantify deficits in autism Nigel Goldenfeld 1, Sally Wheelwright 2 and Simon Baron-Cohen 2 1 Department of Applied Mathematics and Theoretical Physics,

More information

Working memory in. development: Links with learning between. typical and atypical populations. TRACY PACKIAM ALLOWAY Durham University, UK

Working memory in. development: Links with learning between. typical and atypical populations. TRACY PACKIAM ALLOWAY Durham University, UK Working memory in 00 000 00 0 000 000 0 development: Links with learning between typical and atypical populations 4 2 5 TRACY PACKIAM ALLOWAY Durham University, UK Outline 00 000 00 0 000 000 0 Working

More information

The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children. Overview

The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children. Overview The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children Jessica Greenson, Ph.D. Autism Center University of Washington Overview Diagnostic Criteria Current: Diagnostic & Statistical

More information

Definitions of Nature of Science and Scientific Inquiry that Guide Project ICAN: A Cheat Sheet

Definitions of Nature of Science and Scientific Inquiry that Guide Project ICAN: A Cheat Sheet Definitions of Nature of Science and Scientific Inquiry that Guide Project ICAN: A Cheat Sheet What is the NOS? The phrase nature of science typically refers to the values and assumptions inherent to scientific

More information