The functional locus of the lateralized readiness potential

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "The functional locus of the lateralized readiness potential"

Transcription

1 Psychophysiology, 41 (2004), Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2004 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: /j x The functional locus of the lateralized readiness potential HIROAKI MASAKI, a NELE WILD-WALL, b JÖRG SANGALS, b and WERNER SOMMER b a Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Tokyo, Japan b Humboldt-University, Institute for Psychology, Berlin, Germany Abstract The lateralized readiness potential (LRP) is considered to reflect motor activation and has been used extensively as a tool in elucidating cognitive processes. In the present study, we attempted to more precisely determine the origins of the LRP within the cognitive system. The response selection and motor programming stages were selectively manipulated by varying symbolic stimulus response compatibility and the time to peak force of an isometric finger extension response. Stimulus response compatibility and time to peak force affected response latency, as measured in the electromyogram, in a strictly additive fashion. The effects of the experimental manipulations on stimulus- and response-synchronized LRPs indicate that the LRP starts after the completion of response-hand selection and at the beginning of motor programming. These results allow a more rigorous interpretation of LRP findings in basic and applied research. Descriptors: Lateralized readiness potential, Response selection, Motor programming, Functional locus The lateralized readiness potential (LRP) is regarded as a useful and powerful tool in the study of human information processing. It is extracted from event-related brain potentials (ERP) and considered to reflect the activation of response-related processes following stimulus-related processing (Coles, 1989; De Jong, Wierda, Mulder, & Mulder, 1988; Gratton, Coles, Sirevaag, Eriksen, & Donchin, 1988). In previous research, the LRP has been successfully employed to investigate, for example, information transmission between perception- and response-related processes (e.g., Coles, Gratton, & Donchin, 1988; Miller & Hackley, 1992), motor programming (e.g., Leuthold, Sommer, & Ulrich, 1996), overlapping task processing (e.g., Osman & Moore, 1993), or dual route processes in spatial stimulus response compatibility (e.g., Stu rmer, Leuthold, Soetens, Schröter, & Sommer, 2002). Although the somewhat global notion of LRP onset as marker of response activation is quite sufficient for many issues in cognitive psychology, there are other applications where a higher degree of precision as regards the functional significance of this component would be most welcome. For example, Sommer, Leuthold, and Schubert s (2001) assessment Portions of this article were presented at the 42nd Annual Meeting of the Society for Psychophysiological Research, Washington, DC, October 2 6, This study was supported by a fellowship of the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science to the first author. The authors would like to thank Allen Osman for valuable discussions, Kathrin Pusch for her assistance in collecting the data, and William Gehring, Toby Mordkoff, Birgit Stürmer, and one anonymous reviewer for their helpful comments on an earlier draft of this article. Address reprint requests to: Dr. Hiroaki Masaki, School of Sport Sciences, Waseda University, , Mikajima, Tokorozawa, Saitama, Japan, masaki@waseda.jp. of a motoric bottleneck after response initiation (De Jong, 1993) depended crucially on the locus of the LRP after or during response selection but prior to response initiation. The present study attempts to provide further information on how the onset of the LRP is related to the cognitive and motoric processes that have been suggested during choice reaction time tasks. The LRP is derived by recording ERPs from above the motor cortices in tasks that call for left- and right-hand or -foot responses. The ERP above the cortex ipsilateral to the effector required in a given trial is subtracted from the contralateral ERP. When these difference waves are averaged across hands, they yield the LRP, reflecting pure hand-related ERP asymmetry. In general terms, the LRP is considered as a measure of response activation or preparation (cf. Coles, 1989). The interval between a stimulus and the onset of the stimulus-synchronized LRP (stimulus-lrp interval) is a relative measure for the duration of premotoric processes, including perception and at least some aspects of response selection. In contrast, the interval between the onset of the response-synchronized LRP and the response (LRP-response interval) is a relative measure for the duration of the subsequent, that is, the motoric processes (Osman, Moore, & Ulrich, 1995). The LRP is considered to be generated at least partially in the primary motor cortex (e.g., Coles, 1989; Miller & Hackley, 1992). Note however, that even if LRP were exclusively generated in M1, it would, according to current neuroanatomical knowledge, not unambiguously specify its meaning, because the precise functions of M1 are themselves a matter of debate (e.g., Graziano, Taylor, & Moore, 2002). To determine the functional significance of ERP components, it has been suggested to assess the effects of experimental factors with known locus of action within the information processing system on the ERP component in question (e.g., Meyer, Osman, 220

2 Functional locus of the LRP 221 Irwin, & Yantis, 1988). If, for example, manipulation of an experimental factor increases the interval between the stimulus and the onset of the component in question but not the interval between the onset of the component and the response, it can be concluded that the component is elicited in a stage or in stages following the locus of action of the experimental factor. For choice reaction time tasks, Sanders (1980, 1990, 1998) proposed three perceptual stages (preprocessing, feature extraction, and identification) and at least three response-related stages (response selection, motor programming, and motor adjustment). Of primary interest in the present context are responserelated stages. The first one is the so-called response selection or decision stage. In this stage, the stimulus code extracted during the perceptual analysis is assigned to an abstract response code. The manipulation most unambiguously affecting the duration of stimulus response assignment is the compatibility between stimuli and responses (S-R compatibility). Most clearly this holds true for so-called symbolic S-R compatibility, for example when letters or words, meaning left or right have to be assigned to responses performed at the side designated by the stimulus (compatible assignment) or at the opposite side (incompatible assignment). Less well suited for present purposes are variants of spatial S-R compatibility tasks, including the Simon task (Simon & Rudell, 1967) because here, a second, unconditional or direct, processing route may come into play (Kornblum, Hasbroucq, & Osman, 1990), undermining the research logic suggested by Meyer et al. (1988). Following the response selection stage, a motor programming stage has been suggested. Here, a more detailed specification of the kinetic parameters of the response takes place that was established during response selection (Zelaznik & Franz, 1990). Proposed experimental variables related to the motor programming stage are, for example, crossed hand placement, movement direction, and movement velocity (Sanders, 1998). The third motor-related process is the so-called motor adjustment stage that deals with the transition from central to peripheral motor activity. It is considered to be affected by foreperiod duration and instructed muscle tension. In screening the available reports about experimental effects on the onset of the LRP for the sake of interpreting its functional significance, only a limited set of designs is useful. In general, almost all experiments have to be ruled out that have used the LRP as a tool to identify the effects of experimental factors on information processing. In these studies, at least a rough idea about the functional significance of the LRP is a prerequisite and gaining knowledge about the locus of the experimental effects is the aim. In contrast, the present study is concerned with specifying more closely the significance of the LRP by using experimental effects with a well-known locus of action. In addition, we also have to exclude all designs that might induce parallel or dual route processing because they do not allow an unambiguous interpretation of experimental effects on LRP onset. In particular, we cannot take into consideration studies that used multiple stimuli or stimuli with several attributes. This concerns, for example, most studies investigating information transmission (e.g., Gratton et al., 1988; Miller & Hackley, 1992; Osman, Bashore, Coles, Donchin, & Meyer 1992; Smid, Mulder, & Mulder, 1987), spatial S-R compatibility (Wascher, Reinhard, Wauschkuhn, & Verleger, 1999), and the Simon effect (e.g., Stürmer et al., 2002). The studies that can be taken into consideration because they manipulated experimental factors with a commonly accepted locus of action in unambiguous single-route processing are astonishingly few in number. In a two-choice task using the words left or right, Smulders, Kok, Kenemans, and Bashore (1995) factorially manipulated stimulus quality (clear vs. degraded) and response complexity (single keystroke vs. three keystrokes with index, ring, and middle fingers); stimulus quality was taken to affect the feature extraction stage and response complexity was considered to relate to motor programming. As expected, the two experimental factors had additive effects on reaction time (RT). Stimulus degradation increased the stimulus-lrp interval but not the LRP-response interval; conversely, response complexity increased only the LRP-response interval, but not the stimulus- LRP interval. The authors concluded that the LRP succeeds the start of response choice but precedes the start of response programming. On the other hand, the authors noted that the LRP might also emerge within rather than after the stage of response choice. Another problem with this study is that the association of the factor response complexity with the response or motor programming stage is not generally accepted. As several studies suggested (e.g., Verwey, 1994), a sequence of key presses may cause the insertion of additional motoric processing stages into the system rather than merely increasing the time demands for the motor programming stage. This assertion also makes the findings about larger LRP amplitudes for complex as compared to simple responses by Hackley and Miller (1995) and Stief, Leuthold, Miller, Sommer, and Ulrich (1998) difficult to interpret for present purposes. Smulders (1993, chapter 7) manipulated stimulus quality in the same way as Smulders et al. (1995) in combination with symbolic S-R compatibility. Both factors affected the stimulus- LRP interval; the results about the LRP-response interval were not quite as clear because this interval was increased by degraded incompatible stimuli. Miller and Ulrich (1998) reported a manipulation of symbolic S-R compatibility. Participants had to make six-choice responses with index, middle, and ring fingers of the right and left hands. Whereas the choice of hand was always symbolically compatible and easy, the assignment of the stimuli to the fingers within each hand was either easy (compatible) or difficult (incompatible); this within-hand compatibility manipulation affected the LRPresponse interval but not the stimulus-lrp interval, indicating a locus for the LRP onset after the termination of hand selection but prior to finger selection. A similar observation was made by Hackley and Valle-Inclán (1999), who manipulated the number of fingers to be selected within a hand; the number of alternatives affected both the stimulus-lrp and LRP-response intervals. Whereas the former result is compatible with the notion that number of alternatives has effects on both perception and response selection (Sanders, 1998), the latter finding conforms with the one by Miller and Ulrich (1998) that finger selection within a hand follows LRP onset. An additional useful observation is the increase of LRP amplitude and the reduction of the LRP-response interval with the number of prespecified movement parameters in movement precuing tasks (Leuthold et al., 1996; Sangals, Sommer, & Leuthold, 2002; Ulrich, Leuthold, & Sommer, 1998). These findings argue for effects of motor programming processes on the LRP but do not specify the initiation point of the LRP. Together, because of the effects of stimulus degradation and S-R compatibility on the stimulus-lrp interval, it appears rather safe to conclude that the LRP is generated postperceptually and also after at least a considerable amount of response selection. In addition, based on the precuing effects on LRP

3 222 H. Masaki et al. amplitude, it seems that the LRP emerges prior to or during motor programming. In other words, the earliest plausible locus of the LRP onset is during response selection and the latest locus is during motor programming; LRP might also emerge after completion of response selection and at the beginning of motor programming. Because of the residual ambiguities in the results of previous studies, it was the aim of the present experiment to provide more specific information about the relationship of LRP onset to the response selection and the motor programming stages. To determine the locus of the LRP onset, the present study selectively manipulated in a factorial design the time demands for the response hand selection and the motor programming stages. Additivity of the reaction time effects of these manipulations would demonstrate their selectivity. To manipulate response hand selection, we chose symbolic S-R compatibility, as used by Smulders (1993). The choice of the factor acting on motor programming was more difficult. Because of the ambiguities in the interpretation of response complexity mentioned above, we adopted instructed movement velocity in order to manipulate motor programming. Evidence that this variable selectively affects motor programming comes, for example, from studies of the RTs for sliding arm movements by Spijkers and coworkers (Spijkers, 1987, 1989; Spijkers & Sanders, 1984; Spijkers & Steyvers, 1984). Instructed speed of the sliding movement reliably affected RT (shorter RT for faster movements); these effects were additive to those of S-R compatibility and foreperiod duration, segregating the effects of speed manipulation from those presumably affecting response selection and motor adjustment, respectively. In the present study, the S-R compatibility manipulation was realized by presenting the letters R and L, requiring isometric responses (force pulses) with the left and right middle fingers that were either compatible (R! right; L! left) or incompatible (R! left; L! right). Instructed speed was manipulated by requiring in a separate condition that the time to peak force should be within an early, narrow time window or within a later, wider time window; peak force was to be kept within specified and constant limits. Although movement velocity is operationalized here differently than done by Spijkers and coworkers, we take it to be a valid manipulation of the motor programming stage. According to the parallel force unit model proposed by Ulrich and Wing (1991), brief force pulses at the lower limit of time to peak force (i.e., about 100 ms) have only one degree of freedom, that is, only the number of force units has to be defined by the motor program. On the other hand, slower force pulses require the programming of both the number of force units and their timing to obtain the correct force curve. Therefore, the short time-to-peak-force conditions should require less motor programming than long time-to-peak-force conditions. Given that S-R compatibility and movement velocity have independent effects on reaction time, we can make diagnostic predictions for the LRP depending on the functional locus of the origin of this component. The predictions primarily concern the effects of the S-R compatibility and the time-to-peak-force manipulation on the stimulus-lrp interval and the LRPresponse interval. As far as the S-R compatibility effects are concerned, these onset-related predictions can be supplemented by predictions about the slopes of the LRP. However, because it cannot be excluded that time to peak force may affect the amplitude of the LRP (see below), slope-related predictions were prudently confined to effects of S-R compatibility. In principle, five possible loci can be distinguished with the present design. Two of these appear to be highly unlikely on the basis of previous findings. Thus, we do not expect the LRP to start at the beginning of response hand selection, in which case there would be effects of both S-R compatibility and instructed velocity on the LRP-response interval but none on the stimulus- LRP interval. Likewise, we do not expect the LRP to start only after motor programming, which would be indicated by exclusive effects of both variables on the stimulus-lrp interval but none on the LRP-response interval. This leaves us with three realistic alternatives. First, if the LRP starts immediately after the response selection stage and at the beginning of motor programming, the S-R compatibility manipulation should only affect the stimulus-lrp interval with longer onset latencies in the incompatible conditions; also, the slopes of the stimulus- and response-synchronized LRPs should be unaffected by S-R compatibility because the activation processes should be postponed, but their time course should remain the same. Conversely, the time-to-peak-force manipulation should only affect the LRPresponse interval, showing longer intervals in the slow time-topeak-force conditions. Second, if the LRP starts somewhere during response selection, one should expect any manipulation that increases the total time demand in this stage to also increase the time preceding the elicitation of this component; it should, however, also increase the interval between the onset of the component and the execution of the response. Therefore we should expect both the stimulus-lrp and the LRP-response intervals to be longer in the incompatible than in the compatible conditions. The time-topeak-force manipulation, however, should only affect the LRPresponse interval but not the stimulus-lrp interval. Just as one should predict an effect of S-R compatibility on the LRPresponse interval, one might also expect more gradual slopes of both stimulus- and response-synchronized LRPs in the incompatible condition. A similar kind of reasoning holds true for the third alternative, namely that the LRP emerges not at the beginning but during motor programming. In this case, S-R compatibility effects should be confined to the stimulus-lrp interval. Time to peak force, however, should affect both the stimulus-lrp interval and the LRP-response interval. The time-to-peak-force manipulation employed in the present study also offers an opportunity to consider an issue that has yielded controversial results. This issue concerns the influence of kinetic response parameters on LRP amplitude. Sommer, Leuthold, and Ulrich (1994) had required unspeeded responses with short or long time to peak force (100 vs. 200 ms on average) and peak forces of 10 or 50% of maximal force. No effects of the kinetic factors on LRP amplitude were found. Recently Mordkoff and Grosjean (2001) replicated these results in a speeded task by sorting responses according to kinetic parameters. In contrast, the force growth rate had a strong effect on LRP amplitude in an unspeeded task where isometric responses of long duration were compared (Ray, Slobounov, Mordkoff, Johnston, & Simon, 2000). One possibility to explain these results is the difference in time-to-peak-force values between the studies of Sommer et al. and Mordkoff and Grosjean on the one hand and Ray et al. on the other hand. Possibly, time to peak force affects LRP amplitude only if the movement cannot be completely programmed in advance but has to be regulated during its execution. The present study uses speeded responses

4 Functional locus of the LRP 223 with either short-duration ballistic movements and longer duration ramp movements that might require online regulation. If the hypothesis above is correct that LRP amplitude is modulated by online-regulation of the ongoing movement, we should expect LRP amplitude to be larger in the long time-topeak-force condition. Finally, we would like to make a comment on method. One intrinsic problem of the present study is its reliance on onsetmeasures on the one hand and the use of a movement condition with a slowly rising force ramp on the other hand. Clearly, onsets can be determined most easily and most precisely when the rising flank of the parameter is steep. To safeguard against problems in determining force onsets, we required finger extensions and recorded the electromyogram from the musculus extensor digitorum to use it as an alternative indicator of reaction time and also for calculating response-synchronized LRPs (e.g., Masaki, Takasawa, & Yamazaki, 2000). Using the EMG for calculating response-synchronized LRPs has the additional advantage of eliminating any variability that occurs after the start of initial peripheral movement execution. Method Participants Twelve participants (4 men, 8 women), between 19 and 28 years of age (mean SD years), were recruited from Humboldt-University population. All participants were righthanded with mean handedness scores of 181 (Oldfield, 1971) and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Informed consent was obtained. Stimuli and Response Measurement The letters R or L subtending approximately served as imperative stimuli. They were presented in white at the center of a computer monitor placed 100 cm in front of the participant. Responses were measured by means of force-sensitive keys. Leaf springs ( mm) were held by clamps at one end, whereas the other end remained free. The thimblelike finger holders were attached on top of the free end of each spring. Strain gauges (Type 6/120 LY 41, Hottinger Baldwin Messtechnik) were attached near the fixed end. Any force applied to the leaf spring via the finger holders at the free end was reflected by an analogous signal, which was digitized along with the electrophysiological signals (see below). The keys allowed for near isometric recording of the required middle finger extensions. Procedure Participants comfortably rested both forearms and palms on flat boards and placed their middle fingers into the holders of the force-sensitive keys. Each trial began with the presentation of a fixation cross for 0.5 s, after which it was replaced by one of the letters, remaining in view until a response occurred. In the compatible conditions, participants responded to the letters L and R by lifting the left or right middle finger, respectively. In the incompatible conditions, the assignment of letters and fingers was reversed. Whereas peak force could always vary between 1.5 and 7.5 N, time to peak force was limited to a range between 70 to 140 ms in fast and 210 to 420 ms in slow conditions. One second after the response, visual feedback was given if time to peak force had been too short ( Zu rasch ) or too long ( Zu langsam ), if peak force had been too weak ( Zu schwach ) or too strong ( Zu stark ), or if the incorrect finger had been used ( Falsche Hand ). Intervals between a response and the next fixation point ranged between 2 and 3 s. Prior to the experiment proper, each participant practiced the different responses for each hand in compatible conditions. In each condition, consecutive blocks of 30 trials each were conducted until performance reached 75% correct in that block. In addition to the various types of error feedback described above, confirmatory feedback was given in the training session whenever the response was correct, consisting of the word OK. Only those participants who reached criterion in all conditions of the training session were used in the experimental session to follow on the next day with one exception where one day elapsed between sessions. The experimental session consisted of eight blocks of 60 trials each with the first 6 trials serving as warm-up after the short rest separating the blocks. The four conditions, consisting of the factor combinations of compatibility (compatible, incompatible) and movement velocity (short time to peak force, long time to peak force) were administered in counterbalanced order across participants. The required response fingers always varied randomly within each block and were equiprobable. Recording The electroencephalogram (EEG) was recorded from F3, F4, C3 0,C4 0 (4 cm to the left and right of Cz, respectively), P3, P4, O1, and O2 with tin electrodes embedded in a nylon mesh cap (Electrode-Cap International; Eaton, OH), according to the International 10/20 system (Jasper, 1958). Left mastoid served as a reference. Horizontal electrooculograms (heog) were recorded from the left and right outer canthi and vertical electrooculograms (veog) from above and below the eyes. The EEG and EOG were amplified by SynAmps (Neuroscan, Inc.). Electrode impedances were below 5 ko. Band pass was set at 0.01 to 30 Hz ( 3dB). The electromyogram (EMG) was bipolarly recorded from the extensor digitorum muscle in the forearms with tin electrodes with a band pass of 13 Hz to 10 khz ( 3dB, Coulbourn Instrument V75-04). The EMG signals were full-wave rectified and integrated with a time constant of 100 ms (Coulbourn Instrument V76-23). All physiological signals, including the force curves, were digitized at a rate of 500 Hz. Data Analysis To obtain LRPs and other lateralized ERPs, signals from lateral EEG (F3/F4, C3 0 /C4 0, P3/P4, and O1/O2) and horizontal EOG electrodes were calculated to response hand specific bipolar derivations in the same way as for the LRP (Coles, 1989). That is, for each response, hand condition signals at ipsilateral recording sites were subtracted from the signals at homologous contralateral recording sites. Separate mean difference wave forms were computed for trials requiring left- and right-hand responses. These difference wave forms were averaged separately for each participant and each experimental condition. This subtraction procedure was employed to calculate both stimulus- and response-synchronized LRPs. Trials with incorrect responses were excluded from the analyses, as were all trials where vertical or horizontal EOG voltages exceeded a threshold of 100 mv during the recording epoch. To determine EMG onsets, we adhered to the procedure suggested by Smid, Mulder, Mulder, and Brands (1992). For each trial, the onset of the EMG response was determined by

5 224 H. Masaki et al. looking backward from the downward slope of the rectified- EMG peak for the time at which the amplitude was equal to or lower than the amplitude at the preceding sampling point. The validity of these onset measures was checked by visual inspection. We used this EMG onset as a trigger for both averaging of the EMG-synchronized LRP and for measuring EMG latency. The same algorithm was applied to the force curves to determine force onsets. Reaction time was measured as the interval between stimulus and response force onsets. LRP onset was determined as the intersection of the best fitting linear regression line through the slope of the LRP with the baseline, defined as the average voltage of the LRP during the 200 ms prior to stimulus onset (Mordkoff & Gianaros, 2000; Schwarzenau, Falkenstein, Hoormann, & Hohnsbein, 1998). The optimal regression line was selected as follows: First an anchor point was chosen; for the EMG-synchronized LRP, one end of the regression line was anchored at the point of peak amplitude; for the stimulus-synchronized LRP, it was anchored at the point on the LRP associated with the mean EMG onset. We did not anchor the last point of the regression line at the peak of the stimulus-synchronized LRP wave as suggested by Mordkoff and Gianaros (2000) because in the slow time-topeak-force condition the LRP did not show a clear peak. Then, series of least squares linear regressions were calculated based on the anchor point and a specified number of data points preceding the anchor points. Regression lines moved freely along the y-axis also at the anchor point. Starting with n 5 2, the number of data points preceding the anchor points was incremented in steps of one until the variance accounted for by the n 1 1st line was less than for the nth line and the algorithm was stopped. At this time, the nth regression line was visually inspected to determine if it was only a local maximum (i.e., not the true best fit). If the nth regression line appeared to be an obviously poor fit, then the algorithm was restarted until the true optimal regression line was determined (maximum variance accounted for). The point at which this optimal regression line intersected with the baseline was taken as the LRP onset. The regression lines were calculated in jackknife averages, according to the procedure suggested by Miller, Patterson, and Ulrich (1988). When subjecting the onset measures from jackknife averages to analyses of variance (ANOVA), F values were corrected as Fc 5 F/(n 1) 2 (Ulrich & Miller, 2001). Results Performance Table 1 shows mean RTs and mean EMG latencies of correct responses. S-R compatibility and time to peak force seem to Table 1. Means and SEM (Milliseconds) of EMG Latency (Stimulus-to-EMG Onset) and Reaction Time (Stimulus-to-Force Onset) as a Function of Compatibility and Movement Velocity EMG latency Reaction time Compatible Fast (17.0) (17.7) Slow (22.9) (21.6) Incompatible Fast (31.5) (29.2) Slow (31.1) (26.6) Table 2. Means and SEM of Hand-Choice Errors (%), Incorrect Response Force and Incorrect Time-to-Peak-Force as a Function of Compatibility and Movement Velocity Hand-choice errors Peak-force errors Time-to-peak-force errors Compatible Fast 0.63 (0.33) 1.67 (0.55) 8.68 (2.05) Slow 0.76 (0.42) 2.36 (0.67) (2.33) Incompatible Fast 3.68 (0.78) 2.71 (0.94) (2.53) Slow 3.95 (0.86) 2.85 (0.51) (2.29) exhibit additive effects on both dependent variables, suggesting that these two factors selectively affected different processing stages. ANOVA on mean RTrevealed a significant main effect of S-R compatibility, F(1,11) , po.001, but no effect of time to peak force, F(1,11) , p There was no interaction between these variables, F(1,11) , p Mean EMG latency, however, yielded significant main effects of both S-R compatibility and time-to-peak-force manipulations, F(1,11) , po.005; F(1,11) , po.005, respectively. Importantly, there was no interaction between these factors, F , confirming independent effects on separate processing stages. Table 2 shows percentages of hand-choice errors and responses failing to meet the time-to-peak-force and force criteria (force errors). For hand-choice errors, ANOVA indicated that incompatible trials induced more hand errors than compatible trials, F(1,11) , po.001. Thus, speed accuracy trade-off did not occur. No reliable effects of the experimental variables were seen for the force errors and time-to-peak-force errors. We should like to point out that these latter types of errors do not relate to the integrity of the transmitted hand information, but merely reflect imperfections of movement execution. Therefore, these errors do not call into question the applicability of the additive factors logic in the present case as an excess of choice errors would. Stimulus-Synchronized LRPs Figure 1 (left panel) shows the grand averaged wave forms of the stimulus-synchronized LRPs over central regions (C3 0 /C4 0 ), other stimulus-synchronized lateralized wave forms, and the lateralized horizontal EOG. As can be seen in this figure, the S-R compatibility manipulation seemed to affect the latencies of the stimulus-synchronized LRPs. Figure 2 (left panel) shows a magnification of the stimulus-synchronized LRPs over central regions with the averaged best-fit regression lines in each condition superimposed. Table 3 presents the mean onsets of the stimulus-synchronized LRP measured with the regression line method in the jackknife grand averages. ANOVA revealed a significant S-R compatibility effect, Fc(1,11) , po.05, showing earlier onsets in the compatible than in the incompatible conditions. Otherwise neither main effect of time to peak force, Fc , nor interaction with S-R compatibility, Fc(1,11) , p4.10, was found. Furthermore, comparison of the slopes of the regression lines through the initial flank of the stimulus-synchronized LRP revealed an S-R compatibility effect, Fc(1,11) , po.01, indicating steeper slopes in compatible than incompatible conditions (Table 4). However, an interaction between variables

6 Functional locus of the LRP 225 Figure 1. Left panel: grand-average stimulus-synchronized lateralized readiness potentials (LRPs) at four homologous electrode positions, for the lateralized horizontal electrooculogram (heog), rectified EMG (remg), and force waveform. The vertical line indicates stimulus onset. C/F: fast compatible condition; C/S: slow compatible; IC/F: fast incompatible; IC/S: slow incompatible. Right panel: grand-average EMG-synchronized LRPs, heog, remg, and force waveform. The vertical line indicates the EMG onset. was also significant, Fc(1,11) , po.05. Considering Table 4, it is clear that this interaction stems from the relatively steep slopes in the fast compatible condition; Bonferoni-corrected pairwise comparisons indicated that this condition differed significantly from both incompatible conditions and as a trend from the slow compatible condition, p Figure 1 (left panel) also indicates a small negative-going parieto-central deflection in the LRP in the compatible conditions about 200 ms after the stimulus. This deflection was absent in the incompatible conditions. This effect was analyzed by measuring mean amplitudes between 150 and 250 ms. ANOVA revealed a significant interaction of S-R Compatibility Electrode, F(2,22) , po.05, indicating a strong trend for a larger negativity in the compatible than the incompatible condition at parietal sites, t(11) , p The reader will note in Figure 1 (left panel) that there was another negative deflection around 350 ms after stimulus onset at parietal sites. Because in this time range there is also strong central activity, it seems to be most sensible to assume that the parietal activity reflects volume-conducted LRP. Response-Synchronized LRPs EMG onsets were used for calculating response-synchronized LRPs (Figures 1 and 2, right panels). Clearly, there are effects of the experimental manipulations on the onsets and the negativegoing flanks of the LRP and also on the time course following EMG onset. Figure 1 (right panel) also suggests that both the force curves and the rectified EMG wave forms were not affected by the S-R compatibility manipulation, whereas both showed greater peak latencies in the slow movement relative to the fast movement, reflecting the task requirements. It appears from the figures that, especially in the fast compatible condition, the LRP may start very early (o 300 ms), increasing at first slowly until about 150 ms and then steeply. Apart from being difficult to explain, such a biphasic time course might call into question the applicability of the linear

7 226 H. Masaki et al. Figure 2. Left panel: grand-average stimulus-synchronized LRPs at central regions with average best-fit regression lines for onset determination superimposed. Stimulus onset is at time zero. Right panel: grand-average EMG-synchronized LRPs with best-fit regression lines. The vertical line indicates EMG onset. regression method. Therefore, we first assessed the onsets of the LRP wave shapes in each of the four experimental conditions by administering serial one-tailed t tests against zero at each digitized time point for the C3 0 /C4 0 electrode pair (e.g., De Jong et al., 1988; Smid et al., 1987; Valle-Inclán & Redondo, 1998). These tests indicated relatively late and very similar significant LRP onsets for the fast compatible and fast incompatible conditions ( 140 and 142 ms, respectively); markedly earlier in onset were the slow compatible and slow incompatible conditions ( 218 and 192 ms, respectively). In addition, ANOVA on the average amplitudes of the 300 to 200 ms time segment including frontal to parietal electrode pairs did not yield any significant effects. These results indicate that the very early deviations ( 400 to 150 ms) from baseline to be seen in the fast and to some extent Table 3. Mean Onset (Milliseconds) and SEM of the Stimulus- Synchronized LRP Relative to the Stimulus and Mean Onset (Milliseconds) of the Response-Synchronized LRP Relative to the EMG Onset Stimulus to LRP interval LRP to response interval Compatible Fast (1.18) (1.66) Slow (1.58) (5.14) Incompatible Fast (1.30) (1.84) Slow (2.20) (1.77) Table 4. Mean Slope (Microvolts per Millisecond) and SEM of Regression Lines Applied to the Flanks of the Stimulus- Synchronized LRP and the Response-Synchronized LRP a Stimulus-synchronized LRP Response-synchronized LRP Compatible Fast (0.0003) (0.0003) Slow (0.0002) (0.0003) Incompatible Fast (0.0003) (0.0003) Slow (0.0002) (0.0003) a More negative values represent steeper slopes. also in the slow compatible condition are not significant, which justifies the use of the regression method for statistical analyses of condition effects. In addition, the numerical pattern of the onsets suggests a sizeable effect of movement velocity, whereas compatibility is much less effective. The small negative-going parietocentral deflection in the LRP in the compatible conditions was also analyzed by measuring mean amplitudes between 300 and 200 ms before the EMG onset. In contrast to the stimulussynchronized LRP, ANOVA showed neither main effect of S-R compatibility nor interaction of S-R compatibility and electrode, Fs(1,11) and 1.99, ps 5.17 and.16, respectively, for this deflection. Table 3 shows the onsets of the grand averaged EMGsynchronized LRPs as measured with the regression method. ANOVA of the onsets thus obtained revealed a significant timeto-peak-force effect, Fc(1,11) , po.05, confirming longer intervals between the LRP and the EMG onsets in the slow relative to the fast time-to-peak-force condition. Neither S-R compatibility effect, Fc , nor interaction between variables, Fc , was observed. ANOVA of the slopes of the regression lines (Table 4) revealed a time-to-peak-force effect, Fc(1,11) , po.01, supporting the observation in the curves (Figure 2, right panel) that the LRP shows a steeper rising flank in the fast relative to the slow time-to-peak-force condition. No S-R compatibility effect on the slopes was present, Fc However, an interaction of S-R Compatibility Time to Peak Force was revealed, Fc(1,11) , po.05, suggesting that the time-to-peak-force effect in the slopes is even more pronounced in the compatible condition. Bonferoni-corrected pairwise post hoc comparisons indicated that the compatible slow condition differed significantly from the compatible fast condition, po.01) andfas a trendffrom the incompatible fast condition, p 5.08); in addition there was also a trend for a difference between the compatible fast and the incompatible slow conditions, p Another interesting observation concerns the time course of the response-synchronized LRP amplitude. Whereas immediately prior to the response there appear to be no pronounced amplitude differences, the LRP after EMG onset was flatter in the slow than in the fast time-to-peak-force conditions. Thus LRP amplitude is largest in the fast condition until about 150 ms

8 Functional locus of the LRP 227 after EMG onset but smallest thereafter. These observations were supported by ANOVAs of mean LRP amplitudes, measured at the central electrodes in four consecutive 100-ms time segments starting at EMG onset. A main effect of time to peak force appeared in the first segment after EMG onset and in the final ( ms) segment, F(1,11) and 43.74, po.05 and o.001, respectively, but not in between, F Interestingly, in the time segment immediately prior to EMG onset, LRP amplitude was significantly larger in the compatible than in the incompatible conditions (M vs mv), F(1,11) , p.05. The effect was present also when the measurement segment was narrowed to 30 to 40 ms prior to EMG onset, F(1,11) , po.05. Importantly, there was no effect of time to peak force on the preresponse LRP amplitude (100-ms segment before EMG onset: F ; 30 to 40 ms before EMG onset: F , respectively). Discussion It was the aim of the present study to achieve a more precise locus of the onset of the LRP within the information processing system. To this aim we factorially manipulated stimulus response compatibility and movement velocity, taken to selectively influence the response selection and motor programming stages, respectively. EMG latency as measured in the EMG clearly showed additive effects of these variables without any interaction, indicating that separate stages had been manipulated selectively (Sternberg, 1969). Thus, the findings of Spijkers (1989) that movement speed affects motor programming are extended here to isometric conditions. The results for RT as measured in force onsets were similar to those for EMG latency as measured in EMG onsets but were less clear, most likely due to the problems noted above in determining the onsets of the force curves in the long time-to-peak-force conditions. A further important requirement for the validity of the present design is that the task is accomplished within a single processing route and does not involve additional response activation via a second, unconditional route. The absence of incorrect LRP activation at central electrodes in the incompatible conditions indicates that this was indeed the case. Together, these findings make us feel confident that the present design did fulfill the important requirement of independent manipulations of the time demands for response hand selection and motor programming in a serial processing chain. The effects of the experimental manipulations on the LRP onsets were quite clear cut. The S-R compatibility manipulation exclusively acted on the stimulus-lrp interval and had no effect on the LRP-response interval. Conversely, movement velocity had no effect on the stimulus-lrp interval but did affect the LRP-response interval in the predicted direction. As outlined in the introduction, this pattern is expected when the LRP starts after the completion of response hand selection and at the beginning of motor programming. A problem for this interpretation may be seen to arise from the effect of compatibility on the slope of the stimulus-synchronized LRPs in the fast movement condition, which would be in line with LRP elicitation during response selection. If this was the case, however, S-R compatibility should also affect the slopes of the response-synchronized LRPs, which it did not. Inspection of Figure 2 and Table 4 shows that the compatibility effect on the slope of the stimulus-synchronized LRP is due to the flattening of the slope in the fast incompatible relative to the fast compatible condition. We suggest that this is due to an increase of variability in the EMG latency specifically in the fast incompatible condition (cf. Table 1), which would smear the averaged wave shape and thus flatten the slope. In line with this interpretation, the slopes and amplitudes are very similar for these conditions in the responsesynchronized LRPs, where the jitter due to RT variability is eliminated. LRP elicitation during response hand selection would also induce an S-R compatibility effect in response-synchronized onsets, which was neither indicated by the serial t tests nor by the regression method. Note that although S-R compatibility appears to influence response-synchronized LRP onset in the slow condition (cf. Table 3), this effect not only fails to achieve statistical significance, it is also opposite in direction to what LRP-elicitation during response hand selection would predict: Rather than being shorter in the compatible as compared to the incompatible condition, the LRP-response interval is actually somewhat longer. From the present findings we can also rule out that the LRP starts at some point during motor programming. In that case, LRP onset relative to the stimulus should be delayed when the time demands for motor programming are increased. This was not the case, however; movement velocity had no effects on stimulus-synchronized LRP onset. Our data also rule out the two other theoretically possible alternatives, beginning of LRP (1) prior to or at the beginning of response hand selectionfin which case one would not expect any experimental effects on the stimulus-synchronized LRP onsetfand (2) after motor programming, as indicated by an absence of experimental effects on the response-synchronized LRP onset. Both predictions are at odds with our data. Therefore, we conclude from the present findings that the onset of the LRP follows response hand selection and starts with the beginning of motor programming. This conclusion is consistent with most of the other available findings. Thus the stimulus-lrp interval is affected by stimulus quality (Smulders, 1993; Smulders et al., 1995) and symbolic compatibility (Smulders, 1993). The LRP-response interval is affected by response-complexity (Smulders et al., 1995), by preliminary information about response parameters (Leuthold et al., 1996), and by the compatibility (Miller & Ulrich, 1998) and number of alternatives (Hackley & Valle-Inclán, 1999) of finger choice at constant levels of hand selection difficulty. The only inconsistency that we can see is the observation of Smulders (1993) that the LRP-response interval is delayed for degraded incompatible stimuli. Are the present findings called into question by the small early parieto-central negative dip in the stimulus-synchronized LRPs in the compatible conditions? Such dips have been reported when lateralized stimulus presentation is confounded with response side (cf. Eimer, 1996) and have been interpreted as indicating automatic response activation by stimulus features that overlap with a response dimension. One might argue that such automatic response tendencies were also elicited by our centrally presented letter stimuli. In this case, we can rule out two possible processes. First, the parietal dip is not an LRP proper because it is larger over parietal than over central sites whereas the LRP proper shows the opposite distribution (cf. Figure 1). Therefore the parietal dip does not reflect immediate motor activation. Second, the dip is also not an N2pc as observed in visuospatial attention (e.g., Luck & Hillyard, 1994; Praamstra & Oostenfeld, 2003; Wascher & Wauschkuhn, 1996) because this component is typically observed with lateralized horizontal stimulus arrays and shows an occipitotemporal scalp distribution. On the other hand, Stu rmer and Leuthold (2003) have recently reported an

9 228 H. Masaki et al. early parietal lateralized ERP component for vertical stimulus arrays. This component could be functionally dissociated from the LRP proper and was suggested to represent externally triggered transformations of spatial stimulus features into spatial response features. Possibly, the letter stimuli of the present study might have triggered spatial response features in a similar vein as did the spatial stimulus features of Stu rmer and Leuthold. The early parietal dip is not necessarily at odds with a single route account of the present paradigm (Kornblum et al., 1990); possibly, the parietal activation represents a processing stage preceding the motor activation reflected in the LRP proper. Because previous studies using symbolic stimuli have rarely recorded lateralized ERPs from electrodes other than central ones, the generality of such parietal activation is hard to determine. Therefore, without further research it cannot be ruled out that the parietal activation takes place in a route that runs parallel to the conditional response selection. Independent of the functional interpretation of the parietal dip, our findings were not affected by its presence because of its small size at central electrodes and because we used the regression method for onset determination, which is robust against small deviations from the baseline. Together, we suggest that the LRP starts as soon as there is complete determination of the response hand by the response selection stage. The response hand code appears to be transmitted to the motor stages in a discrete fashion; if partial information about the response hand were transmitted, for example, while still being determined, we should have observed an effect of stimulus response compatibility also on the responselocked LRP, which was not the case. How can these conclusions be reconciled with the abundant evidence about partial transmission of information and about effects of the number of stimulus alternatives and compatibility on the response-locked LRP? First of all, let us clearly state that the present findings were obtained in a condition where the stimulus contained only a single attribute (letter shape) and where, according to common understanding, there was only a single processing route. Evidence for partial transmission of information into the motor system comes from experiments with multiple stimuli, such as the Eriksen flanker task (Gratton et al., 1998), or with stimuli having several attributes, as used in two-choice go/no-go tasks (e.g., Abdel-Rahman, Sommer, & Schweinberger, 2002; Miller & Hackley, 1992; Osman et al., 1992) or in the Simon task (e.g., Stürmer et al., 2002; Valle-Inclán, 1996; Valle-Inclán & Redondo, 1998). In all these designs, a biphasic or temporary LRP activation may occur. For example, early activation of the incorrect response followed by activation of the correct response is seen for incompatible flankers in the Eriksen task and for noncorresponding stimuli in the Simon task. Temporary response activation is seen in the LRP for no-go stimuli in twochoice go/no-go tasks. According to the present findings, these signs of temporary response activations should all emerge after response hand selection at the beginning of motor programming. Such an explanation fits very well with dual route models as have been proposed to explain the Simon effect or spatial S-R compatibility (Kornblum et al., 1990). In this case, the motor programming stage would receive two volleys of activation, one via the conditional route, involving controlled response selection, and another one via a direct link from perceptual processes, bypassing response selection. Temporary LRP activations in other designs with multiple stimuli or multiple stimulus attributes might be explained similarly with multiple streams of activation reaching the motor programming stage. These streams of activation might be caused by multiple response selections as in dual task designs (e.g., Osman & Moore, 1993; Sommer et al., 2001) or by additional links from processes other than response selection (Hommel, Mu sseler, Aschersleben, & Prinz, 2001). Studies that have shown effects of stimulus response compatibility (Miller & Ulrich, 1998) or number of response alternatives (Hackley & Valle-Inclán, 1999) on the responsesynchronized LRP indicate that response selection itself may consist of several aspects, only one of them being the selection of the response hand. When other aspects in addition to hand, such as response finger, have to be selected as well, these processes appear to follow hand selection at least in the existing studies because they affect the interval after LRP onset. If this interpretation is correct, it also follows that different codes derived during response selection can be transmitted separately. As far as the motor programs are concerned that are presumably assembled and implemented after response selection, it is not completely clear whether the effector-specific programming phase is preceded by a more abstract phase that is independent of the specific muscle groups required for the response (Schmidt, 1975). According to suggestions by Ulrich et al. (1998) and dipole models by Leuthold and Jentzsch (2001, 2002), the LRP reflects effector-specific motor preparation taking place within the premotor area and primary motor cortex. If this holds true, the assembling and selection of abstract movement programs within the supplementary and cingulate motor areas would take place before LRP onset. Future research should test this notion. In this context, it is also interesting to consider variables that affect LRP amplitude. As far as movement kinetics are concerned, the data are somewhat controversial, as outlined in the introduction. Whereas Sommer et al. (1994) and Mordkoff and Grosjean (2001) did not find effects of response force and time to peak force, Ray et al. (2000) reported larger LRPs for higher force growth rates. Above, we hypothesized that these discrepancies may relate to requirements for on-line control over nonballistic movements. In the present experiment, the fast movement with a time to peak force of ms is at the lower limit and therefore is most probably not under control while it is executed. In contrast, the slow movement with a time to peak force of ms not only requires more programming prior to execution (as confirmed by present LRP onset data) but also renders itself to on-line control. Interestingly, we did find an effect of time to peak force on LRP amplitude, but only after response onset. Following EMG onset, LRP amplitude in the slow condition showed a relatively steady activation over time, whereas in the fast condition, the typical phasic activation with a peak around 100 ms appeared. These difference in the time course of the LRPs following response onset may relate either to differential needs for on-line response regulation (Van Donkelaar & Franks, 1991) or in differential reafferent input from cutaneous and muscle receptors. Prior to response onset, however, there was no effect of time to peak force on LRP amplitude, which is in line with the reports of Sommer et al. (1994) and Mordkoff and Grosjean (2001) and rules out an explanation of the results of Ray et al. (2000) in terms of differential preparation for on-line control. In conclusion, manipulating response hand selection and motor programming in a factorial design, we obtained evidence that further specifies the locus of the LRP onset within the information processing system. According to these findings the LRP starts (1) after the completion of response-hand decision and (2) at the beginning of motor programming. If these

Beneficial effects of ambiguous precues: Parallel motor preparation or reduced premotoric processing time?

Beneficial effects of ambiguous precues: Parallel motor preparation or reduced premotoric processing time? Psychophysiology, 41 (24), 231 244. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 24 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 1.1111/j.1469-8986.24.155.x Beneficial effects of ambiguous

More information

The lateralized readiness potential and response kinetics in response-time tasks

The lateralized readiness potential and response kinetics in response-time tasks Psychophysiology, 38 ~2001!, 777 786. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2001 Society for Psychophysiological Research The lateralized readiness potential and response kinetics in

More information

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events Psychophysiology, 40 (2003), 827 831. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2003 Society for Psychophysiological Research BRIEF REPT Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the

More information

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task Psychophysiology, 44 (2007), 913 917. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2007 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00561.x BRIEF REPORT Reward prediction

More information

Preparation of response force and movement direction: Onset effects on the lateralized readiness potential

Preparation of response force and movement direction: Onset effects on the lateralized readiness potential Psychophysiology, 37 ~2000!, 507 514. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2000 Society for Psychophysiological Research Preparation of response force and movement direction: Onset

More information

Conflict monitoring and feature overlap: Two sources of sequential modulations

Conflict monitoring and feature overlap: Two sources of sequential modulations Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (4), 742-748 Conflict monitoring and feature overlap: Two sources of sequential modulations Çağlar Akçay and Eliot Hazeltine University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa Recent

More information

A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task

A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task Psychological Research (2004) 68: 1 17 DOI 10.1007/s00426-003-0132-y ORIGINAL ARTICLE Bernhard Hommel Æ Robert W. Proctor Kim-Phuong L. Vu A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon

More information

Auditory Warning Signals Affect Mechanisms of Response Selection

Auditory Warning Signals Affect Mechanisms of Response Selection Research Article Auditory Warning Signals Affect Mechanisms of Response Selection Evidence from a Simon Task Rico Fischer, 1 Franziska Plessow, 1 and Andrea Kiesel 2 1 Technische Universität Dresden, Germany

More information

The Effects of Temporal Preparation on Reaction Time

The Effects of Temporal Preparation on Reaction Time University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School January 2013 The Effects of Temporal Preparation on Reaction Time Glen Robert Forester University of South

More information

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling Supplementary materials 1 Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling analyses placed the source of the No Go N2 component in the dorsal ACC, near the ACC source

More information

An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search

An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search Steven Phillips (steve@ni.aist.go.jp) Neuroscience Research Institute (AIST), Tsukuba Central 2, 1-1-1 Umezono, Tsukuba, Ibaraki 305-8568

More information

Measurement of ERP latency differences: A comparison of single-participant and jackknife-based scoring methods

Measurement of ERP latency differences: A comparison of single-participant and jackknife-based scoring methods Psychophysiology, 45 (28), 25 274. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 27 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 1.1111/j.1469-8986.27.618.x Measurement of ERP latency differences:

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Item and Category-Based Attentional Control During Search for Real-World Objects: Can You Find the Pants Among the Pans? Rebecca Nako,

More information

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Psychon Bull Rev (2013) 20:296 301 DOI 10.3758/s13423-012-0353-4 BRIEF REPORT Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Risa Sawaki & Steven J. Luck Published online: 20 December 2012 #

More information

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER Memory & Cognition 2008, 36 (7), 1236-1247 doi: 10.3758/MC/36.7.1236 An action sequence held in memory can interfere with response selection of a target stimulus, but does not interfere with response activation

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article. MECHANISMS OF PRIMING BY MASKED STIMULI: Inferences From Event-Related Brain Potentials

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article. MECHANISMS OF PRIMING BY MASKED STIMULI: Inferences From Event-Related Brain Potentials Research Article MECHANISMS OF PRIMING BY MASKED STIMULI: Inferences From Event-Related Brain Potentials Humboldt-University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany Abstract A metacontrast procedure was combined with

More information

Motor programming of response force and movement direction

Motor programming of response force and movement direction Psychophysiology, 35 ~1998!, 721 728. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1998 Society for Psychophysiological Research Motor programming of response force and movement direction

More information

Are fingers special? Evidence about movement preparation from event-related brain potentials

Are fingers special? Evidence about movement preparation from event-related brain potentials Psychophysiology, 4 (3), 7 16. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 3 Society for Psychophysiological Research Are fingers special? Evidence about movement preparation from event-related

More information

Programming of time-to-peak force for brief isometric force pulses: Effects on reaction time

Programming of time-to-peak force for brief isometric force pulses: Effects on reaction time THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2006, 59 (7), 1277 1305 Programming of time-to-peak force for brief isometric force pulses: Effects on reaction time Hannes Schröter University of Tübingen,

More information

Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized. Readiness Potential. Dianna Monteith. Saint Thomas University

Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized. Readiness Potential. Dianna Monteith. Saint Thomas University Extraversion and the LRP 1 Running head: EXTRAVERSION AND THE LRP Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized Readiness Potential Dianna Monteith Saint Thomas

More information

Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences

Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences Psychophysiology, 35 ~1998!, 99 115. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1998 Society for Psychophysiological Research METHODOLOGY Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency

More information

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION Jordan Schoenherr, Craig Leth-Steensen, and William M. Petrusic psychophysics.lab@gmail.com, craig_leth_steensen@carleton.ca, bpetrusi@carleton.ca Carleton

More information

Interaction of facial expressions and familiarity: ERP evidence

Interaction of facial expressions and familiarity: ERP evidence Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Biological Psychology 77 (2008) 138 149 www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho Interaction of facial expressions and familiarity: ERP evidence Nele Wild-Wall 1, *,

More information

Accessory stimuli modulate effects of nonconscious priming

Accessory stimuli modulate effects of nonconscious priming Perception & Psychophysics 2007, 69 (1), 9-22 Accessory stimuli modulate effects of nonconscious priming RICO FISCHER, TORSTEN SCHUBERT, AND ROMAN LIEPELT Humboldt University, Berlin, Germany In a recent

More information

Report. Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Martin Eimer 1, * and Anna Grubert 1 1

Report. Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Martin Eimer 1, * and Anna Grubert 1 1 Current Biology 24, 193 198, January 20, 2014 ª2014 The Authors http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.001 Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects Report Martin

More information

In Search of the Point of No Return: The Control of Response Processes

In Search of the Point of No Return: The Control of Response Processes Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1990, Vol. 16, No. 1, 164-182 In Search of the Point of No Return: The Control of Response Processes Copyright 1990 by the American

More information

The effect of response execution on P3 latency, reaction time, and movement time

The effect of response execution on P3 latency, reaction time, and movement time Psychophysiology, 36 ~1999!, 351 363. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1999 Society for Psychophysiological Research The effect of response execution on P3 latency, reaction time,

More information

Conditional accuracy in response interference tasks: Evidence from the Eriksen flanker task and the spatial conflict task

Conditional accuracy in response interference tasks: Evidence from the Eriksen flanker task and the spatial conflict task 2007 volume 3 no 3 409-417 Advances in Cognitive Psychology Conditional accuracy in response interference tasks: Evidence from the Eriksen flanker task and the spatial conflict task John F. Stins 1,2,

More information

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction Proceedings of the First International Conference on Complex Medical Engineering (CME2005) May 15-18, 2005, Takamatsu, Japan (Organized Session No. 20). Paper No. 150, pp. 715-719. Event-Related Potentials

More information

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Cerebral Cortex March 2006;16:415-424 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi121 Advance Access publication June 15, 2005 Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Mika

More information

Precueing Spatial S-R Correspondence: Is There Regulation of Expected Response Conflict?

Precueing Spatial S-R Correspondence: Is There Regulation of Expected Response Conflict? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2008, Vol. 34, No. 4, 872 883 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.4.872

More information

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Activation of effect codes in response planning - 1 - To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Michael Ziessler

More information

Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence

Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (4), 951-962 doi:10.3758/app.72.4.951 Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence

More information

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search Clayton Hickey 1,2, Vincent Di Lollo 2, and John J. McDonald 2 Abstract & Attentional selection of a target presented among

More information

Dissociation of S-R Compatibility and Simon Effects With Mixed Tasks and Mappings

Dissociation of S-R Compatibility and Simon Effects With Mixed Tasks and Mappings Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2012, Vol. 38, No. 5, 000 2012 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0029923 Dissociation of S-R Compatibility

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes Biological Psychology 66 (2004) 1 20 The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes Bettina Forster, Martin Eimer School of

More information

Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex

Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (5), 834-839 Brief Reports Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex Jeffrey S. Johnson, Geoffrey F. Woodman, Elsie Braun, and Steven

More information

An investigation of the effect of preparation on response execution and inhibition in the go/nogo task

An investigation of the effect of preparation on response execution and inhibition in the go/nogo task University of Wollongong Research Online University of Wollongong Thesis Collection 1954-2016 University of Wollongong Thesis Collections 2005 An investigation of the effect of preparation on response

More information

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study Armita Faghani Jadidi, Raheleh Davoodi, Mohammad Hassan Moradi Department of Biomedical Engineering Amirkabir University

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Location-specific versus hemisphere-specific adaptation of processing selectivity

Location-specific versus hemisphere-specific adaptation of processing selectivity Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2008, 15 (1), 135-140 doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.1.135 Location-specific versus hemisphere-specific adaptation of processing selectivity MIKE WENDT, RAINER H. KLUWE, AND INA VIETZE

More information

RUNNING HEAD: MOTOR ACTIVATION BY INSTRUCTION 1

RUNNING HEAD: MOTOR ACTIVATION BY INSTRUCTION 1 RUNNING HEAD: MOTOR ACTIVATION BY INSTRUCTION 1 Automatic Motor Activation by Mere Instruction Tom Everaert Marijke Theeuwes Baptist Liefooghe Jan De Houwer Ghent University Address correspondence to:

More information

Where did you go wrong? Errors, partial errors, and the nature information processing. Michael G.H. Coles *, Marten K. Scheffers, Lisa Fournier

Where did you go wrong? Errors, partial errors, and the nature information processing. Michael G.H. Coles *, Marten K. Scheffers, Lisa Fournier Ja. acts psychologica lf!b ELSEVIER Acts Psychologica 90 (1995) 129-144 Where did you go wrong? Errors, partial errors, and the nature information processing of human Michael G.H. Coles *, Marten K. Scheffers,

More information

Action Preparation Helps and Hinders Perception of Action

Action Preparation Helps and Hinders Perception of Action Action Preparation Helps and Hinders Perception of Action Clare Press 1,2, Elena Gherri 1, Cecilia Heyes 3, and Martin Eimer 1 Abstract Several theories of the mechanisms linking perception and action

More information

Response preparation and code overlap in dual tasks

Response preparation and code overlap in dual tasks Memory & Cognition 2005, 33 (6), 1085-1095 Response preparation and code overlap in dual tasks IRING KOCH and WOLFGANG PRINZ Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Munich, Germany

More information

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 2008, 8 (2), 195-210 doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.2.195 The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies MIKA

More information

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in Automatic detection - 1 Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1986, 24 (6), 431-434 SIU L. CHOW The University of Wollongong,

More information

Post-response stimulation and the Simon effect: Further evidence of action effect integration

Post-response stimulation and the Simon effect: Further evidence of action effect integration VISUAL COGNITION, 2002, 9 (4/5), 528 539 Post-response stimulation and the Simon effect: Further evidence of action effect integration Marc Grosjean and J. Toby Mordkoff Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania

More information

International Journal of Psychophysiology

International Journal of Psychophysiology International Journal of Psychophysiology 75 (2010) 339 348 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect International Journal of Psychophysiology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ijpsycho Flanker

More information

The Roles of Feature-Specific Task Set and Bottom-Up Salience in Attentional Capture: An ERP Study

The Roles of Feature-Specific Task Set and Bottom-Up Salience in Attentional Capture: An ERP Study Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2009, Vol. 35, No. 5, 1316 1328 2009 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0015872 The Roles of Feature-Specific

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Ingroup categorization and response conflict: Interactive effects of target race, flanker compatibility, and infrequency on N2 amplitude

Ingroup categorization and response conflict: Interactive effects of target race, flanker compatibility, and infrequency on N2 amplitude Psychophysiology, 47 (2010), 596 601. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2010 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.00963.x BRIEF REPORT Ingroup categorization

More information

Electrophysiological Markers of Visual Dimension Changes and Response Changes

Electrophysiological Markers of Visual Dimension Changes and Response Changes Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2008, Vol. 34, No. 3, 531 542 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.3.531

More information

Behavioural and electrophysiological measures of task switching during single and mixed-task conditions

Behavioural and electrophysiological measures of task switching during single and mixed-task conditions Biological Psychology 72 (2006) 278 290 www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho Behavioural and electrophysiological measures of task switching during single and mixed-task conditions Philippe Goffaux a,b, Natalie

More information

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES Carryl L. Baldwin and Joseph T. Coyne Department of Psychology Old Dominion University

More information

Contingent Attentional Capture by Top-Down Control Settings: Converging Evidence From Event-Related Potentials

Contingent Attentional Capture by Top-Down Control Settings: Converging Evidence From Event-Related Potentials Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 8, Vol. 3, No. 3, 59 53 Copyright 8 by the American Psychological Association 9-153/8/$1. DOI: 1.137/9-153.3.3.59 Contingent Attentional

More information

Readiness Potentials Related to Self-Initiated Movement and to Movement Preceded by Time Estimation: A Comparison

Readiness Potentials Related to Self-Initiated Movement and to Movement Preceded by Time Estimation: A Comparison Physiol. Res. 45:235-239, 1996 Readiness Potentials Related to Self-Initiated Movement and to Movement Preceded by Time Estimation: A Comparison M. KUKLETA, P. BUSER1,1. REKTOR, M. LAMARCHE1 M edical Faculty,

More information

The speed of voluntary and priority-driven shifts of visual attention. *Corresponding Author. Malet Street, London, WC1E 7HX, UK

The speed of voluntary and priority-driven shifts of visual attention. *Corresponding Author. Malet Street, London, WC1E 7HX, UK The speed of voluntary and priority-driven shifts of visual attention Michael Jenkins 1 *, Anna Grubert, & Martin Eimer 1 *Corresponding Author 1 Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University

More information

All set, indeed! N2pc components reveal simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colours

All set, indeed! N2pc components reveal simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colours Running head: Control of multiple-colour search All set, indeed! N2pc components reveal simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colours Anna Grubert and Martin Eimer Department of

More information

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Journal: Psychological Science Manuscript ID: PSCI-0-0.R Manuscript Type: Short report Date Submitted by the Author:

More information

The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture

The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture Psychophysiology, 53 (2016), 650 662. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright VC 2016 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12615 The role of top-down spatial attention

More information

Redundancy gains in pop-out visual search are determined by top-down task set: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence

Redundancy gains in pop-out visual search are determined by top-down task set: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence Journal of Vision (2011) 11(14):10, 1 10 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/14/10 1 Redundancy gains in pop-out visual search are determined by top-down task set: Behavioral and electrophysiological

More information

Running Head: VISUAL WORKING MEMORY LOAD AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL

Running Head: VISUAL WORKING MEMORY LOAD AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL Running Head: VISUAL WORKING MEMORY LOAD AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL Visual working memory load disrupts template-guided attentional selection during visual search Nick Berggren* and Martin Eimer Department

More information

Short article The role of response selection in sequence learning

Short article The role of response selection in sequence learning THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2006, 59 (3), 449 456 Short article The role of response selection in sequence learning Natacha Deroost and Eric Soetens Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels,

More information

Final Summary Project Title: Cognitive Workload During Prosthetic Use: A quantitative EEG outcome measure

Final Summary Project Title: Cognitive Workload During Prosthetic Use: A quantitative EEG outcome measure American Orthotic and Prosthetic Association (AOPA) Center for Orthotics and Prosthetics Leraning and Outcomes/Evidence-Based Practice (COPL) Final Summary 2-28-14 Project Title: Cognitive Workload During

More information

Affective-motivational influences on feedback-related ERPs in a gambling task

Affective-motivational influences on feedback-related ERPs in a gambling task available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Affective-motivational influences on feedback-related ERPs in a gambling task Hiroaki Masaki a, Shigeki Takeuchi b, William

More information

Electrophysiological correlates of error correction

Electrophysiological correlates of error correction Psychophysiology, 42 (2005), 72 82. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2005 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00265.x Electrophysiological correlates

More information

Generating spatial and nonspatial attentional control: An ERP study

Generating spatial and nonspatial attentional control: An ERP study Psychophysiology, 42 (2005), 428 439. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2005 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00304.x Generating spatial and

More information

Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return

Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return David J. Prime and Pierre Jolicoeur Abstract & Here we examined the relationship between inhibition of return (IOR) and response-selection

More information

Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming

Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming Biological Psychology 64 (2003) 7 26 Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming Martin Eimer a,, Friederike Schlaghecken b a School of Psychology, Birkbeck College, University of London,

More information

Visual Short-term Memory Capacity for Simple and Complex Objects

Visual Short-term Memory Capacity for Simple and Complex Objects Visual Short-term Memory Capacity for Simple and Complex Objects Roy Luria 1, Paola Sessa 1, Alex Gotler 2, Pierre Jolicœur 3, and Roberto DellʼAcqua 1 Abstract Does the capacity of visual short-term memory

More information

Locus of the intensity effect in simple reaction time tasks

Locus of the intensity effect in simple reaction time tasks Perception & Psychophysics 27, 69 (8), 1334-1343 Locus of the intensity effect in simple reaction time tasks PIOTR JA KOWSKI University of Finance and Management, Warsaw, Poland MARTA KURCZEWSKA Adam Mickiewicz

More information

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, Vol. 28, No. 2, 396 409 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.396

More information

Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR, USA b University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR, USA b University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [National Cheng Kung University] On: 24 January 2 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 79473938] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England

More information

The Locus of Temporal Preparation Effects: Evidence from the Psychological Refractory Period Paradigm. Karin M. Bausenhart.

The Locus of Temporal Preparation Effects: Evidence from the Psychological Refractory Period Paradigm. Karin M. Bausenhart. Temporal Preparation 1 Running Head: TEMPORAL PREPARATION The Locus of Temporal Preparation Effects: Evidence from the Psychological Refractory Period Paradigm Karin M. Bausenhart Bettina Rolke University

More information

Attentional set mixing: Effects on target selection and selective response activation

Attentional set mixing: Effects on target selection and selective response activation Psychophysiology, 43 (26), 43 42. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 26 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI:./j.469-8986.26.49.x Attentional set mixing: Effects on target

More information

The control of attentional target selection in a colour/colour conjunction task

The control of attentional target selection in a colour/colour conjunction task Atten Percept Psychophys (2016) 78:2383 2396 DOI 10.3758/s13414-016-1168-6 The control of attentional target selection in a colour/colour conjunction task Nick Berggren 1 & Martin Eimer 1 Published online:

More information

The Simon effect in vocal responses

The Simon effect in vocal responses Acta Psychologica 121 (2006) 210 226 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The Simon effect in vocal responses Peter Wühr * Friedrich Alexander Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg, Institut für Psychologie I, Kochstrasse

More information

Experience-dependent recovery of vision following chronic deprivation amblyopia. Hai-Yan He, Baisali Ray, Katie Dennis and Elizabeth M.

Experience-dependent recovery of vision following chronic deprivation amblyopia. Hai-Yan He, Baisali Ray, Katie Dennis and Elizabeth M. Experience-dependent recovery of vision following chronic deprivation amblyopia Hai-Yan He, Baisali Ray, Katie Dennis and Elizabeth M. Quinlan a 3. 2.5 2. 1.5.5 Deprived eye Non-deprived VCtx * * b 8.

More information

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity 1,2 Mark A. Elliott, 2 Zhuanghua Shi & 2,3 Fatma Sürer 1 Department of Psychology National University of Ireland Galway, Ireland.

More information

Random visual noise impairs object-based attention

Random visual noise impairs object-based attention Exp Brain Res (2002) 142:349 353 DOI 10.1007/s00221-001-0899-2 RESEARCH ARTICLE Richard A. Abrams Mark B. Law Random visual noise impairs object-based attention Received: 10 May 2000 / Accepted: 4 September

More information

This is an Accepted Manuscript of an article published by Taylor & Francis in The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.

This is an Accepted Manuscript of an article published by Taylor & Francis in The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. This is an Accepted Manuscript of an article published by Taylor & Francis in The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Treccani B., Cubelli R., Della Sala S., Umiltà, C. (2009). Flanker and Simon

More information

Attentional and anatomical considerations for the representation of simple stimuli in visual short-term memory: evidence from human electrophysiology

Attentional and anatomical considerations for the representation of simple stimuli in visual short-term memory: evidence from human electrophysiology Psychological Research (2009) 73:222 232 DOI 10.1007/s00426-008-0214-y ORIGINAL ARTICLE Attentional and anatomical considerations for the representation of simple stimuli in visual short-term memory: evidence

More information

Delayed working memory consolidation during the attentional blink

Delayed working memory consolidation during the attentional blink Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2002, 9 (4), 739-743 Delayed working memory consolidation during the attentional blink EDWARD K. VOGEL and STEVEN J. LUCK University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa After the detection

More information

Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation

Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation Rongjun Yu a and Xiaolin Zhou a,b,c a Department of Psychology, Peking University, b State

More information

An Electrophysiological Study of Attention Capture by Salience: Does Rarity Enable Capture?

An Electrophysiological Study of Attention Capture by Salience: Does Rarity Enable Capture? An Electrophysiological Study of Attention Capture by Salience: Does Rarity Enable Capture? Noesen, B., Lien, M. C., & Ruthruff, E. (2014). An electrophysiological study of attention capture by salience:

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information ten Oever and Sack 10.1073/pnas.1517519112 SI Materials and Methods Experiment 1. Participants. A total of 20 participants (9 male; age range 18 32 y; mean age 25 y) participated

More information

Electrophysiological Evidence for Temporal Overlap Among Contingent Mental Processes

Electrophysiological Evidence for Temporal Overlap Among Contingent Mental Processes Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1992. Vol. 121. No. 2. 195-209 Copyright 1992 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-3445/92/S3.00 Electrophysiological Evidence for Temporal Overlap

More information

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory?

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory? Running Head: Visual Working Memory and the CDA Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory? Nick Berggren and

More information

7 Grip aperture and target shape

7 Grip aperture and target shape 7 Grip aperture and target shape Based on: Verheij R, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ. The influence of target object shape on maximum grip aperture in human grasping movements. Exp Brain Res, In revision 103 Introduction

More information

Acta Psychologica 136 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica. journal homepage:

Acta Psychologica 136 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica. journal homepage: Acta Psychologica 136 (2011) 245 252 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Acta Psychologica journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/ locate/actpsy Keeping Simon simple: Examining the relationship between

More information

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching Memory & Cognition 2009, 37 (7), 1040-1050 doi:10.3758/mc.37.7.1040 Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching ERINA SAEKI AND SATORU

More information

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),???-??? 794-799 What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? ULRICH MAYR University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon Schneider and

More information

Reaction time distribution analysis of spatial correspondence effects

Reaction time distribution analysis of spatial correspondence effects Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:242 266 DOI 10.3758/s13423-011-0053-5 Reaction time distribution analysis of spatial correspondence effects Robert W. Proctor & James D. Miles & Giulia Baroni Published online:

More information

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL for Cardoso et al. 22 The Neuroimaging Signal is a Linear Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. : Appendix: Homogeneous Linear ( Null ) and Modified Linear

More information

Numbers and Space: A Computational Model of the SNARC Effect

Numbers and Space: A Computational Model of the SNARC Effect Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2006, Vol. 32, No. 1, 32 44 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.1.32

More information

Attentional Blink Paradigm

Attentional Blink Paradigm Attentional Blink Paradigm ATTENTIONAL BLINK 83 ms stimulus onset asychrony between all stimuli B T D A 3 N P Z F R K M R N Lag 3 Target 1 Target 2 After detection of a target in a rapid stream of visual

More information

Biological Psychology

Biological Psychology Biological Psychology 87 (2011) 25 34 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Biological Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho Dissociated roles of the anterior cingulate cortex

More information

The initial stage of visual selection is controlled by top-down task set: new ERP evidence

The initial stage of visual selection is controlled by top-down task set: new ERP evidence Atten Percept Psychophys (2011) 73:113 122 DOI 10.3758/s13414-010-0008-3 The initial stage of visual selection is controlled by top-down task set: new ERP evidence Ulrich Ansorge & Monika Kiss & Franziska

More information