Preparation of response force and movement direction: Onset effects on the lateralized readiness potential

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Preparation of response force and movement direction: Onset effects on the lateralized readiness potential"

Transcription

1 Psychophysiology, 37 ~2000!, Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2000 Society for Psychophysiological Research Preparation of response force and movement direction: Onset effects on the lateralized readiness potential HILTRAUT MÜLLER-GETHMANN, GERHARD RINKENAUER, JUTTA STAHL, and ROLF ULRICH Department of General Psychology and Methodology, University of Tübingen, Germany Abstract Two experiments assessed the preparatory effects of advance information about response force and movement direction on the lateralized readiness potential ~LRP!. In a choice reaction time ~RT! task, an imperative stimulus required an isometric flexion or an extension of the left or right index finger. Prior information about response force or about movement direction reduced RT and shortened the interval from the onset of the imperative stimulus up to the onset of the LRP. Advance information, however, about direction but not about force decreased the interval from LRP onset to the onset of the overt response. The identical pattern of results was obtained in a second experiment, in which each participant performed both precue conditions. The findings of both experiments support the notion that response force is specified before movement direction. These results are consistent with the view accordingly different mechanisms are involved in the specification of muscle force and movement direction. Descriptors: Movement preprogramming, Movement precuing technique, Lateralized readiness potential, Reaction time, Movement direction, Response force Covert preparatory processes within the motor system precede overt motor acts and strongly influence the performance of these acts. Because these processes reveal basic principles of the motor system, it is hardly surprising that there is ongoing interest in unraveling them ~Requin, Brener, & Ring, 1991!. A powerful experimental task to assess preparatory processes is Rosenbaum s ~1980, 1983! precuing technique. This technique is a modified choice reaction time ~RT! task, in which a certain imperative stimulus requires a participant to perform a specific movement as quickly as possible. Before the onset of the imperative stimulus a precue provides information about one or several parameters of the forthcoming movement. For instance, the precue may indicate that a finger extension must be performed without specifying the response finger. If the preparatory processes utilize advance information, RT should be shorter with advance information than without it. Such RT shortening has been well documented in the RT literature ~cf. Requin et al., 1991; Rosenbaum, 1983!. Several recent studies have augmented the inferential power of this technique by combining it with the measurement of regional cerebral blood flow ~e.g., Deiber, Ibanez, Sadato, & Hallett, 1996!, the recording of This research was supported by a grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft ~SO ! to Werner Sommer and Rolf Ulrich. We appreciate the discussions with Hartmut Leuthold, Stefan Mattes, Allen Osman, and Werner Sommer. David A. Rosenbaum and three anonymous reviewers provided helpful comments on an earlier version of this article. Address reprint requests to: Hiltraut Müller-Gethmann, Department of General Psychology and Methodology, University of Tübingen, Friedrichstr. 21, Tübingen, Germany. hiltraut.mueller-gethmann@unituebingen.de. single-neuronal activity ~e.g., Riehle, MacKay, & Requin, 1994; Riehle & Requin, 1995!, transcranial magnetic stimulation ~Hasbroucq et al., 1999!, and the recording of event-related brain potentials ~e.g., De Jong, Wierda, Mulder, & Mulder, 1988; MacKay & Bonnet, 1990!. Riehle and collaborators identified neuronal mechanisms involved in movement preparation ~Riehle et al., 1994; Riehle & Requin, 1995!. In both studies monkeys performed a choice RT task. In one study advance information about movement extent and about the level of frictional force opposing the intended movement was provided ~Riehle et al., 1994!, whereas force level and movement direction were precued in the other study ~Riehle & Requin, 1995!. 1 The results provided evidence for the view that different populations of neurons are involved in the specification of force, movement extent, and movement direction. This view is corroborated by the event-related potential investigation of MacKay and Bonnet ~1990!, which showed that the amplitude of the contingent negative variation is sensitive to movement preparation. Specifically, topographical analysis indicated that the parietal cortex is involved in the specification of movement direction, whereas the primary motor cortex is involved in the specification of force. 1 In a physical sense, force is a vector, and by definition, has both magnitude and direction. Because the magnitude of force, however, can be related to muscle activity within a certain muscle group ~extensor or flexor muscles!, it makes sense to distinguish both the magnitude of force and its direction. In a physiological sense, then, the former parameter relates to the amount of muscle activity whereas the latter refers to the muscle group involved in performing the movement. In this paper the term force will relate to the magnitude of force, while the term direction will relate to the direction of the movement. 507

2 508 H. Müller-Gethmann et al. The view that the human primary motor cortex is engaged in the specification of movement force has been strengthened further by the study of Dettmers et al. ~1995!. These authors measured the relative regional cerebral blood flow while their participants flexed the right index finger with different levels of force. This measure revealed two functional populations within the cortex. The activity within one population increased with the level of peak force whereas the activity of the other population was unaffected by variations in peak force. Although the former population included the primary motor cortex, the primary somatosensory cortex, the dorsal bank of the cingulate sulcus, the ventral part of the posterior supplementary motor area, and the cerebellar vermis, the strongest correlation between regional blood flow and force peak was measured within the contralateral primary motor and somatosensory cortex. By contrast, the latter population was restricted to the contralateral putamen, the bilateral insular cortex, the second somatosensory cortex, and the dorsal part of the posterior supplementary motor area. Hence these results indicate that a forceful finger flexion is reflected in widely distributed cerebral activity that is modulated by movement force. This conclusion is consistent with the growing evidence that the neuroanatomical structure of the motor system appears to be more complex than previously assumed ~for a review see Rizolatti, Luppino & Matelli, 1998!. Chronopsychophysiology ~van der Molen, Bashore, Halliday, & Callaway, 1991! provides a complementary, more abstract approach to unravel less the neuroanatomical structure of the motor system but rather the information processing structure within this system. Several recent studies within this field ~De Jong et al., 1988; Leuthold, Sommer, & Ulrich, 1996; Osman, Moore, & Ulrich, 1995; Ulrich, Leuthold, & Sommer, 1998! combined the precuing technique with the recording of the lateralized readiness potential ~LRP!~cf. Coles, 1989!, the physiological origin of which is located within primary motor cortex ~Sasaki, Gemba, & Tsujimoto, 1990; Kristeva, Cheyne, & Deecke, 1991; Riehle & Requin, 1989!. There is strong evidence that the onset of the LRP indicates the moment in time when the response hand becomes centrally activated ~Gratton et al., 1990; Gratton, Coles, Sirevaag, Eriksen, & Donchin, 1988; Kristeva et al., 1991!. Miller and Hackley ~1992! showed that a voluntary response can be aborted after the onset of the LRP but prior to the onset of EMG. In information-processing terms, these results indicate that the emergence of the LRP indexes the onset of relatively early motoric processes and thus can be used to divide the RT into two intervals ~Osman et al., 1995!. The S-LRP interval reflects the duration of those processes from stimulus onset to the onset of the LRP. By contrast, the LRP-R interval encompasses processes intervening between the onset of the LRP and the onset of the overt response itself. Leuthold et al. ~1996! used the LRP to assess whether advance information about movement direction shortens the LRP-R interval as one might expect if preparation facilitates motoric processes. In their study, four imperative stimuli were mapped one-to-one onto four response alternatives consisting of isometric flexions or extensions of the right or left index finger. Relevant for the present issue are their conditions in which the imperative stimulus was preceded by a precue that either was non-informative or provided information about the requested movement direction ~flexion vs. extension! but not about the responding hand. RT was shorter when advance information was given. The manipulation of precue information not only affected RT but also the onset of the LRP. Consistent with the notion that advance information facilitates motoric processes, the LRP-R interval was shorter with directional advance information than without it. This result provides strong evidence for the idea that advance information about movement parameters shortens the motoric portion of RT. Precue information also shortened the S-LRP interval. This effect may emerge on at least two levels within the information processing system. First, the speed of perceptual processes may benefit from precue information, which reduces stimulus uncertainty. Second, precue information also reduces the number of response alternatives and therefore may increase the speed of response selection ~cf. Rosenbaum, 1980!. The aim of the present study was to extend the approach by Leuthold et al. ~1996!. More specifically, we investigated whether advance information about the magnitude of response force will also shorten the LRP-R and the S-LRP interval. As in the study of Leuthold et al., four imperative stimuli were mapped onto four response alternatives. In contrast to their study, these alternatives consisted of weak or strong isometric flexions with the right or left index finger. In half of the trials advance information about the required force level was provided. We hypothesized that when the level of the force output is specified after the response hand has been activated centrally, preparatory force adjustments should facilitate motor processing and thus shorten the LRP-R interval. Alternatively, if the level of response force is specified before the central activation of the response hand, the duration of this interval should be unaffected by advance information about response force. The former outcome would be expected if force and directional precues operate on the same mechanisms. By contrast, the latter outcome would further strengthen the view that different mechanisms are involved in the specification of response force and movement direction ~MacKay & Bonnet, 1990; Riehle & Requin, 1995!. EXPERIMENT 1 Experiment 1 consisted of two conditions with statistical analyses performed separately. In the force condition different required force levels were precued, whereas the direction condition served as a control condition in which movement direction was precued. Method Participants In the force condition 12 female and 8 male students ~mean age 25 years, range years! took part in one practice session lasting about 30 min and one 2.5-hr experimental session on the next day. They received either course credit or money ~10 DM per hour! for their participation. All participants had normal or correctedto-normal vision and the mean handedness score ~Oldfield, 1971! was 65 ~SD 34!. In the direction condition a fresh sample of 12 female and 8 male students ~mean age 26 years, range years! was recruited. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and a mean handedness score of 72 ~SD 47!. Procedure In the force condition each participant was to produce five brisk finger flexions at the beginning of the practice and of the experimental session to establish the participant s average maximal voluntary force. The practice session consisted of five blocks of 40 trials. The experimental session comprised 16 blocks with the first block considered as practice. After each block participants could take a short rest and initiate the next block when they felt ready to proceed. They were told the meaning of the precues and were instructed to use the precue information for response preparation. The digits 1, 2, 3, and 4 served as imperative stimuli and were mapped one-to-one onto the four response alternatives, which con-

3 Preparation of force and movement direction 509 sisted of a weak or strong flexion of the right or left index finger in the force condition. A criterion of 30% maximal voluntary force was defined for each participant. In the low force condition, participants were asked to produce responses with a peak force below this criterion, whereas in the high force condition the peak force had to be above this criterion. In the direction condition, maximal voluntary force was not determined. Here the response alternatives consisted either of a flexion or extension with the right or left index finger. The assignment of the stimuli to the responses was counterbalanced across participants to avoid systematic S-R mapping effects between the ordinal position of the digits and the identity of the responses. Each stimulus and each type of precue appeared equally often within a single block. The presentation order of stimuli and precue types was randomized over the trial sequence in a single block. Each trial started with the presentation of a fixation cross in the center of the computer screen. After 450 ms the fixation cross was replaced by a precue for 300 ms. This precue provided verbal advance information about the required force level ~strong or weak! or about the required direction ~up or down!, respectively, or consisted of seven plus signs when no advance information was given. After a constant foreperiod of 1,400 ms the fixation cross was replaced by the imperative stimulus for 300 ms. Then, 1,100 ms after stimulus offset, feedback was provided on the screen for 400 ms. In the case of a correct response, the word Korrekt replaced the fixation cross. Appropriate feedback was provided in the case of a wrong response ~anticipations, i.e., RTs 100 ms, misses, i.e., RTs 1,000 ms, wrong hand responses, responses with wrong force level, responses in the wrong direction, or bimanual responses!. The next trial started 2,300 ms after feedback offset with the presentation of the fixation cross. Mean RT and the percentage of response errors were computed for each block and given as feedback at the end of each block. Participants were asked to keep their eyes on the fixation cross and not to blink or move their eyes except during the feedback and intertrial period. They were also instructed to respond quickly and to avoid response errors. Stimuli and Precues The white-colored precues and stimuli were presented in the center of the screen on a blue background. The size of the fixation cross was As precues the words stark ~strong, ! or schwach ~weak, ! were displayed in the force condition. In the direction condition the words oben ~up, ! or unten ~down, ! served as precues. The noninformative precue consisted of seven plus signs ~ !. The imperative stimulus ~ ! consisted of a digit from 1 to 4. A constant viewing distance of 80 cm was ensured by a fixed chin rest. Recording of LRP, Electrooculogram (EOG), Electromyogram (EMG), and Response Force Electroencephalogram ~EEG! activity from C39 and C49 ~cf. Coles, 1989!, designating sites 4 cm to the left and right of Cz along the intramural line, and vertical EOG from above the left eye were recorded with the left mastoid as a common reference. 2 Horizontal 2 We also recorded EEG signals at Pz and Cz in each experiment of the present study. Because these data, however, are not particular relevant for assessing the present hypothesis, we will not report the corresponding results in order to save space. EOG was recorded differentially between the left and right outer canthi. EMG activity was recorded at ventral forearm sites 2.5 cm to the left and right of the point that trisected the wrist elbow distance. High-pass filters were set at 0.1 Hz for EMG and vertical EOG recordings and 0.01 Hz for all other channels. Low-pass filters were set at 30 Hz for all channels. The EMG was rectified offline. After averaging, the waveforms were smoothed by lowpass filtering ~cut-off frequency 4 Hz!. Recordings were made with Ag0AgCl electrodes and Acqua-Gel electrode electrolyte. All signals were digitized at a rate of 100 Hz for 3,000 ms starting 200 ms before precue onset. Electrode impedance was kept below 10 kv for EMG leads and below 5 kv for all other sites. For data analysis, all trials with incorrect responses or with ocular artifacts were discarded. A threshold of 70 mv in all EOG channels was chosen for artifact rejection. Because there were no systematic effects of precue information on heog activity, potential precue effects on the LRP cannot be attributed to heog artifacts. The LRP was determined as suggested by Coles ~1989! and averaging of the mean waveforms was performed either timelocked to the stimulus or to the response to compute the S-LRP and the LRP-R, respectively ~Osman & Moore, 1993!. The averaged waveforms were filtered before their onsets were estimated. The jackknife method ~Miller, Patterson, & Ulrich, 1998! was used to estimate the onset latency differences for the stimulus- and for the response-locked LRP. As recommended by Miller et al. the onset of the S-LRP ~LRP-R! was defined as the point in time when a criterion of 50% ~30%! of the amplitude was reached. The force-time function of a response was recorded in each trial by means of force-sensitive keys, which allowed for near-isometric recordings of index finger flexions and extensions. A cantilever beam ~ mm!with a minimal movement was held by an adjustable clamp at one end, while the other remained free. Strain gauges were attached near the fixed end of the leaf spring. One force key was used for each index finger. The fingertip was located in an adjustable thimble-like holder attached to the free end of the leaf spring. A response was registered as soon as the force of a flexion or an extension exceeded a criterion force of 50 cn from the baseline force level being defined as the average force during a 200-ms interval before precue presentation. Both forearms and palms rested on boards. Results Error Rates and RT In the force condition the overall percentage of errors was 4.1% comprising 1.4% misses ~RTs 1,000 ms!, 0.4% anticipations ~RTs 100 ms!, 1.0% wrong-sided keypresses, and 1.3% keypresses with an incorrect magnitude of force. Analysis of variance ~ANOVA! revealed a strong effect of precue category on RT, F~1,19! 329.2, p.001; with precue information RTs were 95 ms shorter compared with the non-informative precue condition ~mean RTs were 461 and 556 ms, respectively!. In the direction condition the overall error rate was 4.2%, which was due to 0.9% misses ~RTs 1,000 ms!, 0.8% anticipations ~RTs 100 ms!, 0.9% wrong-sided keypresses, and 1.6% keypresses in the wrong direction. The percentage of errors was similar to that of the force condition. Again a significant effect of precue category on mean RT was obtained, F~1,19! 336.4, p.001. Mean RT was 531 ms without directional advance information and decreased to 429 ms when this information was provided. The size of this effect ~102 ms! was similar to the one obtained in the force condition. A between-subject ANOVA comparing the effects between the force

4 510 H. Müller-Gethmann et al. and the direction condition revealed that RT was affected neither by condition, F~1,38! 1.87, p.18, nor by the interaction of condition with precue, F~1,38!.91, p.34. LRP Latencies The stimulus-locked LRPs for the two precue conditions of the force condition can be seen in Figure 1 ~left-hand upper panel!. The mean S-LRP latency was 267 ms with precue information about force and 340 ms without advance information; this difference of ms ~95% confidence interval! was highly significant, t~19! 6.84, p.001. This result extends the finding of Leuthold et al. ~1996!, who reported a similar reduction for precues about movement direction. The response-locked LRPs for the two precue conditions of the force condition are shown in Figure 1 ~right-hand upper panel!. Surprisingly, in both conditions LRP activity emerged at about the same point in time before the onset of the response. More specifically, the duration of the LRP-R interval was 178 ms with precue information, and 186 ms without it; this difference of ms was insignificant, t~19! 1.49, p.1. This result indicates that advance information about response force does not shorten the LRP-R interval. Hence, the finding of Leuthold et al. ~1996! that advance information about movement direction shortens the LRP-R interval does not seem to generalize to advance information about response force. Figure 1 depicts the stimulus-locked LRP ~left-hand lower panel! and the response-locked LRP ~right-hand lower panel! for the two precue conditions of the direction condition. The S-LRP was 307 ms for the non-informative precue and was reduced by ms to 248 ms for the direction precue, t~19! 7.1, p.001. In contrast to the results obtained in the force condition, the LRP-R interval was significantly shorter with precue information about movement direction ~157 ms! than without it ~182 ms!, the effect being ms, t~19! 3.6, p.01. Both results corroborate the findings obtained by Leuthold et al. ~1996!. Motor Time To assess whether advance information on movement direction affected even distal processes of the motor system, we averaged EMG time-locked to the onset of the overt response for the direction condition. The EMG-response interval, namely the motor time, was 91.3 ms when prior information on movement direction was given and 91.8 ms without this information. This difference of ms was not significant, t~19! 0.96, p 0.1. Therefore, advance information on movement direction did not shorten late processes within the motor system. Discussion The two conditions of Experiment 1 replicated the precuing effect of response force and movement direction on RT. Moreover, in both conditions the S-LRP interval was shorter when advance information was provided, which indicates an increase in speed of those processes that precede the central activation of the response Figure 1. Grand mean of the lateralized readiness potentials of Experiment 1. S onset of the imperative stimulus; R onset of the overt response. Left-hand panels: Averages are time locked to the onset of the imperative stimulus. Right-hand panels: Averages are time locked to the onset of the overt response. Upper panels: Force condition. Lower panels: Direction condition.

5 Preparation of force and movement direction 511 hand. These processes may involve early motoric stages such as those involved in response selection and perhaps perceptual stages. The precuing effect on the LRP-R interval of movement direction reported by Leuthold et al. ~1996! was also replicated. Interestingly, however, the response-locked LRP was unaffected by precue information about response force. These results imply that precue information about movement direction speeds up motoric processes, which follow the central activation of the response hand yet precede the EMG onset, as the analysis of the response-locked EMG indicates. However, precue information about response force seems not to facilitate these more distal motoric processes in the stimulus-response chain. It might be argued that force precues did not affect late preparatory stages because the required force level of the response was more difficult to specify than its direction and that therefore participants were less able to preprogram force at a later stage of movement preparation. For example, force but not direction may be regarded as a continuous movement parameter and therefore, in contrast to the specification of movement direction, participants have to select an appropriate force level in accordance with the experimentally predefined force criterion. It seems possible that this more complex selection task hampers the specification of force at a later level of movement preparation. If so, the precue effect should be smaller in the force than in the direction condition. The data, however, do not support this view because the precuing effect did not differ between both conditions. It should also be mentioned that the absence of any effect of the force precue on the LRP-R interval cannot be due to hierarchical, serial programming ~cf. Rosenbaum, 1980!. According to a hierarchical model one may assume that the direction of a movement has to be programmed before force can be specified. However, in the present experiment movement direction was always known before stimulus onset and thus could be programmed as a default parameter. Therefore, the absence of any effect of the force precue on the LRP-R interval cannot be attributed to hierarchical motor programming. According to the results of Experiment 1, precue information about movement direction and response force seems to produce differential effects on the LRP-R interval. However, in face of the insignificant effect of the force precue on the LRP-R interval, one might be tempted to argue that this null effect merely reflects a lack of statistical power that disguises a real effect. This suspicion receives some support from the fact that the LRP-R interval obtained was somewhat shorter with precue information than without it. EXPERIMENT 2 In an attempt to rule out the interpretation of insufficient statistical power, we conducted Experiment 2, in which both experimental conditions were performed by each participant. Thus, if the same pattern of results emerged again, this finding would argue against the idea that the insignificant effect of the force precue on the LRP-R interval merely reflects a lack of statistical power. Method Participants A fresh sample of 18 female and 14 male students participated ~mean age 25 years, range years!. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and a mean handedness score of 81 ~SD 37!. Procedure The procedure was virtually identical to that of Experiment 1. The two precue conditions ~force vs. directional precue condition! were performed successively with the order of administration counterbalanced across participants. To further increase the similarity between the two conditions, half of the participants in the direction condition always responded with high force and the other half always with low force; in the force condition half of the participants always responded with an extension and the other half always with a flexion. To avoid cross-over effects from the first to the second condition, a different set of letters served as imperative stimuli in each condition. Participants performed a practice session to familiarize themselves with the mapping of the letters to the responses. The practice session consisted of 8 blocks comprising 40 trials each. The experimental session consisted of 16 blocks; the same letters and the same mapping as in the practice session were employed. Stimuli, Precues, and Recordings Precues and recordings were the same as in Experiment 1. Uppercase letters ~ ! instead of digits were employed as imperative stimuli. For each participant eight letters were randomly selected from the following set of letters, which by inspection appear minimally confusable: A, C, E, H, L, M, P, S, T, U, and X. Four letters were used for the force condition and the remaining ones for the direction condition. Results Error Rates and RT The overall error rate was 8.8%, which was due to 2.8% misses ~RTs 1,000 ms!, 0.8% anticipations ~RTs 100 ms!, 3.2% wrong-sided keypresses, 1.5% keypresses with an incorrect magnitude of force, and 0.5% keypresses in the wrong direction. This higher rate than that found in Experiment 1 can be attributed to the more complex letter-to-response mapping in Experiment 2. A twoway ANOVA with factors precue condition ~force vs. direction! and advance information ~without vs. with! was performed on RT. There was a highly significant main effect of advance information, F~1,31! 405.1, p.001. Mean RT was 577 ms without and 471 ms with advance information. Note that the size of the precue effect ~106 ms! was similar to the one obtained in Experiment 1. There was neither a main effect of condition nor a significant interaction of the two factors, Fs 1. The respective mean RT in the force condition was 582 and 475 ms; in the direction condition it was 573 and 468 ms. LRP Latencies The LRP waveforms for the two conditions are shown in Figure 2. In both precue conditions, the S-LRP interval was again significantly shorter with advance information. In the force condition the effect was ms ~left-hand upper panel!; the mean S-LRP interval was 282 ms with precue information and 368 ms without it, t~31! 9.0, p.001. When advance information about the direction was provided, the S-LRP interval decreased by ms, from 352 ms when no prior information was provided to 285 ms when prior information was available, t~31! 6.32, p.001 ~left-hand lower panel!. As in Experiment 1, the LRP-R interval was shorter when the precue provided information about the direction of the movement ~137 ms! compared with the non-informative precue ~152 ms!; this difference of ms was again statistically reliable, t~31!

6 512 H. Müller-Gethmann et al. Figure 2. Grand mean of the lateralized readiness potentials of Experiment 2. S onset of the imperative stimulus; R onset of the overt response. Left-hand panels: Averages are time locked to the onset of the imperative stimulus. Right-hand panels: Averages are time locked to the onset of the overt response. Upper panels: Force condition. Lower panels: Direction condition. 2.2, p.05 ~right-hand lower panel!. More interestingly and consistent with Experiment 1, the LRP-R interval did not decrease with precue information in the force condition, t~31! 1.2, p.1 ~right-hand upper panel!; the corresponding intervals were 150 and 144 ms with and without advance information, respectively. The 95% confidence interval for this difference was ms. It is important to note that the precue effect of 15 ms obtained on the LRP-R interval in the direction condition was virtually unaffected by the degree of low-pass filtering. Reanalysis of the data with cut-off frequencies of 4, 8, and 12 Hz revealed an effect size of 14.7, 14.2, and 13.5 ms, respectively. This outcome is consistent with the notion that the difference between the S-LRP onsets in the two precue conditions are not influenced by any factor that systematically biases both LRP onsets to the same extent ~e.g., cut-off frequency!. Nevertheless the t-value associated with each difference decreased with increasing cut-off frequency, though each t-value was significant. Such a decrease in the significance level is expected, because the signal-to-noise ratio worsens as the cut-off frequency of the low-pass filter is increased. Motor Time As in Experiment 1, we assessed whether precue information on movement direction shortened the motor time. The EMG-response interval was 71.3 ms when prior information on movement direction was given and 72.6 ms without advance information. This difference of ms was not significant, t~31! 0.94, p 0.1. Thus, consistent with Experiment 1, advance information about movement direction did not influence the speed of late motoric processes. Discussion Experiment 2 replicated the pattern of results obtained in Experiment 1. In particular, the findings concerning the LRP-R interval confirm the former outcome. Precue information about movement direction again reduced the LRP-R interval. In contrast, the LRP-R interval was not shortened by advance information about response force. GENERAL DISCUSSION Like Leuthold et al. ~1996!, we combined the precuing technique with the recording of the LRP. We not only successfully replicated but also considerably extended their findings. As in their study, our participants received prior information about the forthcoming response alternative. In contrast to Leuthold et al., we identified the effects of advance information not only about movement direction but also about the required force level of the response. Consistent with the bulk of behavioral studies, advance information about either movement parameter greatly shortened RT ~cf. Requin et al., 1991!. This RT shortening supports the notion that covert prepa-

7 Preparation of force and movement direction 513 ratory processes benefit from movement advance information to preprogram the response. Because RT encompasses all internal processes from stimulus input to the onset of the associated response, it is impossible to infer from the RT shortening alone whether precue information affects early or late processes. Therefore, we used the LRP to narrow down the possible loci of the RT savings. The results of both experiments revealed clearly that advance information about movement direction and about response force exerts a differential precuing effect on the LRP. Although precue information about both parameters shortened the S-LRP interval, only advance information concerning movement direction also shortened the LRP-R interval. From this pattern of results we can infer that precue information speeds up early processes ~e.g., perceptual and presumably early motoric processes! due to the reduction of stimulus and response uncertainty, but not always late processes, which begin after the central activation of the response hand. Most important for this study, the present results clearly suggest that these late processes gain a speed advantage from advance information about movement direction but not from advance information about force. This differential precuing effect on the LRP-R interval is compatible with the view that distinct neuronal mechanisms are involved in the preparation of different movement parameters such as direction and force ~Riehle & Requin, 1995!. It may appear surprising that the duration of the LRP-R interval is insensitive to prior information about response force. To account for the null-effect, one may argue that force is not specified in advance but rather achieved by a feedback-controlled mechanism ~e.g., Gordon & Ghez, 1987!, which cannot utilize advance information to enhance performance. Such an account, however, seems to be incompatible with neurophysiological evidence suggesting that advance information about force modulates single-cell activity within the motor system ~e.g., Riehle & Requin, 1995!. As reviewed by Ashe ~1997!, the activity of cells within the primary motor cortex is correlated with the magnitude of force, suggesting an involvement of the primary motor cortex in force control. It seems plausible to assume that this control process benefits from advance information about force, reducing the time taken up by relatively late motoric processes. The present results, however, render this view unlikely. One possibility is that preparatory processes in the primary cortex concerning response force control are not responsible for the RT speedup. Another possibility is that preparatory processes within the primary motor cortex can operate only on certain movement parameters. The present results suggest that these preparatory processes are sensitive to prior information about movement direction but not about the magnitude of force. Consistent with this view, Riehle and Requin ~1995! reported that activity changes of many more neurons within the primary and premotor cortex were related to movement direction than to response force during the preparatory period. Consequently, the present results provide indirect evidence for the idea that the activity changes reported by Riehle and Requin also contribute to the precuing effect on the late RT portion. Because the LRP-R interval encompasses the duration of all processes between the central activation of the response hand and the overt response itself, one could argue that the RT speedup emerging from the directional precues resides within the executionrelated part of the motor system. The directional precue allows the specification of the appropriate muscle groups and thus tensing of these muscles, which in turn may shorten motor time, namely the interval between EMG onset and the onset of the overt response. Motor time, however, was virtually unaffected by directional advance information. The present finding, of course, does not preclude the possibility that the RT benefit of the directional precue is mediated by neuronal structures within the cortico-spinal tract. A recent study ~Hasbroucq et al., 1999!, however, using transcranial magnetic stimulation, renders this possibility rather unlikely. According to this study movement preparation ~such as the selection of muscle groups! is more centrally located than the cortico-spinal tract. Force and direction precues may exert differential effects on the nonlateralized and the lateralized portion of the readiness potential. Before the LRP emerges both hemispheres become activated and this is reflected in a negative going shift of the readiness potential ~cf. Coles, 1989!. It seems likely that this bilateral activation represents a more central preprogramming process that does not affect the interval from the onset of the LRP to the onset of the overt response ~Ulrich et al., 1998!. Central programming processes may especially benefit from advance information about movement force. Consequently, one should expect that force precues facilitate these processes more than direction precues. Accordingly, the S-LRP interval should be reduced more by force than by direction precues. An additional analysis of Experiment 2, however, provides no support for this view. The S-LRP interval was not reduced differentially albeit that an effect in the predicted direction could be observed but failed to be significant. 3 In conclusion, then, the present results suggest the following model of the specification of movement direction and response force, which makes certain assumptions about information processing within the motor system. According to this model, the response is selected in compliance with stimulus information in premotoric processing stages. Furthermore, the ensuing motor processes are divided into an early processing stage that is not limbspecific and into a late stage that is limb-specific ~cf. Miller, Coles, & Chakraborty, 1996; Ulrich et al., 1998!. The LRP onset indicates the onset of the limb-specific motor system. Movement direction, and thus the selection of the appropriate muscle groups is specified, at least partially, within this late system. In contrast, the amount of required muscle activation is specified entirely within the early motor stage. In the case of precue information about response force, the processing time within the non limb-specific system is reduced whereas in the case of precue information about movement direction, the processing time within both systems is shortened. 3 Precues shortened the S-LRP interval ms more in the force than in the direction condition. This difference, however, was not significant, t~31! 1.42, p.17. REFERENCES Ashe, J. ~1997!. Force and the motor cortex. Behavioral Brain Research, 86, Coles, M. G. H. ~1989!. Modern mind brain reading: Psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. Psychophysiology, 26, Deiber, M. P., Ibanez, V., Sadato, N., & Hallett, M. ~1996!. Cerebral structures participating in motor preparation in humans: A positron emission tomography study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 75, De Jong, R., Wierda, M., Mulder, G., & Mulder, L. J. M. ~1988!. Use of

8 514 H. Müller-Gethmann et al. partial stimulus information in response processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, Dettmers, C., Fink, G. R., Lemon, R. N., Stephan, K. M., Passingham, R. E., Silbersweig, D., Holmes, A., Ridding, M. C., Brooks, D. J., & Frackowiak, R. S. ~1995!. Relation between cerebral activity and force in the motor areas of the human brain. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74, Gordon, J., & Ghez, C. ~1987!. Trajectory control in targeted force impulses: II. Pulse height control. Experimental Brain Research, 67, Gratton, G., Bosco, C. M., Kramer, A. F., Coles, M. G., Wickens, C. D., & Donchin, E. ~1990!. Event-related brain potentials as indices of information extraction and response priming. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 75, Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., Sirevaag, E. J., Eriksen, C. W., & Donchin, E. ~1988!. Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: A psychophysiological analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, Hasbroucq, T., Osman, A., Possamaï, C. A., Burle, B., Carron, S., Latour, S., & Mouret, I. ~1999!. Cortico-spinal inhibition reflects time but not event preparation: Neuronal mechanisms of preparation dissociated by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Acta Psychologica, 100, Kristeva, R., Cheyne, D., & Deecke, L. ~1991!. Neuromagnetic fields accompanying unilateral and bilateral voluntary movements: Topography and analysis of cortical sources. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 81, Leuthold, H., Sommer, W., & Ulrich, R. ~1996!. Partial advance information and response preparation: Inferences from the lateralized readiness potential. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, MacKay, W. A., & Bonnet M. ~1990!. CNV, stretch reflex, and reaction time correlates of preparation for movement direction and force. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 85, Miller, J., Coles, M. G. H., & Chakraborty, S. ~1996!. Dissociation between behavioral and psychophysiological measures of response preparation. Acta Psychologica, 94, Miller, J., & Hackley, S. A. ~1992!. Electrophysiological evidence for temporal overlap among contingent mental processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, Miller, J., Patterson, T., & Ulrich, R. ~1998!. Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences. Psychophysiology, 35, Oldfield, R. C. ~1971!. The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, Osman, A., & Moore, C. M. ~1993!. The locus of dual-task interference: Psychological refractory effects on movement-related brain potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, Osman, A., Moore, C. M., & Ulrich, R. ~1995!. Bisecting RT with lateralized readiness potentials: Precue effects after the LRP onset. Acta Psychologica, 90, Requin, J., Brener, J., & Ring, C. ~1991!. Preparation for action. In J. R. Jennings & M. G. H. Coles ~Eds.!, Handbook of cognitive psychophysiology: Central and autonomic nervous system approaches ~pp !. New York: Wiley. Riehle, A., & Requin, J. ~1989!. Monkey primary motor and premotor cortex: Single-cell activity related to prior information about direction and extent of an intended movement. Journal of Neurophysiology, 61, Riehle, A., MacKay, W. A., & Requin, J. ~1994!. Are extent and force independent movement parameters? Preparation- and movement-related neuronal activity in the monkey cortex. Experimental Brain Research, 9, Riehle, A., & Requin, J. ~1995!. Neuronal correlates of the specification of movement direction and force in four cortical areas of the monkey. Behavioral Brain Research, 70, Rizolatti, G., Luppino, G., & Matelli, M. ~1998!. The organization of the cortical motor system: New concepts. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 106, Rosenbaum, D. A. ~1980!. Human movement initiation: Specification of arm, direction, and extent. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, Rosenbaum, D. A. ~1983!. The movement precuing technique: Assumptions, applications, and extensions. In R. A. Magill ~Ed.!, Memory and control in motor behavior ~pp !. Amsterdam: North-Holland. Sasaki, K., Gemba, H., & Tsujimoto, T. ~1990!. Cortical field potential associated with hand movement on warning-imperative visual stimulus and cerebellum in the monkey. Brain Research, 519, Ulrich, R., Leuthold, H., & Sommer, W. ~1998!. Motor programming of response force and movement direction: An ERP analysis. Psychophysiology, 35, van der Molen, M. W., Bashore, T. R., Halliday, R., & Callaway, E. ~1991!. Chronopsychophysiology: Mental chronometry augmented by psychophysiological time markers. In J. R. Jennings & M. G. H. Coles ~Eds.!, Handbook of cognitive psychophysiology: Central and autonomic nervous system approaches ~pp !. New York: Wiley. ~Received January 22, 1999; Accepted August 13, 1999!

Motor programming of response force and movement direction

Motor programming of response force and movement direction Psychophysiology, 35 ~1998!, 721 728. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1998 Society for Psychophysiological Research Motor programming of response force and movement direction

More information

The lateralized readiness potential and response kinetics in response-time tasks

The lateralized readiness potential and response kinetics in response-time tasks Psychophysiology, 38 ~2001!, 777 786. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2001 Society for Psychophysiological Research The lateralized readiness potential and response kinetics in

More information

The Effects of Temporal Preparation on Reaction Time

The Effects of Temporal Preparation on Reaction Time University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School January 2013 The Effects of Temporal Preparation on Reaction Time Glen Robert Forester University of South

More information

Beneficial effects of ambiguous precues: Parallel motor preparation or reduced premotoric processing time?

Beneficial effects of ambiguous precues: Parallel motor preparation or reduced premotoric processing time? Psychophysiology, 41 (24), 231 244. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 24 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 1.1111/j.1469-8986.24.155.x Beneficial effects of ambiguous

More information

The Locus of Temporal Preparation Effects: Evidence from the Psychological Refractory Period Paradigm. Karin M. Bausenhart.

The Locus of Temporal Preparation Effects: Evidence from the Psychological Refractory Period Paradigm. Karin M. Bausenhart. Temporal Preparation 1 Running Head: TEMPORAL PREPARATION The Locus of Temporal Preparation Effects: Evidence from the Psychological Refractory Period Paradigm Karin M. Bausenhart Bettina Rolke University

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article. MECHANISMS OF PRIMING BY MASKED STIMULI: Inferences From Event-Related Brain Potentials

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article. MECHANISMS OF PRIMING BY MASKED STIMULI: Inferences From Event-Related Brain Potentials Research Article MECHANISMS OF PRIMING BY MASKED STIMULI: Inferences From Event-Related Brain Potentials Humboldt-University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany Abstract A metacontrast procedure was combined with

More information

Programming of time-to-peak force for brief isometric force pulses: Effects on reaction time

Programming of time-to-peak force for brief isometric force pulses: Effects on reaction time THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2006, 59 (7), 1277 1305 Programming of time-to-peak force for brief isometric force pulses: Effects on reaction time Hannes Schröter University of Tübingen,

More information

Are fingers special? Evidence about movement preparation from event-related brain potentials

Are fingers special? Evidence about movement preparation from event-related brain potentials Psychophysiology, 4 (3), 7 16. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 3 Society for Psychophysiological Research Are fingers special? Evidence about movement preparation from event-related

More information

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events Psychophysiology, 40 (2003), 827 831. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2003 Society for Psychophysiological Research BRIEF REPT Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the

More information

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction Proceedings of the First International Conference on Complex Medical Engineering (CME2005) May 15-18, 2005, Takamatsu, Japan (Organized Session No. 20). Paper No. 150, pp. 715-719. Event-Related Potentials

More information

An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search

An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search Steven Phillips (steve@ni.aist.go.jp) Neuroscience Research Institute (AIST), Tsukuba Central 2, 1-1-1 Umezono, Tsukuba, Ibaraki 305-8568

More information

Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized. Readiness Potential. Dianna Monteith. Saint Thomas University

Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized. Readiness Potential. Dianna Monteith. Saint Thomas University Extraversion and the LRP 1 Running head: EXTRAVERSION AND THE LRP Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized Readiness Potential Dianna Monteith Saint Thomas

More information

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: A Comparison of Electrophysiological and Other Neuroimaging Approaches Leun J. Otten Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology University

More information

The functional locus of the lateralized readiness potential

The functional locus of the lateralized readiness potential Psychophysiology, 41 (2004), 220 230. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2004 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469.8986.2004.00150.x The functional locus

More information

In Search of the Point of No Return: The Control of Response Processes

In Search of the Point of No Return: The Control of Response Processes Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1990, Vol. 16, No. 1, 164-182 In Search of the Point of No Return: The Control of Response Processes Copyright 1990 by the American

More information

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling Supplementary materials 1 Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling analyses placed the source of the No Go N2 component in the dorsal ACC, near the ACC source

More information

Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences

Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences Psychophysiology, 35 ~1998!, 99 115. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1998 Society for Psychophysiological Research METHODOLOGY Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency

More information

Cortical Control of Movement

Cortical Control of Movement Strick Lecture 2 March 24, 2006 Page 1 Cortical Control of Movement Four parts of this lecture: I) Anatomical Framework, II) Physiological Framework, III) Primary Motor Cortex Function and IV) Premotor

More information

Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return

Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return David J. Prime and Pierre Jolicoeur Abstract & Here we examined the relationship between inhibition of return (IOR) and response-selection

More information

Auditory Warning Signals Affect Mechanisms of Response Selection

Auditory Warning Signals Affect Mechanisms of Response Selection Research Article Auditory Warning Signals Affect Mechanisms of Response Selection Evidence from a Simon Task Rico Fischer, 1 Franziska Plessow, 1 and Andrea Kiesel 2 1 Technische Universität Dresden, Germany

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Item and Category-Based Attentional Control During Search for Real-World Objects: Can You Find the Pants Among the Pans? Rebecca Nako,

More information

Accessory stimulation in the time course of visuomotor information processing: Stimulus intensity effects on reaction time and response force

Accessory stimulation in the time course of visuomotor information processing: Stimulus intensity effects on reaction time and response force Acta Psychologica 120 (2005) 1 18 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy Accessory stimulation in the time course of visuomotor information processing: Stimulus intensity effects on reaction time and response

More information

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Activation of effect codes in response planning - 1 - To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Michael Ziessler

More information

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task Psychophysiology, 44 (2007), 913 917. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2007 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00561.x BRIEF REPORT Reward prediction

More information

Neurophysiology of systems

Neurophysiology of systems Neurophysiology of systems Motor cortex (voluntary movements) Dana Cohen, Room 410, tel: 7138 danacoh@gmail.com Voluntary movements vs. reflexes Same stimulus yields a different movement depending on context

More information

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER Memory & Cognition 2008, 36 (7), 1236-1247 doi: 10.3758/MC/36.7.1236 An action sequence held in memory can interfere with response selection of a target stimulus, but does not interfere with response activation

More information

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Psychon Bull Rev (2013) 20:296 301 DOI 10.3758/s13423-012-0353-4 BRIEF REPORT Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Risa Sawaki & Steven J. Luck Published online: 20 December 2012 #

More information

Interaction of facial expressions and familiarity: ERP evidence

Interaction of facial expressions and familiarity: ERP evidence Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Biological Psychology 77 (2008) 138 149 www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho Interaction of facial expressions and familiarity: ERP evidence Nele Wild-Wall 1, *,

More information

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION Jordan Schoenherr, Craig Leth-Steensen, and William M. Petrusic psychophysics.lab@gmail.com, craig_leth_steensen@carleton.ca, bpetrusi@carleton.ca Carleton

More information

Attentional and anatomical considerations for the representation of simple stimuli in visual short-term memory: evidence from human electrophysiology

Attentional and anatomical considerations for the representation of simple stimuli in visual short-term memory: evidence from human electrophysiology Psychological Research (2009) 73:222 232 DOI 10.1007/s00426-008-0214-y ORIGINAL ARTICLE Attentional and anatomical considerations for the representation of simple stimuli in visual short-term memory: evidence

More information

Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the

Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the 1 Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(3), 835-54. doi:10.1037/a0012726

More information

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES Carryl L. Baldwin and Joseph T. Coyne Department of Psychology Old Dominion University

More information

Peripheral facial paralysis (right side). The patient is asked to close her eyes and to retract their mouth (From Heimer) Hemiplegia of the left side. Note the characteristic position of the arm with

More information

Relative contribution of expectancy and immediate arousal to the facilitatory effect of an auditory accessory stimulus

Relative contribution of expectancy and immediate arousal to the facilitatory effect of an auditory accessory stimulus Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research (2004) 37: 1161-1174 Priming by an auditory stimulus ISSN 0100-879X 1161 Relative contribution of expectancy and immediate arousal to the facilitatory

More information

tion in Time course and distribution of movement-related potentials in a movement precueing task

tion in Time course and distribution of movement-related potentials in a movement precueing task Brain (1996), 119, 1689-1704 tion in Time course and distribution of movement-related potentials in a movement precueing task P. Praamstra, 1 A. S. Meyer, 3 A. R. Cools, 2 M. W. I. M. Horstink 1 and D.

More information

Location-specific versus hemisphere-specific adaptation of processing selectivity

Location-specific versus hemisphere-specific adaptation of processing selectivity Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2008, 15 (1), 135-140 doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.1.135 Location-specific versus hemisphere-specific adaptation of processing selectivity MIKE WENDT, RAINER H. KLUWE, AND INA VIETZE

More information

Ingroup categorization and response conflict: Interactive effects of target race, flanker compatibility, and infrequency on N2 amplitude

Ingroup categorization and response conflict: Interactive effects of target race, flanker compatibility, and infrequency on N2 amplitude Psychophysiology, 47 (2010), 596 601. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2010 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.00963.x BRIEF REPORT Ingroup categorization

More information

Do Rare Stimuli Evoke Large P3s by Being Unexpected? A Comparison of Oddball Effects Between Standard-Oddball and Prediction-Oddball Tasks

Do Rare Stimuli Evoke Large P3s by Being Unexpected? A Comparison of Oddball Effects Between Standard-Oddball and Prediction-Oddball Tasks Do Rare Stimuli Evoke Large P3s by Being Unexpected? A Comparison of Oddball Effects Between Standard-Oddball and Prediction-Oddball Tasks Rolf Verleger 1,2 and Kamila Śmigasiewicz 1 1 Department of Neurology,

More information

Readiness Potentials Related to Self-Initiated Movement and to Movement Preceded by Time Estimation: A Comparison

Readiness Potentials Related to Self-Initiated Movement and to Movement Preceded by Time Estimation: A Comparison Physiol. Res. 45:235-239, 1996 Readiness Potentials Related to Self-Initiated Movement and to Movement Preceded by Time Estimation: A Comparison M. KUKLETA, P. BUSER1,1. REKTOR, M. LAMARCHE1 M edical Faculty,

More information

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search Clayton Hickey 1,2, Vincent Di Lollo 2, and John J. McDonald 2 Abstract & Attentional selection of a target presented among

More information

Measurement of ERP latency differences: A comparison of single-participant and jackknife-based scoring methods

Measurement of ERP latency differences: A comparison of single-participant and jackknife-based scoring methods Psychophysiology, 45 (28), 25 274. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 27 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 1.1111/j.1469-8986.27.618.x Measurement of ERP latency differences:

More information

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention Sleep Deprivation and Selective Attention An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention Logan T. Trujillo, PhD 1 ; Steve Kornguth,

More information

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Cerebral Cortex March 2006;16:415-424 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi121 Advance Access publication June 15, 2005 Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Mika

More information

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes Biological Psychology 66 (2004) 1 20 The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes Bettina Forster, Martin Eimer School of

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

Conflict monitoring and feature overlap: Two sources of sequential modulations

Conflict monitoring and feature overlap: Two sources of sequential modulations Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (4), 742-748 Conflict monitoring and feature overlap: Two sources of sequential modulations Çağlar Akçay and Eliot Hazeltine University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa Recent

More information

Electrophysiological Evidence for Temporal Overlap Among Contingent Mental Processes

Electrophysiological Evidence for Temporal Overlap Among Contingent Mental Processes Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1992. Vol. 121. No. 2. 195-209 Copyright 1992 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-3445/92/S3.00 Electrophysiological Evidence for Temporal Overlap

More information

Accessory stimuli modulate effects of nonconscious priming

Accessory stimuli modulate effects of nonconscious priming Perception & Psychophysics 2007, 69 (1), 9-22 Accessory stimuli modulate effects of nonconscious priming RICO FISCHER, TORSTEN SCHUBERT, AND ROMAN LIEPELT Humboldt University, Berlin, Germany In a recent

More information

Motor Functions of Cerebral Cortex

Motor Functions of Cerebral Cortex Motor Functions of Cerebral Cortex I: To list the functions of different cortical laminae II: To describe the four motor areas of the cerebral cortex. III: To discuss the functions and dysfunctions of

More information

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Cognitive Brain Research 18 (2004) 255 272 Research report Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Audrey Duarte a, *, Charan Ranganath

More information

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Journal: Psychological Science Manuscript ID: PSCI-0-0.R Manuscript Type: Short report Date Submitted by the Author:

More information

Cortico-spinal inhibition re ects time but not event preparation: neural mechanisms of preparation dissociated by transcranial magnetic stimulation

Cortico-spinal inhibition re ects time but not event preparation: neural mechanisms of preparation dissociated by transcranial magnetic stimulation Acta Psychologica 101 (1999) 243±266 Cortico-spinal inhibition re ects time but not event preparation: neural mechanisms of preparation dissociated by transcranial magnetic stimulation Thierry Hasbroucq

More information

Running Head: VISUAL WORKING MEMORY LOAD AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL

Running Head: VISUAL WORKING MEMORY LOAD AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL Running Head: VISUAL WORKING MEMORY LOAD AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL Visual working memory load disrupts template-guided attentional selection during visual search Nick Berggren* and Martin Eimer Department

More information

International Journal of Psychophysiology

International Journal of Psychophysiology International Journal of Psychophysiology 75 (2010) 339 348 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect International Journal of Psychophysiology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ijpsycho Flanker

More information

Report. Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Martin Eimer 1, * and Anna Grubert 1 1

Report. Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Martin Eimer 1, * and Anna Grubert 1 1 Current Biology 24, 193 198, January 20, 2014 ª2014 The Authors http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.001 Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects Report Martin

More information

Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation

Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation Rongjun Yu a and Xiaolin Zhou a,b,c a Department of Psychology, Peking University, b State

More information

Study Guide for Test 3 Motor Control

Study Guide for Test 3 Motor Control Study Guide for Test 3 Motor Control These chapters come from Magill s (2010), Motor Learning and Control: Concepts and Application(9 th ) edition textbook. Additional materials regarding these key concepts

More information

Electrophysiological measurement of information flow during visual search

Electrophysiological measurement of information flow during visual search Psychophysiology, 00 (2015), 00 00. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright VC 2015 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12594 Electrophysiological measurement of information

More information

Impact of contingency manipulations on accessory stimulus effects

Impact of contingency manipulations on accessory stimulus effects Perception & Psychophysics 2007, 69 (7), 1117-1125 Impact of contingency manipulations on accessory stimulus effects ANDREA KIESEL Julius-Maximilians-Universität, Würzburg, Germany AND JEFF MILLER University

More information

CHAPTER 6 INTERFERENCE CANCELLATION IN EEG SIGNAL

CHAPTER 6 INTERFERENCE CANCELLATION IN EEG SIGNAL 116 CHAPTER 6 INTERFERENCE CANCELLATION IN EEG SIGNAL 6.1 INTRODUCTION Electrical impulses generated by nerve firings in the brain pass through the head and represent the electroencephalogram (EEG). Electrical

More information

The Physiology of the Senses Chapter 8 - Muscle Sense

The Physiology of the Senses Chapter 8 - Muscle Sense The Physiology of the Senses Chapter 8 - Muscle Sense www.tutis.ca/senses/ Contents Objectives... 1 Introduction... 2 Muscle Spindles and Golgi Tendon Organs... 3 Gamma Drive... 5 Three Spinal Reflexes...

More information

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),???-??? 794-799 What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? ULRICH MAYR University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon Schneider and

More information

Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex

Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (5), 834-839 Brief Reports Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex Jeffrey S. Johnson, Geoffrey F. Woodman, Elsie Braun, and Steven

More information

An investigation of the effect of preparation on response execution and inhibition in the go/nogo task

An investigation of the effect of preparation on response execution and inhibition in the go/nogo task University of Wollongong Research Online University of Wollongong Thesis Collection 1954-2016 University of Wollongong Thesis Collections 2005 An investigation of the effect of preparation on response

More information

The effect of response execution on P3 latency, reaction time, and movement time

The effect of response execution on P3 latency, reaction time, and movement time Psychophysiology, 36 ~1999!, 351 363. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1999 Society for Psychophysiological Research The effect of response execution on P3 latency, reaction time,

More information

Biological Psychology

Biological Psychology Biological Psychology 109 (2015) 73 85 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Biological Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho Deficit in late-stage contingent negative variation

More information

Detecting and identifying response-compatible stimuli

Detecting and identifying response-compatible stimuli Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997, 4 (1), 125-129 Detecting and identifying response-compatible stimuli JOCHEN MÜSSELER and BERNHARD HOMMEL Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research, Munich, Germany

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Response preparation and code overlap in dual tasks

Response preparation and code overlap in dual tasks Memory & Cognition 2005, 33 (6), 1085-1095 Response preparation and code overlap in dual tasks IRING KOCH and WOLFGANG PRINZ Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Munich, Germany

More information

The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences

The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences from the N170 adaptation effect Xiao-hua Cao 1, Chao Li 1, Carl M Gaspar 2, Bei Jiang 1 1. Department of Psychology,Zhejiang Normal

More information

Benjamin Blankertz Guido Dornhege Matthias Krauledat Klaus-Robert Müller Gabriel Curio

Benjamin Blankertz Guido Dornhege Matthias Krauledat Klaus-Robert Müller Gabriel Curio Benjamin Blankertz Guido Dornhege Matthias Krauledat Klaus-Robert Müller Gabriel Curio During resting wakefulness distinct idle rhythms located over various brain areas: around 10 Hz (alpha range) over

More information

Delayed working memory consolidation during the attentional blink

Delayed working memory consolidation during the attentional blink Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2002, 9 (4), 739-743 Delayed working memory consolidation during the attentional blink EDWARD K. VOGEL and STEVEN J. LUCK University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa After the detection

More information

Action Preparation Helps and Hinders Perception of Action

Action Preparation Helps and Hinders Perception of Action Action Preparation Helps and Hinders Perception of Action Clare Press 1,2, Elena Gherri 1, Cecilia Heyes 3, and Martin Eimer 1 Abstract Several theories of the mechanisms linking perception and action

More information

Electrophysiological evidence for preparatory reconfiguration before voluntary task switches but not cued task switches

Electrophysiological evidence for preparatory reconfiguration before voluntary task switches but not cued task switches Psychon Bull Rev (2014) 21:454 461 DOI 10.3758/s13423-013-0499-8 BRIEF REPORT Electrophysiological evidence for preparatory reconfiguration before voluntary task switches but not cued task switches Min-Suk

More information

Short article Intention and attention in ideomotor learning

Short article Intention and attention in ideomotor learning THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2009, 62 (2), 219 227 Short article Intention and attention in ideomotor learning Arvid Herwig Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences,

More information

Biological Psychology

Biological Psychology Biological Psychology 87 (2011) 25 34 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Biological Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho Dissociated roles of the anterior cingulate cortex

More information

The Mechanism of Valence-Space Metaphors: ERP Evidence for Affective Word Processing

The Mechanism of Valence-Space Metaphors: ERP Evidence for Affective Word Processing : ERP Evidence for Affective Word Processing Jiushu Xie, Ruiming Wang*, Song Chang Center for Studies of Psychological Application, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, Guangdong

More information

An Overview of BMIs. Luca Rossini. Workshop on Brain Machine Interfaces for Space Applications

An Overview of BMIs. Luca Rossini. Workshop on Brain Machine Interfaces for Space Applications An Overview of BMIs Luca Rossini Workshop on Brain Machine Interfaces for Space Applications European Space Research and Technology Centre, European Space Agency Noordvijk, 30 th November 2009 Definition

More information

Nerve Conduction Studies NCS

Nerve Conduction Studies NCS Nerve Conduction Studies NCS Nerve conduction studies are an essential part of an EMG examination. The clinical usefulness of NCS in the diagnosis of diffuse and local neuropathies has been thoroughly

More information

Precueing Spatial S-R Correspondence: Is There Regulation of Expected Response Conflict?

Precueing Spatial S-R Correspondence: Is There Regulation of Expected Response Conflict? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2008, Vol. 34, No. 4, 872 883 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.4.872

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

The control of attentional target selection in a colour/colour conjunction task

The control of attentional target selection in a colour/colour conjunction task Atten Percept Psychophys (2016) 78:2383 2396 DOI 10.3758/s13414-016-1168-6 The control of attentional target selection in a colour/colour conjunction task Nick Berggren 1 & Martin Eimer 1 Published online:

More information

Nerve Conduction Studies NCS

Nerve Conduction Studies NCS Nerve Conduction Studies NCS Nerve conduction studies are an essential part of an EMG examination. The clinical usefulness of NCS in the diagnosis of diffuse and local neuropathies has been thoroughly

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2001, 61: 175-179 Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions El bieta Szel¹g 1, Krystyna Rymarczyk 1 and Ernst Pöppel 2 1 Department

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

Psychological Research

Psychological Research Psychol Res (1984) 46:121-127 Psychological Research Springer-Verlag 1984 Research note: Peak velocity timing invariance Alan M. Wing I and Ed Miller 2 1 Medical Research Council Applied Psychology Unit,

More information

The path of visual attention

The path of visual attention Acta Psychologica 121 (2006) 199 209 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The path of visual attention James M. Brown a, *, Bruno G. Breitmeyer b, Katherine A. Leighty a, Hope I. Denney a a Department of Psychology,

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen The following full text is a publisher's version. For additional information about this publication click this link. http://hdl.handle.net/2066/22531

More information

Voluntary Movements. Lu Chen, Ph.D. MCB, UC Berkeley. Outline. Organization of the motor cortex (somatotopic) Corticospinal projection

Voluntary Movements. Lu Chen, Ph.D. MCB, UC Berkeley. Outline. Organization of the motor cortex (somatotopic) Corticospinal projection Voluntary Movements Lu Chen, Ph.D. MCB, UC Berkeley 1 Outline Organization of the motor cortex (somatotopic) Corticospinal projection Physiology of motor neurons Direction representation, population coding

More information

Seizure onset can be difficult to asses in scalp EEG. However, some tools can be used to increase the seizure onset activity over the EEG background:

Seizure onset can be difficult to asses in scalp EEG. However, some tools can be used to increase the seizure onset activity over the EEG background: This presentation was given during the Dianalund Summer School on EEG and Epilepsy, July 24, 2012. The main purpose of this introductory talk is to show the possibilities of improved seizure onset analysis

More information

Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry

Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry International Journal of Undergraduate Research and Creative Activities Volume 5 Article 3 June 2013 Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry Whitney R. Arthur Central Washington

More information

Preparation for Action: one of the Key Functions of Motor Cortex

Preparation for Action: one of the Key Functions of Motor Cortex Chapter 8: Preparation for Action 1 Preparation for Action: one of the Key Functions of Motor Cortex Alexa Riehle Institut de Neurosciences Cognitives de la Méditerranée INCM CNRS, Marseille, France INCM

More information

Differentiation of conversive sensory loss and malingering by P300 in a modified oddball task

Differentiation of conversive sensory loss and malingering by P300 in a modified oddball task Pain 0 0 0 0 0 p Website publication January NeuroReport, () WE applied the methodology of evoked potentials (EP) to reveal the functional level of abnormality in a patient with circumscribed complete

More information

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, Vol. 28, No. 2, 396 409 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.396

More information

Enhanced visual perception near the hands

Enhanced visual perception near the hands Enhanced visual perception near the hands Bachelor thesis Marina Meinert (s0163430) Supervisors: 1 st supervisor: Prof. Dr. Ing. W. B. Verwey 2 nd supervisor: Dr. M. L. Noordzij External supervisor: Dr.

More information

Generating spatial and nonspatial attentional control: An ERP study

Generating spatial and nonspatial attentional control: An ERP study Psychophysiology, 42 (2005), 428 439. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2005 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00304.x Generating spatial and

More information

Motor Systems I Cortex. Reading: BCP Chapter 14

Motor Systems I Cortex. Reading: BCP Chapter 14 Motor Systems I Cortex Reading: BCP Chapter 14 Principles of Sensorimotor Function Hierarchical Organization association cortex at the highest level, muscles at the lowest signals flow between levels over

More information