Modulation of ERP components by task instructions in a cued go/no-go task

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Modulation of ERP components by task instructions in a cued go/no-go task"

Transcription

1 Psychophysiology, 53 (2016), Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. VC 2015 The Authors. Psychophysiology published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. on behalf of Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: /psyp Modulation of ERP components by task instructions in a cued go/no-go task IDA EMILIA AASEN a,b AND JAN FERENC BRUNNER b,c,d a Department of Psychology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), Trondheim, Norway b Department of Neuropsychology, Helgeland Hospital, Mosjøen, Norway c Department of Neuroscience, NTNU, Trondheim, Norway d Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, St. Olavs Hospital, Trondheim University Hospital, Norway Abstract The present study investigated how components of ERPs are modulated when participants optimize speed versus accuracy in a cued go/no-go task. Using a crossover design, 35 participants received instructions to complete the task prioritizing response speed in half of the task, and accurate responding in the other half of the task. Analysis was performed on the contingent negative variation (CNV), P3go, and P3no-go and the corresponding independent components (IC), as identified by group independent component analysis. After speed instructions, the IC CNV late, P3go anterior, P3no-go early, and P3no-go late all had larger amplitudes than after accuracy instructions. Furthermore, both the IC P3go posterior and IC P3go anterior had shorter latencies after speed than after accuracy instructions. The results demonstrate that components derived from the CNV and P3 components are facilitated when participants optimize response speed. These findings indicate that these ERP components reflect executive processes enabling adjustment of behavior to changing demands. Descriptors: ERP, ICA, Go/no-go, Executive control, Optimization, P3, CNV Executive function is a collective term for the neural processes that enable effective performance and adaptation to novel, conflicting, or complex situations (Lezak, Howieson, Bigler, & Tranel, 2012). The cued go/no-go task is an experimental paradigm that requires activation of executive functions to achieve fast and accurate performance. In addition to the effectiveness of specific executive processes, however, it has been suggested that specific mechanisms that facilitate other processes are of key importance for optimal performance (Forstmann et al., 2008; Stuss et al., 2005). In the cued go/no-go task, several executive and facilitating processes can be investigated. Just like the activation of any other cognitive process, the effect of facilitation is reflected in altered neural activity. The present study investigates such alterations when instructing participants to optimize performance speed or accuracy in a cued go/no-go task while keeping other task aspects unchanged. That way, the executive processes needed to perform The authors would like to thank John Polich for helpful comments on previous versions of this manuscript. The Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics in Norway approved the present study. Both authors contributed equally to this work, and the order of authorship is arbitrary. Address correspondence to: Ida Emilia Aasen, Department of Psychology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, NO-7491 Trondheim, Norway. ida.aasen@ntnu.no This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoNoDerivs License, which permits use and distribution in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non-commercial and no modifications or adaptations are made. 171 the task are the same in both conditions, while the relative facilitation of the different processes is affected. The neural correlates of optimization of speed or accuracy can be investigated by computing ERPs from EEG recorded during task performance. Previous studies have found that such manipulations can modulate both the latencies and amplitudes of several ERP components (e.g., Band, Ridderinkhof, & van der Molen, 2003; Boehm, van Maanen, Forstmann, & van Rijn, 2014; Christensen, Ivkovich, & Drake, 2001; Kutas, McCarthy, & Donchin, 1977; Pfefferbaum, Ford, Johnson, Wenegrat, & Kopell, 1983). A methodological challenge in ERP research, however, is the fact that ERPs represent the sum of activity from various locations occurring at overlapping points in time and representing different cognitive processes (Kappenman & Luck, 2012). A common approach to this challenge is to manipulate a task in a manner changing the overlap between different subcomponents (e.g., Hohnsbein, Falkenstein, & Hoormann, 1995; Verleger, Baur, Metzner, & Smigasiewicz, 2014; Verleger, Metzner, Ouyang, Smigasiewicz, & Zhou, 2014). These methods, however, necessitate knowledge about how to affect the latencies of the underlying components differentially, and do not enable separation of components that always follow each other closely in latency. Such manipulations can also not rule out that other unknown components affect the components of interest. Another approach to solving the problem of component overlap is the use of blind source separation methods, such as principal or independent component analysis (ICA; Makeig & Onton, 2012; Spencer, Dien, & Donchin, 2001). ICA is a method that uses the

2 172 I.E. Aasen and J.F. Brunner input data to learn how to separate the mixed signals recorded from multiple scalp locations into several temporally independent and spatially fixed information sources. ICA defines each information source by a spatial filter. When these filters are applied to data collected from many scalp locations, each independent component (IC) filter focuses on one source of information in the data, canceling out the contributions of all but one of the source signals contributing to the multichannel data (Makeig & Onton, 2012). In the present study, three well-studied ERP components, the contingent negative variation (CNV), P3go, and P3no-go were decomposed using group ICA to enable studying how the underlying components are modulated when individuals are instructed to optimize speed versus accuracy in the cued go/no-go task. In the following, the traditional ERP components will be referred to as raw ERP components, whereas the ICA-derived components will be referred to as ICs. The CNV Introduction of a cue activates preparatory processes that significantly reduce reaction time (RT; Niemi & N a at anen, 1981; Sanders & Wertheim, 1973). The CNV is a slow negative potential elicited in the time interval between the cue and the imperative stimulus. This potential consists of multiple components reflecting different preparatory processes (Brunia, van Boxtel, & B ocker, 2012; Rohrbaugh et al., 1997). CNVs presenting with the largest negativities early in the interstimulus interval have been linked to encoding of relevant stimulus characteristics (Bender et al., 2012; Ruchkin et al., 1997), as well as maintenance and rehearsal processes related to the cue information (Rohrbaugh et al., 1997). The late CNV presents largest negativity immediately before the imperative stimulus. When interstimulus intervals are short, the early and late CNV cannot be identified as separate components in the raw ERP. These components can, however, be separated by using ICA, even in paradigms with 1-s interstimulus intervals (Brunner et al., 2015; Jervis et al., 2007). The late CNV has been suggested to reflect activation of the contingency templates, or schema, for when and how to respond to the upcoming target stimulus (Brunia et al., 2012; Strack, Kaufmann, Kehrer, Brandt, & Sturmer, 2013). We refer to this schema as the task set. Unlike the early CNV, this component has been found to be larger under instructions for speed than for accuracy (Loveless & Sanford, 1974). Because it is modulated in response to time pressure, this potential has been suggested to be facilitated by effortful allocation of attention (Falkenstein, Hoormann, Hohnsbein, & Kleinsorge, 2003), or energization (Brunner et al., 2015). We prefer the concept of energization because it is specified as a facilitation process that is under voluntary control, and required for initiating and maintaining responses (Stuss & Alexander, 2007). This process is different from other, more automatic facilitation processes, such as arousal, emotional valence, or motivation, which also intensify processing, but do this effortlessly. In addition to facilitating the task set proactively, Brunner et al. (2015) suggested that energization is also required for processes occurring after the imperative stimulus. The P3 Family The most studied group of ERP components elicited after an imperative stimulus is the P3 family. These components are all regarded as reflecting cognitive processes, although the exact processes reflected in the different P3 components are a matter of much debate. What is better known are the effects that different manipulations have on these components. P3 amplitude is larger when participants devote more attention or effort to a task, but is smaller when tasks become more perceptually or cognitively demanding (Johnson, 1986; Luck, 2005). It is therefore important to differentiate between the cognitive load on the specific P3 process (the amount of information that must be handled by the P3 process) and the facilitation (i.e., the intensity) of this process when investigating P3 components experimentally. Often, these effects become mixed. For instance, making a task more difficult can simultaneously increase both cognitive load and facilitation. In the present study, cognitive load is therefore kept constant, while facilitation processes are affected by instructing participants to optimize speed or accuracy. In the cued go/no-go paradigm, two P3 components can be identified following the imperative stimulus: the P3go and the P3no-go. These components differ both in latency and topography, suggesting that they reflect different cognitive processes (Falkenstein, Hoormann, & Hohnsbein, 1999; Pfefferbaum, Ford, Weller, & Kopell, 1985). The processes reflected by different P3 components may therefore not be affected in the same way by a speed accuracy manipulation. Prediction of the effects of the speed accuracy manipulation on specific P3 components therefore depends on an understanding of which processes the different components reflect. The P3go: Functional significance and subcomponents. The parietal P3go has been suggested to be similar to the target P3, or P3b, from the oddball paradigm (Barry & Rushby, 2006). The latency of this component has often been said to reflect the time it takes to classify an incoming stimulus as a target (see Polich, 2007; Verleger, 1988). According to the context-updating theory (Donchin, 1981; Donchin & Coles, 1988; Polich, 2003, 2007), the P3b is suggested to index an attention-driven comparison process that evaluates a task-related, and subjectively unexpected, stimulus in working memory. This theory has received some criticism, and Verleger (1988) argued that the P3b is better described as being evoked by all target events rather than unexpected events only. There has been some debate on whether this ERP component is only related to stimulus evaluation, or whether it is also related to the specific response to be made. Using a simple RT paradigm, as well as two- and four-choice RT tasks, Falkenstein and colleagues (Falkenstein, Hohnsbein, & Hoormann, 1993, 1994; Hohnsbein et al., 1995) identified two partially overlapping P3 components the stimulus-related P-SR, and the choice-related P-CR. Whereas the P-SR had a central distribution and was not affected by RT or complexity of the choice, the P-CR had a parietal distribution and was clearly delayed with increasing RT and choice complexity. They interpreted this finding as indicating that the P-SR is related to stimulus classification, whereas the P-CR is related to response selection. Comparing stimulus- and response-locked components, however, Verleger, Jaskowski, and Wascher (2005) found that P3b amplitudes calculated as stimulus- and response-locked components were equally large. The amplitude of the centrally located component (P3a), however, was clearly larger for stimulus- than response-locked components. They therefore argued that the P3b represents a link between perceptual classification and response selection, rather than being related to perceptual processes or response selection alone. They concluded that the P3b might reflect the process of monitoring whether the stimulus is classified correctly to the appropriate response. Recently, Verleger, Baur et al.

3 Modulation of ERP components by task instructions 173 (2014) found further support for the view that the P3b represents a reactivation of a learned stimulus-response (S-R) link. They demonstrated that the P3b amplitude dramatically decreased when the S-R link was made less straightforward. Other processes in the go condition. Evaluative processes are frequently suggested to be active after errors and in conflict situations. The go condition does not involve the possibility of commission errors. This condition does, however, involve the possibility of slow responses errors of speed (Luu, Flaisch, & Tucker, 2000). The cause of errors of speed is inattention in the time interval just prior to and during the presentation of the target stimulus. Optimization of speed depends on detecting this type of inattention to enable maintaining or adjusting task-set activation as needed in the following trials (i.e., a process of self-monitoring). The existence of such a monitoring process that intercedes to bias attention back to the task-related schema when performance decays due to inattention has previously been suggested by Manly and colleagues (Manly, Robertson, Galloway, & Hawkins, 1999). Possibly, the two overlapping P3 components that have been observed in parietal and central areas may differentially reflect reactivation of the S-R link and self-monitoring. Speed accuracy effects on the P3go. Several studies have demonstrated that target-p3 latency is shorter under high as compared to low time pressure (Kutas et al., 1977; Pfefferbaum et al., 1983; Ramautar, Kok, & Ridderinkhof, 2004), which is in line with the suggestion that the P3b reflects an S-R link. Some studies have also found increased target-p3 amplitudes in conditions where response speed is increased (Carrillo-de-la-Pe~na & Cadaveira, 2000; Falkenstein et al., 1993; Friedman, Vaughan, & Erlenmeyer- Kimling, 1978; Roth, Ford, & Kopell, 1978). These amplitude changes could be the result of increased overlap in P3 subcomponents due to latency changes, or reflect real amplitude changes in one or several subcomponents. The use of ICA to separate P3go subcomponents when studying the effects of speed and accuracy instructions can contribute to answering these questions, and give indications about the functional significance of the different components. The P3no-go. The frontocentral P3no-go from cued go/no-go tasks is evoked when a no-go imperative stimulus unexpectedly follows a go cue. The exact process reflected in this ERP component is still debated. The P3no-go has been associated with inhibitory (Fallgatter & Strik, 1999; Gajewski & Falkenstein, 2013; Kok, Ramautar, De Ruiter, Band, & Ridderinkhof, 2004; Smith, Johnstone, & Barry, 2006; Wessel & Aron, 2014) and evaluative processes (Bruin, Wijers, & van Staveren, 2001; Schmajuk, Liotti, Busse, & Woldorff, 2006; Sehlmeyer et al., 2010). A recent review of studies on the P3no-go, however, concluded that the latter hypothesis has received more empirical support than the former (Huster, Enriquez-Geppert, Lavallee, Falkenstein, & Herrmann, 2013). In that review, it was suggested that the P3no-go could reflect either the evaluation of the outcome of inhibition, or an evaluation of the inhibitory process itself. Like the CNV and P3go, it has also been suggested that the P3no-go is comprised of overlapping components reflecting different processes. Brunner et al. (2013) noted that the two subcomponents that they identified seemed to reflect consecutive processes, with one of the components always appearing shortly after the other (IC P3no-go early and IC P3no-go late ). They suggested that these components might independently reflect the proposed inhibitory and evaluative functions of the P3no-go. In a follow-up study, however, the inhibitory hypothesis of the IC P3no-go early was not supported, whereas the monitoring hypothesis of the IC P3no-go late was supported (Brunner et al., 2015). Rather than being related to inhibition, it was suggested that the IC P3no-go early might reflect implementation of an alternative, subdominant task set. Others have also argued that processes evoked by the no-go condition may be better understood within a broader framework of response selection and execution processes, rather than inhibition only (Jasinska, 2013; Rae, Hughes, Weaver, Anderson, & Rowe, 2014). Speed accuracy effects on the P3no-go. The effects of speed and accuracy instructions on the P3no-go have not been previously studied, although several studies have found larger P3no-go amplitudes in fast as compared to slow responders (Dimoska, Johnstone, & Barry, 2006; Smith et al., 2006). An interesting study by Benikos and Johnstone (2009) also showed that the P3no-go amplitude is larger under fast as compared to slow event rates in healthy controls, indicating that faster responding may modulate the P3no-go amplitude intraindividually. Brunner et al. (2015) demonstrated that the IC P3no-go early correlated significantly with neuropsychological indexes of energization, including reaction time. This finding could indicate that the IC P3no-go early may be the subcomponent of the P3no-go that is modulated by response speed. The IC P3no-go late was not related to the energization indices, but was highly related to accuracy (negatively correlated to errors) and therefore suggested to be the subcomponent of the P3no-go reflecting a monitoring process. This component was suggested to be modulated by emotional factors. Although monitoring is often considered to be a cognitive process without any explicit emotional content, conflict situations and errors have been associated with emotional responses and activity in brain regions associated with negative affect (Ichikawa et al., 2011; Shackman et al., 2011). There is also some evidence that there is a relationship between the P3no-go amplitude and anxiety (Sehlmeyer et al., 2010). Study Aims and Hypotheses This study investigates whether ICs from the cued go/no-go task are differentially modulated when participants are instructed to optimize either speed or accuracy. A schematic model presenting the proposed processes involved in the different conditions of the task is presented in Figure 1. In line with the findings by Falkenstein et al. (2003) and Loveless and Sanford (1974), the raw CNV component was expected to be larger in the speed than in the accuracy condition. The amplitude IC CNV late, but not the IC CNV early, was hypothesized to be facilitated by the speed instructions. The processes proposed to be reflected in the CNV ICs are sketched on the left side of Figure 1. The effects on the raw P3go were expected to reflect previous findings, which have demonstrated latency effects on parietal, but not central, parts of the target P3 when RT is affected (Falkenstein et al., 1993; Verleger et al., 2005). Decomposing the P3go should result in one IC corresponding to the parietally maximal P3b, which was expected to have shorter latency in the speed as compared to the accuracy condition. The amplitude of the P3b component was not expected to be affected by the manipulation, as correct target-response mapping should be equally important to obtain both fast and accurate responses. It was also hypothesized that a subcomponent of the P3go, reflecting self-monitoring of attention, would show larger

4 174 I.E. Aasen and J.F. Brunner Figure 1. Schematic illustration of processes hypothesized to be involved in the cued go/no-go task. The CNV is thought to reflect at least two processes: encoding of cue information and attention to the dominant task set. In the go condition, the P3go is thought to reflect reactivation of the target-response link (A 5 go) and evaluation of whether the preparatory attention to the go task set was optimal. In the no-go condition, the P3no-go is thought to reflect implementation of the subdominant task set, and subsequent evaluation of the level of control over this implementation process. Several of these processes are hypothesized to be modulated by instructions to optimize speed or accuracy. The late CNV and the early P3no-go are suggested to be facilitated by an effortful, energetic process (energization). The monitoring process in the no-go condition is suggested to be modulated by emotional processes. The speed instruction is also hypothesized to result in quicker reactivation of the target-response link and more resources invested in evaluating preparatory attention. amplitude in the speed as compared to the accuracy condition. This is because the use of information from the current trial to finetune energization of the late CNV in the following trials would be particularly important in the speed condition. If a P3go IC reflects such a self-monitoring process, the amplitude of this subcomponent should also correlate with the IC CNV late amplitude. The proposed processes reflected in the P3go ICs are sketched on the right side, lower panel of Figure 1. The raw P3no-go amplitude was hypothesized to be larger in the speed than in the accuracy condition in line with studies finding larger P3no-go amplitudes in fast as compared to slow responders (Dimoska et al., 2006; Smith et al., 2006). The amplitude of the centrally maximal IC P3no-go early was hypothesized to be facilitated more in the speed than the accuracy condition, reflecting increased energization to implement a subdominant task set. If our working hypothesis is wrong and this component reflects response inhibition, however, the component should have shorter latency than the mean RT in both conditions and have a shorter latency in the speed than in the accuracy condition. The type of monitoring reflected in the amplitude of the frontocentral IC P3no-go late could be related either to the focus on evaluating whether the (non)response was correct, or to the evaluation of how close one was to committing a premature response in the given trial. Whereas the first alternative should result in larger amplitudes when focusing on accuracy than speed, the second alternative should result in larger amplitudes under high response speed. As previous studies of the P3no-go have only indicated increases in amplitude related to increased response speed, only the latter alternative is sketched in Figure 1. Participants Method A total of 26 female and 9 male university students (mean age years, SD 5 2.6) was recruited to participate in the experiment through presentations during lectures that the participants were attending. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to their participation in the experiment. Criteria for exclusion were a history of neurological or psychiatric disorder, or previous experience with the cued go/no-go task. No participants were excluded from the study. The spatial filters for the independent components were obtained through application of ICA to ERPs recorded from the 35 participants, as well as 193 healthy adult volunteers (109 females, mean age years, SD 5 4.2, previously reported in Brunner et al., 2015). These volunteers completed the same cued go/no-go task that was used in the experiment, but under instructions to balance speed and accuracy (mean RT ms, mean number of commission errors 5 1.1). The volunteers were recruited among acquaintances of researchers and staff, family, and friends of braininjured patients, and a healthy control group from a study on prematurely born children. The resulting spatial filters were then applied to the ERPs of the 35 students recruited for the experiment. EEG Recording EEG was recorded using a 21-channel Mitsar ( EEG system, with a 19-channel tin electrode cap with electrodes Fz, Cz, Pz, Fp1/2, F3/4, F7/8, T3/4, T5/6, C3/4, P3/ 4, and O1/2, fitted according to the International system, and the cap electrodes were referenced to earlobe electrodes. The ground electrode was placed on the forehead (between the Fp1/2 and Fz electrodes). Impedance was kept below 5 kx. The bandpass was Hz, and the sampling rate was 250 Hz. It has previously been argued that slow wave components such as the CNV can be reliably extracted, even when such low frequency cutoffs are used (Padilla, Wood, Hale, & Knight, 2006). ERPs were computed offline in the common average montage. Trials with omission and commission errors were excluded from averaging. Behavioral Task Participants sat approximately 1.5 m from a computer screen while performing the cued go/no-go task, which consisted of 400 pairs of images, with a new pair presented every 3 s (the task used by Brunner et al., 2015). An illustration of the task and the stimulus categories can be seen in Figure 2. The task contained three categories of stimuli animals (A), plants (P), and humans (H), presented

5 Modulation of ERP components by task instructions 175 Figure 2. Illustration of the cued go/no-go task. Left: Participants were instructed to respond to all animal-animal (A-A) pairs (top) and withhold responses to the other three stimulus combinations. The cue and imperative stimulus in A-A and plant-plant (P-P) pairs were always identical. Human stimuli in P-H pairs were combined with a sound. Right: The size of the stimuli relative to the computer screen. in four equally probable combinations: A-A, P-P, A-P, or P-H. The first and second stimulus in A-A and P-P pairs were always identical. There were 20 different images in each category to reduce habituation due to repetition of stimuli. Also, to maintain alertness, the H stimuli were paired with different 70 db sounds consisting of different frequencies (500, 1,000, 1,500, 2,000, and 2,500 Hz). Each stimulus was presented for 100 ms, and the interstimulus interval between the first and second image in a pair was 1,000 ms. The images were selected from children s textbooks and were of approximately equal luminance and size. All images were presented against a white background, and a blank screen occupied the interstimulus intervals. The trials were grouped into four blocks of 100 trials. Each block contained a new set of five images of each category. The image pairs were presented pseudorandomly so that each block contained an equal number of trials of each type. Participants were instructed to press the left mouse button for every A-A (go) trial as quickly as possible and to withhold the response on every A-P (no-go) trial. All participants responded using the index finger on the right hand. No response was required for the P-P or P-H trials. All trials starting with A required subjects to continue attending to the second stimulus in the pair. Verbal speed accuracy manipulations have the possible disadvantage that the effect of these instructions wane over time in favor of the individuals automatic response tendencies (Heitz, 2014). To minimize this effect, participants had a short break every 50 trials, during which the task instructions were repeated. Mean RT, SD of the RT, and the number of omission and commission errors were calculated separately for each participant. A response was regarded as correct if it occurred 200 to 1,000 ms after the second stimulus in go trials. Instructions Under both conditions, participants were instructed to respond to the target stimuli as quickly and accurately as possible. In one condition, they were told to optimize speed and, in the other, to optimize accuracy. The instructions were as follows: Accuracy: I want you to respond as quickly and accurately as you can to the target stimuli, but most of all, it is very important that you do not commit a single mistake! Speed: I want you to respond as quickly and accurately as possible to the target stimuli. In this condition, however, I want you to really push yourself to be as fast as possible, even to the point where you might commit one or two premature responses. The most important thing is that you respond very quickly to all targets. Thus, although the weighting of speed and accuracy was manipulated, the instructions in both conditions were designed to keep participants within the limits of maintaining the conflicting demands of fast and accurate responses. The order in which participants received their instructions was randomized: 18 participants were instructed to focus on speed in the first half of the task, and 17 participants were instructed to focus on accuracy during the first half. After 200 trials, the instructions were reversed. Correction of EEG Artifacts Eyeblink artifacts were corrected by applying ICA to the raw EEG, zeroing the activation curves of ICs corresponding to eyeblinks (see Jung et al., 2000; Vigario, 1997). Epochs of EEG with excessive absolute amplitude in unfiltered EEG (> 100 lv), epochs of excessive faster frequency activity (> 35 lv infrequencybandsof Hz), or excessive slower frequency activity (> 50 lv infrequency bands of 0 1 Hz) were automatically marked and excluded from further analysis. After artifact rejection, the mean number of artifact- and error-free trials used to compute ERPs was 95.5 (SD 5 5.8; range ) for the accuracy cue condition (all trials starting with an animal, used to compute the CNV), and 90.7 (SD 5 6.0; range ) for the speed cue condition. In the go condition, the ERPs were computed for a mean of 47.7 trials (SD ; range ) in the accuracy condition, and 47.0 trials (SD ; range ) in the speed condition. In the no-go condition, the mean number of trials for computing the ERPs was 47.8 (SD 5 3.1; range ) for the accuracy condition, and 43.7 (SD 5 3.3; range ) for the speed condition.

6 176 I.E. Aasen and J.F. Brunner Decomposing ERPs Into Independent Components ICA can be applied to continuous EEG, single trial ERPs, or ERPs averaged across trials. By applying ICA to averaged ERPs from many individuals (group ICA; Kropotov & Ponomarev, 2009; Liu et al., 2009; Olbrich, Maes, Valerius, Langosch, & Feige, 2005), one can obtain ICs that are less affected by oscillatory activity and noise than ICA applied to single trial ERPs or EEG. Another advantage of group ICA is that, by applying the same spatial filters to all participants ERPs, the possibility of comparing the latency and amplitude of an IC peak across individuals is maintained. This method, however, assumes that any particular component has approximately the same topography in all individuals. This is an assumption that ignores differences in individual cortical configuration that may project equivalent potentials to different scalp areas in different subjects (Makeig & Onton, 2012). In the present study, ICA was conducted using the infomax algorithm (Makeig, Jung, Bell, Ghahremani, & Sejnowski, 1997) on a collection of 19-channel ERPs from 228 individuals. To obtain a reliable decomposition, ICA depends on a large number of training points to learn the weights in the unmixing matrix and, in general, larger amounts of clean input data result in more reliable decompositions. Onton and Makeig (2006) suggest that the number of training points should, as a rule of thumb, be larger than the number of electrodes squared (N 2 ) times a factor, k, whichagain depends on the number of electrodes, the algorithm used, data quality, and frequency range. For data collected from a large number of channels (> 64), Makeig and Onton (2012) suggest that k could be set at 20. The same heuristic has, however, not been investigated in data from fewer channels. Because of this, it was expected that a number of training points falling well above but below should be sufficient to obtain a reliable decomposition. In the present study, a sufficient number of training points was achieved by using ERPs from a large number of subjects (n 5 228) as input for the ICA decomposition. ICA was conducted separately for three different conditions. In the cue condition, ERPs were selected from the time interval of 0 1,100 ms after onset of the first stimulus (S1). In go and no-go trials, ERPs were selected from the time interval of ms after onset of the second stimulus (S2). ICA was performed on the ERP Scalp Location 3 Time series matrix. The output of the ICA is a number of temporally independent and spatially fixed components. The number of components equals the number of channels (19 in this case). The raw data for the group ICA, the ERPs, are expected to consist of a mixture of a few large source components plus many smaller source components. The ICA is expected to separate large source components into different components well, whereas the remaining components consist of mixtures of smaller source components and noise (Makeig et al., 1999). The ICAs for the go and no-go conditions were therefore calculated separately, to maximize the probability of finding all the relevant P3go and P3no-go components. Taking into account the 19 electrodes, the sampling rate of 250 Hz, and the time interval of 0.7 s, the matrix for go and no-go conditions consisted of 19 rows and columns, giving 39,900 points in time for computing the unmixing matrices for these conditions. ICA for the relevant (A) and irrelevant (P) cue condition was calculated conjointly to enable selecting components that differentiated between relevant and irrelevant cues. For ICA application in the cue condition, a time interval of 1.1 s was used, resulting in a matrix consisting of 19 rows and columns, giving 125,400 points in time for computing the unmixing matrix. Further details of this method are described in Kropotov and Ponomarev (2009). Measurement of Latency and Amplitude of ERP Components The amplitudes of the raw ERP components were measured in the three midline electrodes (Fz, Cz, Pz), with the exception of the P3 in the go condition, where no clear component could be observed at Fz. As argued by Luck (2005), only electrodes where the component of interest is present should be included in the analyses to avoid excessive noise. As the ICs are characterized by fixed topographies and time courses, the amplitude of the back-projected ICs were only measured at the electrode where the signal was maximal (Cz for IC CNV late,icp3go anterior,icp3no-go early, and IC P3no-go late, and Pz for IC CNV early and IC P3go posterior ). For the P3 components, amplitude was measured at the maximum local peak in the defined time interval (Luck, 2005). This time interval was selected for the P3go and P3no-go components separately based on the onset and offset of the grand mean potentials from the entire group of 35 participants across the speed and accuracy conditions in the midline electrodes. Being slow fluctuations without clear peaks, the CNV components were measured as the mean amplitude in a time interval. The amplitude of the raw CNV and IC CNV late were measured as the mean in the 100 ms before the presentation of S2, while the amplitude of IC CNV early was measured as the mean in the time interval of 600 to 800 ms after S1. This time window was chosen based on the group s mean IC CNV early, which had largest negativity in this time interval. Baseline was adjusted to the mean voltage 100 ms before stimulus presentation relative to S1 for the CNV components and relative to S2 for the P3go and P3no-go components. To ensure that effects on the amplitudes of post-s2 components were not simply the result of differences in the pre-s2 baseline period (CNV), the amplitudes of the P3go and P3no-go components were calculated relative to both pre-s2 and pre-s1 baselines. Analyses of the P3 components relative to a pre-s1 baseline are included in the Appendix. The latencies of the P3go and P3no-go components were measured using a relative criterion version of the fractional area (FA) approach. The method was chosen because it has been proven to be more accurate than peak latency measures (Hansen & Hillyard, 1980; Kiesel, Miller, Jolicoeur, & Brisson, 2008; Luck, 2005), and has been shown to result in very high test-retest reliability when measuring the latency of the P3no-go (Brunner et al., 2013). The onset of the time window for measuring FA latency for each individual is found by identifying the largest amplitude within a defined time window, going back to the lowest point within this time window, and then finding the point in time where a prespecified percentage of this difference is reached (50% for the P3no-go and 70% for the P3go). The offset is defined as the point in time within the time window where the amplitude returns to, or comes closest to the onset point again. The latency is then defined as the median of the FA. The total time window for calculation of the FA was limited according to the observed onset and offset of the grand mean ERP component in the midline electrodes. The time interval was thus limited to ms for the P3go components. For the P3no-go components, the FA time window was limited to ms. Statistics All data were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics 21.0 for Mac. Tests of significant differences among parameters in the speed and accuracy conditions and among the three midline electrodes were calculated using repeated measures analyses of variances (ANOVAs) with electrode (Fz, Cz, Pz) and condition (speed vs.

7 Modulation of ERP components by task instructions 177 Figure 3. Grand mean raw ERPs and independent components (ICs) for the group of healthy subjects (n 5 228) in the visual cued go/no-go task. a: Schematic illustration of the task with time (ms) and amplitude (lv) scale for the ERPs. b: ERPs at Fz, Cz, and Pz electrodes for go (A-A, green), no-go (A-P, red), and irrelevant (P-P, black) conditions. Arrows indicate points in time and conditions under which the topographic maps are made. c: Maps of the raw CNV, go, and no-go components taken at their peak amplitudes as indicated by arrows in (b). d: Topographies (left) and time courses (right) of the two CNV ICs back-projected to Fz, Cz, and Pz. e: Topographies (left) and time courses (right) of the two P3go ICs backprojected to Fz, Cz, and Pz. f: Topographies (left) and time courses (right) of the two P3no-go ICs back-projected to Fz, Cz, and Pz. g: Illustration of the timing and overlap of the raw CNV and ICs, P3go and ICs, and P3no-go and ICs at the Cz and Pz electrodes. accuracy) as within-subject factors. To correct for the nonsphericity of the electrode factor, degrees of freedom were corrected using the Greenhouse-Geisser epsilon adjustment (Jennings & Wood, 1976). Only corrected probability values are reported. Significant main effects of electrode position and significant Condition 3 Electrode interactions were then explored using Scheffe s procedure. Planned comparisons between ERP parameters and between ERP and behavioral parameters were conducted using Pearson s or Spearman s correlations as appropriate for parametric and nonparametric variables. Alpha was set at.05. Results Independent Components of the CNV The grand mean ERPs from the group of 228 subjects and a topographic map of the raw CNV are shown in Figure 3b. As can be observed in Figure 3c, the late CNV had a centroparietal distribution. Applying ICA to the ERPs in the interstimulus interval resulted in three ICs with negative slow fluctuations specific to the cue condition, thus qualifying as CNV components. Of these components, one was much weaker in relative power than the two others, and was therefore discarded from further analysis. The two CNV ICs and topographic maps are shown in Figure 3d. One component, referred to as IC CNV early, had a parietal distribution, with a positive peak at 400 ms followed by a negative peak at 700 ms. The second component, IC CNV late, had a centroparietal distribution, with the largest negativity preceding the second stimulus. Independent Components of the P3Go The grand mean raw P3go component from the group of 228 subjects had a centroparietal distribution at the mean peak latency of 325 ms at Pz, which can be seen in Figure 3b,c. Applying ICA to the collection of individual ERPs from the go condition resulted in two ICs with positivity coinciding in latency with observed peaks

8 178 I.E. Aasen and J.F. Brunner Table 1. Descriptives, Behavioral Parameters Accuracy Speed n Mean (SD) Mean (SD) F 2 g p RT (42.5) 270 (22.6) 86.77***.72 SD RT (21.0) 50.8 (18.4) 8.28**.20 Commissions (.55) 4.56 (2.18) ***.79 Omissions (.77).44 (.96) Note. Means and SDs of the behavioral parameters of the 35 participants in the speed and accuracy conditions. RT 5 reaction time; g p 2 5 partial eta squared. **p <.01. ***p <.001. in the raw P3go component. Figure 3e shows the two components and their topographies. One, referred to as IC P3go posterior, had a parietal distribution and a mean peak latency of 325 ms, matching the description of P3b. The second component, IC P3go anterior,had a centroparietal distribution and a mean peak latency of 330 ms. Independent Components of the P3No-Go The grand mean raw P3no-go and topographic map from the group of 228 subjects is shown in Figure 3b,c. The P3no-go had a central distribution at the mean peak latency of 340 ms at Cz. Applying ICA to the collection of individual ERPs for the no-go condition resulted in two components contributing to the central positive P3no-go component. These components and their topographies are shown in Figure 3f. One of these, referred to as IC P3no-go early, had a central distribution and a mean peak latency of 320 ms. The second component, referred to as IC P3no-go late, had a frontocentral distribution and a mean peak latency of 380 ms. Effects of the Speed Versus Accuracy Instructions on the Behavioral Data There was a significant effect of the instructions on the behavioral data, with the speed condition resulting in shorter RTs, F(1,34) , p <.001, smaller SDs of the RT, F(1,34) , p <.01, and more commission errors, F(1,34) , p <.001. The descriptive data for the behavioral parameters in the two conditions are presented in Table 1. Table 2. Descriptives, Raw ERP Parameters Amplitude (lv) Latency (ms) Accuracy Speed Accuracy Speed n Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) CNV 35 Fz.45 (1.13).63 (1.33) Cz (1.25) (1.25) Pz (1.08) (1.10) P3go 35 Cz 7.82 (3.12) 8.63 (2.93) (25.9) (28.8) Pz 9.05 (2.72) 9.66 (2.60) (20.4) (21.5) P3no-go 35 Fz 6.57 (3.39) 7.68 (3.60) (26.3) (23.1) Cz (3.85) (3.48) (23.3) (21.2) Pz 7.79 (2.69) 8.31 (2.93) (53.0) (58.5) Note. Means and SDs of the ERP amplitudes and latencies in the speed and accuracy conditions for the 35 participants. Effects of the Speed Versus Accuracy Instructions on ERP Amplitudes Table 2 shows the descriptive data for the raw ERP components in the speed and accuracy conditions. Table 3 shows the descriptive data for the independent components. CNV amplitudes in the speed and accuracy conditions. The 2 3 3(Condition3 Electrode) repeated measures ANOVA on the raw CNV amplitudes revealed a significant main effect of electrode position, F(2,68) , p <.001, as well as a significant interaction between the instructional manipulation and electrodes, F(2,34) , p <.01. Summary of the ANOVA results for the raw ERP component amplitudes can be seen in Table 4. Post hoc tests using Scheffe s procedure revealed that the amplitude was significantly larger at the Cz (M , SD ) compared to the Fz (M 5.54, SD ) electrode (p <.001), and larger at the Pz (M , SD ) compared to the Fz electrode (p <.001). No significant difference was found between the amplitude at the Cz compared to the Pz electrode. Furthermore, pairwise comparisons of the CNV amplitude using Scheffe s procedure in each electrode in the speed versus accuracy conditions revealed a significant effect of the instruction at the Pz electrode (p <.01), with significantly larger amplitudes in the speed as compared to the accuracy condition. The effect of the instructional manipulation was not significant at Fz or Cz. Figure 4 demonstrates the differences in amplitudes for the raw CNV and independent components in the two conditions. The amplitude of the IC CNV late, F(1,34) , p <.01, was significantly larger in the speed than in the accuracy condition. There was no significant effect of the instructional manipulation on the amplitude of the IC CNV early. Table 5 shows the summary of the ANOVA results for the ICs. P3go amplitudes in the speed and accuracy conditions. The repeated measures ANOVA showed a significant main effect of the instructions, F(1,34) , p <.01, on the raw P3go with significantly larger amplitudes in the speed as compared to the accuracy condition. 1 There was also a main effect of electrode position, F(1,34) , p <.05, with larger amplitudes at the Pz as compared to the Cz electrode. There was no significant interaction between instruction and electrode position. There was also a significant effect of the instructions on the IC P3go anterior, F(1,32) , p <.01, with larger amplitudes in the speed than in the accuracy condition, but not on the IC P3go posterior amplitude. Figure 5b demonstrates the effect of the instructions on the P3go components. As the IC CNV late was hypothesized to reflect preparation for the dominant task set (A-A 5 go), and the IC P3go anterior was expected to reflect evaluation of this preparatory attention, it was investigated whether the amplitudes of these components correlated with each other. The IC CNV late correlated significantly with the IC P3go anterior amplitude in both the accuracy (r 52.42, p <.05) and the speed condition (r 52.41, p <.05), with larger IC CNV late amplitudes being related to larger IC P3go anterior amplitudes. P3no-go amplitudes in the speed and accuracy conditions. Figure 5c shows the effect of the instructions on the raw P3no-go 1. This effect was not significant when the ANOVA was conducted on amplitudes measured relative to a pre-s1 baseline. See Appendix for details.

9 Modulation of ERP components by task instructions 179 Table 3. Descriptives, Independent Components Amplitude (lv) Latency (ms) Accuracy Speed Accuracy Speed n Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) IC CNV early (Pz) (.97) (1.24) IC CNV late (Cz) (.96) (1.12) IC P3go anterior (Cz) (2.68) 7.33 (3.65) (27.5) (24.6) IC P3go posterior (Pz) (2.73) 6.78 (3.08) (20.9) (23.6) IC P3no-go early (Cz) (3.85) 9.56 (3.96) (25.4) (22.9) IC P3no-go late (Cz) (3.00) 9.35 (3.26) (22.9) (21.2) Note. Means and SDs of the independent component (IC) ERP amplitudes and latencies in the speed and accuracy conditions for the 35 participants. and ICs. The repeated measures ANOVA revealed significant main effects of the instructions, F(1,34) , p <.001, and electrode position, F(2,68) , p <.001, as well as a significant Electrode 3 Condition interaction, F(2,68) , p <.05. Post hoc analyses using Scheffe s procedure revealed that the amplitude was significantly larger at the Cz (M , SD ) than at the Fz (M , SD ) electrode (p <.001), and larger at the Cz than at the Pz (M , SD ) electrode (p <.001), but not significantly different at the Fz compared to the Pz electrode. Pairwise comparisons of the raw P3no-go in each electrode in the speed versus accuracy conditions using Scheffe s procedure revealed a significant effect of the instruction at the Fz (p <.05) and Cz (p <.001) electrodes, with significantly larger amplitudes in the speed as compared to the accuracy condition. The effect of the instruction was not significant at Pz. Significant effects of the instruction on the IC P3no-go early, F(1,34) , p <.05, and IC P3no-go late, F(1,34) , p <.001, amplitudes were also found, with significantly larger amplitudes in the speed than in the accuracy condition. Effects of the Speed Versus Accuracy Instructions on ERP Latencies Table 4. Summary of Repeated Measures ANOVA (Condition 3 Electrode) Results for the Raw ERP Parameters in the Speed and Accuracy Conditions Amplitude df F 2 g p Latency df F g p 2 CNV (2 3 3) Condition 1, Electrode 2, ***.72 C 3 E 2, **.15 P3go (2 3 2) Condition 1, **.25 1, Electrode 1, *.12 1, C 3 E 1, , P3no-go (2 3 3) Condition 1, ***.31 1, Electrode 2, ***.72 2, C 3 E 2, *.11 2, Note. F values and effect sizes (partial eta squared, g p 2 ) for the effects of condition (C, accuracy vs. speed) and electrode position (E) on amplitude and latency variables. The degrees of freedom (df) are adjusted using a Greenhouse-Geisser correction where the assumptions of sphericity are not met. Only uncorrected df are reported. *p <.05. **p <.01. ***p <.001. Table 2 shows the means and SDs of the latencies of the raw ERP components and ICs in the speed and accuracy conditions. Figure 5 shows the grand mean P3go and P3no-go components in the midline electrodes, as well as the P3go and P3no-go ICs. The repeated measures ANOVA revealed no significant results of instruction, electrode position, or interaction effects on the latency of the raw P3go component. The instructional manipulation did have a significant effect on the IC P3go anterior, F(1,32) , p <.001, and IC P3go posterior, F(1,34) , p <.001, latencies, however. For both ICs, the latencies were significantly later in the accuracy as compared to the speed condition, which can be observed in Figure 5b. As shown in the scatter plots in Figure 6, the latency of the IC P3go posterior correlated significantly with RT in the accuracy (r 5.53, p <.001) condition. The correlation was also significant in the speed condition (r 5.44, p <.01). Based on the outlier labeling rule (Hoaglin & Iglewicz, 1987), however, one subject had an RT above the upper bound and was defined as an outlier. When excluding this subject, the correlation was no longer significant (r 5.27, p 5.12). In spite of the IC P3go anterior latency being shorter after speed than accuracy instructions, the latency of this component was not significantly related to RT in either condition. Regarding the raw P3no-go, the repeated measures ANOVA revealed no significant effects of instruction or electrode position, neither were there any significant effects of the instruction on the latencies of the IC P3no-go early or IC P3no-go late. Discussion The goal of this study was to investigate the modulating effects of instructions to optimize speed or accuracy on CNV and P3 ERP parameters elicited in the cued go/no-go task. The behavioral data confirmed that the manipulation was successful, as all participants had shorter RTs and higher rates of commission errors in the speed as compared to the accuracy condition. The optimization of speed compared to accuracy led to significantly larger amplitudes of the raw late CNV, as well as the P3go and P3no-go. No significant changes in the latencies of the P3go or P3no-go were found. The existence of overlapping components when measuring raw ERP components, however, makes it difficult to draw certain conclusions about how the instructions affect the underlying processes. Indeed, the investigation of the ICs derived from these ERP components gave a more differentiated picture. The CNV Independent Components The IC CNV late had larger amplitude in the speed as compared to the accuracy condition, whereas the IC CNV early amplitude remained unchanged across conditions. This is in line with our predictions and the results of previous findings (Loveless & Sanford, 1974), indicating that the proactive processes necessary for

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction Proceedings of the First International Conference on Complex Medical Engineering (CME2005) May 15-18, 2005, Takamatsu, Japan (Organized Session No. 20). Paper No. 150, pp. 715-719. Event-Related Potentials

More information

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln 1 DATA MANAGEMENT Backups Storage Identification Analyses 2 Data Analysis Pre-processing Statistical Analysis

More information

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task Psychophysiology, 44 (2007), 913 917. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2007 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00561.x BRIEF REPORT Reward prediction

More information

1. Department of clinical neurology, Tbilisi State Medical University, Tbilisi, Georgia.

1. Department of clinical neurology, Tbilisi State Medical University, Tbilisi, Georgia. Impact of EEG biofeedback on event-related potentials (ERPs) in attention-deficit hyperactivity (ADHD) children. S. Bakhtadze1, M. Janelidze1, N. Khachapuridze2. 1. Department of clinical neurology, Tbilisi

More information

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Cerebral Cortex March 2006;16:415-424 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi121 Advance Access publication June 15, 2005 Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Mika

More information

Processed by HBI: Russia/Switzerland/USA

Processed by HBI: Russia/Switzerland/USA 1 CONTENTS I Personal and clinical data II Conclusion. III Recommendations for therapy IV Report. 1. Procedures of EEG recording and analysis 2. Search for paroxysms 3. Eyes Open background EEG rhythms

More information

From Single-trial EEG to Brain Area Dynamics

From Single-trial EEG to Brain Area Dynamics From Single-trial EEG to Brain Area Dynamics a Delorme A., a Makeig, S., b Fabre-Thorpe, M., a Sejnowski, T. a The Salk Institute for Biological Studies, 10010 N. Torey Pines Road, La Jolla, CA92109, USA

More information

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study Armita Faghani Jadidi, Raheleh Davoodi, Mohammad Hassan Moradi Department of Biomedical Engineering Amirkabir University

More information

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Psychon Bull Rev (2013) 20:296 301 DOI 10.3758/s13423-012-0353-4 BRIEF REPORT Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Risa Sawaki & Steven J. Luck Published online: 20 December 2012 #

More information

NeuroImage 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

NeuroImage 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage: NeuroImage 50 (2010) 329 339 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Switching associations between facial identity and emotional expression:

More information

Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA

Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA 1 Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA CONTENTS I Personal and clinical data II Conclusion. III Recommendations for therapy IV Report. 1. Procedures of EEG recording and analysis 2. Search for

More information

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling Supplementary materials 1 Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling analyses placed the source of the No Go N2 component in the dorsal ACC, near the ACC source

More information

Event-related potentials as an index of similarity between words and pictures

Event-related potentials as an index of similarity between words and pictures Psychophysiology, 42 (25), 361 368. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 25 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 1.1111/j.1469-8986.25.295.x BRIEF REPORT Event-related potentials

More information

ABSTRACT 1. INTRODUCTION 2. ARTIFACT REJECTION ON RAW DATA

ABSTRACT 1. INTRODUCTION 2. ARTIFACT REJECTION ON RAW DATA AUTOMATIC ARTIFACT REJECTION FOR EEG DATA USING HIGH-ORDER STATISTICS AND INDEPENDENT COMPONENT ANALYSIS A. Delorme, S. Makeig, T. Sejnowski CNL, Salk Institute 11 N. Torrey Pines Road La Jolla, CA 917,

More information

Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics. Scott Makeig. sccn.ucsd.edu

Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics. Scott Makeig. sccn.ucsd.edu Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics Scott Makeig Institute for Neural Computation University of California San Diego La Jolla CA sccn.ucsd.edu Talk given at the EEG/MEG course

More information

Cardiac and electro-cortical responses to performance feedback reflect different aspects of feedback processing

Cardiac and electro-cortical responses to performance feedback reflect different aspects of feedback processing Cardiac and electro-cortical responses to performance feedback reflect different aspects of feedback processing Frederik M. van der Veen 1,2, Sander Nieuwenhuis 3, Eveline A.M Crone 2 & Maurits W. van

More information

BRAIN RESEARCH 1104 (2006) available at

BRAIN RESEARCH 1104 (2006) available at available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Comparative analysis of event-related potentials during Go/NoGo and CPT: Decomposition of electrophysiological markers

More information

Electrophysiological Evidence for Different Inhibitory Mechanisms When Stopping or Changing a Planned Response

Electrophysiological Evidence for Different Inhibitory Mechanisms When Stopping or Changing a Planned Response Electrophysiological Evidence for Different Inhibitory Mechanisms When Stopping or Changing a Planned Response Ulrike M. Krämer 1,2, Robert T. Knight 1, and Thomas F. Münte 2,3 Abstract People are able

More information

An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search

An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search Steven Phillips (steve@ni.aist.go.jp) Neuroscience Research Institute (AIST), Tsukuba Central 2, 1-1-1 Umezono, Tsukuba, Ibaraki 305-8568

More information

The auditory P3 from passive and active three-stimulus oddball paradigm

The auditory P3 from passive and active three-stimulus oddball paradigm Research paper Acta Neurobiol Exp 2008, 68: 362 372 The auditory P3 from passive and active three-stimulus oddball paradigm Eligiusz Wronka 1,2 *, Jan Kaiser 1, and Anton M.L. Coenen 2 1 Institute of Psychology,

More information

Behavioural and electrophysiological measures of task switching during single and mixed-task conditions

Behavioural and electrophysiological measures of task switching during single and mixed-task conditions Biological Psychology 72 (2006) 278 290 www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho Behavioural and electrophysiological measures of task switching during single and mixed-task conditions Philippe Goffaux a,b, Natalie

More information

Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return

Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return David J. Prime and Pierre Jolicoeur Abstract & Here we examined the relationship between inhibition of return (IOR) and response-selection

More information

Modifying the Classic Peak Picking Technique Using a Fuzzy Multi Agent to Have an Accurate P300-based BCI

Modifying the Classic Peak Picking Technique Using a Fuzzy Multi Agent to Have an Accurate P300-based BCI Modifying the Classic Peak Picking Technique Using a Fuzzy Multi Agent to Have an Accurate P3-based BCI Gholamreza Salimi Khorshidi School of cognitive sciences, Institute for studies in theoretical physics

More information

Novel single trial movement classification based on temporal dynamics of EEG

Novel single trial movement classification based on temporal dynamics of EEG Novel single trial movement classification based on temporal dynamics of EEG Conference or Workshop Item Accepted Version Wairagkar, M., Daly, I., Hayashi, Y. and Nasuto, S. (2014) Novel single trial movement

More information

What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3

What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3 * Manuscript-title pg, abst, fig... Debener et al. Independent components of the auditory novelty oddball What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3 event-related

More information

EEG based biomarkers in pediatric neuropsychiatry: ADHD autism (ASD)

EEG based biomarkers in pediatric neuropsychiatry: ADHD autism (ASD) EEG based biomarkers in pediatric neuropsychiatry: ADHD autism (ASD) Neuropsychologist PhD Geir Ogrim NORWAY geir.ogrim@so-hf.no Affiliations Neuroteam, Child psychiatry service, Østfold Hospital Trust

More information

Do Rare Stimuli Evoke Large P3s by Being Unexpected? A Comparison of Oddball Effects Between Standard-Oddball and Prediction-Oddball Tasks

Do Rare Stimuli Evoke Large P3s by Being Unexpected? A Comparison of Oddball Effects Between Standard-Oddball and Prediction-Oddball Tasks Do Rare Stimuli Evoke Large P3s by Being Unexpected? A Comparison of Oddball Effects Between Standard-Oddball and Prediction-Oddball Tasks Rolf Verleger 1,2 and Kamila Śmigasiewicz 1 1 Department of Neurology,

More information

Final Summary Project Title: Cognitive Workload During Prosthetic Use: A quantitative EEG outcome measure

Final Summary Project Title: Cognitive Workload During Prosthetic Use: A quantitative EEG outcome measure American Orthotic and Prosthetic Association (AOPA) Center for Orthotics and Prosthetics Leraning and Outcomes/Evidence-Based Practice (COPL) Final Summary 2-28-14 Project Title: Cognitive Workload During

More information

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 2008, 8 (2), 195-210 doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.2.195 The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies MIKA

More information

Time Course of Processes and Representations Supporting Visual Object Identification and Memory

Time Course of Processes and Representations Supporting Visual Object Identification and Memory Time Course of Processes and Representations Supporting Visual Object Identification and Memory Haline E. Schendan 1 and Marta Kutas 2 Abstract & Event-related potentials (ERPs) were used to delineate

More information

Dual Mechanisms for the Cross-Sensory Spread of Attention: How Much Do Learned Associations Matter?

Dual Mechanisms for the Cross-Sensory Spread of Attention: How Much Do Learned Associations Matter? Cerebral Cortex January 2010;20:109--120 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp083 Advance Access publication April 24, 2009 Dual Mechanisms for the Cross-Sensory Spread of Attention: How Much Do Learned Associations

More information

Stefan Debener a,b, *, Scott Makeig c, Arnaud Delorme c, Andreas K. Engel a,b. Research report

Stefan Debener a,b, *, Scott Makeig c, Arnaud Delorme c, Andreas K. Engel a,b. Research report Cognitive Brain Research 22 (2005) 309 321 Research report What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3 event-related potential as revealed by independent component

More information

The Effects of Temporal Preparation on Reaction Time

The Effects of Temporal Preparation on Reaction Time University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School January 2013 The Effects of Temporal Preparation on Reaction Time Glen Robert Forester University of South

More information

The effect of response execution on P3 latency, reaction time, and movement time

The effect of response execution on P3 latency, reaction time, and movement time Psychophysiology, 36 ~1999!, 351 363. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1999 Society for Psychophysiological Research The effect of response execution on P3 latency, reaction time,

More information

The EEG Analysis of Auditory Emotional Stimuli Perception in TBI Patients with Different SCG Score

The EEG Analysis of Auditory Emotional Stimuli Perception in TBI Patients with Different SCG Score Open Journal of Modern Neurosurgery, 2014, 4, 81-96 Published Online April 2014 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/ojmn http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/ojmn.2014.42017 The EEG Analysis of Auditory Emotional

More information

Beware misleading cues: Perceptual similarity modulates the N2/P3 complex

Beware misleading cues: Perceptual similarity modulates the N2/P3 complex Psychophysiology, 43 (2006), 253 260. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2006 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00409.x Beware misleading cues:

More information

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES Carryl L. Baldwin and Joseph T. Coyne Department of Psychology Old Dominion University

More information

F E M M Faculty of Economics and Management Magdeburg

F E M M Faculty of Economics and Management Magdeburg OTTO-VON-GUERICKE-UNIVERSITY MAGDEBURG FACULTY OF ECONOMICS AND MANAGEMENT Is brain activity observable that leads to an evaluation of a probability of 0.5 that is different from 0.5 in binary lottery

More information

The mind s eye, looking inward? In search of executive control in internal attention shifting

The mind s eye, looking inward? In search of executive control in internal attention shifting Psychophysiology, 40 (2003), 572 585. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2003 Society for Psychophysiological Research The mind s eye, looking inward? In search of executive control

More information

From single-trial EEG to brain area dynamics

From single-trial EEG to brain area dynamics Neurocomputing 44 46 (2002) 1057 1064 www.elsevier.com/locate/neucom From single-trial EEG to brain area dynamics A. Delorme a;, S. Makeig a, M. Fabre-Thorpe b, T. Sejnowski a a The Salk Institute for

More information

Power-Based Connectivity. JL Sanguinetti

Power-Based Connectivity. JL Sanguinetti Power-Based Connectivity JL Sanguinetti Power-based connectivity Correlating time-frequency power between two electrodes across time or over trials Gives you flexibility for analysis: Test specific hypotheses

More information

Electrophysiological evidence for preparatory reconfiguration before voluntary task switches but not cued task switches

Electrophysiological evidence for preparatory reconfiguration before voluntary task switches but not cued task switches Psychon Bull Rev (2014) 21:454 461 DOI 10.3758/s13423-013-0499-8 BRIEF REPORT Electrophysiological evidence for preparatory reconfiguration before voluntary task switches but not cued task switches Min-Suk

More information

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Cognitive Brain Research 18 (2004) 255 272 Research report Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Audrey Duarte a, *, Charan Ranganath

More information

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Journal: Psychological Science Manuscript ID: PSCI-0-0.R Manuscript Type: Short report Date Submitted by the Author:

More information

ERP Correlates of Feedback and Reward Processing in the Presence and Absence of Response Choice

ERP Correlates of Feedback and Reward Processing in the Presence and Absence of Response Choice Cerebral Cortex May 2005;15:535-544 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh153 Advance Access publication August 18, 2004 ERP Correlates of Feedback and Reward Processing in the Presence and Absence of Response Choice

More information

Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention.

Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention. Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention. All behaviors unfold over time; therefore, our ability to perceive and adapt our behavior according to the temporal constraints

More information

Aerobic Fitness and Cognitive Development: Event-Related Brain Potential and Task Performance Indices of Executive Control in Preadolescent Children

Aerobic Fitness and Cognitive Development: Event-Related Brain Potential and Task Performance Indices of Executive Control in Preadolescent Children Illinois Wesleyan University From the SelectedWorks of Jason R. Themanson, Ph.D 2009 Aerobic Fitness and Cognitive Development: Event-Related Brain Potential and Task Performance Indices of Executive Control

More information

The observation of contingent negative variation with reaction times in non-human. primates. Nam Soo Kim. Thesis. Submitted to the Faculty of the

The observation of contingent negative variation with reaction times in non-human. primates. Nam Soo Kim. Thesis. Submitted to the Faculty of the The observation of contingent negative variation with reaction times in non-human primates By Nam Soo Kim Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate School of Vanderbilt University in Partial fulfillment

More information

Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation

Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation Rongjun Yu a and Xiaolin Zhou a,b,c a Department of Psychology, Peking University, b State

More information

An investigation of the effect of preparation on response execution and inhibition in the go/nogo task

An investigation of the effect of preparation on response execution and inhibition in the go/nogo task University of Wollongong Research Online University of Wollongong Thesis Collection 1954-2016 University of Wollongong Thesis Collections 2005 An investigation of the effect of preparation on response

More information

COMPENSATING FATIGUE BY CAFFEINE

COMPENSATING FATIGUE BY CAFFEINE COMPENSATING FATIGUE BY CAFFEINE J. Snel 1, M.B. de Ruiter 1, J. Ruijter 1 and M.M. Lorist 2 1 Department of Psychonomics, Faculty of Psychology, University of Amsterdam 2 Faculty of Psychology, Rijks

More information

Working Memory Impairments Limitations of Normal Children s in Visual Stimuli using Event-Related Potentials

Working Memory Impairments Limitations of Normal Children s in Visual Stimuli using Event-Related Potentials 2015 6th International Conference on Intelligent Systems, Modelling and Simulation Working Memory Impairments Limitations of Normal Children s in Visual Stimuli using Event-Related Potentials S. Z. Mohd

More information

Dissociable Neural Correlates of Intention and Action Preparation in Voluntary Task Switching

Dissociable Neural Correlates of Intention and Action Preparation in Voluntary Task Switching Cerebral Cortex February 2014;24:465 478 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs326 Advance Access publication October 26, 2012 Dissociable Neural Correlates of Intention and Action Preparation in Voluntary Task Switching

More information

Support Vector Machine Classification and Psychophysiological Evaluation of Mental Workload and Engagement of Intuition- and Analysis-Inducing Tasks

Support Vector Machine Classification and Psychophysiological Evaluation of Mental Workload and Engagement of Intuition- and Analysis-Inducing Tasks Support Vector Machine Classification and Psychophysiological Evaluation of Mental Workload and Engagement of Intuition- and Analysis-Inducing Tasks Presenter: Joseph Nuamah Department of Industrial and

More information

Jan Kaiser, Andrzej Beauvale and Jarostaw Bener. Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, 13 Golcbia St., ?

Jan Kaiser, Andrzej Beauvale and Jarostaw Bener. Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, 13 Golcbia St., ? The evoked cardiac response as 0.0 1 1. a runction or cognitive load in subjects differing on the individual difference variable of reaction time Jan Kaiser, Andrzej Beauvale and Jarostaw Bener Institute

More information

Frontal midline theta and N200 amplitude reflect complementary information about expectancy and outcome evaluation

Frontal midline theta and N200 amplitude reflect complementary information about expectancy and outcome evaluation Psychophysiology, 50 (2013), 550 562. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2013 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12040 Frontal midline theta and N200 amplitude

More information

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Activation of effect codes in response planning - 1 - To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Michael Ziessler

More information

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL for Cardoso et al. 22 The Neuroimaging Signal is a Linear Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. : Appendix: Homogeneous Linear ( Null ) and Modified Linear

More information

How Effective are Monetary Incentives for Improving Context Updating in Younger and Older Adults?

How Effective are Monetary Incentives for Improving Context Updating in Younger and Older Adults? Hannah Schmitt International Research Training Group Adaptive Minds Saarland University, Saarbruecken Germany How Effective are Monetary Incentives for Improving Context Updating in Younger and Older Adults?

More information

John E. Richards* Department of Psychology, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC 29208, United States

John E. Richards* Department of Psychology, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC 29208, United States International Journal of Psychophysiology 54 (2004) 201 220 www.elsevier.com/locate/ijpsycho Recovering dipole sources from scalp-recorded event-related-potentials using component analysis: principal component

More information

Neural Correlates of Response Inhibition in Early Childhood: Evidence from a Go/No-Go Task

Neural Correlates of Response Inhibition in Early Childhood: Evidence from a Go/No-Go Task University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory - Faculty and Staff Publications Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory

More information

EEG-Rhythm Dynamics during a 2-back Working Memory Task and Performance

EEG-Rhythm Dynamics during a 2-back Working Memory Task and Performance EEG-Rhythm Dynamics during a 2-back Working Memory Task and Performance Tsvetomira Tsoneva, Davide Baldo, Victor Lema and Gary Garcia-Molina Abstract Working memory is an essential component of human cognition

More information

Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude

Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School 2004 Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude Tim Skinner University of South Florida Follow this and

More information

BENEFITS OF MULTI-DOMAIN FEATURE OF MISMATCH NEGATIVITY EXTRACTED BY NONNEGATIVE TENSOR FACTORIZATION FROM EEG COLLECTED BY LOW-DENSITY ARRAY

BENEFITS OF MULTI-DOMAIN FEATURE OF MISMATCH NEGATIVITY EXTRACTED BY NONNEGATIVE TENSOR FACTORIZATION FROM EEG COLLECTED BY LOW-DENSITY ARRAY International Journal of Neural Systems World Scientific Publishing Company BENEFITS OF MULTI-DOMAIN FEATURE OF MISMATCH NEGATIVITY EXTRACTED BY NONNEGATIVE TENSOR FACTORIZATION FROM EEG COLLECTED BY LOW-DENSITY

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information ten Oever and Sack 10.1073/pnas.1517519112 SI Materials and Methods Experiment 1. Participants. A total of 20 participants (9 male; age range 18 32 y; mean age 25 y) participated

More information

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention Sleep Deprivation and Selective Attention An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention Logan T. Trujillo, PhD 1 ; Steve Kornguth,

More information

Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized. Readiness Potential. Dianna Monteith. Saint Thomas University

Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized. Readiness Potential. Dianna Monteith. Saint Thomas University Extraversion and the LRP 1 Running head: EXTRAVERSION AND THE LRP Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized Readiness Potential Dianna Monteith Saint Thomas

More information

The Influence of Induced Anxiety on the P3 Event Related Brain Potentials of Athletes and Novices in a Go/NoGo Task

The Influence of Induced Anxiety on the P3 Event Related Brain Potentials of Athletes and Novices in a Go/NoGo Task The Influence of Induced Anxiety on the P3 Event Related Brain Potentials of Athletes and Novices in a Go/NoGo Task Conley Waters and Keri Weed University of South Carolina Aiken An elite athlete employs

More information

Attentional Blink Paradigm

Attentional Blink Paradigm Attentional Blink Paradigm ATTENTIONAL BLINK 83 ms stimulus onset asychrony between all stimuli B T D A 3 N P Z F R K M R N Lag 3 Target 1 Target 2 After detection of a target in a rapid stream of visual

More information

Tracking pattern learning with single-trial event-related potentials

Tracking pattern learning with single-trial event-related potentials Clinical Neurophysiology 117 (2006) 1957 1973 www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph Tracking pattern learning with single-trial event-related potentials Marijtje L.A. Jongsma a, *, Tom Eichele b, Clementina M.

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 11: Attention & Decision making Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis

More information

International Journal of Neurology Research

International Journal of Neurology Research International Journal of Neurology Research Online Submissions: http://www.ghrnet.org/index./ijnr/ doi:1.1755/j.issn.313-511.1..5 Int. J. of Neurology Res. 1 March (1): 1-55 ISSN 313-511 ORIGINAL ARTICLE

More information

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention University of New Mexico UNM Digital Repository Psychology ETDs Electronic Theses and Dissertations 2-9-2010 Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention David B.

More information

Provided by the author(s) and NUI Galway in accordance with publisher policies. Please cite the published version when available. Title Prefrontal cortex and the generation of oscillatory visual persistence

More information

Rapid Context-based Identification of Target Sounds in an Auditory Scene

Rapid Context-based Identification of Target Sounds in an Auditory Scene Rapid Context-based Identification of Target Sounds in an Auditory Scene Marissa L. Gamble and Marty G. Woldorff Abstract To make sense of our dynamic and complex auditory environment, we must be able

More information

The influence of predictive value of cues in the endogenous orienting paradigm examined with event-related lateralizations

The influence of predictive value of cues in the endogenous orienting paradigm examined with event-related lateralizations The influence of predictive value of cues in the endogenous orienting paradigm examined with event-related lateralizations Franka Roorda First supervisor: Rob van der Lubbe Second supervisor: Suzanne Vosslamber

More information

EEG changes accompanying learned regulation of 12-Hz EEG activity

EEG changes accompanying learned regulation of 12-Hz EEG activity TNSRE-2002-BCI015 1 EEG changes accompanying learned regulation of 12-Hz EEG activity Arnaud Delorme and Scott Makeig Abstract We analyzed 15 sessions of 64-channel EEG data recorded from a highly trained

More information

ERROR PROCESSING IN CLINICAL POPULATIONS IN CONTRAST TO ADHD J.J.VAN DER MEERE

ERROR PROCESSING IN CLINICAL POPULATIONS IN CONTRAST TO ADHD J.J.VAN DER MEERE ERROR PROCESSING IN CLINICAL POPULATIONS IN CONTRAST TO ADHD J.J.VAN DER MEERE 1 THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK OF ERROR PROCESSING Mean RT x error x 500 ms x x x RTe+1 2 CEREBRAL PALSY an umbrella term covering

More information

Supplementary Information on TMS/hd-EEG recordings: acquisition and preprocessing

Supplementary Information on TMS/hd-EEG recordings: acquisition and preprocessing Supplementary Information on TMS/hd-EEG recordings: acquisition and preprocessing Stability of the coil position was assured by using a software aiming device allowing the stimulation only when the deviation

More information

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),???-??? 794-799 What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? ULRICH MAYR University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon Schneider and

More information

Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology

Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology Rui Cao (caorui.beilia@gmail.com) Thomas A. Busey (busey@indiana.edu) Robert M. Nosofsky (nosofsky@indiana.edu) Richard

More information

Sequential P3 effects in a Posner s spatial cueing paradigm: Trial-by-trial learning of the predictive value of the cue

Sequential P3 effects in a Posner s spatial cueing paradigm: Trial-by-trial learning of the predictive value of the cue Research paper Acta Neurobiol Exp 2009, 69: 155 167 Sequential P3 effects in a Posner s spatial cueing paradigm: Trial-by-trial learning of the predictive value of the cue Carlos M. Gómez*, Angélica Flores,

More information

Seizure onset can be difficult to asses in scalp EEG. However, some tools can be used to increase the seizure onset activity over the EEG background:

Seizure onset can be difficult to asses in scalp EEG. However, some tools can be used to increase the seizure onset activity over the EEG background: This presentation was given during the Dianalund Summer School on EEG and Epilepsy, July 24, 2012. The main purpose of this introductory talk is to show the possibilities of improved seizure onset analysis

More information

EEG Recordings from Parent/Child Dyads in a Turn-Taking Game with Prediction Error

EEG Recordings from Parent/Child Dyads in a Turn-Taking Game with Prediction Error EEG Recordings from Parent/Child Dyads in a Turn-Taking Game with Prediction Error Julia Anna Adrian Department of Cognitive Science University of California San Diego La Jolla, 92093 jadrian@ucsd.edu

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Forsyth et al. 10.1073/pnas.1509262112 SI Methods Inclusion Criteria. Participants were eligible for the study if they were between 18 and 30 y of age; were comfortable reading in

More information

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory?

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory? Running Head: Visual Working Memory and the CDA Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory? Nick Berggren and

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Teaching brain-machine interfaces as an alternative paradigm to neuroprosthetics control

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Teaching brain-machine interfaces as an alternative paradigm to neuroprosthetics control SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Teaching brain-machine interfaces as an alternative paradigm to neuroprosthetics control Authors: Iñaki Iturrate 1,2, Ricardo Chavarriaga 2, Luis Montesano 1, Javier Minguez 1,

More information

Address for correspondence: School of Psychological Sciences, Zochonis Building, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, M139PL, Manchester, UK

Address for correspondence: School of Psychological Sciences, Zochonis Building, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, M139PL, Manchester, UK BBS-D-15-00891_ Mather_ Talmi & Barnacle Emotionally arousing context modulates the ERP correlates of neutral picture processing: An ERP test of the Glutamate Amplifies Noradrenergic Effects (GANE) model

More information

Electrophysiological evidence of two different types of error in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

Electrophysiological evidence of two different types of error in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Cognitive Neuroscience 10, 1±5 (1999) THE speci city of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) for assessing frontal lobe pathology remains controversial, although lesion and cerebral blood ow studies

More information

Event-related potential (ERP) asymmetries to emotional stimuli in a visual half-field paradigm

Event-related potential (ERP) asymmetries to emotional stimuli in a visual half-field paradigm Psychophysiology, 34 (1997), 414-426. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1997 Society for Psychophysiological Research Disclaimer: This file is for the convenience of interested

More information

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Intro Examine attention response functions Compare an attention-demanding

More information

The relation of aerobic fitness to neuroelectric indices of cognitive and motor task preparation

The relation of aerobic fitness to neuroelectric indices of cognitive and motor task preparation Psychophysiology, 7 (21), 11. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 21 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 1.1111/j.169-96.21.992.x The relation of aerobic fitness to neuroelectric

More information

Neural Strategies for Selective Attention Distinguish Fast-Action Video Game Players

Neural Strategies for Selective Attention Distinguish Fast-Action Video Game Players Neural Strategies for Selective Attention Distinguish Fast-Action Video Game Players - Online First - Springer 7/6/1 1:4 PM Neural Strategies for Selective Attention Distinguish Fast-Action Video Game

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

The Roles of Feature-Specific Task Set and Bottom-Up Salience in Attentional Capture: An ERP Study

The Roles of Feature-Specific Task Set and Bottom-Up Salience in Attentional Capture: An ERP Study Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2009, Vol. 35, No. 5, 1316 1328 2009 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0015872 The Roles of Feature-Specific

More information

Presented at the 44th Annual Meeting of the Society for Psychophysiological Research, October 20-24, 2004, Santa Fe, New Mexico, USA.

Presented at the 44th Annual Meeting of the Society for Psychophysiological Research, October 20-24, 2004, Santa Fe, New Mexico, USA. Measures and Models for Estimating and Predicting Cognitive Fatigue Leonard J. Trejo, NASA Ames Research Center Rebekah Kochavi, QSS Group, Inc. Karla Kubitz, Towson University Leslie D. Montgomery, NASA

More information

The error-related potential and BCIs

The error-related potential and BCIs The error-related potential and BCIs Sandra Rousseau 1, Christian Jutten 2 and Marco Congedo 2 Gipsa-lab - DIS 11 rue des Mathématiques BP 46 38402 Saint Martin d Héres Cedex - France Abstract. The error-related

More information

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes Biological Psychology 66 (2004) 1 20 The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes Bettina Forster, Martin Eimer School of

More information

The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture

The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture Psychophysiology, 53 (2016), 650 662. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright VC 2016 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12615 The role of top-down spatial attention

More information