On the right side? A longitudinal study of left- versus right-lateralized semantic dementia

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "On the right side? A longitudinal study of left- versus right-lateralized semantic dementia"

Transcription

1 doi: /brain/awv387 BRAIN 2016: 139; On the right side? A longitudinal study of left- versus right-lateralized semantic dementia Fiona Kumfor, 1,2,3 Ramon Landin-Romero, 1,3 Emma Devenney, 1,3,4 Rosalind Hutchings, 1,2,3 Roberto Grasso, 1 John R. Hodges 1,2,3 and Olivier Piguet 1,2,3 The typical presentation of semantic dementia is associated with marked, left predominant anterior temporal lobe atrophy and with changes in language. About 30% of individuals, however, present with predominant right anterior temporal lobe atrophy, usually accompanied by behavioural changes and prosopagnosia. Here, we aimed to establish whether these initially distinct clinical presentations evolve into a similar syndrome at the neural and behavioural level. Thirty-one patients who presented with predominant anterior temporal lobe atrophy were included. Based on imaging, patients were categorized as either predominant left (n = 22) or right (n = 9) semantic dementia. Thirty-three Alzheimer s disease patients and 25 healthy controls were included for comparison. Participants completed the Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination, a Face and Emotion Processing Battery and the Cambridge Behavioural Inventory, and underwent magnetic resonance imaging annually. Longitudinal neuroimaging analyses showed greater right temporal pole atrophy in left semantic dementia than Alzheimer s disease, whereas right semantic dementia showed greater orbitofrontal and left temporal lobe atrophy than Alzheimer s disease. Importantly, direct comparisons between semantic dementia groups revealed that over time, left semantic dementia showed progressive thinning in the right temporal pole, whereas right semantic dementia showed thinning in the orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate. Behaviourally, longitudinal analyses revealed that general cognition declined in all patients. In contrast, patients with left and right semantic dementia showed greater emotion recognition decline than Alzheimer s disease. In addition, left semantic dementia showed greater motivation loss than Alzheimer s disease. Correlational analyses revealed that emotion recognition was associated with right temporal pole, right medial orbitofrontal and right fusiform integrity, while changes in motivation were associated with right temporal pole cortical thinning. While left and right semantic dementia show distinct profiles at presentation, both phenotypes develop deficits in emotion recognition and behaviour. These findings highlight the pervasive socio-emotional deficits in frontotemporal dementia, even in patients with an initial language presentation. These changes reflect right anterior temporal and orbitofrontal cortex degeneration, underscoring the role of these regions in social cognition and behaviour. 1 Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney, Australia 2 School of Medical Sciences, the University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia 3 ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Sydney, Australia 4 Prince of Wales Clinical School, the University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia Correspondence to: Dr Fiona Kumfor, Neuroscience Research Australia, PO Box 1165, Randwick, Sydney, NSW, 2031, Australia f.kumfor@neura.edu.au Keywords: frontotemporal dementia; temporal lobe; social cognition; behaviour Abbreviations: ACE = Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination; CBI = Cambridge Behavioural Inventory; LME = linear mixed effects; SD = semantic dementia Received September 14, Revised October 21, Accepted November 6, Advance Access publication January 25, 2016 ß The Author (2016). Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Guarantors of Brain. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please journals.permissions@oup.com

2 Left- versus right-lateralized SD BRAIN 2016: 139; Introduction The clinical manifestation of dementia varies widely, depending on the pattern of initial brain changes and the spread of pathology with disease progression. Frontotemporal dementia is characterized by a progressive neurodegeneration of the frontal and/or temporal lobes. Disproportionate atrophy of the temporal lobe is usually associated with a generalized loss of semantic knowledge, and is clinically known as semantic-variant primary progressive aphasia (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2011) or semantic dementia (SD) (Snowden et al., 1989; Hodges et al., 1992). The primary complaint in patients with SD is a loss of memory for words/anomia, with this profound loss of semantic knowledge evident irrespective of the modality of testing (Hodges et al., 1992). Typically, individuals diagnosed with SD show a bilateral but asymmetric pattern of brain atrophy, where the left anterior temporal lobe regions are markedly more affected than the right (Mion et al., 2010). Studies of these patients with typical, left predominant SD have directly influenced our understanding of the representation of semantic knowledge in humans, and identified the unique contribution of the left anterior temporal lobe in the representation of language (Lambon Ralph et al., 2001; Patterson et al., 2007). Importantly, in a proportion (30%) of patients, the anterior temporal lobe involvement is reversed, whereby atrophy is more pronounced in the right than the left anterior temporal lobe region (Chan et al., 2009). The clinical presentation of these predominant right SD cases is heterogeneous and includes inappropriate behaviour, prosopagnosia and/or semantic deficits (Chan et al., 2009; Josephs et al., 2009). Indeed, differentiating patients with right SD from the behavioural-variant of frontotemporal dementia is challenging, with the nosology of right SD remaining unclear (Josephs et al., 2009; Whitwell et al., 2009; Kamminga et al., 2015). One of the key unresolved issues is whether left and right SD represent distinct clinicopathological entities (Seeley et al., 2005; Josephs et al., 2009) and the extent that these syndromes evolve into a similar profile with disease progression. Longitudinal studies are scant but existing evidence suggests that, over time, individuals with right SD develop language features, whereas individuals with left SD may exhibit behavioural changes (Seeley et al., 2005). These reports, however, relied on clinical case reviews to determine the presence and nature of symptoms, which were not objectively measured. The authors proposed that the apparent evolution of behavioural and language features may reflect spreading of disease into the contralateral temporal lobe (Seeley et al., 2005). Neuroimaging data, however, were not available to explore this hypothesis. Few studies have attempted to quantify patterns of atrophy change in left SD over time. In one, the mean rate of volume loss over an 18-month period was reported to be significantly greater in the right temporal lobe and right hippocampus specifically, when compared to these structures in the left hemisphere (Rohrer et al., 2008). Atrophy progression in left SD, however, is not always restricted to the right hemisphere with changes in the left hemisphere also observed over time (Brambati et al., 2015), some even reporting greater left than right hemisphere degeneration (Rogalski et al., 2014). Thus, existing longitudinal studies of left SD have yielded mixed findings about the pattern of disease progression. In right SD, case studies have suggested that atrophy progresses from the right fusiform and parahippocampal gyrus anteriorly to the right temporal pole as well as the left anterior temporal regions, albeit to a lesser extent than the right (Joubert et al., 2004; Henry et al., 2012). To our knowledge, only one group study has investigated progressive grey matter atrophy in six patients with right SD and 13 patients with left SD (Brambati et al., 2009). Over a 1- year period, both groups showed atrophy in the hemisphere contralateral to that predominantly affected at presentation (Brambati et al., 2009). Patterns of atrophy between left and right SD, however, were not directly compared. Furthermore, how atrophy progresses in these individuals beyond 1 year remains unknown. Thus, while some evidence suggests that these syndromes may evolve into a similar clinical phenotype over time, comprehensive longitudinal clinical and neurological examination of these syndromes is lacking. Moreover, comparisons with other neurodegenerative brain disorders have not been conducted, and thus the extent that these changes are specific to left and right SD is unknown. The comparison of right and left SD is not only of interest from a clinical and nosological perspective, but may also help shed light on the function of the right temporal lobe. Damage to the right temporal lobe has been variably associated with impaired visual recognition (Milner, 1968), visual learning deficits (Helmstaedter et al., 1991), musical timbre discriminations (Samson and Zatorre, 1994), recognition of famous tunes (Hsieh et al., 2011) and memory for complex visual scenes (Pigott and Milner, 1993). More recently, however, a potential role in social cognition has been put forward (Olson et al., 2007, 2013). How the right temporal lobe supports social behaviour and how it interacts with other regions of the social brain such as the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex is yet to be fully elucidated. Here, we aimed to longitudinally examine a group of individuals who presented with predominant anterior temporal lobe atrophy (either left of right) and map the pattern of progression using a combination of neuroimaging and behavioural measures, with respect to a group of patients with Alzheimer s disease and healthy controls. The Alzheimer s disease group was included as a disease control group, and to demonstrate that even in a state of advanced neurodegeneration, SD and Alzheimer s disease remain entities that are clinically and neurologically distinct. Direct comparisons with healthy matched controls were conducted at baseline to determine patterns of neurodegeneration and behavioural impairment, specific to each patient group.

3 988 BRAIN 2016: 139; F. Kumfor et al. Our first aim was to determine the common and divergent baseline and longitudinal patterns of atrophy in these syndromes. The second aim was to longitudinally examine general cognition, face perception, emotion processing and behaviour, to understand how these abilities become affected over time. We hypothesized that left and right SD are essentially manifestations of the same pathological disorder and that over time their initially asymmetrical cortical thinning would extend into parallel structures in the contralateral hemisphere to create equivalent patterns of atrophy. Behaviourally, we hypothesized that individuals with right SD would show greater social cognition and behavioural impairment than left SD and Alzheimer s disease at baseline. Longitudinally, we predicted left SD would also develop social cognition and behavioural deficits. Materials and methods Participants Thirty-one dementia cases that presented to FRONTIER, the frontotemporal dementia research clinic in Sydney, with predominant anterior temporal lobe atrophy were included in this study. Twenty-two patients were classified as left SD and nine patients as right SD (Supplementary material). These individuals were compared with 33 age- and education-matched Alzheimer s disease patients and 25 healthy matched controls. All patients were assessed by the multidisciplinary team and diagnosis was reached by consensus according to current clinical diagnostic criteria (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2011; McKhann et al., 2011; Rascovsky et al., 2011) following a comprehensive clinical assessment including: neurological examination, neuropsychological assessment, structural brain MRI, informant report and independent review of the clinical files by an experienced behavioural neurologist (E.D. or J.R.H.). The Frontotemporal Dementia Rating Scale (FRS) was used to measure disease severity/functioning (Mioshi et al., 2010). Controls were recruited from the Neuroscience Research Australia Volunteer database and local community clubs. Controls scored 588/100 on the Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination-Revised (ACE-revised) (Mioshi et al., 2006) and 0 on the sum of boxes score of the Clinical Dementia Rating scale (Morris, 1993). Exclusion criteria for patients and controls included concurrent psychiatric disturbance, other neurodegenerative conditions or neurological disorders and/or history of substance abuse. The South Eastern Sydney Local Health District and the University of New South Wales ethics committees approved the study. Participants or their person responsible provided informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants volunteered their time and were reimbursed for travel costs. MRI acquisition Twenty-seven patients with SD (19 left SD and eight right SD), 32 patients with Alzheimer s disease and 25 controls underwent whole-brain structural MRI with a 3 T Phillips scanner using a standard 8-channel head coil. Three dimensional high-resolution turbo field echo T 1 -weighted sequences were acquired with the following parameters: coronal orientation, matrix , 200 slices, 1 mm 2 in-plane resolution, slice thickness 1 mm, echo time/repetition time 2.6/5.8 ms, flip angle a =19. Among the study participants, 24 patients with SD (17 left SD and seven right SD) and 25 patients with Alzheimer s disease underwent annual MRI (range 1 4 years, median number of MRI scans = 2). A total of 76 SD (54 left SD and 22 right SD) and 92 Alzheimer s disease consecutive scans were included in the study (Table 1). MRI scans were obtained on average within 2 months of the behavioural assessment. Classification of patients The baseline MRI scans of the SD participants were rated using a previously validated MRI rating scale by two trained raters (E.D., R.G.), blind to clinical diagnosis (Kipps et al., 2007; Davies et al., 2009). Inter-rater reliability was high for both raters (Cronbach s a = 0.9). The region of interest selected was the anterior temporal lobe. To reduce potential bias in ratings, all MRI scans were rated in both the correct orientation and a mirror image, and the left and right hemispheres were rated separately. Then, the average score for each rater on the correct and mirror image scans were averaged. Finally, the two raters scores were averaged and the participant was classified as predominantly left- or right-lateralized according to the side with greater atrophy. Research MRI scan was unavailable for four patients (one had pacemaker in situ, three had other metallic implants which prevented MRI scanning). For these individuals, classification into left or right SD group was established based on clinical magnetic resonance or CT images (see Supplementary material for additional information). Preprocessing of baseline and longitudinal imaging data Before analyses, the two T 1 volumes were merged and averaged to increase the signal-to-noise ratio and the grey matter white matter contrasts in brain structures. Freesurfer software, version ( was used for surface-based cortical processing (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999) using standard methods (Fischl and Dale, 2000). Cortical thickness was automatically extracted for each hemisphere at each time point. For the longitudinal processing, an unbiased within-subject template (Reuter and Fischl, 2011) was created using robust, inverse consistent registration between the available time points for each individual (Reuter et al., 2010). Several preprocessing steps, such as skull stripping, Talairach transformations, atlas registration, as well as spherical surface maps and parcellations were initialized with common information from the within-subject template, significantly increasing reliability and statistical power (Reuter et al., 2012). Cortical thickness was smoothed with a 20 mm full-width at half-height Gaussian kernel. This level of blurring kernel was chosen to reduce the impact of imperfect alignment between cortices and thereby improve the signal-to-noise ratio (Lerch and Evans, 2005). All the resulting

4 Left- versus right-lateralized SD BRAIN 2016: 139; Table 1 Demographics at baseline in left SD, right SD and Alzheimer s disease compared with controls Left SD n =22 Right SD n =9 AD n =33 Controls n =25 F P-value Post hoc M:F 13:9 5:4 21:12 12: # ns Age (years) ns Education (years) ns Disease duration (years) a ns FRS (Rasch score) b ns Number of behavioural visits right SD 5 left SD; left SD = AD Number of MRI scans ns Values are mean standard deviation. ns = P # Chi-square value. AD = Alzheimer s disease; FRS = Frontotemporal Dementia Rating Scale. Missing data: a Date of onset missing for one Alzheimer s disease participant; b FRS score missing for one left SD participant. images were visually inspected and manually corrected in the event of tissue segmentation errors. Subjects who had excessive surface segmentation errors after manual correction were excluded from the subsequent baseline (one control, one Alzheimer s disease patient) and follow-up (two Alzheimer s disease patients) statistical analyses. Neuropsychological and behavioural assessment All patients were assessed at least twice and, where possible, followed up annually. All data were included until the point of drop out. The outcome variables were obtained approximately annually. The ACE was used to assess general cognition, with the total score /100. Recently, the ACE-revised (Mioshi et al., 2006) has been updated (ACE-III) (Hsieh et al., 2013). Because the two versions are largely equivalent, we analysed either the ACErevised (R) score or the ACE-III score, to maximize the available data. We analysed both the total score, as a measure of general cognitive ability, as well as performance on the five subtests embedded within the ACE tapping into specific cognitive domains (attention/orientation, memory, language, visuospatial function, fluency). In addition, we examined language function with the Sydney Language Battery (SYDBAT) (Savage et al., 2013), focusing on the Naming, Comprehension and Semantic Association subtests. Finally, we used the copy of the Rey Complex Figure (Rey, 1941) to examine visuospatial ability. We used the Face and Emotion Processing Battery to assess socio-emotional function, which has been described previously (Miller et al., 2012; Kumfor et al., 2014). This battery assesses face perception and emotion decoding ability and consists of four subtests (i.e. Face-Perception, Face- Matching, Emotion-Matching, Emotion-Selection). During the Face-Perception task, individuals are shown 40 pairs of faces and are required to determine whether the images are identical or different. The remaining three tasks consist of 42 trials each. In the Face-Matching task, again, participants view pairs of faces expressing different emotions and are asked to determine whether the faces are the same person. In the Emotion-Matching task, participants view pairs of faces of different people and are asked to determine whether they are expressing the same emotion. Finally, in the Emotion-Selection task, individuals view an array of seven faces showing the six basic emotions (i.e. sadness, surprise, anger, disgust, fear, happiness) and a neutral expression and are asked to point to the face expressing the emotion that corresponds to the emotional label given verbally by the examiner. Images are from the NimStim stimulus set (www. mac-brain.org) and were cropped to remove any non-facial information (e.g. hair). All images are presented in greyscale. Responding is untimed and no feedback is provided. Behavioural change was assessed using the Cambridge Behavioural Inventory-Revised informant questionnaire (Wear et al., 2008; Wedderburn et al., 2008). This questionnaire assesses 10 domains of behaviour (e.g. mood, motivation, abnormal behaviour). For each item the informant reports how frequent the behaviour is on a 5-point scale from never (0) to constantly (4). Here, we focused on the subscales relevant to social function: abnormal behaviour (e.g. Shows socially embarrassing behaviour, Finds humour or laughs at things others do not find funny ), motivation (e.g. Shows reduced affection, Fails to maintain motivation to keep in contact with friends and family ), and stereotypical behaviour (e.g. Is rigid and fixed in her/his ideas, Develops routines from which s/he cannot easily be discouraged ). Statistical analyses Baseline and longitudinal comparisons of cortical thickness Baseline comparisons of cortical thickness between groups were performed using a vertex-wise general linear model (GLM) including cortical thickness as a dependent factor and group (healthy controls, left SD, right SD and Alzheimer s disease) as an independent factor, to identify patterns of neurodegeneration in each disease syndrome with respect to healthy controls. For the longitudinal analyses of cortical thinning, vertexwise comparisons of annual rate of change of cortical thickness among patient groups (left SD, right SD and Alzheimer s disease) were analysed using the Spatiotemporal Linear Mixed Effects (LME) Matlab tools (FreeSurfer version onwards) (Bernal-Rusiel et al., 2013a, b). We assumed minimal change in brain integrity in our control group over time, and therefore did not include them in the longitudinal analyses. LME models provide a powerful and flexible approach for analysing longitudinal data while handling variable missing rates, nonuniform timing and making use of subjects with a single time point to characterize population-level regionally specific differences in cortical thickness (Bernal-Rusiel et al., 2013a).

5 990 BRAIN 2016: 139; F. Kumfor et al. The mean response trends over time among the diagnostic groups (left SD, right SD and Alzheimer s disease) across the cortex were plotted. Visual inspection of the plots revealed linear trajectories over time for the three groups so we fitted a spatiotemporal LME model of cortical atrophy with two random effects: (i) the intercept; and (ii) time from baseline MRI acquisition (expressed in days). As the groups were matched at baseline in age, sex, disease duration and education, no covariates were included in the model. The null hypotheses of no change in cortical thickness over time and no Group Time interaction (i.e. group-specific atrophy rate) were tested. The significance maps for each contrast were written for visualization and postprocessing using tksurfer included in the Freesurfer software suite. We used the two-stage adaptive false discovery rate (FDR) procedure with an array of q- values (Benjamini et al., 2006) to control for multiple comparisons and the results were thresholded at a corrected P-value of Baseline and longitudinal behavioural analyses Analyses were conducted using SPSS Statistics, version 22.0 (IBM). One-way analyses of variance were conducted to examine demographic variables as well as the outcome variables at baseline (ACE-R, Face and Emotion Processing Battery, CBI). Categorical variables were analysed using chi-square analyses. Post hoc analyses were corrected for multiple comparisons using a Sidak adjustment. For the baseline analyses, we compared the left and right SD groups with the Alzheimer s disease and control groups, to determine patterns of cognitive and social impairment across disease groups, with respect to a well-characterized older adult population. LME models were used to model the change in outcome variable according to diagnosis over time. The fixed effects in the model included diagnosis, follow-up time, and the interaction between diagnosis and follow-up time. The only random effect included was the individual variability associated with the patient at baseline (where we used the random intercept model). Residual errors of the model and the random intercepts for each participant at baseline were assumed to be normally distributed. All participants were assumed to be independent. The longitudinal analyses compared left SD, right SD and Alzheimer s disease patients, as we assumed minimal change in cognition and social functioning in our control group over time. Finally, to determine the relationship between cortical thinning and behavioural performance we calculated weighted correlation coefficients for each individual, to enable examination of correlations between subjects across repeated observations (Bland and Altman, 1995). Pearson two-tailed correlational analyses were then conducted with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons P 5a=k = Results Demographics As shown in Table 1, no significant differences in basic demographics, including age, sex and education, were observed across diagnostic groups (all P-values ). All patient groups were matched for disease duration and functional ability as measured by the FRS (P-values ). A difference in the number of followup visits between patient groups was, however, observed with the right SD cases having on average one follow-up visit fewer than the left SD group (P = 0.02). Baseline neuroimaging findings The baseline neuroimaging findings in each patient group compared to controls are reported in the Supplementary material. In brief, these analyses confirmed the typical profiles of cortical thinning with left SD showing bilateral asymmetrical cortical thinning with the left temporal pole more affected than the right. The right SD group showed a similar pattern but lateralized to the right temporal pole. Finally, patients with Alzheimer s disease showed extensive cortical thinning in the posterior lateral and medial surfaces bilaterally. Next, we compared patterns of cortical thinning between patient groups at baseline (Fig. 1). Compared with Alzheimer s disease, left SD showed relatively circumscribed cortical thinning in the bilateral anterior temporal lobes, with the left temporal lobe more affected than the right (shown in red-yellow), whereas the Alzheimer s disease group (shown in blue) showed greater precuneus/posterior cingulate and bilateral posterior parietal cortical thinning compared to left SD. Right SD showed cortical thinning in the right anterior temporal lobe on both the medial and lateral surfaces, with similar but less extensive regions identified in the left temporal lobe compared with Alzheimer s disease (shown in red-yellow). In contrast, compared with right SD, the Alzheimer s disease group showed a few small clusters of cortical thinning in the left posterior cingulate (shown in blue). Critically, comparisons between left and right SD confirmed that although these groups showed bilateral temporal cortical thinning, significant differences in the pattern and degree of cortical thinning were present between groups. Specifically, left SD showed greater left anterior temporal thinning on both the lateral and medial surfaces of the temporal lobe than right SD (shown in redyellow), whereas right SD showed greater cortical thinning in the right anterior temporal lobe than left SD (shown in blue) (Fig. 1). Longitudinal neuroimaging findings Within-group longitudinal changes from baseline are depicted in Fig. 2A. In left SD, progressive cortical thinning was observed in the temporal lobes bilaterally, with more widespread thinning in the left temporal lobe extending posteriorly into the left temporoparietal junction. In addition, progressive thinning was observed in the left superior orbitofrontal and superior frontal cortices. In right SD, extensive cortical thinning was observed across the lateral surface of the right hemisphere,

6 Left- versus right-lateralized SD BRAIN 2016: 139; Figure 1 Baseline neuroimaging findings showing regions of cortical thinning between diagnostic groups. 4 denotes the group with greater cortical thinning. AD = Alzheimer s disease. together with somewhat more circumscribed thinning in the left lateral temporal lobe, anterior temporal pole and the bilateral cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex. In Alzheimer s disease, progressive cortical thinning was observed in the bilateral parietal and occipital regions, together with the posterior cingulate/precuneus and the inferior temporal gyri. Next, we compared the differential patterns of longitudinal cortical thinning across patient groups (Fig. 2B). Compared with Alzheimer s disease, left SD showed disproportionate cortical thinning in the right anterior temporal pole only, whereas Alzheimer s disease showed greater thinning in the right posterior cingulate/precuneus compared with left SD. In contrast, right SD showed greater cortical thinning in the left middle temporal gyrus extending into the superior temporal gyrus, together with the bilateral orbitofrontal cortices than Alzheimer s disease. In the right hemisphere, greater widespread cortical thinning was present across the lateral surface including the superior temporal gyrus, medial orbitofrontal cortex, and middle frontal gyrus, compared with Alzheimer s disease. No regions of greater cortical thinning were observed in Alzheimer s disease when compared with right SD. Finally, direct comparisons between left SD and right SD revealed distinct patterns of cortical thinning according to group. The patients with left SD showed greater cortical thinning in the right inferior temporal gyrus extending into the right fusiform when compared with right SD. In contrast, right SD showed greater thinning in the right medial orbitofrontal cortex and cingulate, right superior temporal gyrus and right middle frontal gyrus, when compared with left SD. Additional regions of cortical thinning were also identified in the right pre- and postcentral gyri and the right supramarginal gyrus in right SD compared to left SD. Importantly, these differences in progression do not appear to be due to a delayed diagnosis in the right SD group. A follow-up analysis comparing a subset of the patients with more severe left SD with the right SD group showed remarkably consistent findings as the overall analysis (Supplementary material). Baseline behavioural findings Cognition We investigated how these patterns of cortical thinning manifested clinically at baseline and over time across syndromes. At baseline, all patient groups showed overall cognitive impairment on the ACE-R compared to controls (all P-values ), with left SD also impaired on this task compared to Alzheimer s disease (P ) (Table 2). Detailed cognitive assessment indicated that left SD showed impaired performance in language, fluency, memory and attention on the ACE, as well as on the SYDBAT measures compared with both healthy controls and Alzheimer s disease. Right SD also showed impaired language, fluency and memory on the ACE and impaired naming, comprehension and semantic association on the SYDBAT, compared with controls and Alzheimer s disease. Alzheimer s disease patients in contrast showed impaired attention and memory, with fluency reduced compared to controls, but to a lesser extent than left SD. Alzheimer s disease also showed impaired visuospatial function on the RCF copy compared with controls but no impairments on the SYDBAT were observed (Supplementary material). Social function A significant effect of diagnosis was observed on the Face- Perception task (P = 0.045), with patients with right SD performing worse than controls (P = 0.037), whereas left SD and Alzheimer s disease performed within normal limits (P-values ). On the Face-Matching task,

7 992 BRAIN 2016: 139; F. Kumfor et al. Figure 2 Longitudinal changes in cortical thickness according to diagnostic group. (A) Within-group and (B) between-group longitudinal changes in cortical thickness in left SD, right SD and Alzheimer s disease (AD) patients. 4 denotes the group with greater cortical thinning. again a significant effect of diagnosis was observed (P = 0.002) with right SD (P = 0.002) impaired on this task, while the left SD (P = 0.242) and Alzheimer s disease groups (P = 0.062) performed within normal limits. A main effect of diagnosis was again present on the Emotion-Matching task (P ). Here, right SD (P = 0.002) and left SD (P = 0.001) were impaired, while patients with Alzheimer s disease performed similarly to controls (P = 0.200). No significant differences were present between patient groups (all P-values ). Finally, all patient groups performed worse than controls on the Emotion-Selection task, (all P-values ). Notably, right SD also performed significantly worse than the Alzheimer s disease group on this task (P = 0.001) and tended to also perform worse than left SD (P = 0.056). No difference between left SD and Alzheimer s disease was observed (P = 0.569). On the CBI Abnormal Behaviour subscale, the right SD group showed more frequent abnormal behaviour than the Alzheimer s disease group (P = 0.015). No difference was observed between left and right SD (P = 0.634), although a trend for more frequent abnormal behaviour in left SD than Alzheimer s disease was observed (P = 0.059). Right SD also exhibited decreased motivation compared to Alzheimer s disease (P = 0.022). No difference in motivation was reported between right and left SD (P = 0.311) or left SD and Alzheimer s disease groups (P = 0.378). Finally, no difference in the level of stereotypical behaviour amongst patient groups was observed at baseline (P = 0.105) (Table 2).

8 Left- versus right-lateralized SD BRAIN 2016: 139; Table 2 Baseline performance on the primary behavioural outcome variables according to diagnostic group Left SD n =22 Right SD n =9 AD n =33 Control n =25 F P-value Post hoc ACE Total Patients 5 controls; left SD 5AD Face-perception a Right SD 5 controls Face-matching a Right SD 5 controls Emotion-matching a Left SD, right SD 5 controls Emotion-selection a Patients 5 controls; right SD 5AD CBI-abnormal behaviour b Right SD 4AD CBI-motivation b Right SD 4AD CBI-stereotypical behaviour b ns Values are mean standard deviation. AD = Alzheimer s disease. Missing data: a Baseline scores on the Face and Emotion Processing subtests include individuals with at least one follow-up. Data available for: 19 left SD, eight right SD, 26 Alzheimer s disease and 25 controls. b Baseline scores on the CBI include individuals who had at least one follow-up on this measure: Data available for: 21 left SD, nine right SD and 32 Alzheimer s disease. Controls did not complete the CBI. Longitudinal behavioural performance Cognition The modelled trajectories for performance on the ACE, SYDBAT and RCF are reported in the Supplementary material. In brief, the only significant Time Diagnosis interaction on the ACE was on the language subtest (P = 0.002), with left SD (P ) declining more over time compared with Alzheimer s disease. Right SD also declined compared with Alzheimer s disease, but not significantly so (P = 0.326). On the SYDBAT Naming, Comprehension and Semantic Association subtests both left and right SD declined more than Alzheimer s disease (all P-values ). In contrast, on the RCF copy, patients with Alzheimer s disease declined significantly more than left SD (P = 0.003), but not right SD (P = 0.113) (Supplementary material). Social function The modelled trajectories for performance on the Face and Emotion Processing Battery and the CBI in left SD, right SD and Alzheimer s disease are shown in Table 3. These analyses revealed a significant change in performance over time on all tasks (P-values ranging between and ), with the exception of the Face-Matching task (P = 0.278). Absence of significant interactions between time and diagnosis on the ACE, Face-Perception task, Emotion-Matching task and the Abnormal Behaviour subscale of the CBI indicated that all patient groups declined at a similar rate on these measures. In contrast, a significant interaction between time and diagnosis was observed on the Emotion-Selection task (P =0.004), the CBI Motivation subscale (P = 0.026) and the CBI Stereotypical Behaviour subscale (P ) (Fig. 3). On the Emotion-Selection task, both left SD (P = 0.003) and right SD (P = 0.028) declined at a faster rate than Alzheimer s disease. The left SD group showed greater changes in motivation over time than patients with Alzheimer s disease (P = 0.008), whereas the rate of change in right SD did not differ significantly from Alzheimer s disease (P = 0.228). Both left (P ) and right (P = 0.002) SD groups showed significantly increased stereotypical behaviours with disease progression, compared with Alzheimer s disease. Correlations between cortical thinning and behavioural performance Finally, we investigated the associations between the behavioural variables (Emotion-Selection, CBI-motivation, CBIstereotypical) and the regions of cortical thinning (medial orbitofrontal cortex, temporal pole, fusiform cortex), which showed a significant Time Diagnosis interaction. Across all patients, correlational analyses revealed that emotion recognition ability was positively associated with cortical thickness of the right fusiform gyrus (r = 0.517, P ) and the right temporal pole (r = 0.450, P = ), with association with the right medial orbitofrontal cortex approaching significance (r = 0.354, P = ) (Table 4). In addition, reduced cortical thickness in the right temporal pole was associated with increased motivational difficulties (r = 0.409, P = ) and increased stereotypical behaviour (r = 0.354, P = ) as measured by the CBI. Correlation between the left temporal pole and motivation (r = 0.354, P = ) approached significance. Correlations separated by group did not reach statistical significance following correction for multiple comparisons. Correlations between cortical thinning and cognitive variables are reported in the Supplementary material. Discussion This study aimed to investigate longitudinal cortical changes in patients with SD with either predominant leftor right-lateralized temporal atrophy at initial presentation, and performance on aspects of social cognition and behaviour over time. Our results revealed divergent patterns of neurodegeneration both at baseline and longitudinally. Importantly, while left SD and right SD initially showed

9 994 BRAIN 2016: 139; F. Kumfor et al. Table 3 Longitudinal analyses of outcome variables in left SD, right SD and Alzheimer s disease Follow-up time Diagnosis follow-up time interaction Parameter estimation coefficients F P-value F P-value Left SD Right SD AD ACE total Face-perception Face-matching Emotion-matching Emotion-selection CBI abnormal behaviour CBI motivation CBI stereotypical behaviour Data available on at least two time points for 22 left SD, nine right SD and 33 Alzheimer s disease on the ACE; 19 left SD, eight right SD and 26 Alzheimer s disease on the Face and Emotion Processing Battery; and 21 left SD, nine right SD and 32 Alzheimer s disease on the CBI. Note, higher positive scores on the CBI indicate greater behavioural abnormalities. The parameter estimation coefficients represent the estimated rate of annual percentage change according to diagnostic group. Figure 3 Longitudinal performance on the behavioural variables with significant Time Diagnosis interaction. Markers represent individuals. Lines represent line of best fit according to diagnosis using linear regression. Baseline (Day 0) is determined with respect to the first neurology appointment and ACE assessment. predominantly asymmetric anterior temporal lobe involvement, involvement of the contralateral hemisphere became apparent with disease progression. Notably, direct comparisons between left and right SD revealed progressive degeneration of the right anterior temporal lobe in left SD, while right SD showed right orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate changes, together with diffuse lateral cortical thinning with disease progression. These patterns were in contrast to that seen in Alzheimer s disease, with cortical thinning affecting primarily posterior brain regions bilaterally in this group. Behaviourally, the most significant findings were a change in the rate of degradation of emotion recognition and social behaviour across patients over time. Here, we discuss how our findings inform the characterization of these subtypes of frontotemporal dementia, with respect to syndrome-specific patterns of atrophy and current understanding of the role of the right anterior temporal lobe and orbitofrontal cortex in social cognition. Our findings revealed that in left SD, progressive cortical thinning of the temporal lobes bilaterally occurred over time, in line with prior studies (Brambati et al., 2007, 2009, 2015; Lam et al., 2014). Importantly, here, we have demonstrated for the first time a disproportionate degradation of the right anterior temporal pole and right fusiform gyrus in this group compared to right SD and Alzheimer s disease. At the behavioural level, this manifested as decline across the majority of social cognition and behavioural measures, with significantly faster decline in emotion recognition and greater social behaviour changes than Alzheimer s disease. Moreover, we found a significant association between right temporal pole integrity and emotion recognition and behaviour. While individuals with left SD typically present with semantic impairment, recent studies have reported impaired emotion processing in these patients (Rosen et al., 2002; Kumfor and Piguet, 2012; Kumfor et al., 2013a), in addition to the relatively circumscribed and slowly progressing changes in language (Leyton et al., 2013). Our neuroimaging and behavioural findings support the view that involvement of the right anterior temporal lobe is responsible for the emergence of social cognition deficits in these patients (see also Irish et al., 2014). The functions of the right temporal lobe

10 Left- versus right-lateralized SD BRAIN 2016: 139; Table 4 Correlations between regions of cortical thinning and social functioning in patient groups Medial OFC Left Temporal pole Right Fusiform Medial OFC Temporal pole Fusiform Emotion-selection a * 0.450** 0.517** CBI-motivation b * ** CBI- stereotypical b * OFC = orbitofrontal cortex. Data missing: Emotion Selection test one left SD and two Alzheimer s disease; CBI for one left SD. **P 5 Bonferroni a/k = ; *P a degrees of freedom = 60; b degrees of freedom = 62. continue to be somewhat elusive to neuroscientists (Olson et al., 2007, 2013), with its primary role still contentious (Gainotti, 2015). Some argue that this region is necessary for representation of non-verbal information (with verbal information primarily subserved by the left temporal lobe), whilst others have suggested that the right temporal lobe is specialized for social cognition (for review of these arguments see Gainotti, 2015). Indeed, this region has been previously associated with abnormal emotional responses in the behavioural-variant of frontotemporal dementia and in clinical Alzheimer s disease (Kumfor et al., 2013a; Sturm et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2014). Our results align with the proposition that social cognition is lateralized to the right anterior temporal lobe. In right SD, our findings revealed widespread bilateral cortical thinning with disease progression. Importantly, compared to both Alzheimer s disease and left SD, right SD showed significant cortical thinning in the right orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate. Until now, the pattern of pathological spreading in right SD has been relatively unclear (see also Brambati et al., 2009). Behaviourally our right SD cohort exhibited widespread impairments in social cognition and behaviour at first presentation, including aspects of basic face and identity recognition, emotion perception and behavioural change, findings that concur with recent studies (Irish et al., 2013; Kumfor et al., 2015; Kamminga et al., 2015). Importantly, our findings revealed that with disease progression, individuals with right SD continue to decline significantly faster across most aspects of social cognition and behaviour, than patients with Alzheimer s disease. In addition, patients with right SD show additional loss of language abilities with disease progression. The continuing decline of social function converges with the documented cortical thinning in the right orbitofrontal cortex and right anterior cingulate in these patients. The degradation of the orbitofrontal cortex has been reliably associated with the manifestation of socioemotional disturbance in frontotemporal dementia, especially in individuals with the behavioural variant (Viskontas et al., 2007; Kumfor et al., 2013b; Hutchings et al., 2015). Indeed, here we found an association between right medial orbitofrontal cortex integrity and emotion recognition. The orbitofrontal cortex is thought to be involved in emotion processing, reward and decision-making (Bechara et al., 2000; Rolls, 2004) and may be particularly important for tagging the emotional nature of stimuli (Kumfor et al., 2013b). Our results suggest that while degradation of the right temporal pole likely underpins the manifestation of the initial symptoms in right SD, including prosopagnosia, with disease progression, encroachment into the right orbitofrontal cortex leads to further disintegration of social function in these patients. Of relevance here are the dense connections between the orbitofrontal cortex and the anterior temporal pole, via the uncinate fasciculus (Von Der Heide et al., 2013). How pathology spreads across different neuropathological syndromes still remains poorly understood (Fletcher and Warren, 2011). The pattern of progression in both left and right SD observed here, together with the previously reported degree of bilateral involvement in both these syndromes (Galton et al., 2001; Mion et al., 2010), however, point towards pathology spreading between functionally connected regions via commissural fibres (Fletcher and Warren, 2011). Fine-grained assessment of these syndromes with a focus on the ongoing degradation of social function and behaviour will be key to further unravelling the organization and function of the social brain. In Alzheimer s disease, social cognition and behaviour remained relatively spared despite the pervasive atrophy and progressive decline in general cognitive ability. These findings closely reflect anecdotal and clinical reports of maintained social graces in Alzheimer s disease even in the moderate disease stages (for review see Kumfor and Piguet, 2013). In contrast to the current findings, one study reported decline in emotion recognition over a 40- month period in seven patients with Alzheimer s disease (Lavenu and Pasquier, 2005). Here, with a large and well-characterized sample, we demonstrate minimal decline in social cognition and behaviour in Alzheimer s disease. This encouraging finding suggests that novel behavioural interventions may be able to harness this spared ability to improve the quality of life of Alzheimer s disease patients and their families (Kumfor et al., 2013b). Pathologically the vast majority of left SD patients have frontotemporal lobar degeneration with TAR DNA binding protein 43 (FTLD-TDP) pathology (Chare et al., 2014) but few studies have examined the pathological basis of right SD to date. A recent case study reported FTLD-TDP

11 996 BRAIN 2016: 139; F. Kumfor et al. pathology in an individual who initially presented with right SD, pointing towards a common pathological basis for these two clinical phenotypes (Henry et al., 2012), in keeping with previous studies (Hodges et al., 2004; Davies et al., 2005). Other reports, however, have suggested that only a subset of right SD cases with co-occurring semantic deficits have FTLD-TDP pathology, whereas those with a behavioural presentation were associated with tau mutations (Josephs et al., 2009). Future studies combining longitudinal clinical, neuroimaging and pathological findings will be essential to further understand the pathophysiological basis of these related syndromes. A further caveat is that our study included a relatively small number of right SD cases. Nevertheless, here the data were maximized using LME modelling. This approach handles unbalanced data (with variable missing rates across time points and imperfect timing), utilizes participants with a single assessment to characterize inter-subject variation and allows the representation of the group mean trajectory and covariance structure between serial measurements. Compared to alternative approaches, LME models offer superior statistical power in detecting longitudinal group differences (Bernal-Rusiel et al., 2013a), which is important when examining these rare clinical syndromes. In conclusion, our findings provide support for the hypothesis that, over time, left and right SD evolve into a similar clinical profile, with social cognition affected in both these phenotypes, despite showing divergent patterns of cortical thinning at baseline and with disease progression. Our findings also offer novel insights into the social brain, providing convergent evidence that both the right anterior temporal pole and orbitofrontal cortex are key players in the successful engagement in and contribution to complex human social interactions. These findings are important for clinicians to bear in mind when considering the disease course and prognosis of individuals with left and right SD and demonstrate the importance of studying these dementia syndromes to uncover new insights into brain-behaviour relationships. Acknowledgements The authors are grateful to the participants and their families for supporting our research. Funding This work was supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia (NHMRC) Research Project Grant (510106) and by funding to ForeFront, a collaborative research group dedicated to the study of frontotemporal dementia and motor neuron disease, from the NHMRC (APP ) and the Australia Research Council (ARC) Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders Memory Node (CE ). F.K. is supported by an Alzheimer s Australia Dementia Research Foundation Postdoctoral Fellowship. E.D. is supported by a UNSW PhD scholarship and the Motor Neuron Disease Association UK. R.H. is supported by an Australia Postgraduate Award. O.P. is supported by an NHMRC Career Development Fellowship (APP ). Supplementary material Supplementary material is available at Brain online. References Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 2000; 10: Benjamini Y, Krieger AM, Yekutieli D. Adaptive linear step-up procedures that control the false discovery rate. Biometrika 2006; 93: Bernal-Rusiel JL, Greve DN, Reuter M, Fischl B, Sabuncu MR; Alzheimer s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Statistical analysis of longitudinal neuroimage data with linear mixed effects models. Neuroimage 2013a; 66: Bernal-Rusiel, JL, Reuter, M, Greve, DN, Fischl, B, Sabuncu, MR; Alzheimer s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Spatiotemporal linear mixed effects modeling for the mass-univariate analysis of longitudinal neuroimage data. Neuroimage 2013b; 81: Bland JM, Altman DG. Calculating correlation coefficients with repeated observations: part 2 correlation between subjects. BMJ 1995; 310: 633. Brambati SM, Amici S, Racine CA, Neuhaus J, Miller Z, Ogar J, et al. Longitudinal gray matter contraction in three variants of primary progressive aphasia: a tenser-based morphometry study. Neuroimage Clin 2015; 8: Brambati SM, Rankin KP, Narvid J, Seeley WW, Dean D, Rosen HJ, et al. Atrophy progression in semantic dementia with assymmetric temporal involvement: a tensor-based morphometry study. Neurobiol Aging 2009; 30: Brambati SM, Renda NC, Rankin KP, Rosen H, Seeley WW, Ashburner J, et al. A tensor based morphometry study of longitudinal gray matter contraction in FTD. Neuroimage 2007; 35: Chan D, Anderson V, Pijenburg Y, Whitwell JL, Barnes J, Scahill R, et al. The clinical profile of right temporal lobe atrophy. Brain 2009; 132: Chare L, Hodges JR, Leyton CE, McGinley C, Tan RH, Kril JJ, et al. New criteria for frontotemporal dementia syndromes: clinical and pathological diagnostic implications. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2014; 85: Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis I: segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 1999; 9: Davies RR, Hodges JR, Kril JJ, Patterson K, Halliday G, Xuereb JH. The pathological basis of semantic dementia. Brain 2005; 2005: Davies RR, Scahill V, Graham A, Williams G, Graham K, Hodges J. Development of an MRI rating scale for multiple brain regions: comparison with volumetrics and with voxel-based morphometry. Neuroradiology 2009; 51: Fischl B, Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2000; 97: Fischl B, Sereno MI, Dale AM. Cortical surface-based analysis II: inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage 1999; 9:

O Connor 1. Appendix e-1

O Connor 1. Appendix e-1 O Connor 1 Appendix e-1 Neuropsychiatric assessment The Cambridge Behavioural Inventory Revised (CBI-R) 1, 2 is a proxy behavioural questionnaire that has been extensively used in studies involving FTD

More information

Piano playing skills in a patient with frontotemporal dementia: A longitudinal case study

Piano playing skills in a patient with frontotemporal dementia: A longitudinal case study International Symposium on Performance Science ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 The Author 2009, Published by the AEC All rights reserved Piano playing skills in a patient with frontotemporal dementia: A longitudinal

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Devenney E, Bartley L, Hoon C, et al. Progression in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia: a longitudinal study. JAMA Neurol. Published online October 26, 2015. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.2061.

More information

Dr Fiona Kumfor University of Sydney

Dr Fiona Kumfor University of Sydney Social Cognition in Dementia: Informing Diagnosis, Prognosis and Management Dr Fiona Kumfor University of Sydney CHANGES IN DEMENTIA Memory Social cognition Language ELEMENTS OF SOCIAL COGNITION Face processing

More information

doi: /brain/awu246 Brain 2014: 137; Degradation of emotion processing ability in corticobasal syndrome and Alzheimer s disease

doi: /brain/awu246 Brain 2014: 137; Degradation of emotion processing ability in corticobasal syndrome and Alzheimer s disease doi:10.1093/brain/awu246 Brain 2014: 137; 3061 3072 3061 BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY Degradation of emotion processing ability in corticobasal syndrome and Alzheimer s disease Fiona Kumfor, 1,2,3, * Laurie-Anne

More information

Published in: Neuropsychologia. DOI: /j.neuropsychologia Document Version Peer reviewed version

Published in: Neuropsychologia. DOI: /j.neuropsychologia Document Version Peer reviewed version Do I know you? Examining face and object memory in frontotemporal dementia Kumfor, F., Hutchings, R., Irish, M., Hodges, J. R., Rhodes, G., Palermo, R., & Piguet, O. (2015). Do I know you? Examining face

More information

Frontotemporal Dementia: Towards better diagnosis. Frontotemporal Dementia. John Hodges, NeuRA & University of New South Wales, Sydney.

Frontotemporal Dementia: Towards better diagnosis. Frontotemporal Dementia. John Hodges, NeuRA & University of New South Wales, Sydney. I.1 I.2 II.1 II.2 II.3 II.4 II.5 II.6 III.1 III.2 III.3 III.4 III.5 III.6 III.7 III.8 III.9 III.10 III.11 III.12 IV.1 IV.2 IV.3 IV.4 IV.5 Frontotemporal Dementia: Towards better diagnosis Frontotemporal

More information

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis (OA). All subjects provided informed consent to procedures

More information

Differential diagnosis of Frontotemporal Dementia FTLD using visual rating scales

Differential diagnosis of Frontotemporal Dementia FTLD using visual rating scales Differential diagnosis of Frontotemporal Dementia FTLD using visual rating scales Poster No.: C-0491 Congress: ECR 2016 Type: Scientific Exhibit Authors: S. Manouvelou 1, G. ANYFANTAKIS 2, V. Koutoulidis

More information

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion Neuron, Volume 70 Supplemental Information Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion Luke J. Chang, Alec Smith, Martin Dufwenberg, and Alan G. Sanfey Supplemental Information

More information

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis International Journal of Innovative Research in Computer Science & Technology (IJIRCST) ISSN: 2347-5552, Volume-2, Issue-6, November-2014 Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using

More information

Automated detection of abnormal changes in cortical thickness: A tool to help diagnosis in neocortical focal epilepsy

Automated detection of abnormal changes in cortical thickness: A tool to help diagnosis in neocortical focal epilepsy Automated detection of abnormal changes in cortical thickness: A tool to help diagnosis in neocortical focal epilepsy 1. Introduction Epilepsy is a common neurological disorder, which affects about 1 %

More information

Visual Rating Scale Reference Material. Lorna Harper Dementia Research Centre University College London

Visual Rating Scale Reference Material. Lorna Harper Dementia Research Centre University College London Visual Rating Scale Reference Material Lorna Harper Dementia Research Centre University College London Background The reference materials included in this document were compiled and used in relation to

More information

CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE

CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE 5.1 GENERAL BACKGROUND Neuropsychological assessment plays a crucial role in the assessment of cognitive decline in older age. In India, there

More information

Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018)

Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018) 1 / 22 Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018) Jérôme Dockès, ussel Poldrack, Demian Wassermann, Fabian Suchanek, Bertrand

More information

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni VISUAL AGNOSIA -damage to the extrastriate visual regions (occipital, parietal and temporal lobes) disrupts recognition of complex visual stimuli

More information

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm Author's response to reviews Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm Authors: Julia Miro (juliamirollado@gmail.com) Pablo Ripollès (pablo.ripolles.vidal@gmail.com)

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) 743 748 WCPCG 2014 Differences in Visuospatial Cognition Performance and Regional Brain Activation

More information

Investigating the impact of midlife obesity on Alzheimer s disease (AD) pathology in a mouse model of AD

Investigating the impact of midlife obesity on Alzheimer s disease (AD) pathology in a mouse model of AD Brain@McGill Prize for Neuroscience Undergraduate Research Colleen Rollins Supervisor: Dr. Mallar Chakravarty Revised: August 8, 2017 Investigating the impact of midlife obesity on Alzheimer s disease

More information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information Supplementary Information The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice Joseph W. Kable and Paul W. Glimcher a 10 0 b 10 0 10 1 10 1 Discount rate k 10 2 Discount rate k 10 2 10

More information

Quantitative Neuroimaging- Gray and white matter Alteration in Multiple Sclerosis. Lior Or-Bach Instructors: Prof. Anat Achiron Dr.

Quantitative Neuroimaging- Gray and white matter Alteration in Multiple Sclerosis. Lior Or-Bach Instructors: Prof. Anat Achiron Dr. Quantitative Neuroimaging- Gray and white matter Alteration in Multiple Sclerosis Lior Or-Bach Instructors: Prof. Anat Achiron Dr. Shmulik Miron INTRODUCTION Multiple Sclerosis general background Gray

More information

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism Justin H G Williams Morven McWhirr Gordon D Waiter Cambridge Sept 10 th 2010 Joint attention in autism Declarative and receptive aspects initiating

More information

How preserved is episodic memory in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia?

How preserved is episodic memory in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia? How preserved is episodic memory in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia? M. Hornberger, PhD O. Piguet, PhD A.J. Graham, PhD, MRCP P.J. Nestor, MD, FRACP J.R. Hodges, MD, FRCP Address correspondence

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Redlich R, Opel N, Grotegerd D, et al. Prediction of individual response to electroconvulsive therapy via machine learning on structural magnetic resonance imaging data. JAMA

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

Neurodegenerative diseases that degrade regions of the brain will eventually become

Neurodegenerative diseases that degrade regions of the brain will eventually become Maren Johnson CD 730 Research Paper What is the underlying neurological explanation for overeating in Frontotemporal Dementia? Does the change in overeating affect swallowing? Neurodegenerative diseases

More information

Neural Correlates of Episodic Memory in Behavioral Variant Frontotemporal Dementia

Neural Correlates of Episodic Memory in Behavioral Variant Frontotemporal Dementia Journal of Alzheimer s Disease 24 (2011) 261 268 DOI 10.3233/JAD-2010-101668 IOS Press 261 Neural Correlates of Episodic Memory in Behavioral Variant Frontotemporal Dementia Catherine Pennington a, John

More information

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease Supplemental informations Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease Stéphanie Lefebvre, PhD^1,2, Guillaume Baille, MD^4, Renaud Jardri MD, PhD 1,2 Lucie Plomhause, PhD

More information

FRONTOTEMPORAL DEGENERATION: OVERVIEW, TRENDS AND DEVELOPMENTS

FRONTOTEMPORAL DEGENERATION: OVERVIEW, TRENDS AND DEVELOPMENTS FRONTOTEMPORAL DEGENERATION: OVERVIEW, TRENDS AND DEVELOPMENTS Norman L. Foster, M.D. Director, Center for Alzheimer s Care, Imaging and Research Chief, Division of Cognitive Neurology, Department of Neurology

More information

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation

Cover Page. The handle  holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation Cover Page The handle http://hdl.handle.net/1887/32078 holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation Author: Pannekoek, Nienke Title: Using novel imaging approaches in affective disorders

More information

Confronting the Clinical Challenges of Frontotemporal Dementia

Confronting the Clinical Challenges of Frontotemporal Dementia Confronting the Clinical Challenges of Frontotemporal Dementia A look at FTD s symptoms, pathophysiology, subtypes, as well as the latest from imaging studies. By Zac Haughn, Senior Associate Editor Ask

More information

fmri and Voxel-based Morphometry in Detection of Early Stages of Alzheimer's Disease

fmri and Voxel-based Morphometry in Detection of Early Stages of Alzheimer's Disease fmri and Voxel-based Morphometry in Detection of Early Stages of Alzheimer's Disease Andrey V. Sokolov 1,3, Sergey V. Vorobyev 2, Aleksandr Yu. Efimtcev 1,3, Viacheslav S. Dekan 1,3, Gennadiy E. Trufanov

More information

Diffusion Tensor Imaging in Dementia. Howard Rosen UCSF Department of Neurology Memory and Aging Center

Diffusion Tensor Imaging in Dementia. Howard Rosen UCSF Department of Neurology Memory and Aging Center Diffusion Tensor Imaging in Dementia Howard Rosen UCSF Department of Neurology Memory and Aging Center www.memory.ucsf.edu Overview Examples of DTI findings in Alzheimer s disease And other dementias Explore

More information

MOTOR FUNCTION AND BEHAVIOUR ACROSS THE ALS-FTD SPECTRUM

MOTOR FUNCTION AND BEHAVIOUR ACROSS THE ALS-FTD SPECTRUM MOTOR FUNCTION AND BEHAVIOUR ACROSS THE ALS-FTD SPECTRUM Dinuksha De Silva, 1, 2 Sharpley Hsieh 4, Jashelle Caga, 4 Felicity V.C. Leslie, 1,2,3 Matthew C. Kiernan, 1, 4 John R. Hodges, 1, 2, 3 Eneida Mioshi,

More information

Role of TDP-43 in Non-Alzheimer s and Alzheimer s Neurodegenerative Diseases

Role of TDP-43 in Non-Alzheimer s and Alzheimer s Neurodegenerative Diseases Role of TDP-43 in Non-Alzheimer s and Alzheimer s Neurodegenerative Diseases Keith A. Josephs, MD, MST, MSc Professor of Neurology 13th Annual Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) Symposium: Alzheimer and Non-Alzheimer

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Table 1 Patient characteristics Preoperative. language testing

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Table 1 Patient characteristics Preoperative. language testing Categorical Speech Representation in the Human Superior Temporal Gyrus Edward F. Chang, Jochem W. Rieger, Keith D. Johnson, Mitchel S. Berger, Nicholas M. Barbaro, Robert T. Knight SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

More information

Discriminative Analysis for Image-Based Studies

Discriminative Analysis for Image-Based Studies Discriminative Analysis for Image-Based Studies Polina Golland 1, Bruce Fischl 2, Mona Spiridon 3, Nancy Kanwisher 3, Randy L. Buckner 4, Martha E. Shenton 5, Ron Kikinis 6, Anders Dale 2, and W. Eric

More information

Funding: NIDCF UL1 DE019583, NIA RL1 AG032119, NINDS RL1 NS062412, NIDA TL1 DA

Funding: NIDCF UL1 DE019583, NIA RL1 AG032119, NINDS RL1 NS062412, NIDA TL1 DA The Effect of Cognitive Functioning, Age, and Molecular Variables on Brain Structure Among Carriers of the Fragile X Premutation: Deformation Based Morphometry Study Naomi J. Goodrich-Hunsaker*, Ling M.

More information

Supplemental Information. Direct Electrical Stimulation in the Human Brain. Disrupts Melody Processing

Supplemental Information. Direct Electrical Stimulation in the Human Brain. Disrupts Melody Processing Current Biology, Volume 27 Supplemental Information Direct Electrical Stimulation in the Human Brain Disrupts Melody Processing Frank E. Garcea, Benjamin L. Chernoff, Bram Diamond, Wesley Lewis, Maxwell

More information

Brain & Language 127 (2013) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Brain & Language

Brain & Language 127 (2013) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Brain & Language Brain & Language 127 (2013) 121 126 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Brain & Language journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/b&l Patterns of longitudinal brain atrophy in the logopenic

More information

Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Tables and Supplementary References

Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Tables and Supplementary References Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Tables and Supplementary References Supplementary Information Supplementary Figure 1. The mean parameter

More information

Four Tissue Segmentation in ADNI II

Four Tissue Segmentation in ADNI II Four Tissue Segmentation in ADNI II Charles DeCarli, MD, Pauline Maillard, PhD, Evan Fletcher, PhD Department of Neurology and Center for Neuroscience, University of California at Davis Summary Table of

More information

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Brain electrical activity methods description and explanation of method state effects trait effects Positron emission tomography

More information

The behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia phenocopy syndrome is a distinct entity - evidence from a longitudinal study

The behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia phenocopy syndrome is a distinct entity - evidence from a longitudinal study Devenney et al. BMC Neurology (2018) 18:56 https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-018-1060-1 RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access The behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia phenocopy syndrome is a distinct entity

More information

Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications.

Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications. Dr. Peter J. Fiester November 14, 2012 Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications. Briefly discuss a few examples

More information

FRONTAL LOBE. Central Sulcus. Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Sulcus. Cingulate Sulcus. Lateral Sulcus

FRONTAL LOBE. Central Sulcus. Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Sulcus. Cingulate Sulcus. Lateral Sulcus FRONTAL LOBE Central Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Cingulate Lateral Lateral View Medial View Motor execution and higher cognitive functions (e.g., language production, impulse inhibition, reasoning

More information

Executive function in progressive and nonprogressive behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia

Executive function in progressive and nonprogressive behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia Executive function in progressive and nonprogressive behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia Michael Hornberger, PhD Olivier Piguet, PhD Christopher Kipps, MD, FRACP John R. Hodges, MD, FRCP Address

More information

Clinical phenotypes in autopsy-confirmed Pick disease

Clinical phenotypes in autopsy-confirmed Pick disease Clinical phenotypes in autopsy-confirmed Pick disease O. Piguet, PhD G.M. Halliday, PhD W.G.J. Reid, PhD B. Casey, PhD R. Carman, MPhil Y. Huang, PhD J.H. Xuereb, MD J.R. Hodges, FRCP J.J. Kril, PhD Address

More information

Title Progression in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia: a longitudinal study

Title Progression in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia: a longitudinal study 1 Title Progression in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia: a longitudinal study Running Title Progression in frontotemporal dementia Authors and Affiliations 1,2,3, E. Devenney, 1 L. Bartley,

More information

doi: /brain/aws024 Brain 2012: 135; Altered brain mechanisms of emotion processing in pre-manifest Huntington s disease

doi: /brain/aws024 Brain 2012: 135; Altered brain mechanisms of emotion processing in pre-manifest Huntington s disease doi:10.1093/brain/aws024 Brain 2012: 135; 1165 1179 1165 BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY Altered brain mechanisms of emotion processing in pre-manifest Huntington s disease Marianne J. U. Novak, 1 Jason D.

More information

Automated Whole Brain Segmentation Using FreeSurfer

Automated Whole Brain Segmentation Using FreeSurfer Automated Whole Brain Segmentation Using FreeSurfer https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/ FreeSurfer (FS) is a free software package developed at the Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging used for three

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. Authors Year Patients Male gender (%) Mean age (range) Adults/ Children

More information

Perspectives on Frontotemporal Dementia and Primary Progressive Aphasia

Perspectives on Frontotemporal Dementia and Primary Progressive Aphasia Perspectives on Frontotemporal Dementia and Primary Progressive Aphasia Bradley F. Boeve, M.D. Division of Behavioral Neurology Department of Neurology Mayo Clinic Rochester, Minnesota Alzheimer s Disease

More information

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia Supplemental Information Table of Contents 2 Behavioral Data 2 Table S1. Participant demographics

More information

MITELMAN, SHIHABUDDIN, BRICKMAN, ET AL. basic necessities of life, including food, clothing, and shelter. Compared to patients with good-outcome schiz

MITELMAN, SHIHABUDDIN, BRICKMAN, ET AL. basic necessities of life, including food, clothing, and shelter. Compared to patients with good-outcome schiz Article MRI Assessment of Gray and White Matter Distribution in Brodmann s Areas of the Cortex in Patients With Schizophrenia With Good and Poor Outcomes Serge A. Mitelman, M.D. Lina Shihabuddin, M.D.

More information

White matter hyperintensities correlate with neuropsychiatric manifestations of Alzheimer s disease and frontotemporal lobar degeneration

White matter hyperintensities correlate with neuropsychiatric manifestations of Alzheimer s disease and frontotemporal lobar degeneration White matter hyperintensities correlate with neuropsychiatric manifestations of Alzheimer s disease and frontotemporal lobar degeneration Annual Scientific Meeting Canadian Geriatric Society Philippe Desmarais,

More information

Discriminative Analysis for Image-Based Population Comparisons

Discriminative Analysis for Image-Based Population Comparisons Discriminative Analysis for Image-Based Population Comparisons Polina Golland 1,BruceFischl 2, Mona Spiridon 3, Nancy Kanwisher 3, Randy L. Buckner 4, Martha E. Shenton 5, Ron Kikinis 6, and W. Eric L.

More information

Online appendices are unedited and posted as supplied by the authors. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

Online appendices are unedited and posted as supplied by the authors. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Appendix 1 to Sehmbi M, Rowley CD, Minuzzi L, et al. Age-related deficits in intracortical myelination in young adults with bipolar SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Supplementary Methods Intracortical Myelin (ICM)

More information

Structural And Functional Integration: Why all imaging requires you to be a structural imager. David H. Salat

Structural And Functional Integration: Why all imaging requires you to be a structural imager. David H. Salat Structural And Functional Integration: Why all imaging requires you to be a structural imager David H. Salat salat@nmr.mgh.harvard.edu Salat:StructFunct:HST.583:2015 Structural Information is Critical

More information

I do not have any disclosures

I do not have any disclosures Alzheimer s Disease: Update on Research, Treatment & Care Clinicopathological Classifications of FTD and Related Disorders Keith A. Josephs, MST, MD, MS Associate Professor & Consultant of Neurology Mayo

More information

Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4

Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4 Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4 Table S1: Brain regions involved in the adapted classification learning task Brain Regions x y z Z Anterior Cingulate

More information

The Impact of Dementia Severity on Caregiver Burden in Frontotemporal Dementia and Alzheimer Disease

The Impact of Dementia Severity on Caregiver Burden in Frontotemporal Dementia and Alzheimer Disease ORIGINAL ARTICLE The Impact of Dementia Severity on Caregiver Burden in Frontotemporal Dementia and Alzheimer Disease Eneida Mioshi, PhD,*w David Foxe, BPsych,* Felicity Leslie, BPsych,* Sharon Savage,

More information

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group 1 Supplemental Data Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group Additional inclusion criteria for the major depressive disorder group were: age of onset of

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Van Laere K, Vanhee A, Verschueren J, et al. Value of 18 fluorodeoxyglucose positron-emission tomography in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. JAMA Neurol. Published online March

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/324/5927/646/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Self-Control in Decision-Making Involves Modulation of the vmpfc Valuation System Todd A. Hare,* Colin F. Camerer, Antonio

More information

Different regional patterns of cortical thinning in. Alzheimer s disease and frontotemporal dementia (FTD) are

Different regional patterns of cortical thinning in. Alzheimer s disease and frontotemporal dementia (FTD) are doi:10.1093/brain/awm016 Brain (2007), 130, 1159^1166 Different regional patterns of cortical thinning in Alzheimer s disease and frontotemporal dementia An-Tao Du, 1, * Norbert Schuff, 1,2 Joel H. Kramer,

More information

Supplementary Materials for

Supplementary Materials for Supplementary Materials for Folk Explanations of Behavior: A Specialized Use of a Domain-General Mechanism Robert P. Spunt & Ralph Adolphs California Institute of Technology Correspondence may be addressed

More information

Social Neuroscience Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:

Social Neuroscience Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: This article was downloaded by: [Olivier Piguet] On: 30 September 11, At: 15:49 Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Subjects The experiment included twelve subjects: ten sighted subjects and two blind. Five of the ten sighted subjects were expert users of a visual-to-auditory

More information

Screening for Cognitive Dysfunction in Corticobasal Syndrome: Utility of Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination

Screening for Cognitive Dysfunction in Corticobasal Syndrome: Utility of Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination Original Research Article DOI: 10.1159/000327169 Accepted: March 8, 2011 Published online: April 8, 2011 Screening for Cognitive Dysfunction in Corticobasal Syndrome: Utility of Addenbrooke s Cognitive

More information

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex PARIETAL LOBE The Parietal Lobes develop at about the age of 5 years. They function to give the individual perspective and to help them understand space, touch, and volume. The location of the parietal

More information

Differentiation of semantic dementia and Alzheimer s disease using the Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination (ACE)

Differentiation of semantic dementia and Alzheimer s disease using the Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination (ACE) INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF GERIATRIC PSYCHIATRY Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008; 23: 370 375. Published online 4 September 2007 in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com).1887 Differentiation of semantic

More information

Fibre orientation dispersion in the corpus callosum relates to interhemispheric functional connectivity

Fibre orientation dispersion in the corpus callosum relates to interhemispheric functional connectivity Fibre orientation dispersion in the corpus callosum relates to interhemispheric functional connectivity ISMRM 2017: http://submissions.mirasmart.com/ismrm2017/viewsubmissionpublic.aspx?sei=8t1bikppq Jeroen

More information

Brain tissue and white matter lesion volume analysis in diabetes mellitus type 2

Brain tissue and white matter lesion volume analysis in diabetes mellitus type 2 Brain tissue and white matter lesion volume analysis in diabetes mellitus type 2 C. Jongen J. van der Grond L.J. Kappelle G.J. Biessels M.A. Viergever J.P.W. Pluim On behalf of the Utrecht Diabetic Encephalopathy

More information

FTD basics! Etienne de Villers-Sidani, MD!

FTD basics! Etienne de Villers-Sidani, MD! FTD basics! Etienne de Villers-Sidani, MD! Frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD) comprises 3 clinical syndromes! Frontotemporal dementia (behavioral variant FTD)! Semantic dementia (temporal variant

More information

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation

Cover Page. The handle   holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation Cover Page The handle http://hdl.handle.net/1887/39720 holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation Author: Hafkemeijer, Anne Title: Brain networks in aging and dementia Issue Date: 2016-05-26

More information

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression Supplemental Information Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression Roselinde H. Kaiser, Ph.D., Susan Whitfield-Gabrieli, Ph.D., Daniel G. Dillon, Ph.D., Franziska Goer, B.S., Miranda

More information

Functional Elements and Networks in fmri

Functional Elements and Networks in fmri Functional Elements and Networks in fmri Jarkko Ylipaavalniemi 1, Eerika Savia 1,2, Ricardo Vigário 1 and Samuel Kaski 1,2 1- Helsinki University of Technology - Adaptive Informatics Research Centre 2-

More information

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Supplementary Methods Materials & Methods Subjects Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Dartmouth community. All subjects were native speakers of English,

More information

Henry Molaison. Biography. From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Henry Molaison. Biography. From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Henry Molaison From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Henry Gustav Molaison (February 26, 1926 December 2, 2008), known widely as H.M., was an American memory disorder patient who had a bilateral medial

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Schlaeger R, Papinutto N, Zhu AH, et al. Association between thoracic spinal cord gray matter atrophy and disability in multiple sclerosis. JAMA Neurol. Published online June

More information

Neural activity to positive expressions predicts daily experience of schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms in adults with high social anhedonia

Neural activity to positive expressions predicts daily experience of schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms in adults with high social anhedonia 1 Neural activity to positive expressions predicts daily experience of schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms in adults with high social anhedonia Christine I. Hooker, Taylor L. Benson, Anett Gyurak, Hong Yin,

More information

Supplementary Material. Functional connectivity in multiple cortical networks is associated with performance. across cognitive domains in older adults

Supplementary Material. Functional connectivity in multiple cortical networks is associated with performance. across cognitive domains in older adults Supplementary Material Functional connectivity in multiple cortical networks is associated with performance across cognitive domains in older adults Emily E. Shaw 1,2, Aaron P. Schultz 1,2,3, Reisa A.

More information

DEMENTIA 101: WHAT IS HAPPENING IN THE BRAIN? Philip L. Rambo, PhD

DEMENTIA 101: WHAT IS HAPPENING IN THE BRAIN? Philip L. Rambo, PhD DEMENTIA 101: WHAT IS HAPPENING IN THE BRAIN? Philip L. Rambo, PhD OBJECTIVES Terminology/Dementia Basics Most Common Types Defining features Neuro-anatomical/pathological underpinnings Neuro-cognitive

More information

doi: /brain/awr099 Brain 2011: 134; White matter damage in primary progressive aphasias: a diffusion tensor tractography study

doi: /brain/awr099 Brain 2011: 134; White matter damage in primary progressive aphasias: a diffusion tensor tractography study doi:10.1093/brain/awr099 Brain 2011: 134; 3011 3029 3011 BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY White matter damage in primary progressive aphasias: a diffusion tensor tractography study Sebastiano Galantucci, 1,2

More information

Behavioral Task Performance

Behavioral Task Performance Zacks 1 Supplementary content for: Functional Reorganization of Spatial Transformations After a Parietal Lesion Jeffrey M. Zacks, PhD *, Pascale Michelon, PhD *, Jean M. Vettel, BA *, and Jeffrey G. Ojemann,

More information

The specificity of visual recognition impairments following focal brain damage

The specificity of visual recognition impairments following focal brain damage University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Theses and Dissertations 2007 The specificity of visual recognition impairments following focal brain damage Jessica Lee Wisnowski University of Iowa Copyright 2007

More information

Word Length Processing via Region-to-Region Connectivity

Word Length Processing via Region-to-Region Connectivity Word Length Processing via Region-to-Region Connectivity Mariya Toneva Machine Learning Department, Neural Computation Carnegie Mellon University Data Analysis Project DAP committee: Tom Mitchell, Robert

More information

Geri R Hall, PhD, GCNS, FAAN Advanced Practice Nurse Emeritus Banner Alzheimer s Institute

Geri R Hall, PhD, GCNS, FAAN Advanced Practice Nurse Emeritus Banner Alzheimer s Institute Geri R Hall, PhD, GCNS, FAAN Advanced Practice Nurse Emeritus Banner Alzheimer s Institute Anosognosia Purpose: To introduce caregivers and health professionals to symptoms of lack of awareness and other

More information

Review of Longitudinal MRI Analysis for Brain Tumors. Elsa Angelini 17 Nov. 2006

Review of Longitudinal MRI Analysis for Brain Tumors. Elsa Angelini 17 Nov. 2006 Review of Longitudinal MRI Analysis for Brain Tumors Elsa Angelini 17 Nov. 2006 MRI Difference maps «Longitudinal study of brain morphometrics using quantitative MRI and difference analysis», Liu,Lemieux,

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Green SA, Hernandez L, Tottenham N, Krasileva K, Bookheimer SY, Dapretto M. The neurobiology of sensory overresponsivity in youth with autism spectrum disorders. Published

More information

Structural MRI in Frontotemporal Dementia: Comparisons between Hippocampal Volumetry, Tensor- Based Morphometry and Voxel-Based Morphometry

Structural MRI in Frontotemporal Dementia: Comparisons between Hippocampal Volumetry, Tensor- Based Morphometry and Voxel-Based Morphometry : Comparisons between Hippocampal Volumetry, Tensor- Based Morphometry and Voxel-Based Morphometry Miguel Ángel Muñoz-Ruiz 1,Päivi Hartikainen 1,2, Juha Koikkalainen 3, Robin Wolz 4, Valtteri Julkunen

More information

Exam 1 PSYC Fall 1998

Exam 1 PSYC Fall 1998 Exam 1 PSYC 2022 Fall 1998 (2 points) Briefly describe the difference between a dualistic and a materialistic explanation of brain-mind relationships. (1 point) True or False. George Berkely was a monist.

More information

Supplementary materials. Appendix A;

Supplementary materials. Appendix A; Supplementary materials Appendix A; To determine ADHD diagnoses, a combination of Conners' ADHD questionnaires and a semi-structured diagnostic interview was used(1-4). Each participant was assessed with

More information

Is DTI Increasing the Connectivity Between the Magnet Suite and the Clinic?

Is DTI Increasing the Connectivity Between the Magnet Suite and the Clinic? Current Literature In Clinical Science Is DTI Increasing the Connectivity Between the Magnet Suite and the Clinic? Spatial Patterns of Water Diffusion Along White Matter Tracts in Temporal Lobe Epilepsy.

More information

Copyright 2002 American Academy of Neurology. Volume 58(8) 23 April 2002 pp

Copyright 2002 American Academy of Neurology. Volume 58(8) 23 April 2002 pp Copyright 2002 American Academy of Neurology Volume 58(8) 23 April 2002 pp 1288-1290 Improved executive functioning following repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation [Brief Communications] Moser,

More information

doi: /brain/awp077 Brain 2009: 132;

doi: /brain/awp077 Brain 2009: 132; doi:10.1093/brain/awp077 Brain 2009: 132; 2566 2578 2566 BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY Combined magnetic resonance imaging and positron emission tomography brain imaging in behavioural variant frontotemporal

More information

Brain Imaging Applied to Memory & Learning

Brain Imaging Applied to Memory & Learning Brain Imaging Applied to Memory & Learning John Gabrieli Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences Institute for Medical Engineering & Sciences McGovern Institute for Brain Sciences MIT Levels of Analysis

More information

Localizing lesion locations to predict extent of aphasia recovery. Abstract

Localizing lesion locations to predict extent of aphasia recovery. Abstract Localizing lesion locations to predict extent of aphasia recovery Abstract Extensive research has related specific lesion locations to language impairment in aphasia. However, far less work has focused

More information

Reading Words and Non-Words: A Joint fmri and Eye-Tracking Study

Reading Words and Non-Words: A Joint fmri and Eye-Tracking Study Reading Words and Non-Words: A Joint fmri and Eye-Tracking Study A N N - M A R I E R A P H A I L S R E B C S, S K I D M O R E C O L L E G E D R. J O H N H E N D E R S O N U N I V E R S I T Y O F S O U

More information