Intransitivity on Paired-Comparisons Instruments:

Similar documents
What are Indexes and Scales

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison

equation involving two test variables.

3 CONCEPTUAL FOUNDATIONS OF STATISTICS

Standard Scores. Richard S. Balkin, Ph.D., LPC-S, NCC


Statistics is the science of collecting, organizing, presenting, analyzing, and interpreting data to assist in making effective decisions

Statistics is the science of collecting, organizing, presenting, analyzing, and interpreting data to assist in making effective decisions

Still important ideas

so that a respondent may choose one of the categories to express a judgment about some characteristic of an object or of human behavior.

Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 11 Online readings: Appendix D, E & F Plous Chapters 10, 11, 12 and 14

Analysis and Interpretation of Data Part 1

Development of the O*NET Paper-and-Pencil Work Importance Locator

Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 13 (emphasis on Chapter 12) Online readings: Appendix D, E & F

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Still important ideas

Adaptive Testing With the Multi-Unidimensional Pairwise Preference Model Stephen Stark University of South Florida

Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science

On the purpose of testing:

Examining the ability to detect change using the TRIM-Diabetes and TRIM-Diabetes Device measures

ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES ( ) PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology

The Use of Unidimensional Parameter Estimates of Multidimensional Items in Adaptive Testing

Louis Leon Thurstone in Monte Carlo: Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison

VIEW: An Assessment of Problem Solving Style

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo

MEASURING AFFECTIVE RESPONSES TO CONFECTIONARIES USING PAIRED COMPARISONS

Summary & Conclusion. Lecture 10 Survey Research & Design in Psychology James Neill, 2016 Creative Commons Attribution 4.0

Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 4 Online readings: Appendix D, E & F Online readings: Plous - Chapters 1, 5, 6, 13

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

An Empirical Study on Causal Relationships between Perceived Enjoyment and Perceived Ease of Use

SPSS Portfolio. Brittany Murray BUSA MWF 1:00pm-1:50pm

A Comparison of Three Measures of the Association Between a Feature and a Concept

Table of Contents. Plots. Essential Statistics for Nursing Research 1/12/2017

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review

Data and Statistics 101: Key Concepts in the Collection, Analysis, and Application of Child Welfare Data

Color Difference Equations and Their Assessment

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

Chapter 14: More Powerful Statistical Methods

The relationship between job satisfaction, perceived organizational support and organizational cynicism of ÖYP research assistants

11/24/2017. Do not imply a cause-and-effect relationship

Statistical analysis DIANA SAPLACAN 2017 * SLIDES ADAPTED BASED ON LECTURE NOTES BY ALMA LEORA CULEN

AP Psych - Stat 2 Name Period Date. MULTIPLE CHOICE. Choose the one alternative that best completes the statement or answers the question.

Business Statistics Probability

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES

STATISTICS AND RESEARCH DESIGN

THOMAS R. STEWAB'P University of Illinoh

CHAPTER 3 DATA ANALYSIS: DESCRIBING DATA

Effect of Sample Size on Correlation and Regression Coefficients

Empirical Knowledge: based on observations. Answer questions why, whom, how, and when.

International Core Journal of Engineering Vol.3 No ISSN:

A Cross-Cultural Study of Psychological Well-being Among British and Malaysian Fire Fighters

Quantitative Methods in Computing Education Research (A brief overview tips and techniques)

32.5. percent of U.S. manufacturers experiencing unfair currency manipulation in the trade practices of other countries.

Pushing the Right Buttons: Design Characteristics of Touch Screen Buttons

A COMPARISON BETWEEN MULTIVARIATE AND BIVARIATE ANALYSIS USED IN MARKETING RESEARCH

Ch. 11 Measurement. Paul I-Hai Lin, Professor A Core Course for M.S. Technology Purdue University Fort Wayne Campus

investigate. educate. inform.

LEDYARD R TUCKER AND CHARLES LEWIS

Best on the Left or on the Right in a Likert Scale

Survey research (Lecture 1) Summary & Conclusion. Lecture 10 Survey Research & Design in Psychology James Neill, 2015 Creative Commons Attribution 4.

Survey research (Lecture 1)

Guru Journal of Behavioral and Social Sciences

Quantitative Research. By Dr. Achmad Nizar Hidayanto Information Management Lab Faculty of Computer Science Universitas Indonesia

Chapter 1: Explaining Behavior

CHAPTER 4 THE QUESTIONNAIRE DESIGN /SOLUTION DESIGN. This chapter contains explanations that become a basic knowledge to create a good

SURVEY TOPIC INVOLVEMENT AND NONRESPONSE BIAS 1

Discrimination Weighting on a Multiple Choice Exam

9 research designs likely for PSYC 2100

Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1

Validity and Reliability. PDF Created with deskpdf PDF Writer - Trial ::

The degree to which a measure is free from error. (See page 65) Accuracy

Ch. 11 Measurement. Measurement

Sampling for Success. Dr. Jim Mirabella President, Mirabella Research Services, Inc. Professor of Research & Statistics

CHAPTER - III METHODOLOGY CONTENTS. 3.1 Introduction. 3.2 Attitude Measurement & its devices

Adjusting for mode of administration effect in surveys using mailed questionnaire and telephone interview data

Psychology Research Process

computation and interpretation of indices of reliability. In

Skew and Internal Consistency

Life Quality and Job Satisfaction: A Case Study on Job Satisfaction of Bike Participants in Chiayi County Area

AP Psych - Stat 1 Name Period Date. MULTIPLE CHOICE. Choose the one alternative that best completes the statement or answers the question.

RELIABILITY RANKING AND RATING SCALES OF MYER AND BRIGGS TYPE INDICATOR (MBTI) Farida Agus Setiawati

Introduction to the HBDI and the Whole Brain Model. Technical Overview & Validity Evidence

AGE IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF CLOSURE ABILITY IN CHILDREN

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 205 ( 2015 ) th World conference on Psychology Counseling and Guidance, May 2015

Statistics as a Tool. A set of tools for collecting, organizing, presenting and analyzing numerical facts or observations.

Comparison of the Null Distributions of

The Relationship of Competitiveness and Achievement Orientation to Participation in Sport and Nonsport Activities

Chapter 2 Norms and Basic Statistics for Testing MULTIPLE CHOICE

MTH 225: Introductory Statistics

CHAPTER-III METHODOLOGY

Issues That Should Not Be Overlooked in the Dominance Versus Ideal Point Controversy

Workplace Personality Inventory II

MBA 605 Business Analytics Don Conant, PhD. GETTING TO THE STANDARD NORMAL DISTRIBUTION

The Geography of Viral Hepatitis C in Texas,

The Lens Model and Linear Models of Judgment

Prepared by: Assoc. Prof. Dr Bahaman Abu Samah Department of Professional Development and Continuing Education Faculty of Educational Studies

HARRISON ASSESSMENTS DEBRIEF GUIDE 1. OVERVIEW OF HARRISON ASSESSMENT

Georgina Salas. Topics EDCI Intro to Research Dr. A.J. Herrera

Transcription:

Intransitivity on Paired-Comparisons Instruments: The Relationship of the Total Circular Triad Score to Stimulus Circular Triads Darwin D. Hendel Measurement Services Center, University of Minnesota Intransitivity associated with the method of paired comparisons for scaling stimulus objects has been hypothesized in previous research to relate to the psychological and/or physical distance between stimulus objects. The purpose of the present study was to determine whether paired-comparisons intransitivity is a function of intransitivity associated with specific stimulus objects rather than a function of the entire set of stimulus objects. Three 190- item, paired-comparisons instruments with diverse content (i.e., vocational needs, mate preferences, and food preferences) were designed to examine the relationship between Stimulus Circular Triads and the Total Circular Triad score and were administered to 276 high school and 358 college students. Results of univariate correlational analyses and multiple-regression techniques suggested that paired-comparisons intransitivity relates to individual differences variables associated with the respondent, although there were differences in the absolute level of intransitivity associated with each of the three sets of stimuli. The method of paired comparisons, an application of Thurstone s (1927) &dquo;law of paired comparisons,&dquo; has been used as a measurement technique in several areas of applied psychology. Paired-comparison methodology has been used to rate employee job performance (Lawshe, Kephart, & McCormick, 1949), to measure personality characteristics (Edwards, 1963), and to measure vocational needs (Gay, Weiss, Hendel, Dawis, & Lofquist, 1972). One feature of the method of paired comparisons is that it yields a measure of response intransitivity associated with the set of stimulus objects. Explanations of paired-comparisons intransitivity have tended to focus on characteristics of the stimulus objects in the paired-comparisons instrument rather than on individual differences variables associated with the respondent. For example, Mosteller (1951) illustrated the concept of unidimensionality of stimulus objects in his paired-comparison instrument of eight American League baseball teams. Coombs (1958) discussed intransitivity as a function of the psychological distance between stimulus objects and found circular-triad intransitivity to be greater for color preferences for sets of similar colors than for colors more widely separated. Coombs, Greenberg, and Zinnes (1961) made the distinction between unilateral intransitivity (both stimuli are on the same side of an individual s preference distribution) and bilateral intransitivity (stimuli are on opposite sides of an individual s preference distribution). Previous research (e.g., Ace & Dawis, 1972; Hendel, 1975) has also examined the relation- scores derived from ships among intransitivity different instruments. Results generally have indicated that the tendency to respond intransitively does generalize across different domains 403

404 of instrument content and across different groups of subjects. A related question concerns the extent to which intransitivity is consistent or stable over time. Although previous studies (e.g., Berdie, 1969) have examined stability of measures of response variability, little research has examined the long-range stability of intransitivity on paired-comparisons instruments. The majority of studies of paired-comparisons intransitivity have used the total circular triad (TCT) score as calculated by Kendall s (1955, p. 125) formula, where xi represents the number of times that the individual voted for stimulus i, to calculate intransitivity. Another index, the coefficient of consistence (Ferguson, 1966; Kendall, 1955; Moran, 1947), has been developed to reflect paired-comparisons intransitivity. Since this index is merely a linear transformation of the TCT score, it, too, reflects intransitivity associated with the set of stimulus objects. Another approach to measuring paired-comparisons intransitivity is to determine the intransitivity associated with subsets of the stimulus objects. Slater (1961 ) suggested that some types of intransitive triads would receive more weight than others. One such subdivision would be to separate the total circular triad score into stimulus circular triad (SCT) scores, one SCT score for each of the stimulus objects. SCT scores refer to the number of times each of the stimulus objects is involved in a circular triad. No single stimulus is considered intransitive in and of itself, since intransivity occurs in relationship to other stimuli. Because each circular triad must involve three stimulus objects (i.e., A > B, B > C. C > A), the number of SCTs across a set of n stimulus objects would be equal to three times the TCT score. Although no research data were found which investigated SCT scores in relationship to the TCT scores, SCT scores have been used in some applied contexts. DeWitt and Weiss (1969), for example, used the distribution of SCT scores across 20 stimulus objects as a method to differentiate between individuals who respond to a paired-comparison instrument in true random fashion from those who may have had difficulty discriminating among some of the stimuli. The primary purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between SCT and TCT scores to determine whether or not SCT scores, across a set of stimulus objects, are differentially related to the TCT scores. In contrast to previously noted theoretical discussions of intransitivity (e.g., Coombs, 1958), the present study describes the results of an empirical study of three sets of real data. Method Three paired-comparisons instruments were used to obtain the indices of response intransitivity. Each instrument contained 20 statements in a complete paired-comparisons format, which resulted in 190 paired-comparisons items (((n - 1))/2 items where n equals the number of statements). The first instrument was the Minnesota Importance Questionnaire (MIQ; Gay, et al., 1972) which contains statements of vocational needs (e.g., &dquo;i could do something that makes use of my abilities&dquo;). The second instrument, designed by the author, was the Mate Selection Questionnaire (M SQ), which contains qualities presumed to be used frequently in choosing a mate (e.g., &dquo;physical attractiveness&dquo;). The third instrument, also designed by the author, was the Food Preference Questionnaire (FPQ). which obtained preferences for main course meals (e.g., &dquo;hot beef sandwiches&dquo;). Data concerning the psychometric characteristics of the MSQ and the FPQ suggested that the three instruments were comparable in terms of the internal consistency reliabilities of the 20 scales (Hendel, 1972); median scale reliabilities ranged from.79 to.88. TCT scores werv calculated according to Equation 1 for calculating TCT scores from the &dquo;votes for&dquo; scores for the stimulus objects. The 20 scores for each of the three inventories re-

405 flected the number of times each of the stimulus items was preferred in the total set of 190 paired-comparisons items; scale scores could range from zero to 19. A computer program was written to obtain the 20 SCT scores for each of the paired-comparisons instruments by counting the number of times each of the stimulus items was involved in an intransitive set of preferences. The maximum number of circular triads on a instrument was 20-item, paired-comparisons 330, as calculated by Equation 2. where A maximum of 990 SCTs (330 x 3) was possible for each of the three paired-comparisons instruments. The relationship between SCT scores and the TCT score was examined in each of two groups of subjects. The first group consisted of 276 students from Cooper Senior High School in the Robbinsdale (Minnesota) school district; the students participated in the questionnaire session as part of their class work in vocational education. The mean age was 17.1 years (SD.67); = 96 (35%) of the students were males and 180 (65%) were females. The second group consisted of 358 students enrolled in an introductory psychology class at the University of Minnesota; students received experimental points for participating in the questionnaire session. The mean age was 19.9 years (SD 2.48); 168 (47%) of the = students were males and 190 (53%) were females. Pearson product-moment correlations were calculated between each of the 20 SCT scores and the TCT score for each of the three instruments for each of the two groups of subjects. SCT means and standard deviations were calculated for each of the 20 stimuli on each paired-comparisons instrument for each of the two groups of subjects. Multiple regression analysis was used to obtain the beta weights for each of the stimuli on each instrument; the dependent variable was the TCT score for the instrument. Because the multiple correlation between the 20 SCT scores and the TCT score would be 1.00, it was not possible to obtain beta weights for all of the 20 stimuli; beta weights were obtained for 19 of the 20 stimuli on each of the paired-comparisons instruments. Results Table 1 contains the correlations, means and standard deviations, and beta weights for the 20 stimuli on the MIQ. As the table suggests, intransitivity for the high school group was much more prevalent, as indicated by higher means for each of the 20 stimuli and for the TCT score, compared with the = college student group (t 8.40, p <.001 for the TCT score). The standard deviations for all stimuli were relatively large and, in some cases, greater than the mean &dquo;votes for&dquo; the stimulus. Examination of the distribution of SCTs acorss individuals in each group indicated a positively skewed distribution with the mode at or near zero. The correlations between SCT scores and the TCT score on the MIQ ranged from a low of.81 for Moral Values to a high of.91 for Supervision-Technical and Variety for the high school group and from a low of.68 for Social Status to a high of.82 for Responsibility and Working Conditions for the college student group. Beta weights ranged from a low of.0549 for Abilitv Utilization to a high of.0691 for Authoritv for the high school group and from a low of.0569 for Activity to a high of.0849 for Moral Values for the college group. Because of the part-whole nature of the SCT- TCT correlations, tests of the significance of the correlations were not possible. The ranges of the correlations were quite small (.10 for the high school group and.14 for the college student group) and suggest little differentiation among the correlations. The beta weights for the two groups of subjects were also generally of the same magnitude. Examination of the SCT means for the two groups indicated some similarity for the stimuli with the highest and lowest means. The three highest SCT means for the high school group were for Companv Policies and Practices, Coworkers, and Working Conditions: the order of

406 Table 1 Correlations Between Stimulus Circular Triad Scores and the Total Circular Triad Score: Means, Standard Deviations, and Beta Weights for the Minnesota Importance Questionnaire

,, 407 the second and third highest means were reversed for the college group. The three stimuli with the lowest SCT means varied more between the two groups. The three lowest stimuli were Independence, Authority, and Social Status for the high school group and Abilitv Utilization. Achievement, and Social Status for the college group., Table 2 contains similar data for the 20 stimuli on the MSQ. Although the high school group consistently had higher mean SCT scores and TCT scores than the college student group (t = 8.15, p <.001 for TCT), the means were considerably lower than for the parallel stimuli on the MIQ. These results suggest that both groups of subjects were more transitive in their mate preference than they were for vocational need preferences. Correlations ranged from a low of.75 for Nationality to a high of.87 for Tvpes of Friends for the high school group and from a low of.55 for Nationalitv to a high of.80 for Alertness for the college student group. Beta weights ranged from.0529 for Personalitv to.0769 for Interests for the high school group and from a low of.061 for Personal Values to a high of.0903 for Familv Background for the college student group. Although the range in correlations was relatively small (.11) for the high school group, the correlations were more varied for the college student group (.25). The three highest and three lowest SCT stimuli were identical for the two groups of subjects, although there were slight changes in the rank orders of the three highest and three lowest stimuli. This finding was generally similar to the results obtained concerning vocational need preferences. Table 3 contains the correlations, means, standard deviations, and beta weights for the 20 stimuli on the FPQ. Both groups were less intransitive about their food preferences than they were about either their vocational needs or their mate preferences. Comparisons between the two on groups the TCT score indicated, however, that the high school group was more intransitive than the college student group (t 4.29, p < =.001). Correlations ranged from a low of.72 for Tacos to a high of.87 for Meat Loaf for the high school group and from a low of.60 for Tacos to a high of.80 for Hamburger and Hot Beef sandwiches for the college student group. Beta weights for the high school group ranged from a low of.0529 for Beef Stew to a high of.0839 for Macarofii and Cheese. Beta weights for the college student group ranged from a low of.0629 for Cheeseburgers to a high of.0822 for French Fried Shrimp. As for the other two sets of stimulus objects, the stimulus objects with the three highest SCT means (i.e., Hot Bc~cet~Sandwiches, Hamburger, and Barbecued Beef) were identical for the two groups, and the three lowest were highly similar for the two groups. Discussion and Conclusions The present study investigated the relationship between SCT scores and the TCT score on paired-comparisons instruments, in order to determine whether intransitivity is a function of the entire instrument rather than a function of intransitivity associated with specific stimulus objects. Results of bivariate correlational analyses and multiple regression analyses indicated similar correlations and beta weights for each of 20 stimuli on three paired-comparisons instruments which varied widely in content-vocational needs, mate preferences, and food preferences. Results also indicated large differences in the absolute level of intransitivity associated with each of the three sets of stimuli and large differences between intransitivity of the high school and college students. Intransitivity was greatest for both groups of students for vocational need preferences, followed by mate preferences and food preferences, respectively. Results also indicated, however, that the stimuli with the highest and lowest SCT means tended to be virtually identical. Within each set of stimuli, there was a considerable range in SCT means which suggests that some of the stimuli contributed more than others to the overall intransitivity associated with the set of stimuli. If the stimuli within each instrument had been approximately equally spaced along a stimulus similarity con-

408 Table 2 Correlations Between Stimulus Circular Triad Scores and the Total Circular Triad Score: Means, Standard Deviations, and Beta Weights for the Mate Satisfaction Questionnaire

409 Table 3 Correlations Between Stimulus Circular Triad and the Total Circular Triad Score: Means, Standard Deviations, and Beta Weights for the Food Preference Questionnaire

410 tinuum, one could hypothesize that the SCTs would be almost equal for each of the stimuli. Results indicated large variability among the SCTs for each of the three sets of stimuli for both groups of subjects. Future research might investigate the relationship between SCT scores and subjects preference (i.e., the &dquo;votes for&dquo; the stimulus). Previously reported results (Hendel, 1975) have indicated highly significant intercorrelations among the three TCT scores for both groups of subjects, which suggests consistency across the three domains of content with respect to the TCT measure of transitivity. Results of the present study suggest that intransitivity may be associated with the complexity of the stimuli being rated and, presumably, a subject s familiarity and experience with the stimulus objects. Additionally, intransitivity is consistently observed across sets of stimuli. High school and college students were more similar in intransitivity associated with food preferences (a dimension with which both groups presumably had considerable experience)---than for either the mate or vocational need preferences (dimensions which one could hypothesize that high school students had considerably less experience with than college students). With young and naive subjects, intransitivity may be viewed as the result of confusion among the subsets of stimuli. being rated. The use of SCT scores does not separate the stimulus confusion into independent segments since, by definition, each circularity of preference involves three different stimuli. This circularity is reflected in higher SCT scores among all of the confused stimuli rather than in specific stimuli primarily responsible for the circularity. With older and more mature subjects such as the college students, circularity may occur because subjects have changed their frame of reference (i.e., focused on a different dimension of the multi-dimensional stimulus object) during the process of completing the paired-comparisons instrument. Examining the relationship between SCT scores and the TCT score poses other difficulties in the interpretation of the relationship between the two types of scores. For example, the absence of &dquo;lower limits&dquo; on the correlations between SCT scores and the TCT score and the part-whole nature of those correlations precluded making conclusions about the level of the correlations, the statistical significance of the correlations, and differences among the correlations associated with the 20 stimulus objects. The use of contrived data obtained by using monte carlo techniques could alleviate this problem to an extent, but careful experimental examination of the genesis of circularity of preference seems to be necessary to determine the relationship between intransitivity associated with specific stimuli and intransitivity associated with the total set of stimulus objects. As previously noted, since each circular triad involves three stimuli, it is extremely difficult to interpret intransitivity in this context. Other topics for research might further illuminate the meaning of intransitivity. if examined within the specific content in which it occurs, e.g., determining which specific triads are intransitive, examining which triads are characteristic of some persons and not others, or analyzing whether some triads are intransitive and others are not. References Ace, M. E., & Dawis, R. V. The contributions of questionnaire length, format, and type of score to response inconsistency. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 1972, 32, 1003-1011. Berdie, R. F. The consistency and generalizability of intra-individual variability. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1969, 53, 35-41. Coombs, C. H. On the use of inconsistency of preference in psychological measurement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1958, 55 1-7. Coombs, C. H., Greenberg, M., & Zinnes, J. A double law of comparative judgment for the analysis of preferential choice and similarities data. Psychometrika, 1961, 25, 165-172. DeWitt, L. J., & Weiss, D. J. Applications of circular triad data in individual measurement. Proceedings of the 77th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, 1969, 147-148. Edwards, A. L. Edwards Personal Preference Schedule. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1963.

411 Ferguson, G. A. Statistical analysis in psychological education. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1966. Gay, E. G., Weiss, D. J., Hendel, D. D., Dawis, R. V., & Lofquist, L. H. Manual for the Minnesota Importance Questionnaire. Minnesota Studies in Vocational Rehabilitation, 1972, 28. Hendel, D. D. Paired comparisons: Intransitivity, inconsistency, and compulsivity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, 1972. Hendel, D. D. Paired comparisons intransitivity: Trends across domains of content and across groups of subjects. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 1975, 35, 865-868. Kendall, M. G. Rank correlation methods. New York: Hafner, 1955. Lawshe, C. H., Kephart, H. C., & McCormick, E. J. The paired comparison technique for rating the performance of industrial employees. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1949, 33, 69-77. Moran, P. A. P. On the method of paired comparisons. Biometrika, 1947, 34, 363-365. Mosteller, F. Remarks on the method of paired comparisons : III. A test of significance for paired comparisons when equal standard deviations and equal correlations are assumed. Psychometrika, 1951, 16, 207-218. Slater, P. Inconsistencies in a schedule of paired comparisons, Biometrika, 1961, 48, 303-312. Thurstone, L. L. A law of comparative judgment. Psychological Review, 1927, 34, 273-286. Author s Address Darwin D. Hendel, Measurement Services Center, University of Minnesota, 9 Clarence Avenue SE, Minneapolis, MN 55414