Online Supplemental Material. paying appropriate attention (Johnson, 2005), we established a minimum completion time

Similar documents
Statistical inference provides methods for drawing conclusions about a population from sample data.

Chapter 23. Inference About Means. Copyright 2010 Pearson Education, Inc.

Checking the counterarguments confirms that publication bias contaminated studies relating social class and unethical behavior

BAYESIAN HYPOTHESIS TESTING WITH SPSS AMOS

Student Performance Q&A:

appstats26.notebook April 17, 2015

Type I Error Of Four Pairwise Mean Comparison Procedures Conducted As Protected And Unprotected Tests

Promoting Academic Integrity

Profile Analysis. Intro and Assumptions Psy 524 Andrew Ainsworth

AP STATISTICS 2008 SCORING GUIDELINES (Form B)

BATMAN/MR. FREEZE SUBZERO (GOLDEN LOOK-LOOK BOOK) BY SHELAGH CANNING

Guidelines for reviewers

The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose. Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX

11B Crazy Traits. What role does chance play in an organism s heredity? 1. Determining the genotype. 2. Stop and Think. Investigation 11B.

Elementary statistics. Michael Ernst CSE 140 University of Washington

Previously, when making inferences about the population mean,, we were assuming the following simple conditions:

ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE (ANOVA): TESTING DIFFERENCES INVOLVING THREE OR MORE MEANS

Before we get started:

EXERCISE: HOW TO DO POWER CALCULATIONS IN OPTIMAL DESIGN SOFTWARE

Sheila Barron Statistics Outreach Center 2/8/2011

PROBABILITY Page 1 of So far we have been concerned about describing characteristics of a distribution.

Summary Report: The Effectiveness of Online Ads: A Field Experiment

INFORMATION SYSTEMS: A MANAGER'S GUIDE TO HARNESSING TECHNOLOGY, V. 4.0 BY JOHN GALLAUGHER

CHAPTER THIRTEEN. Data Analysis and Interpretation: Part II.Tests of Statistical Significance and the Analysis Story CHAPTER OUTLINE

Margin of Error = Confidence interval:

Comparing Direct and Indirect Measures of Just Rewards: What Have We Learned?

Section 1: Goals and Attitudes

Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) Program Transcript

Psychological. Influences on Personal Probability. Chapter 17. Copyright 2005 Brooks/Cole, a division of Thomson Learning, Inc.

THE REFERENTIAL NATURE OF RULES AND INSTRUCTIONS: A RESPONSE TO INSTRUCTIONS, RULES, AND ABSTRACTION: A MISCONSTRUED RELATION BY EMILIO RIBES-IÑESTA

Probability and Inheritance PSI Biology

bivariate analysis: The statistical analysis of the relationship between two variables.

Chi Square Goodness of Fit

Registered Reports - Guidelines for reviewers

Acknowledge the depth of the pain that your affair brought to your marriage

PERHAPS LOVE FROM HAL LEONARD - SHEET MUSIC - CHORAL SATB - 8 PAGES - - CHORAL SATB CHERRY LANE MUSIC CL1030 PERHAPS LOVE JOHN DENVER AUD

2. Collecting agromyticin hormonal as a measure of aggression may not be a measure of aggression: (1 point)

Registered Reports guidelines for reviewers and authors. Guidelines for reviewers

ANOVA in SPSS (Practical)

Chapter 12: Introduction to Analysis of Variance

15.301/310, Managerial Psychology Prof. Dan Ariely Recitation 8: T test and ANOVA

Quantifying the Association Between Discrete Event Time Series

Lesson Overview 11.2 Applying Mendel s Principles

Chapter 20 Confidence Intervals with proportions!

Does chewing gum have an impact on student performance? : An analysis of quiz grades

When Intuition. Differs from Relative Frequency. Chapter 18. Copyright 2005 Brooks/Cole, a division of Thomson Learning, Inc.

Probabilities and Research. Statistics

SEED HAEMATOLOGY. Medical statistics your support when interpreting results SYSMEX EDUCATIONAL ENHANCEMENT AND DEVELOPMENT APRIL 2015

VIEW: An Assessment of Problem Solving Style

Independent Variables Variables (factors) that are manipulated to measure their effect Typically select specific levels of each variable to test

5.2 ESTIMATING PROBABILITIES

Experimental evidence of massive-scale emotional contagion through social networks

Missy Wittenzellner Big Brother Big Sister Project

LAB ASSIGNMENT 4 INFERENCES FOR NUMERICAL DATA. Comparison of Cancer Survival*

Title: A robustness study of parametric and non-parametric tests in Model-Based Multifactor Dimensionality Reduction for epistasis detection

04/12/2014. Research Methods in Psychology. Chapter 6: Independent Groups Designs. What is your ideas? Testing

What is probability. A way of quantifying uncertainty. Mathematical theory originally developed to model outcomes in games of chance.

Probability and Sample space

Chapter 9. Producing Data: Experiments. BPS - 5th Ed. Chapter 9 1

Optimizing Communication of Emergency Response Adaptive Randomization Clinical Trials to Potential Participants

Applied Statistical Analysis EDUC 6050 Week 4

Analysis of Variance (ANOVA)

Lessons in biostatistics

Chapter 5: Producing Data

SAMPLE SIZE AND POWER

Stat 111 Final. 1. What are the null and alternative hypotheses for Alex and Sarah having the same mean lap time?

To open a CMA file > Download and Save file Start CMA Open file from within CMA

Supporting Information

NO LIFE OF MY OWN: AN AUTOBIOGRAPHY BY FRANK CHIKANE DOWNLOAD EBOOK : NO LIFE OF MY OWN: AN AUTOBIOGRAPHY BY FRANK CHIKANE PDF

Chapter 1: Alternative Forced Choice Methods

Quantitative Methods in Computing Education Research (A brief overview tips and techniques)

Q1 How would you rate the quality of Hollis Adams Water Aerobics?

PROBABILITY and MENDELIAN GENETICS

Post Hoc Analysis Decisions Drive the Reported Reading Time Effects in Hackl, Koster-Hale & Varvoutis (2012)

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Instructor Guide to EHR Go

Chapter 10. Experimental Design: Statistical Analysis of Data

A closer look at the Semantic Web journal s review process

Lab 5: Testing Hypotheses about Patterns of Inheritance

Teaching Statistics with Coins and Playing Cards Going Beyond Probabilities

Motor Programs Lab. 1. Record your reaction and movement time in ms for each trial on the individual data Table 1 below. Table I: Individual Data RT

PRINTABLE VERSION. Quiz 10

Abdul Latif Jameel Poverty Action Lab Executive Training: Evaluating Social Programs Spring 2009

Improving Informed Consent to Clinical Research. Charles W. Lidz Ph.D.

Bayesian and Classical Hypothesis Testing: Practical Differences for a Controversial Area of Research

On the Performance of Maximum Likelihood Versus Means and Variance Adjusted Weighted Least Squares Estimation in CFA

Psychology 2019 v1.3. IA2 high-level annotated sample response. Student experiment (20%) August Assessment objectives

Please copy into your agenda:

KENNY ROGERS..."LADY"...SHEET MUSIC. BY LIONEL RICHIE DOWNLOAD EBOOK : KENNY ROGERS..."LADY"...SHEET MUSIC. BY LIONEL RICHIE PDF

Merck Index 13th Edition Guiglakatsarava

Investigating the robustness of the nonparametric Levene test with more than two groups

Research paper. One-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) Research paper. SPSS output. Learning objectives. Alcohol and driving ability

Manifestation Of Differences In Item-Level Characteristics In Scale-Level Measurement Invariance Tests Of Multi-Group Confirmatory Factor Analyses

A Brief (very brief) Overview of Biostatistics. Jody Kreiman, PhD Bureau of Glottal Affairs

Demonstrating Client Improvement to Yourself and Others

WDHS Curriculum Map Probability and Statistics. What is Statistics and how does it relate to you?

Chapter 5 & 6 Review. Producing Data Probability & Simulation

About Reading Scientific Studies

Monday, February 8. Please copy into your agenda:

Health Insight: A Consumer s Guide to Taking Charge of Health Information

Transcription:

Bryan, Adams & Monin; Online Supplemental Material 1 Online Supplemental Material Pilot time trial Because online samples often include participants who rush through studies without paying appropriate attention (Johnson, 2005), we established a minimum completion time (MCT) criterion for inclusion in the final sample for our two online experiments (2 and 3). One conventional procedure for eliminating outliers uses distributional criteria (e.g., 2 SDs above or below the mean). This method is not suitable here, however, because completion times are limited on the low end but not on the high end there is a minimum time in which it is possible to complete an experiment in good faith but there is no practical maximum. Instead, we conducted a pilot test to determine the shortest time in which one could reasonably participate in good faith. We asked 5 colleagues who were unfamiliar with the experiment to complete the online experiment as quickly as possible while still reading all essential instructions and questions but skipping the consent form, having a coin immediately accessible for flipping, and ignoring two open-ended questions at the end of the study to ensure they completed the study as quickly as possible. The mean completion time by our rushed testers was 3.09 minutes (SD = 0.55); this was set as our MCT. We emphasize that this is not the mean time in which a good faith participant would be expected to complete the experiment. Rather, it is the mean time in which a good faith participant could possibly be expected to complete the experiment. An even more conservative criterion (but, we believe, an unrealistic one given the already conservative instructions pilot participants were given) is the time in which our fastest rushed tester completed the experiment: 2.38 minutes. We report results of analyses using the 3.09-minute MCT in the main text. Analyses using the highly conservative 2.38-

Bryan, Adams & Monin; Online Supplemental Material 2 minute criterion yield similar results and are reported in the Additional Analyses section below, as are results with no time-based exclusions. In Experiment 2, the 3.09-minute criterion excludes 5 people from the sample and the 2.38-minute criterion excludes 3. In Experiment 3, those two criteria exclude 32 and 19 people, respectively. The higher rate of time-based exclusions in this study was likely due to the ad hoc nature of the sample: Whereas participants in Experiment 2 had registered to take part in surveys regularly and had presumably set time aside for the study, the Experiment 3 sample consists of casual internet browsers who landed on the survey somewhat unexpectedly, clicking an ad they discovered while browsing Facebook. Therefore, the sample likely included a contingent of people who just clicked through quickly to see what the study was about. Analyses Using Different MCT Criteria Analyses using the 2.38-minute completion-time criterion In Experiment 2, using the highly conservative 2.38 minute time criterion, participants in the cheating condition claimed, on average, to have obtained 5.48 heads (SD=1.24) while those in the cheater condition claimed to have obtained 4.90 heads (SD=1.37), t(79)=1.97, p=0.053, d=0.44. The number of heads reported in the cheating condition was still significantly higher than the 5.00 expected by chance, t(39)=2.42, p=0.020, d=0.39, and the number of heads reported in the cheater condition was still not different from chance, t(40)=0.45, p>0.6. In Experiment 3, using the highly conservative 2.38-minute criterion, the omnibus effect of condition remained significant, F(2, 109)=4.62, p=0.012, as did the differences between the cheating and cheater conditions, t(109)=2.56, p=0.012, d=0.67, and between the baseline and cheater conditions, t(109)=2.81, p=0.006, d=0.71. The

Bryan, Adams & Monin; Online Supplemental Material 3 difference between the baseline and cheating conditions remained non-significant, t(109)=0.25, p>0.80. Further, the number of heads claimed in both the cheating and baseline conditions remained significantly higher than chance, t(39)=4.81, p<0.0005, d=0.76 and t(41)=5.15, p<0.0005, d=0.79, respectively. The number of claimed heads in the cheater condition was still not significantly different from chance, t(29)=1.47, p>0.15. Analyses with no time-based exclusions In Experiment 2, including in the sample the 3 people who completed the experiment more quickly even than our highly conservative 2.38-minute criterion also did not change the results meaningfully. Participants in the cheating condition claimed, on average, to have obtained 5.59 heads (SD=1.41) while those in the cheater condition claimed to have obtained 4.86 head (SD=1.36), t(82)=2.40, p=0.019. The number of heads reported in the cheating condition was still significantly higher than the 5.00 expected by chance, t(40)=2.65, p=0.011, and the number of heads reported in the cheater condition was still not different from chance, t(42)=0.68, p>0.5. In Experiment 3, including in the sample the 19 people who completed the experiment more quickly even than our highly conservative 2.38-minute criterion did mask our effect, however. Including them, the omnibus effect of condition is no longer significant, F(2, 128)=0.94, p=0.394, nor are the differences between the cheating and cheater conditions, t(128)=1.25, p=0.215, or between the baseline and cheater conditions, t(128)=1.16, p=0.248. The difference between the baseline and cheating conditions remained non-significant, t(128)=0.11, p>0.91. Finally, the number of heads claimed in the cheater (M=5.82), cheating (M=6.27) and baseline (M=6.23)

Bryan, Adams & Monin; Online Supplemental Material 4 conditions were all significantly higher than chance, t(38)=5.82, p<0.005, t(45)=6.27, p<0.0005, and t(48)=6.23, p<0.0005, respectively, suggesting that the 19 people who completed the experiment so quickly cheated at a much higher rate than our good-faith participants. Tests of Distribution in Studies 2 and 3 Please refer to Figures S1 and S2 for the full distribution of responses in Experiments 2 and 3. Below we present statistical analyses of these distributions beyond the mean differences reported in the main text. Test of Standard Deviations It might appear, on first glance, that the standard deviation in the cheater condition in Experiment 3 (SD=1.18) is low compared to the expected value if participants were indeed reporting their coin tosses honestly (E(s)=sqrt[Np(1- p)]=sqrt(10*.5*.5)=1.58), raising the possibility that participants in that condition opted to report the most honest-sounding number of heads (i.e., 5) without even tossing a coin. However, a computer simulation with 5,000 samples of 27 participants, each flipping a fair coin 10 times yields a 95% confidence interval for the SD of {1.16; 2.00}, which includes the observed SD. Thus the observed variability is within the range of what can be reasonably expected, and we cannot reject the null to assume a restriction of the range. Participants seem indeed to be reporting their tosses honestly in the cheater condition. Moreover, similar simulations show that the other two SDs in this study (as well as all SDs observed in Experiment 2) fall well within the expected 95% confidence intervals corresponding to those cell sizes.

Bryan, Adams & Monin; Online Supplemental Material 5 Skewness and Kurtosis To further probe this question, we tested the distributions of numbers of claimed heads in the cheater conditions of both Experiments 2 and 3 (see supplementary figures S1 and S2). There is no significant skew or kurtosis in the cheater condition distributions in either experiment, z skew =0.47, p=0.64, z kurtosis =-0.47, p=0.64 (Experiment 2) and z skew =1.57, p=0.12, z kurtosis =1.59, p=0.11 (Experiment 3). Likelihood of Reporting Exactly 5 Heads Finally, we examined directly whether there was a difference between conditions, in either Experiment 2 or 3, in the likelihood of reporting exactly 5 heads. In Experiment 2, 35.0% of cheater condition participants reported exactly 5 and 35.9% of cheating condition participants did, χ 2 (1)=0.007, p=0.93. In Experiment 3, the difference was somewhat larger but still did not approach significance: 32.4% of cheater condition participants reported exactly 5 heads, 23.3% of cheating condition participants did, and 17.0% of baseline condition participants did, χ 2 (2)=2.73, p=0.26.

Bryan, Adams & Monin; Online Supplemental Material 6 Reference Johnson, J. A. (2005). Ascertaining the validity of individual protocols from Web-based personality inventories. Journal of Research in Personality, 39, 103-129. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2004.09.009

Bryan, Adams & Monin; Online Supplemental Material 7 Figure S1. Distributions of the numbers of heads participants claimed to have obtained in the cheating and cheater conditions in Experiment 2.

Bryan, Adams & Monin; Online Supplemental Material 8 Figure S2. Distributions of the numbers of heads participants claimed to have obtained in the cheater, cheating, and baseline conditions in Experiment 3.