Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Similar documents
Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Visual Perception. Agenda. Visual perception. CS Information Visualization January 20, 2011 John Stasko. Pre-attentive processing Color Etc.

IAT 355 Perception 1. Or What You See is Maybe Not What You Were Supposed to Get

Human Perception. Topic Objectives. CS 725/825 Information Visualization Fall Dr. Michele C. Weigle.

A Comparison of Three Measures of the Association Between a Feature and a Concept

Comment on W.S. Cleveland, A Model for Studying Display Methods of Statistical Graphics. Leland Wilkinson SYSTAT, Inc. and Northwestern University

Lecture 12: Psychophysics and User Studies

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

IAT 355 Visual Analytics. Encoding Information: Design. Lyn Bartram

VISUAL PERCEPTION & COGNITIVE PROCESSES

Visual Perception. Agenda. Visual perception. CS Information Visualization August 26, 2013 John Stasko. Pre-attentive processing Color Etc.

Presence and Perception: theoretical links & empirical evidence. Edwin Blake

CHAPTER 4 THE QUESTIONNAIRE DESIGN /SOLUTION DESIGN. This chapter contains explanations that become a basic knowledge to create a good

Using the ISO characterized reference print conditions to test an hypothesis about common color appearance

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Six Sigma Glossary Lean 6 Society

Modeling Human Perception

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Are there Hemispheric Differences in Visual Processes that Utilize Gestalt Principles?

APPLICATION OF FUZZY SIGNAL DETECTION THEORY TO VIGILANCE: THE EFFECT OF CRITERION SHIFTS

Supporting Information

Color Repeatability of Spot Color Printing

MEASURING AFFECTIVE RESPONSES TO CONFECTIONARIES USING PAIRED COMPARISONS

Alignment and Category Learning

Dynamics of Color Category Formation and Boundaries

Perception and Cognition

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

VISUAL PERCEPTION OF STRUCTURED SYMBOLS

CHANGES IN VISUAL SPATIAL ORGANIZATION: RESPONSE FREQUENCY EQUALIZATION VERSUS ADAPTATION LEVEL

Analysis of the Role of Associative Inhibition in Perceptual Learning by Means of the Same Different Task

Main Study: Summer Methods. Design

JUDGMENTAL MODEL OF THE EBBINGHAUS ILLUSION NORMAN H. ANDERSON

Chapter 1: Explaining Behavior

CS Information Visualization September 7, 2016 John Stasko. Identify visual features that are and are not pre-attentive

Online publication date: 08 June 2010

CHAPTER 2. MEASURING AND DESCRIBING VARIABLES

Differences of Face and Object Recognition in Utilizing Early Visual Information

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion

SPRING GROVE AREA SCHOOL DISTRICT. Course Description. Instructional Strategies, Learning Practices, Activities, and Experiences.

psychology of visual perception C O M M U N I C A T I O N D E S I G N, A N I M A T E D I M A G E 2014/2015

Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay*

Group Assignment #1: Concept Explication. For each concept, ask and answer the questions before your literature search.

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

Phil 490: Consciousness and the Self Handout [16] Jesse Prinz: Mental Pointing Phenomenal Knowledge Without Concepts

AP STATISTICS 2009 SCORING GUIDELINES

Visual Processing (contd.) Pattern recognition. Proximity the tendency to group pieces that are close together into one object.

University of Alberta. The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm. Shawn Tan

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex

Using Perceptual Grouping for Object Group Selection

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Table 1 Patient characteristics Preoperative. language testing

Measuring Focused Attention Using Fixation Inner-Density

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli

Measuring the User Experience

EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH. Laurence D. Smith University of Maine

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison

..." , \ I \ ... / ~\ \ : \\ ~ ... I CD ~ \ CD> 00\ CD) CD 0. Relative frequencies of numerical responses in ratio estimation1

Intelligent Object Group Selection

Still important ideas

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity

Statistics. Nur Hidayanto PSP English Education Dept. SStatistics/Nur Hidayanto PSP/PBI

Gathering and Repetition of the Elements in an Image Affect the Perception of Order and Disorder

Animated Scatterplot Analysis of Time- Oriented Data of Diabetes Patients

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

Sensory Thresholds and Signal Detection. Lecture 13

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo

Decline of the McCollough effect by orientation-specific post-adaptation exposure to achromatic gratings

Color Difference Equations and Their Assessment

How Far Away Is That? It Depends on You: Perception Accounts for the Abilities of Others

CS160: Sensori-motor Models. Prof Canny

Categorical Perception

Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge

The Color of Similarity

Business Statistics Probability

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Detection Theory: Sensitivity and Response Bias

Rajeev Raizada: Statement of research interests

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 2

S"TX. Revised - A Study of Comparative Judgments. of Numerical and Verbal Attribute Labels. Faculty Working Paper

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

9 research designs likely for PSYC 2100

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES, COGNITIVE ABILITIES, AND THE INTERPRETATION OF AUDITORY GRAPHS. Bruce N. Walker and Lisa M. Mauney

PERFORMANCE OPERATING CHARACTERISTICS FOR SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL DISCRIMINATIONS: COMMON OR SEPARATE CAPACITIES?

Rachael E. Jack, Caroline Blais, Christoph Scheepers, Philippe G. Schyns, and Roberto Caldara

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

Application of ecological interface design to driver support systems

Experimental Psychology

An experimental investigation of consistency of explanation and graph representation

Feature encoding and pattern classifications with sequentially presented Markov stimuli*

STUDY OF THE BISECTION OPERATION

Discrete Resource Allocation in Visual Working Memory

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

Practice Test Questions

Sequential similarity and comparison effects in category learning

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

COLOR DISTANCE ON MAPS

2.4.1 STA-O Assessment 2

Transcription:

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Sung Park and Richard Catrambone 2 School of Psychology & Graphics, Visualization, and Usability Center (GVU) Georgia Institute of Technology ABSTRACT We identify critical issues in comparative research on relational information displays (RIDs). The key argument is that when conducting an analysis of the cognitive process of people viewing different displays, their perceptual processes must be held constant so that they do not affect the results. We propose that in order to help researchers more easily compare display types (e.g., graphs) for how effectively they convey information, two factors must be considered. First, each element (e.g., each bar in a bar graph) in graphs that are being compared has to be equally discriminable. Second, the number of elements in the graphs being compared has to be the same; the maximum number of elements is limited by from the graph that uses a presentation format (e.g., density) that has the fewest number of discriminable levels. We present a psychophysics experiment that identified differential discrimination thresholds for density levels. CR Categories: I. 3. 6 [Computer Graphics]: Methodology and Techniques-Ergonomics Keywords: relational information display, human factors, psychophysics INTRODUCTION People often have trouble finding the right information when looking at graphs. Sometimes this leads to frustration, causing people to discontinue their search or resulting in them acquiring false information due to the poor display design. A clear understanding of which display is best suited for various types of information enhances the accuracy and efficiency of the user s task. A variety of graphic displays have been used to represent different types of information, such as line graphs, bar charts, pie charts, scatter plots, matrices, tables, networks, maps, and many others. These displays can be categorized under a common name, relational information displays (RIDs), which are displays that represent relations among types of information (e.g., a bar graph including a person's name on the x-axis and her finish in a race on the y-axis) []. To calibrate the effectiveness among different RIDs, welldesigned comparative research is required. In this study, critical issues in designing comparative research on RIDs will be discussed by analyzing prior RID research. This will lead to an argument for why additional grounding research in psychophysics is critical. Finally, a psychophysics experiment was conducted that provides initial foundational data. 2 THEORY Many studies were conducted on RIDs (for a review, see [2]). Some studies are considered as foundational work because they provide basic understanding on how to leverage our capabilities and limitations of visual perception so that we can design effective graphs or displays. Of this research, we focus on studies on displays associated with different types of data (data conveying different scale types such as quantitative, ordinal, and nominal). 2. Ranking Quantitative Tasks Cleveland and McGill [3] developed principles for statistical graphics. They developed the ranking of quantitative tasks based on psychophysics and their own empirical study (See quantitative ranking in Figure ). In psychophysics, Steven s Law portrays the relationship between an actual physical magnitude, x, and its perceived magnitude, p(x): p(x) = c x β The β weight differs from the particular physical magnitude being perceived. Experiments indicate that the average β for length judgments range from to.; for area, from to ; for volume, from to. This difference indicates that people in general are more accurate at length judgments than area and volume judgments. Cleveland and McGill also conducted an empirical study where participants were asked to judge the percent difference of each display. The result supported the quantitative ranking in Figure. email: gtg6s@mail.gatech.edu 2 email: rc7@prism.gatech.edu

Due to this distance effect, ordinal size perception has a severe stepping limitation meaning that the maximum number of values that can be encoded without the distance effect is limited. Finally, color hue was best for nominal information because color perception is highly selective and known to be processed in the preattentive stage of the visual process. This enables an accurate distinction among nominal values. Combining the results of Cleveland and McGill [3] and Mackinlay [4], an effectiveness ranking for different aproaches for three types of information (quantitative, ordinal, and nominal) can be created (See Figure ). It should be noted that in the original rankings, the two studies placed position as the best approach across all three information types. However, this approach is not considered in the present analysis because the position approach is known to be processed by a different system and this study wanted to control for this factor. That is, there are two separate systems for optic processing: visual and spatial information processing [5]. While visual information processing follows a projection from the occipital to the temporal cortex, spatial information processing follows a projection from the occipital to the parietal cortex. Accordingly, the present study focused on graphical presentations that are processed by the visual information system. 2.3 Represented and Representing Dimensions Study Figure. Ranking of different approaches for displaying information. The higher an approach is in the column, the better its accuracy. The approaches shown in the gray boxes are not relevant to these types of data. The ranking of quantitative data (A) is analyzed by Cleveland and McGill [3] and the rankings of ordinal and nominal data (B and C) are investigated by Mackinlay [4]. 2.2 Ranking Non-Quantitative (Ordinal and Nominal) Tasks Mackinlay [4] conducted a psychological analysis of displays presenting ordinal and nominal information to complement Cleveland and McGill s study (See ordinal and nominal rankings in Figure ). He suggested that density conveys ordinal information better than length primarily because length judgment suffers from the distance effect while density does not. For example, in Figure 2, the comparison of size (ordinal information) in lines (length) is easily perceived in (A) but is more difficult to do so in (B). On the other hand, density judgment does not suffer from the distance effect. Zhang [] suggested that the mapping between the type of display and type of data can be studied effectively by the distributed cognition framework because cognitive activity in RID related tasks is distributed into two different representations: an internal representation (propositions, schemas, productions, mental images) and an external representation (physical symbols, graphs, objects). For a review on distributed cognition, see Zhang & Norman [6]. In an example of a RID task (see Figure 3), the density of the circle codes the value it represents. Viewers are told that the denser the circle, the more value it represents. By visual inspection of both external representations, the perceptual difference that (a) has more density than (b) can easily be identified. However, the decision of whether (a) represents more value than (b) can be made only by referring to the internal representation (i.e., a rule that has been learned) that the denser the circle, the more value it represents. Thus, RID tasks relate two distributed components: an internal representation and an external representation. (a) 75 (b) 50 Figure 2. An example of the distance effect Figure 3. An example of a RID task. Each display represents an assigned number and follows the given rule that the denser the circle, the more value it represents. One of the most promising methodologies that has been adopted in distributed cognition is representational analysis which investigates representational effect. The representational effect refers to the phenomenon that different representations of a common formal structure can cause dramatically different cognitive behaviors. Zhang [] argued that representational analysis could Size indicates elementary tasks such as length, angle, slope, area, and volume collectively.

effectively investigate RIDs as well. For each type of data (quantitative, ordinal, nominal) different features (length, density, color) conveying isomorphic information can be compared to see how they lead to differences in cognitive processing. Accordingly, he provided a theoretical framework that accounts for the representational effects of all RIDs (See Figure 4). The framework suggests that although there are no optimal displays that are efficient for all types of tasks, there can be a correct or incorrect mapping between the representation of a display and the structure of a task. While the first and third hypotheses were supported, the second was only partially supported in that there was not a significant difference in performance between line graph (length) and density when an ordinal task was given. Some potential problems that might have affected the results can be identified by thoroughly investigating the task set that was used in their experiment. 2.4 Issues in Comparative Studies on RIDs Zhang s [] original framework focused on the information retrieval and interpretation of the viewer at the cognitive level rather than the perceptual level. This is because he was interested in how the match or the mismatch of the scale between display and data would affect the viewer s cognitive behavior. Park and Catrambone [7] attempted to capture cognitive behavior through two measures, correctness (whether or not participants produced the right answer) and reaction time. Consider the tasks represented in Figures 5 and 6. Both tasks are ordinal tasks with different types of display. This was a direct comparison experiment because in the analysis, Park and Catrambone [7] compared the mean values (correctness and reaction time) of the two displays. Figure 5 is shows a flaw though: the two lengths representing Chris and Alexis are somewhat difficult to discriminate. Obviously, this is a case where distance effect of length occurs, which might have affected the results as an extraneous variable. Figure 4. The mapping between the represented and representing dimensions. The scale type of each entity is inside the parentheses. Records are from a fictional women s 400 meter freestyle swimming competition. There are two dimensions: represented and representing dimensions. The represented dimensions of a RID are the data (e.g., times) while the representing dimensions are the display (e.g., length of bar). Zhang [] argued that in order for a representation to be efficient and accurate, the represented and representing dimensions should match in scale (A, E, I in Figure 4). In an attempt to examine the implications of Zhang s framework, Park and Catrambone [7] conducted an empirical study. The following three predictions were tested to verify the key hypothesis that when the represented and representing dimensions match, a person s performance will be best: Figure 5. Ordinal task with Line Graph (length) Performance in retrieving quantitative information is optimal when the display has the same quantitative scale property. Performance in retrieving ordinal information is optimal when the display has the same ordinal scale property. Performance in retrieving nominal information is optimal when the display has the same nominal scale property. Figure 6. Ordinal task with Density

To avoid this problem when conducting an analysis of cognitive process in comparative research on RIDs, each element (e.g., a single line that represents Alexis in Figure 5) has to be readily discernable. However, the word readily is not discrete and definitely would not assist in designing definite tasks or stimuli. As a result, a need to quantify the interval among the elements arises (e.g., how large does the interval between the lines or the interval between levels of density need to be). It is also important that the ease of discriminating the elements in each task should be held constant. (i.e., the level of difficulty to discriminate lines in Figure 5 and density in Figure 6 should be the same). Thus, the first proposition in our framework is: ) When conducting an analysis of cognitive process in comparative research on RIDs, each element in different graphs being compared by researchers has to be readily discernable and should have the same level of discrimination difficulty. When looking at Figures 5 and 6, the number of graphical elements in the two graphs is set to 8. The number of elements in two graphs being compared should be held constant because the numbers affect participants searching process. The question is then how many elements can be used. While the lower boundary is open and a task can be scaled down to decrease difficulty (i.e., in Figures 5 and 6 there can be fewer than 8 elements), the upper boundary might be less flexible. In Park and Catrambone s [7] experiment, they stated that when levels go from black to white in density, there are a limited number of separate levels that can be shown while keeping them discriminable. Each RGB level for gray saturation increases together from 0 to 255 in an IBM PC (e.g. R = 0, G = 0, B = 0 for black and R = 255, G = 255, B = 255 for white). Informal pilot testing in their study suggested that when the interval between RGB levels becomes less than about 36, the visual distinction among levels starts to break down. Therefore, their experiment used 8 density levels (i.e., a set of 8 choices) that had an interval greater than 36. It is interesting to note that the line graph (length) suffers less severely than density from this limitation meaning that the number of elements in line graph (Figure 5) ought to be greater than the maximum number for density. This suggests the second proposition in the framework: 2) When conducting an analysis of cognitive process in comparative research on RIDs, the number of elements in different graphs being compared by researchers has to be the same and the maximum number of elements has to be derived from the graphic presentation that has the least discriminable levels (e.g., density). Another important notion to consider is Steven s Law that the perceived magnitude of a difference should increase as the absolute physical magnitude does. In other words, the interval of 36 at the lower end and the higher end of density might not provide an identically perceived magnitude difference. While the main goal of the experiment is to study the differential thresholds across levels of density, the secondary goal is to test the hypothesis of whether Steven s Law affects the interval as density increases. 3 METHOD 3. Participants Participants were 40 undergraduate students who were enrolled in introductory psychology courses at the Georgia Institute of Technology who volunteered for the experiment to earn course credit. 3.2 Materials Participants viewed a pair of density levels. A Java application was created to implement the condition sets on IBM PCs. 3.3 Procedure Each participant had two density levels presented side by side on the screen. One was a "standard" density and the other was a "comparison" density. Participants were asked to determine which density level was denser. They pressed Z for the left stimulus and / for the right stimulus. While the standard density remained constant in levels within each block, the comparison density changed in each trial. The comparison density changed by an interval of 59 from the RGB level of the standard density (e.g., if the standard density has a RGB level of 64, the range of comparison density will be from 35 to 93). The step size of the comparison density was two. This study adopted the method of constant stimuli in psychophysics so the comparison density randomly varied with the restriction of range and step size in each trial. There were five blocks of trials with 94 trials each. Five blocks represented five different standard densities: RGB level of 0, 64, 28, 92, and 255. This was done to equally spread different density levels across a spectrum from 0 to 255. One goal was to test the hypothesis of whether the differential threshold is affected by Steven s Law. 3.4 Design The experiment was a one factor within subject design with five levels (RGB level of 0, 64, 28, 92, and 255) serving as the standards. A Latin square was used to determine the order of the levels. Participants were evenly distributed to each of the following task orders: Table. Latin square orders # Participants Order of Standard Density 8 0, 64, 28, 92, 255 8 64, 28, 92, 255, 0 8 28, 92, 255, 0, 64 8 92, 255, 0, 64, 28 8 255, 0, 64, 28, 92 4 RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The Latin square order had no effect and all results were collapsed over this variable. Density judgment accuracies involving the different standard densities as a function of the comparison densities are shown in Figures 7 to.

.05 5 5 5 5 5 5 3 5 7 9 3 5 7 9 2 23 25 27 29 5 226 228 230 232 234 236 238 240 242 244 246 248 250 252 254 Figure 7. Percent Correct at standard density of 0 Figure Figure. 0. Percent Correct at standard density of 255 Figure. 5 5 5 5 35 39 43 47 5 55 Figure 8. Percent Correct at standard density of 64 5 59 63 67 7 75 79 83 87 9 The upper and lower difference threshold was obtained by drawing a vertical line at 90% percent correct. While the traditional psychophysics study considers 50% as the differential threshold level, this study decided to set at 90% because as stated in proposition # in the framework, we are seeking a value that enables the viewer to readily discriminate the graph presentation. For example, in Figure 8, a density level of 5 or less (lower differential threshold) and 8 or more (upper differential threshold) can be readily discriminated when compared to with the standard density of 64. It is interesting to note that in the lowest density (i.e., density of 0, black) only a slight increase in the comparison density enabled a clear discrimination (See Figure 7), whereas in the highest density (i.e., density of 255, white) the lower differential threshold was relatively far away at 226 (See Figure ). This difference in threshold range is very noticeable when all the density levels are compared (See Figure 2). 5 5 5 5 99 03 07 5 9 23 Figure 9. Percent Correct at standard density of 28 27 3 35 39 43 47 5 55 250The upper and lower difference threshold was obtained by drawing a vertical line at 90% percent correct. While the 226 29 traditional psychophysics study considers 50% as the differential threshold 200 level, this study decided to set at 90% because as stated in proposition # in the framework, we are seeking a value that 7 enables the viewer to readily discriminate the graph presentation. 50 For example, in Figure 47 8, a density level of 5 or less (lower differential threshold) and 8 or more (upper differential threshold) can be 3 readily discriminated when compared 00 to with the standard density of 64. It is interesting to note 8 that in the lowest density (i.e., density of 0, black) only a slight increase in the comparison density enabled a 50clear discrimination 5(See Figure 7), whereas in the highest density (i.e., density of 255, white) Upper the lower Differential differential Thresholds threshold Lower Differential Thresholds was relatively far away at 226 (See Figure ). 0This difference 3 in threshold range is very noticeable when all 0 64 28 92 255 the density levels are compared (See Figure 2). 5 5 5 63 67 7 75 79 83 87 9 95 99 203 207 2 25 29 Figure 2. Upper and Lower Differential Threshold for the Standard Densities. Notice that the interval between the lower and upper threshold increases as a function of standard density. For example, the interval at standard density of 64 is 3 (i.e., 8 5 +) whereas the interval at standard density of 28 is 35 and of 92 is 49. This indicates that it becomes more difficult to discriminate as the Figure 0. Percent Correct at standard density of 92

density level increases. This is consistent with Steven s law. We discovered the lower and upper thresholds of various standard densities. We also found that the interval between the differential threshold and the standard density increases as a function of the standard density. This implies that the step size among different levels of density should be carefully determined when density graphs are compared with other types of graphs. For example, fixing the step size of the density graph to 20 is not a good idea. It might work for density levels below 200 but above this would no longer readily discriminable. There are two ways to leverage this foundational data when conducting comparative research on RIDs. The first is to fix the step size of density graphs to the threshold that works for all levels. We learned that 226 is the lower differential threshold for the standard density of 255 (white). This interval (i.e., 30) is the highest interval across all density levels. The second is to vary the step sizes based on the density levels. This will require a precise control of the presentation but will potentially guarantee more elements (number of density levels in a graph) available for use. Dimensions in Relational Information Displays. In Proceedings of 8 th International Conference on Information Visualization, 605-62. 2004. 5 CONCLUSION This study outlined some critical issues in comparative research on RIDs. The assumption is that when conducting an analysis of the cognitive process of participants viewing different displays, the perceptual process of these participants has to be held constant so that it does not affect the results. The framework suggested two propositions when conducting an analysis of cognitive process on different RIDs. First, each element in different graphs that are being compared for information display qualtiy has to be readily discernable and should have the same level of discrimination difficulty. Second, the number of elements in the different graphs has to be the same and the maximum number of elements has to be derived from the graphic presentation that has the least discriminable levels. The framework, in conjunction with the present study, particularly suggested that the number of elements has to be set equal to the density display because density has the fewest number of discriminable levels. Finally, a psychophysics experiment was conducted in order to determine differential thresholds across various density levels. We learned that Steven s power law plays a key role in deriving the differential thresholds and determining the number of elements that can used in comparative research on relational information displays. REFERERNCES [] Zhang, J. A representational analysis of relational information displays. In International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 45, 59-74. 996. [2] Carswell, C. M. & Wickens, C. D. Comparative graphics: History and applications of perceptual integrality theory and the proximity compatibility hypothesis (TR ARL-88-2/AHEL-88-). Aviation Research Laboratory, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. [3] Cleveland, W. S. & McGill, R. Graphical perception and graphical methods for analyzing scientific data, In Science, 828-833. 985. [4] Mackinlay, J. D. Automating the design of graphical presentations of relational information. In ACM Transactions on Graphics, 5(2), 0-4. 986. [5] Ungerleider, L. G. & Macko, K. A. Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways. In Trends in Neurosciences, 6, 44-47. [6] Zhang, J. & Norman, D. A. Representations in distributed cognitive tasks. In Cognitive Science, 8, 87-22. 994. [7] Park, S. & Catrambone, R. Represented and Representing