Self-Esteem Mediates Relationships between Self-Concept Clarity and Perceptions of the Future

Similar documents
Optimistic Versus Pessimistic Life-Orientation Beliefs among University Teachers

A Study of Life Satisfaction and Optimism in Relation to Psychological Well-Being Among Working and Non Working Women

How consumption influences our self-image Effects of material values and loneliness on self-image stability. Bachelor thesis Stephanie Haen ANR

Sikha Naik Mark Vosvick, Ph.D, Chwee-Lye Chng, Ph.D, and John Ridings, A.A. Center for Psychosocial Health

STUDY ON THE CORRELATION BETWEEN SELF-ESTEEM, COPING AND CLINICAL SYMPTOMS IN A GROUP OF YOUNG ADULTS: A BRIEF REPORT

The Big Five and HEXACO questionnaires are full of affect and so, probably, are your questionnaires

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations

ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH

University of Warwick institutional repository:

GOLDSMITHS Research Online Article (refereed)

Distinguishing Affective and Non-Affective Reactions to Daily Events

Level and Instability of Day-to-Day Psychological Weil-Being and Risk for Depression

It won t happen to me. Optimism, biases, and recall of road-risk information

Assignment 4: True or Quasi-Experiment

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Locus of Control and Psychological Well-Being: Separating the Measurement of Internal and External Constructs -- A Pilot Study

CHAPTER III RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

IMPLICATIONS OF THE DIMENSIONALITY OF UNREALISTIC OPTIMISM FOR THE STUDY OF PERCEIVED HEALTH RISKS

CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE

Perceived Likelihood as a Measure of Optimism and Pessimism: Support for the Future Events Scale

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Development of the Web Users Self Efficacy scale (WUSE)

DURHAM AREA SURVEY 2011: REPORT ON OPTIMISM. January, Prepared by: Lyris Antle Natasha Doelman Hannah Scott, Ph.D.

Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping

Alterations in self-perceptions following childhood onset of spinal cord injury

c) Redraw the model and place on it relevant attributions for each of the four boxes.

BRIEF REPORT. The effects of optimism and pessimism on updating emotional information in working memory. Sara M. Levens 1 and Ian H.

multilevel modeling for social and personality psychology

Multiple Act criterion:

Measuring Self-Esteem of Adolescents Based on Academic Performance. Grambling State University

A General Framework for Personality Psychology

DRAFT (Final) Concept Paper On choosing appropriate estimands and defining sensitivity analyses in confirmatory clinical trials

33 Multiple choice questions

Psychological Wellbeing of Management Students with Different Personality Types

Editorial. An audit of the editorial process and peer review in the journal Clinical Rehabilitation. Introduction

Negative Life Events, Self-Perceived Competence, and Depressive Symptoms in Young Adults

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior

A TEST OF SHARKEY AND SINGELIS (1995) MODEL OF SELF-CONSTRUAL AND EMBARRASSABILITY: SITUATIONAL VERSUS DISPOSITIONAL FACTORS 1

Development of a measure of self-regulated practice behavior in skilled performers

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 11 Cognitive Control of Behavior (Cont.)

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 114 ( 2014 ) th World Conference on Psychology, Counselling and Guidance

Piers Harris Children s Self-Concept Scale, Second Edition (Piers-Harris 2)

SITUATIONAL STRUCTURE AND INDIVIDUAL SELF-ESTEEM AS DETERMINANTS OF THREAT-ORIENTED REACTIONS TO POWER. Arthur R» Cohen

Title: The Relationship between Locus of Control and Academic Level and Sex of Secondary School Students

Self-Esteem Accessibility as Attitude Strength: On the Durability and Impactfulness of Accessible Self-Esteem

The Myers Briggs Type Inventory

Human Motivation and Emotion

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK

Pearson Education Limited Edinburgh Gate Harlow Essex CM20 2JE England and Associated Companies throughout the world

Resilience. A Paradigm Shift From At Risk: to At Potential. presented by

Bouncing back from setbacks

The Multifactor Leadership Questionnaire (MLQ) measures a broad range of leadership types from passive leaders, to leaders who give contingent rewards

Chapter 11 Nonexperimental Quantitative Research Steps in Nonexperimental Research

Teacher satisfaction: some practical implications for teacher professional development models

The Relation Between Happiness and Number of Siblings. Michael W. Passer. University of Washington

CLINICAL VS. BEHAVIOR ASSESSMENT

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

Reflect on the Types of Organizational Structures. Hierarch of Needs Abraham Maslow (1970) Hierarchy of Needs

HANDOUTS UNITED REPUBLIC OF TANZANIA MINISTRY OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL WELFARE. HIV and AIDS Voluntary Counselling and Testing MODULE 1.

(CORRELATIONAL DESIGN AND COMPARATIVE DESIGN)

Relationship among Creativity, Motivation and Creative Home Environment of Young Children

AC T I O N - O R I E N T E D H O P E - C E N T E R E D C A R E E R D E V E L O P M E N T

The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence

Quasi-experimental analysis Notes for "Structural modelling".

Formulation of Research Design

An Experimental Comparison of the Fairness Models by Bolton and Ockenfels and by Fehr and Schmidt

Childhood Healthcare Experience, Healthcare Attitudes, and Optimism as Predictors of Adolescents Healthcare Behavior

Thriving in College: The Role of Spirituality. Laurie A. Schreiner, Ph.D. Azusa Pacific University

Heavy drinking among college students is in uenced by anxiety sensitivity, gender, and contexts for alcohol use

A Discussion of the Incongruence Between Optimism and Academic Performance and Its Influence on School Violence

1. Before starting the second session, quickly examine total on short form BDI; note

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION

Meeting high standards: the effect of perfectionism on task performance, self-esteem, and self-efficacy in college students

Impression formation and cooperative behavior

Hardiness and optimism as moderators of cognitive emotion regulation. strategies in coping negative life events by adolescents.

NEARLY 12 MILLION COSmetic

The Attribute Index - Leadership

Visualizing Success: Investigating the Relationship between Ability and Self-Efficacy in the Domain of Visual Processing

Moderating and Mediating Variables in Psychological Research

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

FASD Spring Forum Developing a Purposeful Community

PERCEIVED TRUSTWORTHINESS OF KNOWLEDGE SOURCES: THE MODERATING IMPACT OF RELATIONSHIP LENGTH

Measurement of Constructs in Psychosocial Models of Health Behavior. March 26, 2012 Neil Steers, Ph.D.

Encyclopedia of Psychological Assessment (pp ). Rocio Fernandez-Ballesteros (Ed.). London: Sage, 2002.

Author's response to reviews

Optimism and self-esteem are related to sleep. Results from a large community based sample

Best on the Left or on the Right in a Likert Scale

WHO Quality of Life. health other than the cause of a disease or the side effects that come along with it. These other

ATTRIBUTION THEORY. Attribution - to explain by indicating a cause

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

Emotion Regulation Choice among Undergraduate Students with Posttraumatic Stress Symptoms

The Psychology of Inductive Inference

IS MY THINKING MAKING ME SICK? Donna L. Holland, MD, FAAP, FACP Internal Medicine/Pediatrics Integrative Medicine

Emotional Support LIVING WITH VITILIGO

Dermatology Life Quality Index: Data from Danish Inpatients and Outpatients

A Multidimensional, Hierarchical Self-Concept Page. Herbert W. Marsh, University of Western Sydney, Macarthur. Introduction

Collaborating across Cultures: Cultural Metacognition and Affect- Based Trust in Creative Collaboration

Screener and Opioid Assessment for Patients with Pain- Revised (SOAPP -R)

UNDERSTANDING AND INFLUENCING PUPILS CHOICES AS THEY PREPARE TO LEAVE SCHOOL EXECUTIVE SUMMARY. Dr Kathryn Asbury. University of York

Several studies have researched the effects of framing on opinion formation and decision

Transcription:

doi: 10.15503/jecs20181.99.108 Journal of Education Culture and Society No. 1_2018 99 Self-Esteem Mediates Relationships between Self-Concept Clarity and Perceptions of the Future Adrian Morawiak SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Department of Psychology, ul. Chodakowska 19/31, Warszawa, Poland E-mail address: morawiak.adrian@gmail.com Błażej Mroziński SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Department of Psychology, ul. Chodakowska 19/31, Warszawa, Poland E-mail address: bmrozinski@swps.edu.pl Joanna Gutral SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Department of Psychology, ul. Chodakowska 19/31, Warszawa, Poland E-mail address: jgutral@swps.edu.pl Marzena Cypryańska SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Department of Psychology, ul. Chodakowska 19/31, Warszawa, Poland E-mail address: mcypryanska@swps.edu.pl John B. Nezlek jbnezl@wm.edu SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Department of Psychology, ul. Chodakowska 19/31, Warszawa, Poland; The College of William and Mary, Williamsburg, Virginia, USA E-mail address: jbnezl@wm.edu Abstract Aim. The present study was designed to examine relationships between young people s self-concepts and their perceptions of their futures Methods. High school students (n = 347) completed measures of the two domains of self-concept, the evaluative domain, self-esteem, and the knowledge or structural

100 Dynamics domain, self-concept clarity. They also completed two measures of perceptions of their futures, optimism and future time perspective. Results. Both measures of self-concept were positively correlated with both measures of perception of the future. For both measures of perceptions of the future, regression analyses found that when perceptions of the future were regressed onto the two measures of self-concept perceptions of the future were signi cantly related to only self-esteem. Relationships between perceptions of the future and self-concept clarity were not signi cant. Analyses of mediation revealed that self-esteem mediated the relationship between self-concept clarity and both measures of perceptions of the future. Conclusion. Young people with a clearer sense of self and who have higher selfesteem are more optimistic and perceive a longer future than young people with a less clear sense of self and who have lower self-esteem; however, the effects of self-concept clarity disappear after the relationship between clarity and self-esteem are taken into account. Key words: self-esteem, self-concept clarity, future orientation, optimism Introduction People s perceptions of their futures can serve as important in uences on their present behaviours. For example, if people view their futures as hopeless, they may be unmotivated to try to improve their present circumstances or to make plans for a more adaptive future. Such a possibility is the foundation of one of the leading models of depression, the learned helplessness model (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978). In contrast, if people think of their futures more positively, they are more likely to engage in behaviours that promote both their present and future well-being (e.g., Carver, Scheier, & Segerstrom, 2010). Moreover, self-concept is a critical component of how people formulate their visions of the future. For example, low self-esteem is associated with greater level of distress, particularly depression (Orth, & Robins, 2013), and depression is associated with decreased optimism. Using research on perceptions of the future and research on self-concept as a starting point, the present study examined relationship between self-concept and perceptions of the future among a sample of high-school students. We studied high school students because adolescence can be an important period in life in terms of setting the stage for later life. For example, at this time, people may choose to attend university or not, and this decision can have a profound effect on their options in the future. We present the hypotheses and expectations that guided our study proceeded by relevant literature review. Perceptions of the future The future can be conceptualized in various ways. People can think of a job they might have, of the type of relationships they will have, where they will live, how they think they will feel, and so forth. For present purposes we measured people s perceptions of their futures in terms of two constructs, optimism

Journal of Education Culture and Society No. 1_2018 101 and time perspective. We thought that these two constructs would provide complementary insights into young people s perceptions of their futures. We relied upon a de nition of optimism proposed by Carver, et al. (2010): Optimism is an individual difference variable that re ects the extent to which people hold generalized favorable expectancies for their future (p. 879). As a result of their review, Carver et al. concluded that optimism had numerous positive outcomes, including psychological well-being. Although they did not mention self-esteem per se, the correlates of optimism they described are also part of the nomological network of self-esteem. Interestingly, Carver et al. did not speci cally address causal relationships between optimism and the other constructs with which they were concerned. They assumed that optimism led to certain outcomes rather than reverse. We address this issue below. Another way we conceptualized perceptions of the future relied upon the construct of time perspective introduced by Carstensen and colleagues (e.g., Lang, & Carstensen, 2002). Future time perspective (FTP) refers to the extent to which people see their futures as open or limitless versus limited. Although much of the research on FTP has concerned age differences in FTP (older adults have been found to have more limited FTP than younger adults), some has concerned relationships between self-esteem and FTP. This research suggests that self-esteem is positively related to how open people view their futures to be (e.g., Davis, Hicks, Schlegel, Smith, & Vess, 2015). Moreover, FTP has been found to be positively related to optimism (e.g., Allemand, Hill, Ghaemmaghami, & Martin, 2012). The evaluative and knowledge components of self-evaluation The self is one of the most studied topics in psychology, and the self has been studied using a nearly incomprehensible number of frameworks within the context of an equally large number of theories. Nevertheless, many measures of the self can be understood in terms of the extent to which they concern two broad dimensions (e.g., Nezlek, & Plesko, 2001) of self-concept. The rst, and by far the most commonly studied dimension, is the evaluative dimension, and it refers to how positively people value themselves. Perhaps the prototypical measure of self-evaluation is Rosenberg s (1965) classic measure of self-esteem. The second dimension re ects what is sometimes referred to as the knowledge component, and one way this knowledge component has been conceptualized is in terms of a construct called self-concept clarity. Self-concept clarity (SCC) refers to the extent to which self-beliefs are internally consistent, stable, and clearly de ned (Campbell, 1990; Campbell, at al., 1996). It is important to note that variability in SCC re ects variability in the consistency and con- dence of beliefs about the self. It does not re ect the content or accuracy of beliefs about the self.

102 Dynamics The bulk of research on relationships between self-esteem and SCC had found that the two are positively related (e.g., Campbell et al., 1996; Nezlek, & Plesko, 2001). People who have higher self-esteem, have clearer self-concepts and vice-versa. It is important to note however, that self-concept clarity is distinct from self-esteem. The two constructs covary independently of their covariance with various other measures including the factors of the Five Factor Model of personality and private self-consciousness (Campbell et al., 1996). Moreover, as noted by Campbell et al. (1996) in terms of self-esteem and SCC considered in isolation, there is no reason to assume the causal precedence of one over the other. The present study: Relationships between self-concept and perceptions of the future Although the self serves as an important basis for thinking about the future, relationships between perceptions of the future and self-concept have not received much attention, at least in terms of the evaluative and knowledge components that we have discussed, and the present study was designed to examine such relationships. We collected measures of the evaluative and knowledge components of self-concept, self-esteem and self-concept clarity respectively, and we collected two measures of perceptions of the future, a measure of future time perspective and a measure of optimism. The lack of directly relevant theory and research made it dif cult to pose detailed hypotheses; nevertheless, there was enough to provide a basis for some general expectations. The general principle underlying our expectations was that an individual s present self-concept provides a basis for making predictions about the future. Overall, we expected that both self-esteem and selfconcept clarity would be positively related to both measures of perceptions of the future. Individuals whose self-esteem was higher would be more optimistic and would see a more open future compared to individuals whose self-esteem was lower. Similarly, we expected that individuals when had a clearer sense of who they were, would be more optimistic and would see a more open future compared to individuals who had a less clear sense of who they were. On a somewhat more speculative basis, we also examined mediational relationships among our measures. Would self-esteem mediate relationships between self-concept clarity and perceptions of the future or vice-versa? In this regard, previous research and theory provided no guidance at all, so we truly had no speci c expectations. Our logic also assumes that self-concept leads to perceptions of the future instead of the opposite causal sequence. This assumption was based on the conclusion drawn from the research review on relationships between selfesteem and depression by Orth and Robins (2013). They concluded: Overall, the available evidence provides strong support for the vulnerability model (low self-esteem contributes to depression), weaker support for the scar model

Journal of Education Culture and Society No. 1_2018 103 (depression erodes self-esteem) (p. 455). Given how important perceptions of the future are in the etiology and maintenance of depression, we assumed that self-esteem would be related to this critical feature of depression in the same way it was related to depression itself. Material and methods Participants and procedure Participants were 347 high school students (189 women and 158 men) aged 15 to 17 (M = 16.55, SD = 0.98). Participants were asked to volunteer for a study about their life satisfaction. Participants were under 18 years old and so a parent or legal guardian needed to sign a consent form to allow them to participate. Only students who provided such signed consent were allowed to take part in that study. No incentives were offered for participation nor were there any penalties for not participating. Participants were told that their responses would be anonymous and that they had the right to discontinue participation at any time without giving a reason why. The data were collected during a community meeting that each class had with their lead teacher each week. These meetings lasted 45 minutes, and participants completed the questionnaires at that time. We collected two measures of self-concept. One, the Rosenberg Self-esteem scale (RSE; Rosenberg, 1965), was a measure of the evaluative component of self-concept, and the other, the Self-Concept Clarity scale (SCC; Campbell et al., 1996), was a measure of the knowledge component. We also collected two measures of perceptions of the future. One was the Future Time Perspective Scale (FTP; Lang, & Carstensen, 2002), the other was the Life Orientation Test (LOT; Scheier, Carver, & Bridges, 1994). We used Polish language versions of all measures. Due to the fact that participants were volunteers and the data were collected in classes, we wanted to minimize the total number of items we asked participants to answer. Based on published factor analyses and examination of the items, we reduced the number of items in the RSE, SCC, and LOT. Note that item numbers are taken from the item numbers as presented in the original published English language versions of the scales. For the FTP, we used all ten items from the original scale. For the RSE we used items 1, 6, 7, 8, 9, and 10. For the SCC we used items 1, 3, 4, 8, 11, and 12. For the LOT, we used items 3, 4, 7 and 10. For the FTP, we used a 1-7 scale with endpoints labelled de nitely not true and de nitely true. For the LOT, we used a 0-4 scale with endpoints labelled de nitely does not refer to me and de nitely refers to me. For the RSE, we used a 1-4 scale with endpoints labelled I strongly agree and I strongly disagree. For the SCC, we used a 1-5 scale with endpoints labelled I de nitely disagree and I de nitely agree. The raw data for this study and copies of the scales that we used are available on the Open Science Framework repository.

104 Dynamics Results Before conducting the primary analyses we inspected participant s responses to ensure that they had answered thoughtfully. We de ned a careless responder as someone who provided identical answers to all the items for a measure even when the items would logically require different responses, e.g., positively and negatively valenced items (Meade, & Craig, 2012). Participants who did this on one or more measures were excluded from the analyses, leaving a nal sample of 334. To provide a context for understanding the analyses that examined the relationships among our measures, univariate summary statistics for the measures are presented in Table 1. Before estimating the reliabilities we scored all items so that a higher score represented more of the construct being measured, higher self-esteem, more optimism, and so forth. The resulting reliabilities indicated that our scales had at least fair reliability according to Shrout (1998). Also, as can be seen from the data in Table 1, the mean scores were in the middle of their respective scales suggesting that oor and ceiling effects were not an issue. As expected, the measures we collected were positively correlated, relationships that are consistent with what has been found in the past. These correlations indicated that young people who had a clearer sense who they are, saw their futures more optimistically and as having a longer time perspective than young people with a less clear sense of who they are. Similarly, young people with higher self-esteem saw their futures more optimistically and as having a longer time perspective than young people with lower self-esteem. Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations between measures M SD alpha LOT RSE SCC FTP 4.72.81.75.36.44.24 LOT 2.44.74.64.50.21 RSE 2.77.58.81.54 SCC 2.73.81.76 Given the positive correlation between self-esteem and self-concept clarity, we needed to determine if relationships between perceptions of the future and these two measures were independent. First, we conducted two multiple regressions, one in which FTP scores were the outcome and scores on our measures of self-concept (RSE and SCC) were predictors, and the other in which scores on the LOT were the outcome and scores on the RSE and SCC were predictors. When FTP scores were regressed onto RSE and SCC scores, the test of the of the overall model was signi cant, F(2,331) = 39.80, p <.001, R 2 =.19; however, the coef cient for the SCC was not signi cant ( =.01, t < 1), whereas the coef cient for RSE was signi cant ( =.44, t(331) = 7.53, p <.001). Recall that the zero-order correlation between the FTP and the SCC was signi cant (r =.24, p <.001). This pattern suggests that self-esteem mediated relationships between self-concept clarity and

Journal of Education Culture and Society No. 1_2018 105 future time perspective. The same pattern occurred when LOT was regressed on RSE and SCC. When LOT scores were regressed onto RSE and SCC scores, the test of the of the overall model was signi cant, F(2,331) = 54.68, p <.001, R 2 =.25; however, the coef cient for SCC was not signi cant ( = -.07, t < 1), whereas the coef cient for RSE was signi cant ( =.53, t(331) = 9.44, p <.001). Recall that the zero-order correlation between LOT and SCC scores was signi cant (r =.21, p <.001). This pattern suggests that self-esteem mediated relationships between self-concept clarity and optimism. To examine the possibility that self-esteem mediated relationships between self-concept clarity and our measures of perception of the future, we conducted analyses using the PROCESS (v. 2.16.3) macro for SPSS, a set of procedures developed by Hayes (2013) to examine mediation within multiple regression. We used variants of Model 4 of this procedure with 10,000 bootstrapped samples and 95% con dence intervals. In one analysis the outcome variable was FTP, the mediator was RSE, and the independent variable was SCC. In a second analysis, the outcome variable was LOT, the mediator was RSE, and the independent variable was SCC. The results of these analyses were quite clear and are illustrated in Figure 1. Self-esteem mediated the relationship between future time perspective and self-concept clarity. The direct effect of self-concept clarity on future time perspective was not signi cant, -.001 (se =.059, t = < 1, CI: -.114 to.116), whereas the indirect effect of self-esteem on future time perspective was signi cant,.236 (se =.041, z = 6.29, p <.001, CI:.163 to.327). Similarly, self-esteem mediated the relationship between optimism and self-concept clarity. The direct effect of self-concept clarity on optimism was not signi cant, -.065 (se =.052, t = 1.25, CI: -.166 to.036), whereas the indirect effect of self-esteem on optimism was signi cant,.261 (se =.034, z = 7.29, p <.001, CI:.199 to.336). Figure 1. Standardized regression coef cients for the relationship between self-concept clarity and future time perspective as mediated by self-esteem. Note: The standardized regression coef cient between self-concept clarity and future time perspective, controlling for self-esteem, is in parentheses. *p <.001

106 Dynamics Figure 2. Standardized regression coef cients for the relationship between self-concept clarity and optimism as mediated by self-esteem Note: The standardized regression coef cient between self-concept clarity and optimism, controlling for self-esteem, is in parentheses. *p <.001 Discussion As expected, we found that self-concept was positively related to perceptions of the future. Both self-esteem and self-concept clarity were positively related to optimism and to the length of people s future time perspective. Moreover, we found that self-esteem mediated relationships between selfconcept clarity and people s perceptions of their futures. The mediational relationships we found, suggest that relationships between self-concept clarity and perceptions of the future occurred because of the relationships between self-esteem and self-concept clarity, and that self-concept clarity is not directly related to perceptions of the future. Therefore, we will limit our discussion to relationships between self-esteem and perceptions of the future. When people think of their futures they use the present as part of this construction in part because the present consists of immediately available stimuli and information. Moreover, the self is the mechanism that processes experience making the self one of the most important aspects of the present. Granting this, self-evaluation is an important part of the basis people use to make judgments about the future. For example, we used the following item to measure self-esteem: On the whole, I am satis ed with myself, and we measured optimism with this item: Overall, I expect more good things to happen to me than bad. It would be inconsistent (dissonant) for someone to endorse the rst item strongly while endorsing the second item weakly. Being satis ed with one s self includes (at least to some extent) being satis ed with one s life (e.g., Pavot, & Diener, 2008), and so how people evaluate themselves in the present (self-esteem) should be related to how they suppose their lives will be in the future.

Journal of Education Culture and Society No. 1_2018 107 Relationships between self-esteem and future time perspective may also re ect the variance they share with a third construct: self-ef cacy. As noted by Bandura (1997), self-esteem is positively related to self-ef cacy, and some of the items on the future time perspective scale we used seem to be similar to how self-ef cacy is measured. For example, one item from the future time perspective scale is I could do anything I want in the future, one item from a popular measure of self-ef cacy (Schwarzer & Jerusalem, 1995) is It is easy for me to stick to my aims and accomplish my goals. Believing that one can accomplish things in the present would predispose one to believe that he or she could accomplish things in the present. Limitations and future directions The present study clearly has its limitations. First, our results describe a sample of younger people from a speci c place. Although there is no reason to believe that Polish students differ from students of a similar age who live in other, industrialized and economically developed countries, they might. More likely is the possibility that relationships between the self and perceptions of the future differ from the present results among people who live in cultures in which the self is de ned differently for example in terms of self-construal (Cross, Hardin, & Gercek Swing, 2011). Determining if such differences exist requires studying individuals from such cultures or measuring individual differences in self-construal. Second, there is the limitation inherent in our use of a sample of young people. A considerable body of research has found that the future time perspective of older people is shorter than the future time perspective of younger people (e.g., Lang, & Carstensen, 2002). This research has also found that for older people this shortened time perspective is associated with increased positive affect and well-being because older people focus on the more positive aspects of their lives. This suggests that relationships between future time perspective and self-concept among older people may differ from the relationships between these measures we found in our sample of younger people. In contrast, no age differences have been proposed for relationships between optimism and other constructs. Examining such possibilities will require studying a sample of older people and comparing the results to those from a sample of younger people. There is also the issue of the speci c measures we used. Although we used well-validated and widely used measures of both self-concept and perceptions of the future, it is possible that different measures would have led to different results. Despite the limitations of the present study we believe the results contribute to our understanding of how young people perceive their futures. Although having a clear sense of self may be associated with more positive perceptions of the future, upon closer inspection, such relationships are probably due to the fact that people who have a clearer sense of self have higher self-esteem. We

108 Dynamics hope that the present study has provided some useful insights into the factors that in uence how people construct their futures as well as the types of methods and statistical analyses that can be used to study such topics. References 1. Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87(1), 49 74. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.87.1.49. 2. Allemand, M., Hill, P. L., Ghaemmaghami, P., & Martin, M. (2012). Forgivingness and subjective well-being in adulthood: The moderating role of future time perspective. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 32 39. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2011.11.004 3. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-ef cacy: The exercise of control. New York: W.H. Freeman. 4. Campbell, J. D. (1990). Self-esteem and the clarity of the self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 538-549. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.3.538 5. Campbell, J. D., Trapnell, P. D., Heine, S. J., Katz, I. M., Lavallee, L. F., & Lehman, D. R. (1996). Self-concept clarity: Measurement, personality correlates, and cultural boundaries. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 141-156. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.1.141 6. Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2010). Optimism. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 879 889. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.01.006 7. Cross, S. E., Hardin, E. E., & Gercek Swing, B. (2011). The what, how, why, and where of selfconstrual. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15, 142 179. doi:10.1177/1088868310373752 8. Davis, W. E., Hicks, J. A., Schlegel, R. J., Smith, C. M., & Vess, M. (2015). Authenticity and self-esteem across temporal horizons. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 10(2), 116-126. doi:10. 1080/17439760.2014.910830 9. Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: The Guilford Press. 10. Lang, F. R., & Carstensen, L. L. (2002). Time counts: Future time perspective, goals, and social relationships. Psychology and Aging, 17, 125 139. doi:10.1037//0882-7974.17.1.125 11. Meade, A. W., & Craig, S. B. (2012). Identifying careless responses in survey data. Psychological Methods, 17(3), 437 455. doi:10.1037/a0028085 12. Nezlek, J. B., & Plesko, R. M. (2001). Day-to-day relationships among self-concept clarity, self-esteem, daily events, and mood. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 201-211. doi:10.1177/0146167201272006 13. Orth, U., & Robins, R. W. (2013). Understanding the link between low self-esteem and depression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(6), 455-460. doi:10.1177/0963721413492763 14. Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (2008). The satisfaction with life scale and the emerging construct of life satisfaction. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 3, 137 152. doi:10.1080/17439760701756946 15. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton. 16. Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1063 1078. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1063 17. Shrout, P. E. (1998). Measurement reliability and agreement in psychiatry. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 7, 301 317. doi:10.1191/096228098672090967.