INTRODUCTION PURPOSE METHOD

Similar documents
INTRODUCTION METHODS

INTRODUCTION Use of images in multiple-choice tasks is common in linguistic comprehension

Background. Can APT-3 Improve Reading Impairment in Mild Aphasia?

How difficult is it? How well Adults with Aphasia Perceive Task Demands

Effort Invested in Cognitive Tasks by Adults with Aphasia: A Pilot Study

The inner workings of working memory: Preliminary data from unimpaired populations

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

Training and generalization of complex auditory-visual conditional discriminations in individuals with autism: New procedures using dynamic stimuli.

Contextual bias and inferencing in adults with right hemisphere brain damage It is widely accepted that damage to the right cerebral hemisphere (RHD)

Methods. Preliminary Results

Exploring the relationship between high level anomia, attention and cognitive processing deficits: a retrospective data analysis

Effects of Response Elaboration Training on Increased Length and Complexity of Utterances with Two Participants with Fluent Aphasia

Critical Review: Effectiveness of EMA in improving articulatory accuracy in adults with AOS

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

TITLE: Acquisition and generalization responses in aphasia treatment: Evidence from sentence-production treatment

Disclosures. Background. Equipment 2/4/2015

2.1 Participants 122 stroke patients with aphasia and 25 healthy controls (Table 1). <insert Table 1>

Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities

Higher Cortical Function

Eye-tracking technology in low vision: implications for functional diagnosis and rehabilitation

Using contextual analysis to investigate the nature of spatial memory

Consolidating working memory: Enhancing cognitive performance through effective encoding

NeuroTracker Published Studies & Research

Using Virtual Clinicians to Promote Functional Communication Skills in Aphasia Method. Participants. Experimental design

Critical Review: Group Therapy for Post-Stroke Aphasia Rehabilitation

AUDITORY ATTENTIONAL MECHANISMS IN SCHOOL- AGE CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS

Changes in Chronic Global Aphasia at Impairment and Functional Communication Levels following SGD Practice and Use. Richard. D. Steele Lingraphica

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Clinical Aphasiology Submission 2006

Defining the Parameters of Incidental Learning on a Serial Reaction Time (SRT) Task: Do Conscious Rules Apply?

Concurrent Learning of Temporal and Spatial Sequences

Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task

Balancing Cognitive Demands: Control Adjustments in the Stop-Signal Paradigm

***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully replicate the published version*** Auditory Temporal Processes in the Elderly

Measuring Focused Attention Using Fixation Inner-Density

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features

Sensory Memory Systems. Visual Store. PDF created with pdffactory trial version

Learning Objectives.

Variation in spectral-shape discrimination weighting functions at different stimulus levels and signal strengths

Gait abnormalities as early signs of MCI

APPLIED NEUROLINGUISTICS /A 3 credits. September 11 November 27, 2012

S everal studies indicate that the identification/recognition. Identification Performance by Right- and Lefthanded Listeners on Dichotic CV Materials

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION

Traumatic Brain Injury for VR Counselors Margaret A. Struchen, Ph.D. and Laura M. Ritter, Ph.D., M.P.H.

The role of tone duration in dichotic temporal order judgment (TOJ)

An Eye Movement Study on the Relationship Between Multiple Implicit Sequence Learning and Attention

Improvements in Chronic Broca s Aphasia at Impairment and Functional Communication Levels following SGD Practice and Use

Optical Illusions and Human Visual System: Can we reveal more? CIS Micro-Grant Executive Report and Summary of Results

Maximizing Generalization Effects of Semantic Feature Analysis

PSYCHOMETRIC VALIDATION OF SPEECH PERCEPTION IN NOISE TEST MATERIAL IN ODIA (SPINTO)

Publisher: Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:.

Handheld AAC Program for People with Aphasia. Marjorie Nicholas, PhD Krystal Peralta, MS

Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the

The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR

observed. In spite of the fact that D.H. demonstrated minimal definitive changes on the conversational measures selected, by the end of both

What is aphasia? Katrina Clarkson Principal Speech and Language Therapist, Regional Rehabilitation Unit, Northwick Park Hospital

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources

Validity of Haptic Cues and Its Effect on Priming Visual Spatial Attention

priming disappear, while stays intact? Why does long-term positive long-term negative priming

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract

A PROPOSED MODEL OF SPEECH PERCEPTION SCORES IN CHILDREN WITH IMPAIRED HEARING

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH. University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK

MedRx HLS Plus. An Instructional Guide to operating the Hearing Loss Simulator and Master Hearing Aid. Hearing Loss Simulator

RECOVERY OF LINGUISTIC DEFICITS IN STROKE PATIENTS; A THREE- YEAR-FOLLOW UP STUDY.

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (2), February /98/103(2)/1080/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 1080

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

Hearing Profile of Stroke Patients

REFERENCES. Hemispheric Processing Asymmetries: Implications for Memory

Basic Audiogram Interpretation

Hemisphere Deconnection and Unity in Conscious Awareness

Assessing Hearing and Speech Recognition

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm

CAN WE PREDICT STEERING CONTROL PERFORMANCE FROM A 2D SHAPE DETECTION TASK?

CHAPTER ONE CORRELATION

Measuring Changes in Quality of Life in Persons with Aphasia: Is Communication Confidence a Good Measure?

Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level

Perceptual Load in Different Regions of the Visual Field and its Effect on Attentional Selectivity. Hadas Marciano Advisor: Yaffa Yeshurun

3/25/2010. Multitasking

Child Date. Thinking Skills Inventory (TSI) Specialized Preschool Version

Gender-Based Differential Item Performance in English Usage Items

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention

Critical Review: Do mind-body therapies improve language outcomes in persons with aphasia?*

Adapting Patient Provider. with Communication Disorders

Auditory Stimuli Slow Down Responses and First Fixations: Support for Auditory Dominance in Adults

Attentional control in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Psychology, University of Hull, Hull, UK

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

The role of memory on patients with mild cognitive impairment. Sophia Vardaki, PhD National Technical University of Athens

Psy /16 Human Communication. By Joseline

Competing Streams at the Cocktail Party

Skill Council for Persons with Disability Expository for Speech and Hearing Impairment E004

Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect

MSc Neuroimaging for Clinical & Cognitive Neuroscience

Perception Lab Reports. Style Guide. Aaron Bornstein

Meaning-based guidance of attention in scenes as revealed by meaning maps

shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all of the above.

Speech perception in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer s type (DAT) Mitchell S. Sommers Department of Psychology Washington University

EARLY ONSET FRONTOTERMPORAL DEMENTIA AND ALZHEIMERS DISEASE: DIAGNOSIS, TREATMENT AND CARE

Transcription:

INTRODUCTION Several authors have reported that individuals with aphasia have greater difficulty allocating attention according to task demands than people without neurological disorders (Hula & McNeil, 2008; LaPointe & Erickson, 1991; McNeil et al., 2004; McNeil et al., 2005; Murray, Holland, & Beeson, 1997; Robin & Rizzo, 1988). Studying how attention deficits contribute to neurogenic language deficits is important for clinical practice and research. However, existing methods for indexing attention allocation in people with aphasia pose serious methodological challenges, including demands on comprehension abilities for understanding of dual-task instructions prior to an experiment, and response requirements that may impact participants performance. Eyetracking methods have great potential to address such challenges. Such methods do not require a) understanding of complex instructions; b) responding verbally, in writing, or with gestures; or c) manipulating devices, such as a computer mouse or joystick (Hallowell, Wertz, & Kruse, 2002). These features reduce critical response confounds and improve the validity of assessment tools for indexing attention allocation (Heuer & Hallowell, 2013). Heuer and Hallowell (2013) developed an eyetracking method to assess attention allocation using a dual-task paradigm in individuals with and without aphasia. The dual-task method included a visual search task, in which participants were trained to find a visual target in a display that included one target and three nontarget foils, and an auditory linguistic processing task, in which sentences were presented auditorally. Attention demands were manipulated by varying the complexity of each of the two tasks. Changes in attention demands were indexed through performance on the visual search task using eyetracking measures. Results indicated that the method is sensitive to differences between people with and without aphasia, and that it captures response variations associated with task demands and stimulus complexity. While those results were valuable in establishing the construct validity of the novel eyetracking-based measures, no conclusions could be drawn about the concurrent validity of the method because no previously validated measure of attention allocation had been administered. PURPOSE The purpose of this study was to test the concurrent validity of the novel eyetracking method developed by Heuer and Hallowell (2013) by comparing the performance on single and dual tasks to scores from a standardized traditional attention assessment-the Test of Everyday Attention (TEA, Ridgeway, Robertson, Ward, & Nimmo-Smith, 1994). Participants without neurological impairment were studied to ensure confidence that TEA task performance was not confounded by language or cognitive impairments. It was also important to replicate the findings for individuals free of neurogenic deficits reported in the previous study to ensure that the construct validity of the method was maintained. METHOD Participants. Forty individuals without neurological impairment, who passed a mental status screening (Mini Mental Status Examination; MMSE; Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975), and vision and hearing screenings, participated. None had participated in previous eyetracking experiments.

Eyetracking tasks. The eyetracking single and dual tasks are described in detail by Heuer and Hallowell (2013). See Figure 1 for an example of a single visual search task, and Figure 2 for an example of the dual task. Test of Everyday Attention (TEA, Ridgeway, Robertson, Ward, & Nimmo-Smith, 1994). The TEA assesses a variety of different functions of attention, including focused, divided and sustained attention in visual and auditory modality. The subtests Telephone Search and Telephone Search while counting were administered because these construct targeted by these subtests best mirrors the construct of attention allocation targeted through the novel eyetracking method. In the Telephone Search subtest, participants search for key symbols while ignoring irrelevant symbols in a simulated classified telephone directory. In the Telephone Search with Counting subtest, participants again complete the telephone search, this time in conjunction with a counting task of prerecorded tones. Test stimuli include mock pages from a telephone directory. Auditory stimuli in the counting condition are prerecorded strings of tones. Analysis. Eye movements were recorded using an LC Technologies EyeFollower system with a sampling rate of 120 Hz. The proportion of fixation duration on target stimuli (PFDT) served as the dependent measure (Heuer & Hallowell, 2013). PFDT was defined as the total duration of fixations on the target image, divided by the total of fixation durations on all images in the display. A value close to 1 indicates that the viewer fixated mostly the target, indexing low processing demands; a value close to zero indicates that the viewer distributed fixations equally across targets and foils, not identifying the target, indexing high processing demands. A repeated measures ANOVA and follow-up t-tests comparing single and dual -task performance, and performance during simple and complex stimulus conditions for the eyetracking tasks were conducted. Pearson correlation coefficients for eyetracking measures and TEA scores were also computed. RESULTS Replication of eyetracking-based attention allocation measures The sample mean of -.04 (SD =.06) was significantly different from 0, t(38) = 3.34, p =.002, (two-tailed), d =.53, indicating a significant decrement in PFDT from single todual task processing. The mean PFDT for simple stimuli of.71 (SD =.08) was significantly higher than the mean PFDT for visually complex stimuli of.62 (SD =.10) during the single visual search task, as indicated by a significant paired-samples t-test, t(38) = 11.55, p >.001, d =.90. PFDT significantly decreased with an increase in stimulus complexity as indexed by a significant main effect, F(2, 76) = 62.18, p <.001 (see means and standard deviations for simple, medium, and complex stimulus conditions in Table 1, and pair-wise comparisons incorporating a Holm p-value correction in Table 2). Validation of attention allocation task with the Test of Everyday Attention Pearson Correlation Coefficients were computed for eyetracking measures of visual search single and dual task and the raw and scaled scores for the TEA single (telephone search), and dual task (telephone search while counting). See Table 3 for descriptive data on the performance of individuals on the Telephone search and Table 4 for statistical results. TEA scores correlated significantly with the eyetracking measures, indicating high validity of the

novel attention allocation task. Only the correlation between the difference scores for eyetracking versus TEA measures did not achieve significance. DISCUSSION Results replicated the findings for control participants in the original Heuer and Hallowell (2013) study. With an increase in task demands associated with an increase in stimulus complexity and also with changes from single-to dual-task processing, decreases in the dependent eyetracking measure PFDT were observed. These findings are important because they support the construct validity of the eyetracking method and ensure that the eyetracking method being examined for concurrent validity with an established measure (the TEA) indexes attention allocation as originally purported. Concurrent validation of the novel eyetracking task and associated measures was achieved for single- and dual-task processing. TEA single-task scores correlated significantly with eyetracking measures for the visual search single and dual tasks. Similarly, TEA dual task scores correlated highly with the eyetracking measures for the visual search single and the dual tasks. SUMMARY The concurrent validity of the novel eyetracking method for capturing attention allocation was established. Results are encouraging in terms of the future feasibility of using the new eyetracking-based method as a clinical and research tool to assess attention allocation in individuals with neurogenic deficits that are difficult to assess with traditional assessment tools. REFERENCES Hallowell, B., Wertz, R.T., & Kruse, H. (2002). Using eye movement responses to index auditory comprehension: An adaptation of the Revised Token Test. Aphasiology, 16(4/5/6), 587-594. Heuer, S., & Hallowell, B. (2013). A new eye-tracking method to assess attention allocation in individuals with and without aphasia using a dual-task paradigm. Manuscript submitted for publication. Hula, W. D., & McNeil, M. R. (2008). Models of attention and dual-task performance as explanatory constructs in aphasia. Seminars in Speech and Language, 29(3), 169-187. LaPointe, L. L., & Erickson, R. J. (1991). Auditory vigilance during divided task attention in aphasic individuals. Aphasiology, 5(6), 511-520. Manor, B., & Gordon, E. (2003). Defining the temporal threshold for ocular fixation in freeviewing visuocognitive tasks. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 128, 85-93. McNeil, M. R., Doyle, P. J., Hula, W. D., Rubinsky, H. J., Fossett, T. R. D., & Matthews, C. T. (2004). Using resource allocation theory and dual-task methods to increase the sensitivity of assessment in aphasia. Aphasiology, 18(5,6,7), 521-542. McNeil, M. R., Matthews, C., Hula, W. D., Doyle, P. J., Rubinsky, H. J., & Fossett, T. R. D. (2005). A dual-task tool for quantifying normal comprehension of aphasic connected speech production: A constructive replication. Aphasiology,19(3, 4, 5), 473-484.

Murray, L. L., Holland, A. L., & Beeson, P. M. (1997). Auditory processing in individuals with mild aphasia: A study of resource allocation. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 40(4), 792-808. Ridgeway, V., Robertson, I. H., Ward, T., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1994). Test of everyday attention. Bury St. Edmunds, England: Thames Valley Test Company. Robin, D. A., & Rizzo, M. (1988). The effect of focal cerebral lesions on intramodal and crossmodal orienting of attention. Clinical Aphasiology, 18, 61-74. Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations for Single, Medium and Complex Stimuli during the Dual-task Condition of the Visual Search Task. Stimulus Complexity Mean Standard Deviation N Simple.70.12 39 Medium.61.13 39 Complex.57.13 39 Table 2 Comparisons of Simple, Medium, and Complex Stimulus Conditions in the Dual-task Condition During the Visual Search Task Pairs t df p simple medium 7.15 38 <.001 simple - complex 11.57 38 <.001 Medium complex 3.41 38.002 Table 3 Mean, Standard Deviation and Number of Participants for the TEA Raw Scores of Single and Dual-task Processing. TEA M SD Minimum Maximum N Single 3.28.79 2.00 5.30 37 Dual 3.85 1.38 1.80 7.80 36

Table 4 Pearson Correlation Coefficients for the Raw TEA Data, the Dual-task decrement (DTD) and the Scaled Score Equivalent of the DTD (SSE) of Single and Dual-task and PFDT of the Visual Search Single and Dual Task. PFDT TEA Single N Dual N DTD N SSE N VS r p r p r p r p Single.72** <.001 37.65** <.001 36 -.37*.03 35.21.23 35 Dual.63** <.001 37.65** <.001 36 -.46**.006 35.29.10 35 Diff. score -.20.24 37 -.29.09 36 -.30.08 35.22.21 35 Note VS = Visual search, DTD = Dual-task decrement, SSE = Scaled Score Equivalent, Figure 1. The display is an example of a simple visual search. Participants were instructed to find the different image. The green square is by the black circle. 0 4,000 11,000 Time in ms Figure 2. During the dual task, the visual search display on the left and a verbal stimulus are presented simultaneously, followed by the comprehension display on the right side.