Accounts for the N170 face-effect: a reply to Rossion, Curran, & Gauthier

Similar documents
A defense of the subordinate-level expertise account for the N170 component

Domain specificity versus expertise: factors influencing distinct processing of faces

ERP evidence for task modulations on face perceptual processing at different spatial scales

Perception of Faces and Bodies

Seeing faces in the noise: Stochastic activity in perceptual regions of the brain may influence the perception of ambiguous stimuli

FFA: a flexible fusiform area for subordinate-level visual processing automatized by expertise

The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences

Selective Attention Supports Working Memory Maintenance by Modulating Perceptual Processing of Distractors

Translating From Perceptual to Cognitive Coding

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH. DEGREE (if applicable)

Early lateralization and orientation tuning for face, word, and object processing in the visual cortex

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age

A picture is worth thousands of trials: rendering the use of visual information from spiking neurons to recognition

Linking Brain Response and Behavior to Reveal Top-Down. (and Inside-Out) Influences on Processing in Face Perception. Heather A. Wild. Thomas A.

The Training and Transfer of Real-World Perceptual Expertise

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes

Word Count: Please address correspondence to: James Tanaka, Department of Psychology, University

Revisiting the Role of the Fusiform Face Area in Visual Expertise

Lisa S. Scott 1* James W. Tanaka 2. David L. Sheinberg 3. Tim Curran 1*

(for Trends in Cognitive Science) Cindy Bukach 1,3. Isabel Gauthier 1,3. Michael J. Tarr 2,3. Research Center, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN

Brain Activation during Face Perception: Evidence of a Developmental Change

Beyond faces and modularity: the power of an expertise framework

ASSAF HAREL, PHD CURRICULUM VITAE Department of Psychology Wright State University

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni

Face Perception - An Overview Bozana Meinhardt-Injac Malte Persike

Attentional and Perceptual Factors Affecting the Attentional Blink for Faces and Objects

Dissociation of Category-Learning Mechanisms via Brain Potentials

Effects of Long-Term Object Familiarity on Event-Related Potentials in the Monkey

Two eyes make a pair: facial organization and perceptual learning reduce visual extinction

Using spatial frequency scales for processing face features and face configuration: An ERP analysis

Summary. Multiple Body Representations 11/6/2016. Visual Processing of Bodies. The Body is:

Bodies capture attention when nothing is expected

Are All Types of Expertise Created Equal? Car Experts Use Different Spatial Frequency Scales for Subordinate Categorization of Cars and Faces

Are Greebles like faces? Using the neuropsychological exception to test the rule

The Other-Race Effect and its Influences on the Development of Emotion Processing

The Time Scale of Perceptual Expertise

Rapid Attentional Selection of Non-native Stimuli despite Perceptual Narrowing

RUNNNING HEAD: Pigeons Attend to Features of Human Faces. Pigeons and People Categorize Human Faces Using Similar Features

Normal Greeble Learning in a Severe Case of Developmental Prosopagnosia

Context influences holistic processing of nonface objects in the composite task

Cognition xx (0000) xxx xxx. Brief article. A holistic account of the own-race effect in face recognition: evidence from a cross-cultural study

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln

Commentary on Moran and Desimone's 'spotlight in V4

Acta Psychologica 141 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica

Self-face recognition in attended and unattended conditions: an event-related brain potential study

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception

Gender difference in N170 elicited under oddball task

Cogniţie, Creier, Comportament / Cognition, Brain, Behavior Vol. IX(3), , 2005 Romanian Association of Cognitive Sciences

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition

Vision Research 51 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

The face behind the mask: a developmental study

Face Processing: The N170 ERP Component in Autism

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

The influence of feature conjunction on object inversion and conversion effects

Processing Faces and Facial Expressions

Neurophysiological correlates of memory illusion in both encoding and retrieval phases

Version of record first published: 16 Oct 2012.


Working Memory Load for Faces Modulates P300, N170, and N250r

Attention modulates the processing of emotional expression triggered by foveal faces

Unraveling Mechanisms for Expert Object Recognition: Bridging Brain Activity and Behavior

Inversion and contrast-reversal effects on face processing assessed by MEG

Shlomo Bentin 1,3, Joseph M. DeGutis 2, Mark D Esposito 2, and Lynn C. Robertson 2,3. Abstract INTRODUCTION

Top down processing of faces in human brain: A Behavioral Study

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report

A Drift Diffusion Model of Proactive and Reactive Control in a Context-Dependent Two-Alternative Forced Choice Task

Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

Mutual information of image fragments predicts categorization in humans: Electrophysiological and behavioral evidence q

In what way does the parietal ERP old new effect index recollection?

Eye Movements in Face Recognition 255

ANALYZING EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS

Activation of Preexisting and Acquired Face Representations: The N250 Event-related Potential as an Index of Face Familiarity

Beliefs alter holistic face processing I if response bias is not taken into account

For Non-Commercial Academic Use Only

The neural plasticity of other-race face recognition. James W. Tanaka & Lara J. Pierce. University of Victoria. Victoria, British Columbia CANADA

Are face-responsive regions selective only for faces?

Neural substrates of species-dependent visual processing of faces: use of morphed faces

2. Which of the following is not an element of McGuire s chain of cognitive responses model? a. Attention b. Comprehension c. Yielding d.

Human face preference in Gamma-Frequency EEG activity

SHORT REPORT The Development of Perceptual Expertise for Faces and Objects in Autism Spectrum Conditions

Grace Iarocci Ph.D., R. Psych., Associate Professor of Psychology Simon Fraser University

The measure of an illusion or the illusion of a measure?

RESULTS Human vs Macaques

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention

Gaze Bias Learning II. Linking neuroscience, computational modeling, and cognitive development. Tokyo, Japan March 12, 2012

Running head: FACE PROCESSING DEFICIT IN SCHIZOPHRENIA

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms

The face-sensitive N170 encodes social category information Jonathan B. Freeman a, Nalini Ambady a and Phillip J. Holcomb a

ERP Measures of Multiple Attention Deficits Following Prefrontal Damage

A Biologically Plausible Approach to Cat and Dog Discrimination

PS3019 Cognitive and Clinical Neuropsychology

Neural correlates of non-familiar face processing

Human Cognitive Developmental Neuroscience. Jan 27

Online Publication Date: 15 th July 2012 Publisher: Asian Economic and Social Society

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

The face-sensitive N170 encodes social category information Jonathan B. Freeman, Nalini Ambady and Phillip J. Holcomb

Transcription:

S. Bentin, D. Carmel / Cognition 85 (2002) 197 202 197 COGNITION Cognition 85 (2002) 197 202 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Discussion Accounts for the N170 face-effect: a reply to Rossion, Curran, & Gauthier Shlomo Bentin a,b,c, *, David Carmel a a Department of Psychology, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem 91905, Israel b Center of Neural Computation, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem 91905, Israel c Institute des Sciences Cognitives, CNRS, 60675 Bron, France Abstract In their commentary, Rossion, Curran, and Gauthier (Rossion, B., Curran, T., Gauthier, I. (2002). A defense of the subordinate-level expertise account for the N170 component. Cognition, 85, 197 202) (RC&G) raise a series of arguments against the domain-specificity account for the N170 faceeffect (Carmel, D., & Bentin, S. (2002). Domain specificity versus expertise: factors influencing distinct processing of faces. Cognition, 83, 1 29). This effect consists of a large difference (always significant) observed in the amplitude of a negative component peaking at the lower posteriortemporal sites in response to human faces relative to many other stimulus categories. As an alternative to domain specificity, RC&G advocate a subordinate-level expertise account, by which the N170 effect can be obtained for any type of stimulus for the individual identification of which the perceiver is an expert. While considering some of their arguments well taken and interesting, we believe that, overall, RC&G s interpretation of our current data (as well as some of theirs) and of our position ignores several important aspects and, therefore, their critique is not persuasive. q 2002 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Domain-specificity account; N170 face-effect; Rossion, Curran, and Gauthier Rossion, Curran, and Gauthier (this issue) (RC&G) reject the N170 effect as evidence for domain specificity in face processing, by making the following arguments: 1. Negative potentials at similar latencies are elicited by non-face categories, and the amplitudes of these potentials vary considerably. According to RC&G, this pattern suggests that the N170 might relate to general categorization processes (e.g. Tanaka, 2001). 2. N170 increases in amplitude when the task requires categorization at the subordinate, compared with a basic level (Tanaka, Loo, Weisbord, & Keifer, 1999). * Corresponding author. Institute des Sciences Cognitives, CNRS, 67, Boulevard Pinel, 69675 Bron, Cedex, France. Tel.: 133-4-37-91-12-57; fax: 133-4-37-91-12-10. E-mail address: bentin@isc.cnrs.fr (S. Bentin). 0010-0277/02/$ - see front matter q 2002 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. PII: S0010-0277(02)00102-6

198 S. Bentin, D. Carmel / Cognition 85 (2002) 197 202 3. Comparing the brain activity elicited by faces with that elicited by other objects is moot because faces are automatically categorized at the individual (within category) level whereas other objects are not. 4. Given the complexity and diversity of extrastriate brain regions, it is very likely that the N170 is modulated by a number of brain sources, each involved in a different type of perceptual process. We agree completely with the last point (cf. Sagiv & Bentin, 2001). Indeed, far-field potentials, recorded at the scalp, are almost always modulated by more than one brain source. This is why the consistent N170 discrimination of visual stimuli carrying physiognomic information from any other perceptual category is so striking. It is the researcher s role to use experimental designs and post-hoc analytical methods in order to disentangle the influence of the different sources. This strategy contrasts the view that we should lump together all the effects that may modulate a particular event-related potential (ERP), a view advocated by RC&G. In the present case, the negative component showing the N170 effect, in addition to its association with face-selective visual mechanisms, is most likely modulated also by non-selective visual activity that is always reflected by N1. Consequently, it is not surprising to find that any visual stimulus elicits a negative component at about this latency. Furthermore, being modulated by physical characteristics of the stimulus, the N1 elicited by different stimulus categories may vary in amplitude considerably. Fortunately, however, as we demonstrated in our recent paper (Carmel & Bentin, 2002), the influence of these two sources can be easily disentangled. The N170 effect is biggest at the mastoids, and significantly reduced (sometimes even completely absent) at O1 and O2, sites at which the N1 is maximal. Indeed at the posterior lateral sites the difference between faces and any other stimulus categories (tested under similar experimental conditions) exceeds by far differences between other categories that might be sporadically observed. Failing to make this distinction led RC&G to make arguments 1 and 2. Support for our position can be easily found considering the data of Tanaka et al. (1999). As evident in all the figures and in the BESA analysis, the potential that has been modulated by the type of categorization was limited to the left hemisphere, peaking at posterior parietal sites, far from the sites at which the N170 effect is observed. Ignoring this topographical distinction, RC&G erroneously associated the interesting brain activity reported by Tanaka et al. (1999) with the N170. Regarding the third argument above, we believe that RC&G ignored our position regarding the function of the mechanism to which the N170 is putatively associated. The present evidence strongly suggests that the N170 is sensitive to isolated face parts and it is not sensitive to face familiarity (e.g. Bentin & Deouell, 2000). Hence, we proposed that it is associated with a structural encoding mechanism, which probably precedes the within-category identification processes (Bentin, Allison, Puce, Perez, & McCarthy, 1996; see also Carmel & Bentin, 2002). Our current view is that this component is, in fact, associated with an extrastriate face-detection mechanism that triggers the structural encoding process in the fusiform gyrus (see Sagiv & Bentin, 2001). If our view is correct, the fact that the within-category identification process might be automatic for faces is irrelevant to our present discussion. RC&G criticize our suggestion that domain specificity may account for the absence of

S. Bentin, D. Carmel / Cognition 85 (2002) 197 202 199 task effects on the N170 elicited by faces (while such effects were found on the ERPs elicited by cars at the same latency), by making the following arguments: 1. Faces attract attention automatically; therefore, finding little task-induced modulation of the N170 elicited by faces is not relevant to domain specificity. Attentional effects due to task instructions should be interpreted in the context of attentional differences due to the perceiver s acquired processing biases for a category. 2. In passive (oddball) tasks, faces and objects of expertise should automatically recruit additional processes as compared with categories for which the perceiver is not expert. The possibility that faces attract attention automatically is intriguing, but so far the evidence is not unequivocal. In fact, the major evidence for this claim is the fact that unilateral extinction is reduced when faces are presented in the contralesional field of right-hemisphere damaged patients (Vuilleumier, 2000). In normal participants, however, selective attention did not influence face processing (Cauquil, Edmonds, & Taylor, 2000). Note that in the Eimer (2000a) study cited by RC&G as evidence for attention effects, the task required memorizing and retrieving attended items (detecting repetitions, or matching exemplars to memorized items). This confound could explain the differences found between conditions for faces, as well as the finding that this modulation was mostly evident in components later than the N170 (see Eimer, 2000a, Fig. 3); the N170 was almost identical for attended and unattended faces, with a difference of about 1 mv between them. Moreover, Eimer (2000b) reports that when attention was directed away from faces, the N170 was not modulated by the task. Parenthetically, RC&G seem to ignore that in the study that they cite, as well as in several others, Eimer favors a domainspecificity interpretation of the N170 (Eimer, 2000a d). Finally, we believe that the whole argument regarding attention stems from a misinterpretation of our position. We have never claimed that face processing is totally immune to attention manipulations, and did not use such an argument to discard the expertise view. We simply demonstrated a clear difference between the significant and explainable task effects on the processing of other items (cars) and the absence of such effects for faces, suggesting that this difference supports domain specificity in face processing. Regarding the possibility that faces would recruit different processes than items for which the perceiver is not expert, it seems to us that this is simply an enunciation of the expertise view which is the focus of the present argument and, as detailed below, still needs to be proved. In Experiment 2 (Carmel & Bentin, 2002), we compared the N170 elicited by human faces (for which humans are experts at the within-category identification level) with that elicited by faces of apes (which share the general configuration of the human face but for which our subjects were not experts at the within-category identification level). RC&G criticize our exploration of the putative expertise effects raising the following arguments: 1. We did not actually manipulate expertise because the stimulus type effect was confounded with task relevance. In Experiment 2 the participants were instructed to press a button each time they saw a human face. Hence, despite appearing at equal relative frequencies, human and ape faces were not task-equivalent: furthermore we had no objective (behavioral) evidence for the ability of our participants to individually

200 S. Bentin, D. Carmel / Cognition 85 (2002) 197 202 identify human and ape faces or to the extent at which these stimuli were processed in a holistic manner. 2. Behavioral experiments comparing human and monkey/ape faces do support the expertise hypothesis. For instance, the face inversion effect in animals and humans is found only for faces for which subjects have developed an expertise. Furthermore, in a recent study, face inversion effects on N170 were found for human but not for monkey faces (de Haan, Pascalis, & Johnson, 2002). 3. Expertise modulated the N170 elicited by dogs and birds in a previous experiment (Tanaka & Curran, 2001). This evidence, in concert with fmri (e.g. Gauthier, Skudlarski, Gore, & Anderson, 2000) and behavioral data that showed an effect of expertise (e.g. Gauthier & Tarr, 1997), outweighs our single null result. First, we should specify that showing similarly conspicuous N170 effects (with an identical scalp distribution) for human and ape faces was not a null result. We did not fail to find a predicted difference. Rather, we ran a crucial experiment testing predictions based on two theoretical frameworks, domain specificity versus expertise, and the data supported the former. Further, we should note that the modulation of N170 by expertise to dogs and birds in the Tanaka and Curran (2001) study was considerably smaller (albeit significant) than the modulation usually found for faces. In addition, as far as we can tell from the figures, these small effects were also more dorso-medial than the standard N170 effect. Hence, while very interesting, at the very least these effects need to be replicated, and their relation to the face-n170 further elucidated, before becoming a base for strong theoretical conclusions. Regarding both the fmri and the behavioral evidence, we suggest that these data tapped a different level of processing than that tapped by the N170 effect, a level at which, for example, holistic or component-based processing strategy is relevant. To this end, we should only add that, as the type of processing was not relevant to our study, this issue was completely ignored in our paper. 1 Did we, indeed, manipulate expertise? We agree with RC&G that it would have been nice to have an independent measure of expertise for ape faces. Yet, as the participants in this study never worked in a monkey lab or had any unusual experience with apes, it stands to reason that they were not experts in individual recognition of ape faces (or, at the very least, they were significantly less expert than in identifying human faces). As for the fact that human faces were given a target status in this experiment, note that if this factor affected face processing, it should have augmented the N170 relative to that elicited by ape faces (or the non-target faces of Experiment 1), which is the opposite of what was found. The face inversion effect found for human but not for monkey faces led de Haan et al. (2002) as well as RC&G to conclude that experience drives the specialization of the N170 and that human expertise with human faces does not generalize to faces of apes. However, it is interesting to note that in a different study (Pascalis, Demont, de Haan, & Campbell, 2001) 1 It is interesting to note, however, that data concerning holistic processing for items of expertise are far from convincing (at least as far as disproportionate inversion effects or part-whole paradigms are evidence). For example, Gauthier et al. (2000) found such evidence for car experts but not for bird experts. By contrast, Tanaka and Gauthier (1997) found such evidence for dog experts but not for car experts. Note also that even thousands of trials were not sufficient to develop holistic processing for inverted faces (McKone, Martini, & Nakayama, 2001).

S. Bentin, D. Carmel / Cognition 85 (2002) 197 202 201 the same authors report that human infants at various ages showed an inversion effect for faces of both humans and apes, but not for sheep faces, and concluded that the inversion effect generalizes to primates. Of course, opinions may change in light of new data, but note that the former study is in agreement with a study in chimpanzees (Parr, Dove, & Hopkins, 1998). In that study (cited by RC&G as evidence supporting the expertise view) inversion effects in chimpanzees were found only for human and chimpanzee faces, but not for capucin monkey faces. Whereas the extension of chimpanzees response specificity to human faces could have been, in principle, explained by their experience with human faces (being raised in captivity), for the very same reason their experience with the chimpanzee faces might be doubtful (we have no information about how these chimps were raised). At the very least, these data could be equally explained by both the domain specificity and the expertise views, hence providing no strong support to either. Furthermore, on the basis of this discussion RC&G seem to be in complete agreement with us that without special training humans are not particularly expert for individual identification of apes. If so, how does the expertise hypothesis account for the conspicuous N170 effect elicited by apes in comparison to faces of other animals (cf. Bentin et al., 1996)? Note that, in contrast, the absence of a face inversion effect on the N170 elicited by ape faces in the de Haan et al. (2002) study can be easily accounted for by our model of the N170 effect. Recall that face inversion affects face identification, impeding global processing. This manipulation does not, however, affect component-oriented detection of physiognomic features. Ape faces are similar to human faces in general configuration, but not at the level of individual features. If indeed the N170 effect is associated with the component-based detection of physiognomic information, impeding global face processing by inversion may enhance the amplitude of the N170 for human but not for ape faces. Note that a similar effect was found for schematic faces and accounted for in detail by Sagiv and Bentin (2001). In conclusion, it seems to us that the present critique published by RC&G is not persuasive. Whereas we accept the possibility that extensive expertise with a particular set of stimuli might change the strategy of processing those stimuli, the present evidence does not imply that this change starts at the level of processing at which the N170 effect is modulated. Whether or not the special efficiency that normal people show for face processing is innate is not clear. While reviewing present evidence on this issue, Carmel and Bentin (2002) took no stand. It is clear, however, that, while faces are not more frequently seen than many other stimuli, they are the only visual category to develop an expertise naturally, without explicit training. To this end, we believe that their processing is domain-specific. Acknowledgements We acknowledge the financial support of grant #98-00057 awarded by the US-Israel Binational Foundation to Shlomo Bentin. References Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, A., & McCarthy, G. (1996). Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 551 565.

202 S. Bentin, D. Carmel / Cognition 85 (2002) 197 202 Bentin, S., & Deouell, Y. (2000). Structural encoding and identification in face-processing: ERP evidence for separate mechanisms. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 35 54. Carmel, D., & Bentin, S. (2002). Domain specificity versus expertise: factors influencing distinct processing of faces. Cognition, 83, 1 29. Cauquil, A. S., Edmonds, G. E., & Taylor, M. J. (2000). Is the face-sensitive N170 the only ERP not affected by selective attention? NeuroReport, 11, 2167 2171. de Haan, M., Pascalis, O., & Johnson, M. J. (2002). Specialization of neural mechanisms underlying face recognition in human infants. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 99 209. Eimer, M. (2000). Attentional modulations of event-related brain potentials sensitive to faces. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 103 116. Eimer, M. (2000). Effects of face inversion on the structural encoding and recognition of faces. Evidence from event-related potentials. Cognitive Brain Research, 10, 145 158. Eimer, M. (2000). The face-specific N170 component reflects late stages in the structural encoding of faces. NeuroReport, 11, 2319 2324. Eimer, M. (2000). Event-related brain potentials distinguish processing stages involved in face perception and recognition. Clinical Neurophysiology, 111, 694 705. Gauthier, I., Skudlarski, P., Gore, J. C., & Anderson, A. W. (2000). Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 191 197. Gauthier, I., & Tarr, M. J. (1997). Becoming a Greeble expert: exploring the face recognition mechanism. Vision Research, 37, 1673 1682. McKone, E., Martini, P., & Nakayama, K. (2001). Categorical perception of face identity in noise isolates configural processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 573 599. Parr, L. A., Dove, T., & Hopkins, W. D. (1998). Why faces may be special: evidence of the inversion effect in chimpanzees. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 615 622. Pascalis, O., Demont, E., de Haan, M., & Campbell, R. (2001). Recognition of faces of different species: a developmental study between 5 and 8 years of age. Infant and Child Development, 10, 39 45. Rossion, B., Curran, T., & Gauthier, I. (this issue). A defense of the subordinate level-expertise account for the N170 component. Cognition. Sagiv, N., & Bentin, S. (2001). Structural encoding of human and schematic faces: holistic and part-based processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 937 951. Tanaka, J. W. (2001). The entry point of face recognition: evidence for face expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 534 543. Tanaka, J. W., & Curran, T. (2001). A neural basis for expert object recognition. Psychological Science, 12,43 47. Tanaka, J. W., & Gauthier, I. (1997). Expertise in object and face recognition. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 36, 83 125. Tanaka, J. W., Luu, P., Weisbrod, M., & Kiefer, M. (1999). Tracking the time course of object categorization using event-related potentials. NeuroReport, 10, 829 835. Vuilleumier, P. (2000). Faces call for attention: evidence from patients with visual extinction. Neuropsychologia, 38, 693 700.