Introduction to the special issue

Similar documents
Current Computational Models in Cognition Supplementary table for: Lewandowsky, S., & Farrell, S. (2011). Computational

MODELS FOR RECALL AND RECOGNITION

Utility Maximization and Bounds on Human Information Processing

An Analysis of the Working Memory Capacity Paradox

UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society

Theoretical Note. Automatic and Controlled Processing Revisited

Cognitive Modeling. Lecture 9: Intro to Probabilistic Modeling: Rational Analysis. Sharon Goldwater

Cognitive Modeling. Mechanistic Modeling. Mechanistic Modeling. Mechanistic Modeling Rational Analysis

episodic memory#recognition#recall#hippocampus#neural network models

C U R R I C U L U M V I TA E

Working Memory and Mental Health: A Primer for the Mental Health Professional

Negative Effects of Using List Items as Recall Cues 1

PS3021, PS3022, PS4040

On Trust. Massimo Felici. Massimo Felici On Trust c

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

HOW TO IDENTIFY A RESEARCH QUESTION? How to Extract a Question from a Topic that Interests You?

Retrieval of Autobiographical Information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

Choose an approach for your research problem

A Retrieval Theory of Priming in Memory

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

Strong memories obscure weak memories in associative recognition

Categorization, Recognition, and Unsupervised Learning

Theory, Models, Variables

Attention, Automaticity, and Priority Learning

Moral Personality Exemplified. Lawrence J. Walker. University of British Columbia

CURRICULUM VITAE PUBLICATIONS

Memory: Computation, Genetics, Physiology, and Behavior. James L. McClelland Stanford University

Exam #4 Study Guide. Chapter 7 Memory

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 5. CLINICAL APPROACH TO INTERVIEWING PART 1

Memory 2/15/2017. The Three Systems Model of Memory. Process by which one encodes, stores, and retrieves information

Dr. Braj Bhushan, Dept. of HSS, IIT Guwahati, INDIA

Comparing Exemplar- and Rule- Based Theories of Categorization Jeffrey N. Rouder 1 and Roger Ratcliff 2

DEVELOPING THE RESEARCH FRAMEWORK Dr. Noly M. Mascariñas

Foundations for a Science of Social Inclusion Systems

Cognitive Psychology. Robert J. Sternberg EDITION. Yak University THOIVISOISI * WADSWORTH

Journal of Memory and Language

Chapter 1 What is Psychology?

Whose psychological concepts?

Sperling conducted experiments on An experiment was conducted by Sperling in the field of visual sensory memory.

Bill Wilson. Categorizing Cognition: Toward Conceptual Coherence in the Foundations of Psychology

Psycholinguistics Psychological Mechanisms

The spacing and lag effect in free recall

Single Parameter Model for Free Recall And the Nature of Partially Filled Working Memory. Abstract:

In press: Psychology of Learning and Motivation

Increasing the amount of information that can be held in short-term memory by grouping related items together into a single unit, or chunk.

Dear Participants in the Brunswik Society

Psychology (PSYC) Psychology (PSYC) 1

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

Daniel W. Shuman: A Gift for Collaboration

APPENDIX X POLICY FOR INTEGRITY AND THE RESPONSIBLE CONDUCT OF SCHOLARSHIP AND RESEARCH: GUIDELINES TO ENCOURAGE RESPONSIBLE RESEARCH PRACTICES

CHAPTER 6: Memory model Practice questions at - text book pages 112 to 113

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources

WILLIAM S. YACULLO, Ph.D., CCC-A

Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness

Developmental Evidence for Working Memory as Activated Long-Term Memory

Involuntary and Voluntary Memory Sequencing Phenomena

COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY BY ROBERT J. STERNBERG, KARIN STERNBERG DOWNLOAD EBOOK : COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY BY ROBERT J. STERNBERG, KARIN STERNBERG PDF

Department of Psychology

WSC 2018 SCIENCE. Science of Memory

The Role of Biomedical Knowledge in Clinical Reasoning: Bridging the Gap between Two Theories

Psychology Departmental Mission Statement: Communicating Plus - Psychology: Requirements for a major in psychology:

Cognition, Learning and Social Change Conference Summary A structured summary of the proceedings of the first conference

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in

AN ACTIVATION-BASED THEORY OF IMMEDIATE ITEM MEMORY

DARTMOUTH COLLEGE Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences

Categorisation, Bounded Rationality and Bayesian Inference

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition

Learning to Use Episodic Memory

High-capacity spatial contextual memory

Protocol analysis and Verbal Reports on Thinking

CHAPTER OUTLINE CHAPTER SUMMARY

General Knowledge/Semantic Memory: Chapter 8 1

PSYCHOLOGY (413) Chairperson: Sharon Claffey, Ph.D.

Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces

2. Strategic Knowledge, Chunks, and Cognitive Segments

Mechanisms of long-term repetition priming and skill refinement: A probabilistic pathway model

Alvin Goldman (ed), Readings in Philosophy and Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, 1993.

9/21/2017. Life Without Memory (Clive Wearing) Arlo Clark-Foos, Ph.D.

Arlo Clark-Foos, Ph.D. 2 October

An Empirical Study on Causal Relationships between Perceived Enjoyment and Perceived Ease of Use

Sensation is the conscious experience associated with an environmental stimulus. It is the acquisition of raw information by the body s sense organs

Foreword to Fred Terman at Stanford. Father of Silicon Valley, the words seem to leap from the page.

Poster Presentation at the Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Association. Toronto, August Running Head: CROSS-CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE

24. PSYCHOLOGY (Code No. 037)

How People Estimate Effect Sizes: The Role of Means and Standard Deviations

DARTMOUTH COLLEGE Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences

OCTOBER 4, 2014 COMPREHENSIVE PAIN ASSESSMENT FOR CLINICIANS. Participate in person, or on-line. Gain a deeper understanding of pain intensity scales

Fame: I m Skeptical. Fernanda Ferreira. Department of Psychology. University of California, Davis.

Kauyumari Sanchez Curriculum Vitae

TOD AM and the List-Strength and List-Length Effects: Comment on Murdock and Kahana (1993a)

Irrationality in Game Theory

AP Psychology Scoring Components Page(s)

A Look at National Awards Robin Autenrieth Co-PI, ADVANCE Program Interim Department Head, Civil Engineering

RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY AND SURVEY RESEARCH ~

DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY

Chapter 4: Representation in Memory

Transcription:

Cognitive Science 27 (2003) 335 339 Introduction to the special issue This special issue honors the research and mentorship excellence of Dr. Richard Shiffrin, the 2002 David E. Rumelhart Prize recipient. This prize was instituted in 2001, funded by the Robert J. Glushko and Pamela Samuelson Foundation. The first prize recipient was Geoffrey Hinton. The prize is awarded annually to an individual or collaborative team making a significant contemporary contribution to the formal analysis of human cognition. Mathematical modeling of human cognitive processes, formal analysis of language and other products of human cognitive activity, and computational analyses of human cognition using symbolic or nonsymbolic frameworks all fall within the scope of the award. In 2002, the prize selection committee consisted of Alan Collins, Mark Liberman, Anthony Marley, and James McClelland (chair). This issue of Cognitive Science is the first of what will hopefully become a tradition of special issues related to Rumelhart Prize winners. We have already begun planning a special issue on computational linguistics in honor of the third Rumelhart Prize winner, Dr. Aravind Joshi, at the University of Pennsylvania. In some cases, the special issues will be based upon a single topic central to the research of the prize winner. In other cases, the issues will be comprised of research articles by colleagues, collaborators, or past students of the prize winner. This special issue pays tribute to the diversity and quality of research that Richard Shiffrin s students have gone on to conduct after leaving Dr. Shiffrin s laboratory, and so it is fitting that this issue for Dr. Shiffrin includes research by his previous students and collaborators. Dr. Shiffrin has been the mentor to a surprisingly large number of individuals who have developed noteworthy and fertile research programs. The research topics spanned by the seven articles of this issue include cognitive neuroscience, causal reasoning, human computer interaction, memory, semantic representation, visual perception, priming, attention, and automaticity. Despite the eclectic set of covered topics, shared themes and methods underlie the articles. The researchers strive to develop well-constrained formal models of cognition. The sources of these constraints include evolutionary (Geisler), optimality (Geisler, Huber, Shiffrin, Steyvers, Raaijmakers), neurophysiological (Huber, Schneider), pragmatic (Dumais), and computational (Dumais, Steyvers) considerations. Rather than restricting or holding back the development of theory, these constraints enable theorizing by directing cognitive scientists to positive formalisms and methods. A second theme in common to several of the manuscripts is the fertility of applying rational and Bayesian principles to human cognition. The apparent wisdom of this approach appears on the surface to be cast in doubt by the large number of empirical 0364-0213/03/$ see front matter 2003 Cognitive Science Society, Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/s0364-0213(03)00008-9

336 Introduction to the special issue / Cognitive Science 27 (2003) 335 339 demonstrations of suboptimal and irrational human decision making, including the research for which Daniel Kahneman was awarded a 2002 Nobel Prize in economics. However, Bayesian, rational analyses always begin with limitations and constraints (as mentioned above), significantly reducing the apparent discrepancy between the approaches. Furthermore, failures of rational decision making notwithstanding, enormous progress in many fields has been made with Bayesian modeling. This point is emphasized by the articles in this issue they demonstrate surprising depth to rational analyses of perception, memory, causal reasoning, and information access. Humans may not be built in the best way possible, but comparing our performance to formally devised gold standards of rationality has revealed both informative deviations from optimality and surprising cases where cognition apparently conforms to normative accounts. 1. A brief biography of Richard Shiffrin Dr. Shiffrin received his Ph.D. from the Mathematical Psychology Program in the Department of Psychology at Stanford University in 1968, the year after David Rumelhart received his degree from the same program. Since 1968, he has been on the faculty of the Department of Psychology at Indiana University, where he is now the Luther Dana Waterman Professor of Psychology and distinguished professor. He developed the cognitive science program at Indiana University in the late 1980s, and was its director from its inception in 1989 2002. Shiffrin has accumulated many honors, including memberships in the National Academy of Sciences and the American Academy of Arts and Sciences, the Howard Crosby Warren Award of the Society of Experimental Psychologists, and a MERIT Award from the National Institute of Mental Health. Shiffrin has served the field as editor of the Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, and as a member of the governing boards of several scientific societies. Shiffrin has made many contributions to the modeling of human cognition in areas ranging from perception to attention to learning, but is best known for his long-standing efforts to develop explicit models of human memory. His most recent models use Bayesian, adaptive approaches, building on previous work, but extending it in a critical new manner, and carrying his theory beyond explicit memory to implicit learning and memory processes. The theory has been evolving for about 35 years, and as a result represents a progression similar to the best theories seen in any branch of science. Shiffrin s major effort began in 1968, with research (Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968) that laid out a model of the components of short- and long-term memory and described the processes that control the operations of memory. The Atkinson Shiffrin model encapsulated empirical and theoretical results from a very large number of publications and modeled quantitatively the relation of short- to long-term memory. It achieved its greatest success by showing the critical importance and the possibility of modeling the control processes of cognition. This 1968 chapter remains one of the most cited works in the entire field of psychology. Shiffrin s formal theory was taken forward in a quantum leap with the Search of Associative Memory (SAM) model (Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, 1980, 1981). This was a joint effort with Jeroen Raaijmakers, then a graduate student. The SAM model quantified the nature of retrieval from

Introduction to the special issue / Cognitive Science 27 (2003) 335 339 337 long-term memory, and characterized recall as a memory search with cycles of sampling and recovery. The SAM theory precisely incorporates the notions of interactive cue combination that are now seen to lie at the heart of memory retrieval. Another major step occurred when the theory was extended to recognition memory (Gillund & Shiffrin, 1984). Shiffrin and his former student initiated what has become the standard approach to recognition memory, in which a decision is based on summed activation of related memory traces. It was a major accomplishment that the same retrieval activations that had been used in the recall model could be carried forward and used to predict a wide range of recognition phenomena. The next major step occurred in 1990, when Shiffrin published two articles on the list-length effect with his student Steve Clark and his colleague, Roger Ratcliff (Ratcliff, Clark, & Shiffrin, 1990; Shiffrin, Ratcliff, & Clark, 1990). This research was of critical importance in that it established clearly that experience leads to the differentiation, rather than the mere strengthening, of the representations of items in memory. In 1997, the theory evolved in a radical new direction in an important paper with another former student, Mark Steyvers (Shiffrin & Steyvers, 1997). Although the changes were fundamental, the new model retained the best concepts of its predecessors, so that the previous successful predictions were also a part of the new theory. The Retrieving Effectively from Memory (REM) model added featural representations, to capture similarity relations among items in memory. Building on earlier ideas by John Anderson, and related to ideas developed in parallel by James McClelland and Mark Chappell, Shiffrin used Bayesian principles of adaptive and optimal decision making under constraints to guide the selection of the quantitative form of the activation functions. In addition, storage principles were set forth that provided mechanisms by which episodic experience could coalesce over development and experience into permanent noncontextualized knowledge. This latter development allowed the modeling of implicit memory phenomena, in work that is just now starting to appear in journals, including a theory of long-term (Schooler, Shiffrin, & Raaijmakers, 2001) and short-term (Huber, Shiffrin, Lyle, & Ruys, 2001) priming. The short-term priming research showed that the direction of priming can be reversed by extra study given to particular primes, leading to another conceptual breakthrough. A Bayesian-inspired new model consistent with the REM framework explains this and many other findings by assuming that some prime features are confused with test item features, and that the system attempts to deal with this situation optimally by appropriate discounting of evidence from certain features. 2. An enduring tradition A selected set of references to Richard Shiffrin s research is included here, sampled from a much larger set of over 100 scholarly works. This corpus of research attests to his impact in the specific areas of memory and attention, and more generally to formal modeling in cognitive science. The present set of articles demonstrates that this tradition of effectively combining empirical research on human cognition with formal modeling is alive and well. It is a testament to Shiffrin s effectiveness as a mentor that his students have been able to apply the modeling tools that they learned to areas far removed from Shiffrin s own research. As one of his current graduate students observed, Rich creates an atmosphere that makes it easy

338 Introduction to the special issue / Cognitive Science 27 (2003) 335 339 for people to succeed. The current articles corroborate this claim. The success that Shiffrin has had in his scientific career shines all the brighter for having worn off on his students and colleagues. Selected research publications by Richard Shiffrin Watson, W. W., Howard, A. J., Miller, N. E., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1963). Isotopic thermal diffusion factors for helium and neon at low temperatures. Zeitschrift fur Naturforschung, 18a, 242 245. Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 89 195). New York: Academic Press. Shiffrin, R. M., & Atkinson, R. C. (1969). Storage and retrieval processes in long-term memory. Psychological Review, 79, 179 193. Shiffrin, R. M (1970a). Memory search. In D. A. Norman (Ed.), Models of memory (pp. 375 447). New York: Academic Press. Shiffrin, R. M. (1970b). Forgetting, trace erosion or retrieval failure? Science, 168, 1601 1603. Shiffrin, R. M., & Gardner, G. T. (1972). Visual processing capacity and attentional control. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 72 82. Shiffrin, R. M. (1976). Capacity limitations in information processing, attention and memory. In W. K. Estes (Ed.), Handbook of learning and cognitive processes: Vol. 4. Attention and memory (pp. 177 236). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing. I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1 66. Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing. II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127 190. Penner, M. J., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1980). Nonlinearities in the coding of intensity within the context of a temporal summation model. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 67, 617 628. Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1980). SAM: A theory of probabilistic search of associative memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 14, pp. 207 262). New York: Academic Press. Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1981). Search of associative memory. Psychological Review, 88, 93 134. Shiffrin, R. M., & Dumais, S. T. (1981). The development of automatism. In J. Anderson (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 111 140). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Gillund, G., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1984). A retrieval model for both recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 91, 1 67. Salasoo, A., Shiffrin, R. M., & Feustel, T. C. (1985). Building permanent memory codes: Codification and repetition effects in word identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 50 77. Shiffrin, R. M. (1988). Attention. In R. C. Atkinson, R. J. Herrnstein, G. Lindzey, & R. D. Luce (Eds.), Stevens handbook of experimental psychology (2nd ed., pp. 739 811). New York: Wiley. Shiffrin, R. M., & Thompson, M. (1988). Moments of transition-additive random variables defined on finite regenerative random processes. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 32, 313 340. Ratcliff, R., Clark, S., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1990). The list-strength effect. I. Data and discussion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 163 178. Shiffrin, R. M., Ratcliff, R., & Clark, S. (1990). The list-strength effect. II. Theoretical mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 16, 179 195. Shiffrin, R. M., Huber, D., & Marinelli, K. (1995). Effects of category length and strength on familiarity in recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(2), 267 287. Shiffrin, R. M., & Lightfoot, N. (1997). Perceptual learning of alphanumeric-like characters. In R. L. Goldstone, P.G. Schyns, & D. L. Medin (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 36, pp. 45 82). San Diego: Academic Press.

Introduction to the special issue / Cognitive Science 27 (2003) 335 339 339 Shiffrin, R. M., & Steyvers, M. (1997). A model for recognition memory: REM: Retrieving effectively from memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 4(2), 145 166. Diller, D. E., Nobel, P. A., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2001). An ARC-REM model for accuracy and response time in recognition and recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 414 435. Huber, D. E., Shiffrin, R. M., Lyle, K. B., & Ruys, K. I. (2001). Perception and preference in short-term word priming. Psychological Review, 108, 149 182. Nobel, P. A., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2001). Retrieval processes in recognition and cued recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 27, 384 413. Schooler, L., Shiffrin, R. M., & Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (2001). A model for implicit effects in perceptual identification. Psychological Review, 108, 257 272.