Perception of Faces and Bodies

Similar documents
FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni

The Relation Between Perception and Action: What Should Neuroscience Learn From Psychology?

Vision Research 51 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Accounts for the N170 face-effect: a reply to Rossion, Curran, & Gauthier

Summary. Multiple Body Representations 11/6/2016. Visual Processing of Bodies. The Body is:

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice

A defense of the subordinate-level expertise account for the N170 component

Neural codes PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 12. COC illusion

Face Perception - An Overview Bozana Meinhardt-Injac Malte Persike

(for Trends in Cognitive Science) Cindy Bukach 1,3. Isabel Gauthier 1,3. Michael J. Tarr 2,3. Research Center, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN

Cognition xx (0000) xxx xxx. Brief article. A holistic account of the own-race effect in face recognition: evidence from a cross-cultural study

Human Cognitive Developmental Neuroscience. Jan 27

FFA: a flexible fusiform area for subordinate-level visual processing automatized by expertise

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception

fmri: What Does It Measure?

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report

Identify these objects

Face Inversion Disproportionately Impairs the Perception of Vertical but not Horizontal Relations Between Features

EDGE DETECTION. Edge Detectors. ICS 280: Visual Perception

Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, 102 Gilmer Hall, P.O. Box. Department of Neurology, University of Lübeck, Lübeck, Germany

Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience fmri results in context. Doug Schultz

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age

Revisiting the Role of the Fusiform Face Area in Visual Expertise

The influence of feature conjunction on object inversion and conversion effects

Commentary to Dijkerman & de Haan: Somatosensory processes subserving perception

11, in nonhuman primates, different cell populations respond to facial identity and expression12, and

Impaired face discrimination in acquired prosopagnosia is associated with abnormal response to individual faces in the right middle fusiform gyrus

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

Are face-responsive regions selective only for faces?

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J.

The Brain. Its major systems, How we study them, How they make the mind

BECOMING A GREEBLE EXPERT: EXPLORING MECHANISMS FOR FACE RECOGNITION

Perceptual Disorders. Agnosias

Bodies capture attention when nothing is expected

It Doesn t Take A Lot of Brains to Understand the Brain: Functional Neuroanatomy Made Ridiculously Simple

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 2

Cross-syndrome, cross-domain. comparisons of development trajectories

Extrastriate Visual Areas February 27, 2003 A. Roe

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems

PSY 310: Sensory and Perceptual Processes 1

Beyond faces and modularity: the power of an expertise framework

Key questions about attention

Hierarchically Organized Mirroring Processes in Social Cognition: The Functional Neuroanatomy of Empathy

The face behind the mask: a developmental study

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

THE ENCODING OF PARTS AND WHOLES

UNDERSTANDING THE RECOGNITION OF FACIAL IDENTITY AND FACIAL EXPRESSION

PS3019 Cognitive and Clinical Neuropsychology

Dr. Mark Ashton Smith, Department of Psychology, Bilkent University

Processing of facial identity and expression: a psychophysical, physiological and computational perspective

Linking Brain Response and Behavior to Reveal Top-Down. (and Inside-Out) Influences on Processing in Face Perception. Heather A. Wild. Thomas A.

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception

Who Needs Cheeks? Eyes and Mouths are Enough for Emotion Identification. and. Evidence for a Face Superiority Effect. Nila K Leigh

Sensorimotor Functioning. Sensory and Motor Systems. Functional Anatomy of Brain- Behavioral Relationships

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Normal Greeble Learning in a Severe Case of Developmental Prosopagnosia

Action during body perception: Processing time affects. of self other correspondences.

Social Cognition and the Mirror Neuron System of the Brain

Virtual Brain Reading: A Connectionist Approach to Understanding fmri

Prof. Greg Francis 7/8/08

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544

Dissociation of Category-Learning Mechanisms via Brain Potentials

Seeing faces in the noise: Stochastic activity in perceptual regions of the brain may influence the perception of ambiguous stimuli

A picture is worth thousands of trials: rendering the use of visual information from spiking neurons to recognition

5.8 Departure from cognitivism: dynamical systems

A Model of Biological Motion Perception from Configural Form Cues

Reminders. What s a Neuron? Animals at Birth. How are Neurons formed? Prenatal Neural Development. Week 28. Week 3 Week 4. Week 10.

Developmental Social Cognition Cognitive Development

Disorders of Object and Spatial perception. Dr John Maasch Brain Injury Rehabilitation Service Burwood Hospital.

Unraveling Mechanisms for Expert Object Recognition: Bridging Brain Activity and Behavior

Taking control of reflexive social attention

Separate Face and Body Selectivity on the Fusiform Gyrus

Neuroscience Tutorial

Anatomical limitations in mental transformations of body parts

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

Implicit memory. Schendan, HE. University of Plymouth

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

THE EFFECT OF DIFFERENT TRAINING EXPERIENCES ON OBJECT RECOGNITION IN THE VISUAL SYSTEM. Alan Chun-Nang Wong

Brain Activation during Face Perception: Evidence of a Developmental Change

Young infants detect the direction of biological motion in point-light displays

Are Greebles like faces? Using the neuropsychological exception to test the rule

Chapter 5: Perceiving Objects and Scenes

Cognitive domain: Knowledge Answer location: Introduction: Knowledge from Cognitive Deficits Question type: MS Ans: C

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Review

Study of Imagery. Banned by behaviorists

Phil 490: Consciousness and the Self Handout [16] Jesse Prinz: Mental Pointing Phenomenal Knowledge Without Concepts

Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex. 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations. 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage

Category-specific interference of object recognition with biological motion perception

The Neuroscience of Vision II

The Concept of Simulation in Control-Theoretic Accounts of Motor Control and Action Perception

Last but not least. Perception, 2003, volume 32, pages 249 ^ 252

MSc Neuroimaging for Clinical & Cognitive Neuroscience

Neuroimaging of high-level visual functions. Visual awareness

Cortical Visual Symptoms

PERCEIVING AND ACTING ON THE SOCIAL WORLD

Visual Object Recognition Computational Models and Neurophysiological Mechanisms Neurobiology 130/230. Harvard College/GSAS 78454

Rajeev Raizada: Statement of research interests

Infants Differential Processing of Female and Male Faces Jennifer L. Ramsey-Rennels 1 and Judith H. Langlois 2

Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. Lecture 3: Disorders of Perception

Report. Face Adaptation without a Face

Transcription:

CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Perception of Faces and Bodies Similar or Different? Virginia Slaughter, 1 Valerie E. Stone, 2 and Catherine Reed 3 1 Early Cognitive Development Unit and 2 Cognitive Neuroscience, School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia, and 3 Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Program, Psychology Department, University of Denver ABSTRACT Human faces and bodies are both complex and interesting perceptual objects, and both convey important social information. Given these similarities between faces and bodies, we can ask how similar are the visual processing mechanisms used to recognize them. It has long been argued that faces are subject to dedicated and unique perceptual processes, but until recently, relatively little research has focused on how we perceive the human body. Some recent paradigms indicate that faces and bodies are processed differently; others show similarities in face and body perception. These similarities and differences depend on the type of perceptual task and the level of processing involved. Future research should take these issues into account. KEYWORDS object recognition; face perception; body schema; social information processing Probably the most important and complex objects we perceive are other humans. From the time we are born, other humans capture our attention and elicit complex behaviors from us. We can identify other humans as humans because they possess both a human face and a human body. Further, we identify specific individuals not only on the basis of their unique faces and body shapes, but also on the basis of their characteristic expressions, postures, and movements. Given these functional similarities between faces and bodies, how similar are the visual processing mechanisms used to recognize them? FACES AS SPECIAL OBJECTS It has long been argued that the visual system uses special perceptual processing for faces that is different from the processing used for other objects. The rationale is that faces are such significant social stimuli that natural selection acted to Address correspondence to Virginia Slaughter, School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, 4072 Australia; e-mail: vps@ psy.uq.edu.au. create dedicated face-processing mechanisms in the brain. There is evidence consistent with the idea that faces are special: From early in development, infants are biased to look at faces more than other complex objects (Johnson & Morton, 1991). Also, adult perception of faces reveals unique effects, including the inversion effect (upside-down faces are more difficult to recognize than other complex inverted stimuli) and the caricature effect (a face with its distinctive features exaggerated is easier to recognize than the original face). Event-related potential studies, which measure the timing of the brain s electrical responses to stimuli, show that the brain responds differently to faces than to other objects within 170 ms after they are presented. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) research, showing activity in the brain while participants are actively performing perceptual or cognitive tasks, further suggests that distinct brain areas respond to faces compared with other objects. However, not all researchers agree that these apparently facespecific phenomena genuinely reflect unique processing of faces. Some authors suggest that responses to faces are driven by the abstract perceptual features of faces, such as symmetry or high contrast, rather than their face-ness per se (Turati, Simion, Milani, & Umilta, 2002). Others argue that the apparently special processing faces receive simply reflects the ubiquity and importance of faces, and that the perceptual effects adults exhibit when viewing faces will be evident for any objects that they are highly practiced at perceiving (e.g., cars for car enthusiasts or dogs for dog breeders; Tanaka & Gauthier, 1997). These authors propose that visual expertise changes the way that objects are processed: Within a given domain, novices recognize objects by focusing on their distinctive parts, but experts rely on configural processing, focusing on the spatial relationships between parts. The argument is that configural processing in general can explain perceptual effects that appear to distinguish faces. BODIES ARE SPECIAL, TOO Another class of objects that may be subject to special processing is human bodies. Although there has been relatively little research on perception of the human body compared with Volume 13 Number 6 Copyright r 2004 American Psychological Society 219

Face and Body Perception that of faces, several similarities between faces and bodies suggest that they may be processed similarly. First, bodies and faces share a number of abstract configural properties that may make the perceptual system treat them similarly. All faces share the same set of parts (eyes, nose, mouth, etc.), as do all bodies (arms, legs, torso, etc.). As a result, for both faces and bodies, perceptual distinctions depend on the exact shape and position of component parts. Also, from the front, both faces and bodies are symmetrical along the vertical axis. Further, the spatial relationships between parts of faces and between parts of bodies are relatively fixed. Across individuals, the configural arrangement of the eyes, nose, and mouth of the face is relatively unchanging, as is the arrangement of the head, torso, and limbs. Second, faces and bodies are both salient conveyors of social information. Both provide information about other individuals attentional and emotional states, and inform basic social categorizations, including attributions of age, gender, and attractiveness. Faces and bodies are both used for communication. Finally, our embodied internal experience of both faces and bodies could distinguish them as special object classes. Our ability to move and functionally use our faces and bodies could influence the visual recognition of other faces and bodies. Recent neurophysiological studies with monkeys have revealed a class of mirror neurons, so called because the same neurons are active whether a given motor action is performed or observed. There is some indirect evidence that similar motor-mirroring structures exist in humans (see Gallese & Goldman, 1998, for a review), suggesting that visual and motor representations interact. The ability both to see faces and bodies and to move our own faces and bodies may make them similarly unique compared with other perceptual objects. Despite these arguments for special perceptual processing of bodies, there are also reasons to suppose that faces and bodies may be treated differently by the visual system. The nature of the information conveyed by faces and bodies is arguably different. Faces, although often moving, are perceptually informative even while still. We can make judgments about another person s gender, identity, emotion, attractiveness, and direction of attention (conveyed most saliently in eye gaze) from a still photograph of the face. Bodies, in contrast, are typically moving, and much of the information that bodies convey is in dynamic movement. We can identify another person s gender, emotion, and direction of attention from that person s body most easily if it is in motion. Thus, it remains an open question whether the visual system applies similar or different perceptual processes to faces and bodies. Insight into this question may be gained by considering two factors. First, the various ways that scientists measure face and body perception may place more or less difficult demands on the visual system. Second, patterns in the development of face perception and of body perception may give insight into how and when faces are treated similarly by the visual system. LEVELS OF PERCEPTUAL PROCESSING OF FACES AND BODIES Perception of faces or bodies is a multistage process. The extent to which faces and bodies are treated similarly by the perceptual system depends on the stage of processing, and thus the type of perceptual task participants are asked to perform. It may be useful to distinguish two stages of perceptual processing and two types of perceptual tasks: detection versus recognition. Face or body detection refers to the ability to determine whether a particular stimulus is a face or a body rather than something else, and is an early stage of visual processing. Paradigms that compare participants responses to faces or bodies with their responses to other types of objects are measuring detection. Face or body recognition is a later stage of visual processing and involves making distinctions between individuals within a category. Recognition is often tested by asking participants to distinguish individual faces or bodies. Recognition processes are invoked not only for identifying individual persons, but also for identifying specific body postures (e.g., sitting vs. running) and specific facial expressions (e.g., happy vs. angry). Detection of Faces and Bodies Recent work has shown that the developmental time courses for detecting human faces and bodies are different. When young infants are presented with a typical human face image and a scrambled human face image in which the eyes, nose, and mouth are moved to noncanonical locations, they prefer to look at the typical face (Johnson & Morton, 1991). Thus, in this task, infants detect the presence of a face (as opposed to a scrambled nonface). A recent study used a similar experimental procedure to investigate development of human body perception (Slaughter, Heron, & Sim, 2002). Infants between the ages of 12 and 18 months were shown typical and scrambled images of human bodies (see Fig. 1) as well as facelike stimuli, and their looking preferences were measured. The data indicated differences in the way infants responded to faces and bodies. Infants younger than 18 months of age did not show a preference for typical or scrambled body pictures, suggesting that they did not notice the differences between them, yet these young infants clearly preferred the typical face to the scrambled face. By 18 months of age, infants looked longer at the scrambled than at the typical body pictures, presumably because they found the scrambled images novel or surprising. These developmental data indicate that infants perceptual expectations about typical human faces develop much earlier than their expectations about human bodies. Adults also show a dissociation between face and body detection. In recent fmri studies, distinct brain regions were activated when participants viewed pictures of faces, bodies, or body parts. Detection of faces consistently correlated with activation in the fusiform face area, located in the ventral temporal 220 Volume 13 Number 6

Virginia Slaughter, Valerie E. Stone, and Catherine Reed LOOKING TIMES LOOKING TIMES 4500 4000 3500 3000 2500 2000 1500 1000 500 0 6000 5000 4000 3000 2000 1000 0 12-month-olds 15-month-olds 18-month- olds 12-month-olds 15-month-olds 18-month-olds Fig. 1. Mean looking times (in milliseconds) to images of human bodies (top) and facelike stimuli (bottom) with typical (white bars) and scrambled (black bars) arrangements of parts. Results are presented separately for 12-, 15-, and 18-month-olds. From Slaughter, Heron, and Sim (2002). lobe (the underneath surface of the brain toward the back; Kanwisher, McDermott, & Chun, 1997). In contrast, the extrastriate body area, located in the lateral occipitotemporal cortex (the lower left or right outside surface of the brain toward the back), was active only when participants were shown images of the human body or body parts (Downing, Jiang, Shuman, & Kanwisher, 2001). The extrastriate body area does not respond to images of faces. Thus, both in infancy and in adulthood, there are demonstrated differences in basic perceptual detection of human faces and bodies. Recognition of Faces and Bodies In contrast to the data on detection, data on face and body recognition reveal some similarity in how adults process faces and bodies. Without explicit training, people should be experts at recognizing both individual faces and individual body postures, because of their ubiquity in everyday life. If perceptual expertise means that visual recognition relies on configural processing, then one would expect that both faces and bodies would be most easily recognized by the spatial arrangement of their component parts. The inversion effect, in which recognition of objects is disrupted by turning them upside down, is traditionally considered an indicator of configural processing because inverting a familiar object makes it more difficult to recognize relations between the parts. The inversion effect has been demonstrated for faces and also for other objects when viewed by experts (e.g., dog breeders, car experts). A recent study (Reed, Stone, Bozova, & Tanaka, 2003) demonstrated that adults show similar inversion effects for faces and body postures: Both faces and bodies are more difficult to recognize when presented upside down than when presented right side up, but the same is not true of other complex stimuli, such as houses (see Fig. 2). It appears that both face and body-posture recognition depend on mentally representing the spatial configuration of stimulus parts. Despite this similarity in the recognition of faces and bodies, there are suggestions of differences in recognition processes as well. Evidence from neuropsychological patients indicates a dissociation between face and body recognition. In prosopagnosia, patients are unable to recognize individual faces a recognition problem that can be independent from difficulties with recognition of other objects. This pattern of visual recognition problems suggests that these patients have damage to a specialized face-processing area in the brain. The disorder autotopagnosia (or somatotopagnosia) affects patients ability to recognize, point to, or name specific body parts within the context of a whole body, although these patients have no difficulty naming parts of other complex objects (e.g., Ogden, 1985). Though each of these disorders is distinct from general object recognition problems, prosopagnosia and autotopagnosia do not typically occur together. The existence of these two distinct neuropsychological disorders, one affecting face recognition and the other body-part recognition, suggests that recognition of faces and recognition of bodies involve some distinct processes. CONCLUSIONS The studies reviewed here provide evidence for both similarities and differences in the way we perceive faces and bodies. Detection tasks have demonstrated mostly differences: developmental differences in responses to typical and scrambled faces versus bodies, and activation of different brain areas for face versus body processing in adults. Recognition tasks have shown similarity between perception of faces and perception of bodies, in the effects of expertise and inversion. But there are also some differences in face and body recognition: Prosopagnosia and autotopagnosia reveal independent deficits for recognition of faces and bodies and do not co-occur. Volume 13 Number 6 221

Face and Body Perception Fig. 2. Examples of stimuli and results from a study on ability to recognize upright and inverted stimuli. On each trial, two stimuli, which could be either the same or different, were presented in either upright or inverted orientation. The task was to indicate whether the stimuli were identical. The graphs show the percentage of trials on which participants responded incorrectly. From Reed, Stone, Bozova, and Tanaka (2003). This complex pattern may be explained in terms of the levels of processing involved. Detection tasks appear to tap into relatively basic visual categorization processes, possibly processes depending on simple spatial properties. Thus, the evidence from such tasks suggests the initial identification of faces and bodies as such occurs in distinct areas of the brain. Recognition tasks, in contrast, may recruit several different complex processes that analyze configural properties, identify individuals, and assign meaning. Some processing, such as the configural processing affected by expertise, may operate similarly for faces and bodies. Other recognition processing (e.g., how parts are represented relative to the whole or how the motion of parts is represented) may operate differently for faces and bodies. DIRECTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH Understanding the extent to which faces and bodies are treated similarly in visual processing will require more work that explicitly contrasts responses to faces, bodies, and other complex objects. Furthermore, it will be important for such work to define carefully the level of processing being tested. Detection paradigms involve differentiating faces and bodies from other objects or scrambled stimuli. The developmental work to date has focused on body detection, but can be expanded to explore the development of body recognition. At what stage of development would infants recognize individual, meaningful body postures? Recognition encompasses a variety of processes, depending on what about the face or body is being represented in the mind. Inversion and discrimination studies, for example, test whether participants are sensitive to relatively small changes in configuration, and performance in these studies thus may not depend on representing the whole object. Paradigms that test how well parts are recognized within the whole or individually (e.g., tests used with autotopagnosics) may tap into a different level of processing, at which the structure of the whole object and the relationship of parts to that whole are represented. Processing of bodies and processing of faces may therefore be similar in some recognition tasks, but different in others, depending on the level of processing involved. Detailed work testing recognition at 222 Volume 13 Number 6

Virginia Slaughter, Valerie E. Stone, and Catherine Reed different levels of processing can help clarify the levels at which bodies and faces share processing and mental representations and the levels at which they do not. Finally, further work on this topic should consider the importance of motion. The studies reviewed here all involved detection or recognition of static human faces and bodies. However, static and dynamic information are arguably weighted differently in face and body processing; as noted, static information is more meaningful in faces, whereas dynamic information is more crucial to body perception. For example, static images easily afford recognition of individual faces, but recognition of individual bodies probably has less to do with body shape than with characteristic motion patterns. Prosopagnosics report using motion patterns to recognize familiar people. Recent evidence suggests that facial information is processed by two distinct cognitive streams: a ventral stream (through the lower parts of the temporal lobes, corresponding to brain areas below the ears) that recognizes individuals by static features and a dorsal stream (through the upper parts of the parietal lobes, corresponding to brain areas above the ears) that processes dynamic information (O Toole, Roark, & Abdi, 2002). Different brain areas are activated by static versus dynamic displays of facial expressions. The brain also responds differently to displays of the biomechanical motion of human bodies than to static human bodies, but this may not be a dorsal-ventral differentiation (Vaina, Solomon, Chowdhury, Sinha, & Belliveau, 2001). Thus, perhaps one of the most important future directions for research in this area is the exploration of how visual processes involved in the perception of faces and bodies depend on dynamic information. Recommended Reading Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, E., & McCarthy, G. (1996). Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 551 565. Downing, P., Jiang, Y., Shuman, M., & Kanwisher, N. (2001). (See References) Kanwisher, N., & Moscovitch, M. (2000). The cognitive neuroscience of face processing: An introduction. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 1 11. Reed, C.L., Stone, V.E., Bozova, S., & Tanaka, J. (2003). (See References) Slaughter, V., Heron, M., & Sim, S. (2002). (See References) Acknowledgments We thank Derek Moore for his insightful comments on an early version of this article. REFERENCES Downing, P., Jiang, Y., Shuman, M., & Kanwisher, N. (2001). A cortical area selective for visual processing of the human body. Science, 293, 23 26. Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 493 501. Johnson, M.H., & Morton, J. (1991). Biology and cognitive development: The case of face recognition. Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell. Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 4302 4311. Ogden, J. (1985). Autotopagnosia: Occurrence in a patient with nominal aphasia and with an intact ability to point to parts of animals and objects. Brain, 108, 1009 1022. O Toole, A., Roark, D., & Abdi, H. (2002). Recognizing moving faces: A psychological and neural synthesis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 261 266. Reed, C.L., Stone, V.E., Bozova, S., & Tanaka, J. (2003). The bodyinversion effect. Psychological Science, 14, 302 308. Slaughter, V., Heron, M., & Sim, S. (2002). Development of preferences for the human body shape in infancy. Cognition, 85(3), B71 B81. Tanaka, J., & Gauthier, I. (1997). Expertise in object and face recognition. In R. Goldstone, P. Schyns, & D. Medin (Eds.), Psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 36. Perceptual mechanisms of learning (pp. 83 125). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Turati, C., Simion, F., Milani, I., & Umilta, C. (2002). Newborns preference for faces: What is crucial? Developmental Psychology, 38, 875 882. Vaina, L., Solomon, J., Chowdhury, S., Sinha, P., & Belliveau, W. (2001). Functional neuroanatomy of biological motion perception in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 98, 11656 11661. Volume 13 Number 6 223