Discovery Through the Lens of the Standard Celeration Chart: Informing and Facilitating Inductive Intervention Strategies

Similar documents
Implementing Discrete Trial Teaching

Parent initiated Why evaluate (3) Questions about program appropriateness School initiated Conflict resolution Component of a regular peer review proc

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

Functionality. A Case For Teaching Functional Skills 4/8/17. Teaching skills that make sense

1/20/2015. Maximizing Stimulus Control: Best Practice Guidelines for Receptive Language Instruction. Importance of Effective Teaching

Verbal Behavior 101: A Quick Introduction. Tashenna Gillmore, M.Ed., BCBA, LBA The Heart of Behavior LLC.

COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO

The Effects of a CABAS Conditioning Procedure To Increase Appropriate Toy Play and Decrease Stereotypy

Teaching Communication Across the Day. Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA

Teaching Communication to Individuals with Autism. Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA

Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Melissa A. Ezold

The Effects of Video Modeling on New Staff Training. of Discrete Trial Instruction. A thesis presented. Tamarra Forbes

Teaching for Generaliza0on & Maintenance

AN EVALUATION OF CHOICE ON INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICACY AND INDIVIDUAL PREFERENCES AMONG CHILDREN WITH AUTISM KAREN A. TOUSSAINT TIFFANY KODAK

Pairing & Manding. Vincent J. Carbone Ed.D., BCBA-D NYS Licensed Behavior Analyst

2/2/2017. Understanding Why and How Behavior Changes. Deficits? Strengths? Behaviors? Services/ Interventions? Parent, Professional, Peer prospective?

International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy ISSN: Universidad de Almería España

PASS4TEST. IT Certification Guaranteed, The Easy Way! We offer free update service for one year

The Effects of Question-Present Versus Item Present Conditions on Acquisition of Mands

STRATEGIES FOR SUCCESSFUL INSTRUCTION

Schedules of Reinforcement

LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS

Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present. A Thesis Presented

Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition. A Thesis Presented. Robert Mark Grant

Effective Interventions for Students with ASD: Practical Applications for Classroom Success. Seminar Two

Effective Interventions for Students with ASD: Practical Applications for Classroom Success. Seminar Two. Objectives

Barbara Sosoo. Providing Systematic Instruction

Overall Overview 2/19/17

National University of Ireland, Maynooth

BACB Fourth Edition Task List Assessment Form

Evidence-Based Practices Comparison Chart. National Autism Center (NAC) 1

AN EVALUATION OF PREFERENCE FOR VIDEO AND IN VIVO MODELING KANEEN B. GEIGER AND LINDA A. LEBLANC

APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS (ABA) THE LOVAAS METHODS LECTURE NOTE

PERSPECTIVE TAKING AND NONLITERAL LANGUAGE OUTLINE DERIVED RELATIONAL RESPONDING

Julie Bolton M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

The Functional Analysis of Behavior: History, Applications, and Implications

Thinking Out of the Box 1

12/19/2016. Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview. What is the Autism Spectrum? Autism Spectrum Disorder

Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview

HELPING STUDENTS WITH AUTISM FIND THEIR VOICE FINAL EXAM

Methodological refinements in the behavior-analytic study of distraction: A preliminary investigation

Title: Stimulus Pairing Training in. Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Kosuke Takahashi a. Jun ichi Yamamoto b

Purpose and Objectives of Study Objective 1 Objective 2 Objective 3 Participants and Settings Intervention Description Social peer networks

Teaching Two Children Diagnosed with Autism to Tolerate Physical Contact

A Comparison of Methods for Marking Correct Responses. in Matching to Sample. A Thesis Presented. Kara Schmidt

MANDS FOR INFORMATION USING WHO? AND WHICH? IN THE PRESENCE OF ESTABLISHING AND ABOLISHING OPERATIONS M. ALICE SHILLINGSBURG

Evaluation of a Self-Instructional Package for Teaching Tutors to Conduct Discrete Trials Teaching with Children with Autism.

Answers to end of chapter questions

The Role of Joint Control in Teaching Complex Behavior to Children with Autism

Running Head: VISUAL SCHEDULES FOR STUDENTS WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER

UNIVERSITY OF AKUREYRI

School of Social Work

An Overview of Naturalistic ABA Strategies for Young Children with Autism

DSM V Criteria for Autism Spectrum Disorder

Social Skills and Autism: Using Books in Creative Ways to Reach and Teach in Early Education

Comparing the Teaching Interaction Procedure to Social Stories: A Replication Study

METHOD Participants and Setting The participants in this study were 11 children (10 boys, 1 girl) with an independent

Consequent Interventions

An Introduction to Behavior Management

Starting Strong 2015 Understanding Autism Spectrum Disorders and An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis

Practical Strategies for Families and Educators of Deaf Learners with Autism

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

INCREASING INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICIENCY BY PRESENTING ADDITIONAL STIMULI IN LEARNING TRIALS FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS JASON C.

Evaluating & Teaching Yes/No Responses Based on an Analysis of Functions. Jennifer Albis, M.S., CCC-SLP

VISUAL PERCEPTION OF STRUCTURED SYMBOLS

This is an edited transcript of a telephone interview recorded in March 2010.

Neurobiology and Information Processing Theory: the science behind education

Behavioral Consultation Case Presentation. Cori Brown Psychology 636 Fall 2011 California State University, Chico

What is Behavior? Course Objectives. What is Behavior? 11/2/13. (And how do we manage it?)

Effects of Differential Reinforcement of Short Latencies on Response Latency, Task Completion, and Accuracy of an Adolescent with Autism

Using Antecedent Based Intervention & Reinforcement: From an Elementary Perspective

CARBONE CLINIC VIVIAN ATTANASIO VERBAL BEHAVIOR INSTITUTE AND TAMARA KASPER CENTER FOR AUTISM TREATMENT

Teaching a Scanning Response to a Child with Autism

PECS and Skinner's Verbal Behavior. Andy Bondy, Ph.D. Lori Frost, MS, CCC-SLP

Behaviorism: An essential survival tool for practitioners in autism

FRASER RIVER COUNSELLING Practicum Performance Evaluation Form

J-01 State intervention goals in observable and measureable terms.

The Effects of Precision Teaching With Textual or Tact Relations on Intraverbal Relations

Tasks of Executive Control TEC. Interpretive Report. Developed by Peter K. Isquith, PhD, Robert M. Roth, PhD, Gerard A. Gioia, PhD, and PAR Staff

THE EFFECTS OF TEXTUAL FLUENCY ON THE RATE OF ACQUISITION AND APPLICATION OF INTRAVERBAL RELATIONS. Rachael E. Shrontz, B.A.

10/21/18. Goals for today. With thanks

EBP s for Transitioning: Implementing with Individuals with Autism. Laura Ferguson, M.Ed, BCBA

Tris Gilland, Ed. S., BCBA Douglas County Schools

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Emergence of Equivalence Relations: Comparing Sorting and Match-to-Sample Procedures. Lindsay J. Grimm. The New England Center for Children

Progress Monitoring Handouts 1

Behavioral Treatment Strategies. Yiliana Puerto, M.S., BCBA BCBA Program Manager PsychSolutions, Inc.

What Triggers Disruptive Behaviors? 10/13/18. Practical Ways to Decrease Disruptive Behavior. Background. Lynn Kern Koegel, PhD, CCC-SLP

Staff Development Day 2013

A Comparison of Edible, Social, and No Contrived Reinforcement on the. Acquisition of a Behavior Chain. A Thesis Presented.

VB-MAPP Barriers Assessment

The Relationship Between Motivating Operations & Behavioral Variability Penn State Autism Conference 8/3/2016

Cultivating Motivation During Natural Environment Teaching and Group Instruction for Diverse Learners. Pam Salerno Aja Weston Michael Houck

Glossary.

Psychological Flexibility and the Extension of Knowledge in Contextual Behavioral and Cognitive Therapy. Steven C. Hayes University of Nevada

Acquisition of Picture Exchange-Based vs. Signed Mands and Implications to Teach Functional Communication Skills to Children with Autism

Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge

How Does NUMBER SENSE Begin?

I. Content Presentation. II. Learning Situation. IV. Reflection. III. Observation

Examining the Bouba-Kiki Effect

Transcription:

Behavioral Development Bulletin 2015 American Psychological Association 2015, Vol. 20, No. 2, 150 157 1942-0722/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0101307 Discovery Through the Lens of the Standard Celeration Chart: Informing and Facilitating Inductive Intervention Strategies Staheli Meyer, Tiffany Aninao, Kendra Newsome, and Donny Newsome Fit Learning, Reno, Nevada Key to the success of Precision Teachers is an understanding that accuracy of performance is necessary, but insufficient for skill mastery. Dimensions of behavior occurring in time, such as rate, latency, or duration, must also be included in determinations of competence. By employing the Standard Celeration Chart, which allows assessment of all the above features of responding, Precision Teachers can detect and address barriers to mastery that go beyond issues of accuracy. Included here are examples of successful behavioral interventions whose discovery is attributed to the use of this sensitive measurement tool. The first intervention presented shows the effects of an error defusion intervention on passage reading. The second intervention demonstrates the impact of a discrimination priming on a picture-naming task. In the before mentioned interventions, the Standard Celeration Chart was paramount in the discovery of key performance variables. Keywords: defusion, discrimination, intervention, momentum In the applied domain, the ambition of the practitioner is the design of effective treatments to produce meaningful outcomes for the individuals served. The principles of learning inform our treatments as scientists, but the practitioner is concerned with more than an understanding of principles; the practitioner is concerned with clinical significance and extent of benefit (Azrin, 1977). As an applied science, Precision Teaching has yielded scores of discoveries about teaching and learning (Binder, 1996; Lindsley, 1992; Haughton, 1972). The use of this sensitive, standardized measurement tool has set the occasion for these discoveries and is an ideal tool for the practitioner. The standard celeration chart (SCC) is a tool that optimizes the discovery of key performance variables in the applied arena; it allows assessments of frequency, latency, and duration in real calendar time, facilitating the This article was published Online First September 21, 2015. Staheli Meyer, Tiffany Aninao, Kendra Newsome, and Donny Newsome, Fit Learning, Reno, Nevada. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Staheli Meyer, Fit Learning, 9408 Double R Boulevard, Suite B, Reno, NV 89521. E-mail: staheli@meyer.net detection of barriers to acquisition and mastery, important matters for the practitioner. With its use, trends are quantifiable, and patterns are easily detected across multiple behaviors of interest. Thus, the SCC functions as a powerful measurement tool and means to evaluate of the efficacy of clinical interventions (Binder, 1996; Kubina & Yurich, 2012). The purpose of this article is to illustrate how the SCC, when used to measure behavior in an applied setting, led to the design of interventions that produced meaningful outcomes for the individuals served. In particular, the use of frequency as the primary datum and the depiction of real calendar time along the horizontal axis allowed for important discoveries with respect to our phenomenon of interest: the acquisition of academic behaviors. Because the SCC uses frequency as its primary datum, performance can be evaluated with respect to the frequency of correct responses, as well as the frequency of incorrect responses, a benefit not afforded by percent correct measures. The first intervention described, error defusion, exemplifies how the SCC allowed for the detection of long interresponse times during reading activities and the determination of clinical benefits following intervention. The second intervention demonstrates how the SCC facilitated comparisons between academic programs, 150

DISCOVERY THROUGH THE LENS 151 which revealed differences in a learner s acquisition patterns and led to an inductive approach to ameliorating those differences. Error Defusion Defusion, a technique borrowed from Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, is understood to alter the aversive function of verbal stimuli, including private events. Similar to interventions commonly applied in the treatment of anxiety disorders, defusion protocols involve systematic, repeated exposure to stimuli that have historically evoked avoidance behaviors. The principal of habituation and related techniques such as flooding are instructive conceptual parallels in the traditional behavior analytic literature. The error-defusion protocol described herein represents an attempt to alter the aversive functions of textual stimuli likely to occasion errors and/or general response suppression, thereby reducing the need for prompting and resulting in increasing acquisition frequencies. Method Mike was a 12-year-old boy diagnosed with Selective Mutism. Mike was referred to Fit Learning for reading remediation. His reading skills at intake were assessed to be six grade levels behind his age-matched peers. Based on the intake assessment, a reading program was designed to improve Mike s reading abilities. Sessions were conducted five days per week for two hours per day. During reading sessions, a number of reading components were targeted and plotted on the SCC. These component skills were selected based on the intake assessment and included skills such as letter sound identification, decoding and reading words that follow specific phonetic patterns, and passage reading. Mike s progress toward grade level reading was monitored weekly using Aimsweb reading passages, a standardized curriculum-based measurement tool (CBM). Aimsweb reading CBMs were administered weekly. Mike was presented with a novel CBM passage each week and asked to read aloud for one minute. The number of correctly read words and errors were scored during this 1-min period. If Mike failed to respond to a word within three seconds, the instructor verbally prompted Mike with the full word. This was counted as an error. CBM data were then plotted on the SCC. Through analysis of CBM data, it was discovered that instances of nonresponding reliably occurred in the presence of unfamiliar words. As a result, Mike s overall frequencies of correct responding were low and his frequencies of errors due to nonresponding were high. This pattern of behavior was observed across the majority of his other reading programs as well. Following failed strategies such as prompting Mike to apply mastered decoding skills and/or ask for help. The practitioner identified that in stimulus conditions where incorrect responding was probable, Mike engaged in behavior tantamount to learned helplessness (Abramson et al., 1978). Therefore, an error defusion task was implemented. The rationale for this task was to reduce, or defuse, the aversive functions of stimuli likely to occasion errors attributable to nonresponding (Törneke, 2010). The Error Defusion program was run at the beginning of each day in the context of Mike s reading sessions. Multiple levels or slices of error defusion were sequentially introduced as mastery criteria on the slice in acquisition were met, and each iteration involved a closer approximation to the targeted response during CBM passage reading. The levels are described in order of presentation, and four slices were introduced. During the first slice of error defusion, Mike was presented with a sheet of six colors. The colors randomly repeated throughout the sheet, and each row contained 10 exemplars. Mike was given the instruction to engage in any response similar in topography to the correct response (i.e., another color), but only incorrect responses were reinforced. For example, if the color red was presented, reinforcement was available for naming any color on the sheet except red. For slice 2, Mike was presented with a sheet containing five sight words. The organization of this stimulus sheet followed the same patterns as the sheet used for colors, and the instructions were the same. For example, if the word this was presented, reinforcement was available for the spoken response of any other word on the sheet except this. The next slice in the error defusion sequence utilized multisyllabic words above Mike s cur-

152 MEYER, ANINAO, NEWSOME, AND NEWSOME rent reading and decoding level. In this iteration, Mike was instructed to sound out the presented word. For scoring purposes and reinforcement, the accuracy of Mike s response was irrelevant. Mike s goal was to respond by attempting to decode the word (e.g., sound out). For example, if the word presented was decide Mike could have responded by saying d e k d dekd. Attempts to decode were counted as correct responses regardless of whether the sounds Mike produced corresponded to the sounds of the word. The final slice in the error defusion sequence consisted of sentences with one or two nonsense words embedded in each sentence. A new criterion was stated for this level of training. Correct responses required that Mike meet an interresponse time goal. Thus, reinforcement was contingent on Mike s response to the embedded nonsense word, which had to occur within two seconds of him reading the word that preceded the nonsense word. If the interresponse criterion was met, a correct response was counted. In all phases of error defusion, frequency goals were stated for each practice opportunity, and points were delivered if the frequency goal was obtained. Results Figure 1 shows the effects of the error defusion intervention on Mike s reading CBM. Dots represent individual frequencies of correct responses, whereas represents the frequency of errors scored for nonresponding or reading a word incorrectly. Trends of correct or incorrect responding are visual denoted by a black line. This trend line is quantified with the celeration calculation, which yielded numbers found in Table 1. An increasing frequency is quantified as acceleration ( ), whereas a decreasing frequency is quantified as deceleration ( ). Bounce is a measure of variability in responding and are denoted as green lines, and yielded numbers found in Table 1. Figure 1. Frequency of correct and incorrect words read on a reading CBM.

DISCOVERY THROUGH THE LENS 153 Table 1 Performance Measures Before and After Error Defusion Intervention Measure Before error defusion After error defusion Acceleration of correct responses 1.13 1.11 Deceleration of incorrect responses 1.02 1.13 Bounce of correct responses 3.4 1.8 The acceleration of correct responses before error defusion intervention was 1.13, after intervention celeration was 1.11. The deceleration of incorrect responses before intervention was 1.02, after the intervention deceleration was 1.13. Variability, or bounce, in correct responding before the intervention was 3.4; after the intervention bounce decreased to 1.8 (see Table 1). Before the error defusion programing, correct and incorrect responses were largely undifferentiated. After the implementation of slice 1, an immediate differentiation between these responses is observed; correct responses increased in frequency and incorrect responses decreased. This pattern continued through the implementation of slice 2 and subsequent slices. After the implementation of slice 3, correct responding continued to celerate and Mike s reading speed increase from 28 words per minute after the initial implementation of error defusion slice 1, to a frequency to 55 words per minute at the end of training in slice 3. Following the implementation of slice 4 of the defusion programing, the steepest rate of deceleration of errors due to nonresponding was observed, 1.39. During the months the error defusion intervention was in place the level of the Aimsweb reading passages was increased from kindergarten grade level passages to 2nd grade level passages. Following the implementation of the error defusion programing, Mike s reading frequencies increased by 57 words per minute in 52 weeks. The terminal frequency measured on the last observation period was 85 words per minute on a 2nd grade reading passage. Antecedent Priming Behavioral momentum, a metaphor used to describe a rate of responding and its resistance to change following a [change] in reinforcement conditions (Rosqvist & Hersen, 2005, p. 691), has been used to illustrate effects of an antecedent intervention of presenting high-probability requests in a sequence (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007). The second intervention in this report demonstrates how the SCC enabled the discovery of different acquisition rates between similar academic programs, which informed a momentum-based intervention. Because the SCC is standardized by time, all charts for a given learner s programming are date-synchronized allowing for comparisons between training programs occurring in real time. In this case, a difference in acquisition rates was discovered between naming tasks picture names and category identification. These programs shared many features the stimulus materials were the same (flashcards), the practice intervals were the same (15-s), and the frequency aim was identical (50 60 responses per minute). Thus, in an inductive inquiry, other variables impacting acquisition on the picture-naming tasks were considered. Further analysis revealed that at least two aspects differed: the number of discriminations required and the history of reinforcement with respect to each program. The category identification program required only five differentiated responses, whereas the picture naming task required 18 differentiated responses. Moreover, rates of reinforcement were higher on the category identification task than for the picture-naming task as a result of frequency goals being met more frequently. Method An antecedent intervention was selected to evaluate whether the momentum of high rate responding during a flash card priming task would be effective for increasing frequencies on the picture-naming task. Sally was a 6-year-old girl diagnosed with Downs Syndrome. Sally was referred to Fit Learning for language enrichment and comprehension training. Based on the intake assessment, a relational language pro-

154 MEYER, ANINAO, NEWSOME, AND NEWSOME gram was designed to improve Sally s language and comprehension abilities. During each session, 8 to 10 different skills were targeted for acquisition, including picture and category identification. Sally s session were conducted five days a week, for one hour each day. A priming task was implemented before the picture-naming program wherein only two discriminations were present in the stimulus array to see whether the frequency of responding occasioned during the priming task would facilitate an increased frequency of responding during the picture-naming program. A flashcard deck was constructed that contained two items: cups and airplanes. Sally had a history of reinforcement for correct naming of these objects. The deck included multiple exemplars of cups and airplanes. Sally was given a frequency goal for responses in a 15-s timing. Reinforcement was provided contingent on meeting the frequency goal. The priming task was implemented once a day before Sally s regular picture naming program. Results Figure 2 shows Sally s celeration on a category identification task where only five discriminations were required. Figure 3 shows Sally s celeration on a picture-naming task where 18 discriminations were required. Differences in acquisition rates are observed between category identification tasks ( 1.81), and picture-naming tasks ( 1.08). Figure 4 shows the effects of a discrimination priming on frequency and stability of responding on the aforementioned picture-naming task. Dots represent frequencies of correct responses and the represents frequencies of error responses. An under the record floor indicates no error was recorded. Again, black lines denote Figure 2. Frequency of correct and incorrect on category identification task.

DISCOVERY THROUGH THE LENS 155 Figure 3. Frequency of correct and incorrect on picture-naming task. celerations, and green lines show bounce. The acceleration of correct responses before the intervention was 1.08. After the intervention the celeration was 1.24. Before the intervention the bounce was 2.20. After the intervention bounce was 1.40 (see Table 2). Discussion The SCC offers behavior scientists a system for measuring dimensions of behavior occurring in real time on a standard display, therefore facilitating visual inspection and detection of clinical benefit. This allows the impact of interventions to be easily measured across many related stimulus and response classes. Both of the case studies presented here demonstrate the utility of the SCC in detecting suboptimal performance patterns, informing context-specific intervention strategies, and evaluating the localized effectiveness of those interventions. In both examples discussed, the SCC facilitated the discovery of clinically significant and meaningful outcomes that might have otherwise been masked by the measurement system. This error defusion programing was informed by conceptual principles of learning, but evaluated by the practitioner for the extent of benefit for the individual being served. Patterns in Mike s acquisition data showed long interresponse times to particular stimuli. Without frequency as a unit of measurement, this dimension of responding would have gone undetected. A procedure to alter the aversive function of textual stimuli likely to occasion general response suppression was implemented. This procedure resulted in increased acquisition rates, and a decrease in errors attributable to nonresponding. In this instance, barriers to mastery were detected, and an inductive intervention was implemented.

156 MEYER, ANINAO, NEWSOME, AND NEWSOME Figure 4. Frequency of correct and incorrect on picture-naming task. Table 2 Performance Measures Before and After Prime Measure Before prime After prime Celeration of correct responses 1.08 1.24 Bounce of correct responses 2.20 1.40 In Sally s case, the standardized features of the SCC and the plotting of frequency in real time led to the discovery that similar tasks yielded different acquisition patterns. This discovery guided the implementation of an antecedent priming task, which increased frequency of responding and stability on the targets in acquisition. Practitioners, having identified the differential rates of acquisition, were able to isolate potential variables impacting acquisition rates (e.g., reinforcement history), which informed the use of specific tactics (e.g., behavioral momentum) for altering the acquisition pattern. In the applied domain, it is the goal of the practitioner to design effective treatments that produce meaningful outcomes. Practitioners are only as effective as the tools they use to contact their subject matter. When the SCC is utilized, trends are quantifiable, and patterns are readily detected. Frequency, latency, and duration are all dimensions of behavior. Measurements of these dimensions on a standardized display that yields quantifiable changes in behavior facilitate the detection of barriers to acquisition and mastery, both of which are important considerations for the practitioner. References Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-

DISCOVERY THROUGH THE LENS 157 ogy, 87, 49 74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843x.87.1.49 Azrin, N. (1977). A strategy for applied research: Learning based but outcome oriented. American Psychologist, 32, 140 149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.32.2.140 Binder, C. (1996). Behavioral fluency: Evolution of a new paradigm. The Behavior Analyst, 19, 163 197. Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Applied behavior analysis. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education, Inc. Haughton, E. (1972). Aims Growing and sharing. In J. B. Jordan & L. S. Robbins (Eds.), Let s try doing something else kind of thing: Behavioral principles and the exceptional child (pp. 20 39). A report from the Invisible College Conference on Application of Behavioral Principles in Exceptional Child Education, March, 1971. Arlington, VA: The Council for Exceptional Children. Kubina, R. M., & Yurich, K. K. L. (2012). The precision teaching book. Lemont. PA: Greatness Achieved Publishing Company. Lindsley, O. R. (1992). Precision teaching: Discoveries and effects. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 51 57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1992.25-51 Rosqvist, J., & Hersen, M. (2005). Behavioral momentum. In M. Hersen (Ed.), Encyclopedia of behavior modification and cognitive behavior therapy. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Törneke, N. (2010). Learning RFT: An introduction to relational frame theory and its clinical applications. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications, Inc. Received December 19, 2014 Revision received May 17, 2015 Accepted May 18, 2015