CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a

Similar documents
FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

postreinforcement pause for a minute or two at the beginning of the session. No reduction

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS'

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING

REPEATED MEASUREMENTS OF REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE EFFECTS ON GRADIENTS OF STIMULUS CONTROL' MICHAEL D. ZEILER

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

MCMASTER UNIVERSITY. more values independently of food delivery. In any case, the well known result of Skinner's

SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT'

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Subjects and apparatus

that simple discrimination training to compound given a set to react -to one aspect of a stimulus a "set", a discrimination is "pretrained" along

Variability as an Operant?

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

Extinction. n Operant Extinction: n Ideally combined with DRO (esp. DRI) n No longer reinforcing operant behavioral response

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information

University Microfilms, Inc., Ann Arbor, M ichigan

Behavioural Processes

Measures of temporal discrimination in fixedinterval performance: A case study in archiving data

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN

Instrumental Conditioning I

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues

edited by Derek P. Hendry'

Some Effects of Discrimination Training on a Line Length Dimension

Schedules of Reinforcement 11/11/11

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats

OBSERVING RESPONSES AND SERIAL STIMULI: SEARCHING FOR THE REINFORCING PROPERTIES OF THE S2 ROGELIO ESCOBAR AND CARLOS A. BRUNER

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING. Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A.

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

Stimulus control of behavior during the postreinforcement pause of FI schedules

ASSOCIATIVE FACTORS UNDERLYING THE PIGEON'S KEY PECKING IN AUTO-SHAPING PROCEDURES'

INDUCED POLYDIPSIA1 JOHN L. FALK UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN. a large number of these small meals. Under. CRF, inter-reinforcement time is short and

SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

GENERALIZATION GRADIENTS AS INDICANTS OF LEARNING AND RETENTION OF A RECOGNITION TASK 1

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

acquisition associative learning behaviorism A type of learning in which one learns to link two or more stimuli and anticipate events

ANNA STATE HOSPITAL. peated measurement of the attack. A mechanical

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, &

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1

ASSOCIATED WITH NALORPHINE IN

Reports from the Research Laboratories

TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES SN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION

The Post-Reinforcement Pause and Terminal Rate In Fixed-Interval Schedules

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

Jeremie Jozefowiez. Timely Training School, April 6 th, 2011

Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is

ACQUISITION OF DISCRIMINATED OPERANT RESPONDING: THE EFFECTS OF BRIEF REINFORCEMENT DELAYS. MATTHEW E. ANDRZEJEWSKI University of Wisconsin-Madison

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a

AUTOSHAPING OF KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS

J. E. R. STADDON DUKE UNIVERSITY. The relative inability of the usual differential. to ask whether performance under DRL schedules

Transfer of persistence across reinforced behaviors

A PRACTICAL VARIATION OF A MULTIPLE-SCHEDULE PROCEDURE: BRIEF SCHEDULE-CORRELATED STIMULI JEFFREY H. TIGER GREGORY P. HANLEY KYLIE M.

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q

Classical and Instrumental Conditioning. Lecture 8

Acquisition of bar-pressing under interval and ratio-schedules in the presence and absence of stimuli correlated with water delivery

Some Variables in Conditioned Suppression with Humans

Behavior Shaping. Shaping. Shaping is defined as. Differential Reinforcement. Jesús Rosales-Ruiz UNT. TxABA 2018

INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals

Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE

Omission training compared with yoked controls and extinction in multiple-schedule discrimination learning*

Multidimensional Auditory Stimulus Control in a Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes)

Operant Conditioning

between successive DMTS choice phases.

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus.

A concurrent assessment of the positive and negative properties of a signaled shock schedule*

Coding of feature and no-feature events by pigeons performing a delayed conditional discrimination

Coding of hedonic and nonhedonic samples by pigeons in many-to-one delayed matching

Schedule Induced Polydipsia: Effects of Inter-Food Interval on Access to Water as a Reinforcer

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors

Common Coding in Pigeons Assessed Through Partial Versus Total Reversals of Many-to-One Conditional and Simple Discriminations

The effects of reinforcement upon the prepecking behaviors of pigeons in the autoshaping experiment.

What is Learned? Lecture 9

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2005 November 14.

Cooling as Reinforcing Stimulus in Aplysia

1. A type of learning in which behavior is strengthened if followed by a reinforcer or diminished if followed by a punisher.

Learning the Temporal Dynamics of Behavior

OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS. Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

REINFORCEMENT AT CONSTANT RELATIVE IMMEDIACY OF REINFORCEMENT A THESIS. Presented to. The Faculty of the Division of Graduate. Studies and Research

Transcription:

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 5, NUMBF- 4 OCITOBER, 1 962 THE EFECT OF TWO SCHEDULES OF PRIMARY AND CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT JOAN G. STEVENSON1 AND T. W. REESE MOUNT HOLYOKE COLLEGE Of 23 pigeons, 11 received continuous reinforcement for key pecking, and 12 received an FR 10 schedule of reinforcement. The birds were then tested without food, but with potential conditioned reinforcers presented either on the same schedule as in training, on the other schedule, or not at all. Each bird in the subgroup trained on CRF and tested with Sr"s at FR 10 not only gave more responses in testing than did each bird in both subgroups receiving no S"s, but also gave more responses than did each bird in the Sr subgroup receiving CRF training and S"s at CRF. Cumulative records are presented to show the effects of different schedules of conditioned reinforcers. The purpose of the present experiment was to consider the effect of potential conditioned reinforcers on maintaining an operant response. The conditioned reinforcers (Sr's) are presented without food during testing, either on the same schedule, or on a schedule differing from the one in effect during training. A control, in which no Sr's at all are presented, also follows training. The work of Notterman and Dinsmoor indicated that the training schedule affects the strength of an Sr. Notterman (1951) found that the strength of an Sr is an increasing monotonic function of the total number of SA trials in training. Dinsmoor (1952) tentatively concluded that the Fl training increases the Sr power of a stimulus. Unlike Dinsmoor, Clayton (1956) found "no consistent evidence that a moderate degree of intermittence in reinforcement during either discrimination or extinction phases had favorable effects on secondary reinforcing properties" (1956, p. 380). Clayton suggested that her contrast with Dinsmoor's findings may have been caused by his animals on Fl simply building up a higher rate of responding than his CRF group, so that they made a greater number of responses during extinction. This can be tested only by comparing each Sr group with a control, which differs only in that it receives no Sr during testing. Furthermore, because the Sr in both experiments was an SD for the same operant in training, part of the Sr strength in testing may actually be SD strength. 'Now at Brown University. Following CRF training, Dinsmoor, Kish, and Keller (1953) presented an Sr on either a CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no differences that were due to these schedules of S presentation. Although more responses might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a CRF schedule, the CRF schedule of Sr presentation in the above experiment offers conditions which are more similar to training. According to Bitterman's discrimination hypothesis, the more similarity between training and testing, the more responses emitted in testing. However, this apparent similarity between training and testing schedules is complicated by the fact that in the above experiment the Sr which followed on operant in testing preceded the same operant in training. Thus, many discrepancies exist in the above Sr experiments which consider the same operant in both training and testing. Therefore the present experiment deals with Sr at this level, rather than using Sr to condition a new response. It is desirable to discuss Sr strength by comparison with a No-Sr control; to eliminate any SD effects by having the potential Sr follow the given operant in training as well as in testing; and to test the discrimination hypothesis by having two subgroups with only one difference between training and testing (i.e., removal of primary reinforcement). METHOD An operant-conditioning apparatus was used (Ferster & Skinner, 1957), with grain 505

506 JOAN G. STEVENSON and T. W. REESE presented by a Lehigh Valley Electronics No. 1347 feeder. The location of the response key, 3/4 in. in diameter, was at the center of the panel, 9% in. from the floor. A cumulative recorder and digital counters recorded responses and reinforcements. White noise and houselights were present at all times. Subjects The subjects were 23 naive, male Silver King pigeons, 6 to 7 years old, maintained at 75% free-feeding weight. Procedure In Sessions 1-3 (Table 1), all birds received the same treatment, continuous reinforcement for key pecking. The total number of responses in Session 4, a simple extinction session, served as a criterion for placing birds into six subgroups. Each subgroup contained four birds, except one subgroup, which contained only three. An attempt was made to keep the median number of extinction responses per subgroup equal. During training sessions, each response to be reinforced produced: key light on, feeder light on, noise of feeder coming up, and grain, for 2.5 sec. A total of 65 Sr'S was given in Sessions 1 and 2 (see Table 1); a total of 55 Sr's per session was given in Sessions 3, 5, 6, and 7. During testing, the potential Sr'S consisted of: key light on, feeder light on, and noise of feeder coming up. The feeder, which was empty and covered, was raised for only 0.5 sec. The hole through which the bird gained access to the feeder was not covered. Table 1 shows the schedules on which each subgroup was trained and tested. During training, each bird received the same number of reinforcements per session. After the first 3 days of testing (Sessions 8, 9, and 10), nearly all responses were given in the first 15 min of a 50-min test session; therefore, the last four test sessions were reduced to 20 min. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The Kendall Rank Correlations between the number of responses in Session 4 (simple EXT) and the number of responses in Session 8 (first test day) are not significant for any subgroup. For no subgroup did the bird that gave the fewest responses and the bird that gave most responses in Session 4 remain at the same extremes in Session 8. Thus, the relative number of responses given in simple EXT does not aid in predicting the relative number of responses given in testing (with or without Sr) after retraining. Following either training schedule, testing with Sr's at FR 10 results in more responses than does testing with Sr's at CRF; and either of these Sr schedules results in more responses than does testing with no Sr's at all. For any one of the three test schedules, more responses are given by the subgroup trained on FR 10 than on CRF. These group relations hold not only in terms of the median total number of responses (Table 2), but also in terms of the median number of responses per session (Fig. 1). A noticeable exception occurs on the first test day, where more responses are given by the subgroup trained on CRF than by the subgroup trained on FR 10, with each receiving Sr's at FR 10. The curves were fitted simply to indicate how responding decreases from one session to the next. The sharpest drop for all subgroups occurs from the first test day ible 1 Schema of the Experimental Design Sessions 1-3 Session 4 Sessions 5-7 Sessions 8-15' Subgroup (Training) (Training) (Testing) CRF Sr at CRF CRF-CRF CRF Control (No St) CRF-No Sr Sr at FR 10 CRF-FR10 Simple EXT (40 min) S at CRF FRIO-CRF FR 10 Control (No Sr) FR10-No S' S at FR 10 FRIO-FRI0 Sessions 8-11 lasted 50 min each Sessions 12-15 lasted 20 min each

EFFECT OF TWO SCHEDULES 507. 10 11 12 SESSIONS Fig. eye. 1. Median number of responses for each subgroup during the test sessions. The curves were fitted by to the second, and is largest for subgroup CRF-FRIO. Plots of the number of responses for each bird show a decrease from the first test session to the second, but not all curves continue to decrease as smoothly as indicated in the group curves. For birds trained on CRF, the highest points on any given session are always those of the three CRF-FRIO birds. There are only two exceptions: (1) on the second test session, when one bird from subgroup CRF-CRF gave more responses than did two birds from subgroup CRF-FR 10; and (2) on the last test session (Session 15), when one bird from CRF-FRIO was surpassed by a bird from each of the other two subgroups. However, on this last session, three birds gave zero responses, and the maximum was only 39 responses. Because of the difficulty of reading a figure with 11 or 12 curves, these individual curves are not shown. Table 2 gives the number of responses each bird makes on the first test session, and the total over all the eight test sessions. Like the individual curves, this table shows that each bird in subgroup CRF-FRI0 gave more responses than did any bird in either subgroup CRF-No Sr or CRF-CRF. This finding with Sr is in line with the wellknown observation that FR schedules of primary reinforcement produce more responses than does a CRF schedule. It is not in line with the discrimination hypothesis, since the presentation of Sr's at FR 10 results in a condition which differs more from CRF training than does presentation of Sr's at CRF. Unlike the above findings following CRF training, there is overlap among birds from the different subgroups trained on FR 10 (Table 2). Overlap is even found between subgroup FR 10-FR 10 and subgroup FR 10-No. Sr. Presentation of Sr'S can dramatically reverse the usual differences found in extinction, following FR vs. CRF training. That is, each bird in the subgroup FR 10-No Sr gave more responses than did any bird in the subgroup CRF-No Sr, as might be predicted. However, each bird in the subgroup trained on CRF, with Sr's at FR 10, gave more responses than did any bird in the subgroup trained on FR 10, with no Sr. It has been indicated that birds in subgroup CRF-FR 10 gave not only more responses than any bird in both control subgroups, but also

508 JOAN G. STEVENSON and T. W. REESE Table 2 Total Number of Responses for each Bird: (a) In the First Test Session; (b) In All Eight Test Sessions Subgroup 73 CRF-CRF 159 168 582 50 CRF-No Sr 47 97 211 1024 CRF-FR10 908 1189 FR1O-CRF 17 686 783 1529 FR10-No Sr 267 310 628 828 FR10-FR1O 287 795 1279 887 Number of Responses First All Eight Test Session Test Sessions 217 297 300 1037 84 112 182 377 1489 1706 1956 275 1096 1426 1987 564 620 843 1433 762 1466 2598 2696 147 1706 1261 704 2032 more responses than any bird in the Sr subgroup, CRF-CRF. The superiority of this CRF-FR 10 subgroup is nicely demonstrated by a measure used by Miles (1956, p. 497), who expressed the "relative effectiveness of the secondary reinforcement stimulation" by a ratio of the number of responses for an Sr subgroup over the number of responses for its control subgroup. In Fig. 2, this ratio is plotted against sessions. The ratio does not show a consistent increase or decrease over sessions for any Sr subgroup. Because the curve for subgroup CRF-FRIO lies well above the other three curves, this combination of training on CRF and testing on FR 10 produces a relatively more effective Sr than any of the other three combinations used. The FR 10 training schedule produces more responses in simple extinction than does the CRF training schedule. Therefore, if the training schedule alone were the most important variable during Sr tests, each bird in the subgroup FRIO-CRF should give more responses than does each bird in subgroup CRF-CRF, and similarly for subgroups FRIO-FRIO vs. subgroup CRF-FRIO. This is not the case (Table 2). If the Sr schedule alone were the most important variable during the Sr test, each bird in subgroup CRF-FRIO should give more responses than does each bird in subgroup CRF-CRF, and similarly for subgroups FRIO-FRIO vs. FRIO-CRF. Although this does hold for the first comparison, it does not hold for the latter. The FRIO-FRIO subgroup even showed overlap with the FRIO-No Sr subgroup. If the discrimination hypothesis holds, a 9 l0 o f1 12 13 Is SESSIONS14I Fig. 2. Median number of responses for each S' subgroup divided by the median number of responses for its control for each test session. the CRF-CRF subgroup should give more responses than each of the other two subgroups receiving CRF training; and the FRIO-FRIO subgroup should give more responses than each of the other two subgroups receiving FRlO training. This is not the case. Thus, the evidence argues against explanations on the basis of the training schedule alone, the Sr schedule alone, and the discrimination hypothesis. By all of the above comparisons, the CRF-FRIO combination of schedules is most effective. It is the only combination in which the ratio of responses to reinforcements is higher in testing than in training. It is suggested, therefore, that the most effective combination may be that in which the test schedule requires more responses per reinforcement than does the training schedule.

EFFECT OF TWO SCHEDULES 509 The schedule of Sr presentation affects the shape of the cumulative-response curve. Figure 3 presents sample records from the three most interesting subgroups: FRIO-FR1O, FR1O-CRF, and CRF-FR1O. The hatch marks indicating each Sr presentation are omitted for the FR1O-CRF subgroup. Following FR 10 training, responses tend to occur at a high rate or not at all for the subgroups FRIO-No Sr and FRIO-FR1O, whereas the responses are more evenly distributed for subgroup FR1O- CRF. For the subgroup trained on CRF and tested with Sr at FR 10, the rate is at first slow and irregular, and then becomes fast and more stable, to the extent of being a straight line for one bird. Kelleher (1961) also found that in contrast with other schedules, an FR schedule of Sr presentation produced high response rates. With the two subgroups receiving Sr at FR 10, the pauses that do occur as testing continues frequently follow an Sr presentation. These effects of scheduling imply that the conditioned stimuli are more than discriminative stimuli which simply evoke responding with each presentation. Fig. 3. Individual cumulative-response records made during the first two test sessions by birds from three different subgroups. Hatch marks indicate each Sr presentation, except in the middle record where Sr was presented at CRF. The arrows in the top record indicate pauses after Sr presentation.

510 JOAN G. STEVENSON and T. W. REESE REFERENCES Bitterman, M. E., Feddersen, W. E., and Tyler, D. W. Secondary reinforcement and the discrimination hypothesis. Amer. J. Psychol., 1953, ", 456-464. Clayton, F. L. Secondary reinforcement as a function of reinforcement scheduling. Psychol. Reports. 1956, 2, 377-380. Dinsmoor, J. A. Resistance to extinction following periodic reinforcement in the presence of a discriminative stimulus. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1952, 45, 31-35. Dinsmoor, J. A., Kish, G. B., and Keller, F. S. A comparison of the effectiveness of regular and periodic secondary reinforcement. J. gen. Psychol., 1953, ", 57-66. Ferster, C. B., and Skinner, B. F. Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1957. Kelleher, R. T. Schedules of conditioned reinforcement during experimental extinction. J. exp. Anal. Behav., 1961, 4, 1-5. Miles, R. C. Secondary-reinforcement stimulation throughout a series of spontaneous recoveries. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1956, 49, 496-498. Notterman, J. M. A study of some relations among aperiodic reinforcement, discrimi-.ation training and secondary reinforcement. J. exp. Psychol., 1951, 41, 161-169. Stevenson, Joan G. The effect of two schedules of primary and secondary reinforcement on resistance to extinction. Unpublished honors thesis, Mount Holyoke College, 1960. This thesis may be obtained from the Mount Holyoke College Library on an Interlibrary Loan. Received November 22, 1961