Introduction ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION. K.J. Stanhope C.T. Dourish

Similar documents
PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

"One-trial tolerance" to the anxiolytic actions of benzodiazepines in the elevated plus-maze, or the development of a phobic state?

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Effects of a Novel Fentanyl Derivative on Drug Discrimination and Learning in Rhesus Monkeys

Evidence for Nootropic Effect of BR-16A (Mentat), A Herbal Psychotropic Preparation, in Mice

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock

The Effects of Benzodiazepines on Ingestive Behavior. Hannah Dinnen & Ivy Far

ATTENUATION OF STRESS-INDUCED BEHAVIORAL DEFICITS BY AZADIRACHTA INDICA (NEEM): ROLE OF SEROTONIN

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

Abstract. Introduction. R.N. Takahashi 1, O. Berton 2,3, P. Mormède 2 and F. Chaouloff 2

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives?

Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

GABAergic Influences Increase Ingestion across All Taste Categories. Liz Miller. Molly McGinnis. Lindsey Richardson

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

The Muscarinic Receptor Agonist Xanomeline Has an Antipsychotic-Like Profile in the Rat

Latency to paroxetine-induced anxiolysis in the rat is reduced by co-administration of the 5-HT 1A receptor antagonist WAY100635

Supporting Online Material for

0H-DPAT mediated by 5-HT receptors?

Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: Fear

Unit 06 - Overview. Click on the any of the above hyperlinks to go to that section in the presentation.

Effects of Caffeine on Memory in Rats

A Method for Assessing Attentional Bias in Anxious Rats. A Senior Honors Thesis

VASILE HEFCO 1*, LUCIAN HRITCU 1, ADRIAN TIRON 1, ANDREEA-IOANA HEFCO 1

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

EFFECTS OF NITRIC OXIDE SYNTHASE INHIBITOR N G -NITRO-L-ARGININE METHYL ESTER ON SPATIAL AND CUED LEANING

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Emotion I: General concepts, fear and anxiety

Recent Advances in Energy, Environment, Biology and Ecology

Adolescent Prozac Exposure Enhances Sensitivity to Cocaine in Adulthood INTRODUCTION

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

1. A type of learning in which behavior is strengthened if followed by a reinforcer or diminished if followed by a punisher.

AM281, Cannabinoid Antagonist/Inverse agonist, Ameliorates Scopolamine- Induced Cognitive Deficit

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

Operant Conditioning B.F. SKINNER

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Nat Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2006 September 5.

MDMA Stimulus Generalization to the 5-HT 1A Serotonin Agonist 8-Hydroxy-2- (di-n-propylamino)tetralin

acquisition associative learning behaviorism A type of learning in which one learns to link two or more stimuli and anticipate events

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events.

Cognitive Function Test. NOR & spatial NOR task. Introduction. Novel Object Recognition (NOR) Estrogen Receptor (ER) in Brain

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

How to measure rodent behavior and perform a neurological screen.

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

BR-16A (Mentat), A Herbal Preparation, Improves Learning and Memory Performance in Mice

Efficacy and the discriminative stimulus effects of negative GABA A modulators, or inverse

Behavioral Pharmacology

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

d-amphetamine S EFFECTS ON BEHAVIOR PUNISHED BY TIME-OUT FROM POSITIVE REINFORCEMENT Emily E. Guido

Protective Effect of Mentat (BR-16A) A Herbal Preparation, on Alcohol Abstinence-Induced Anxiety and Convulsions

Previous maze experience required to increase open arms avoidance in rats submitted to the elevated plus-maze model of anxiety

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs

Development of a Touch-Screen-Based Paradigm for Assessing Working Memory in the Mouse

The effects of compounds varying in selectivity as 5-HT 1A receptor antagonists in three rat models of anxiety

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a

Science & Technologies COMPARISON THE EFFECTS OF TACRINE AND GALANTAMINE ON ACTIVE AVOIDANCE TEST IN RATS WITH DIAZEPAM-AMNESIA MODEL

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery

Key words: amphetamine; apomorphine; phencyclidine; 8-OH-DPAT; prepulse inhibition; dopamine; motor behavior; schizophrenia; rats; startle

Associative Learning

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

TITLE: Glutamate transmission enhancement for treatment of PTSD

Reports from the Research Laboratories. of the Department of Psychiatry. Self-Administration of Amphetamine and Cocaine by Rats by ROY PICKENS.

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

Chapter 7 - Learning

Phil Reed. Learn Behav (2011) 39:27 35 DOI /s Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning

Short communication NORADRENERGIC STIMULATION OF THE LATERAL HYPOTHALAMUS AS A REINFORCEMENT IN T MAZE LEARNING IN RATS

Cholinergic influence on memory stages: A study on scopolamine amnesic mice

Scopolamine Effects Under a Titrating-Delayed-

Journal of Chemical and Pharmaceutical Research

Effects of the dopamine D2 agonist, quinpirole, on time and number processing in rats

Schedule Induced Polydipsia: Effects of Inter-Food Interval on Access to Water as a Reinforcer

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training

A concurrent assessment of the positive and negative properties of a signaled shock schedule*

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Effects of d-amphetamine and Scopolamine on Activity Before and After Shock in Three Mouse Strains'

Learning. Learning is the relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience.

Psychology in Your Life

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Prevention of Development of Tolerance and Dependence to Opiate in Mice by BR-16A (Mentat) A Herbal Psychotropic Preparation

The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine

I. Classical Conditioning

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

Pharmacological Profile of BR-16A (Mentat)

PERCEPTION: Gain Control & Integration. Mark A. Geyer, Ph.D. Departments of Psychiatry & Neurosciences University of California, San Diego

Subjects. Thirty-five adult male Lister Hooded rats (Charles River, Kent, UK),

Extinction of the Context and Latent Inhibition

Conditioned Stimulus Familiarity Determines Effects of MK-801 on Fear Extinction

ASSOCIATED WITH NALORPHINE IN

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas

EFFECT OF ILLUMINATION CONDITION ON RISK ASSESSMENT BEHAVIORS OF MICE

Transcription:

Psychopharmacology (1996) 128 : 293 303 Springer-Verlag 1996 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION K.J. Stanhope C.T. Dourish Effects of 5-HT 1A receptor agonists, partial agonists and a silent antagonist on the performance of the conditioned emotional response test in the rat Received: 13 March 1996/ Final version: 8 July 1996 Abstract In the present study, the effects of 5-HT 1A receptor ligands with varying degrees of intrinsic activity at the 5-HT 1A receptor were examined in the conditioned emotional response (CER) test and their effects compared to those of the benzodiazepine receptor agonists, diazepam and chlordiazepoxide. Diazepam (3.0 mg/kg) and chlordiazepoxide (3.0 mg/kg), and the 5-HT 1A receptor partial agonists, ipsapirone (10.0 mg/ kg) and gepirone (3.0 mg/ kg), alleviated conditioned suppression of lever pressing. The 5-HT 1A receptor partial agonist, buspirone (0.1 1.0 mg/kg), the 5-HT 1A receptor agonist, 8-OH-DPAT (0.01 0.10 mg/kg), and the 5-HT 1A receptor antagonist, WAY-100635 (0.03 3.0 mg/kg), had no effects on conditioned fear. Neither enhancing the level of food deprivation nor pretreatment with the amnesic agent scopolamine induced anxiolytic-like effects in the present CER test. The anxiolytic-like effects of ipsapirone in this test were completely reversed by WAY-100635. These results indicate that 5-HT 1A agonist, but not antagonist actions, induce an anxiolytic effect in the CER test in rats. Key words Conditioned emotional response (CER) 5-HT 1A receptor agonists and antagonists WAY-100635 Anxiety Fear Introduction The role of 5-HT 1A receptors in the modulation of anxiety is controversial. Although compounds with partial agonist actions at the 5-HT 1A receptor such as the K.J. Stanhope 1 (*) C.T. Dourish Department of Neuropharmacology, Wyeth Research Ltd, Huntercombe Lane South, Taplow, Maidenhead, Bucks SL6 OPH, UK Present address: 1 Cerebrus Ltd., Ascot, Berkshire SL5 7NP, UK pyrimidinylpiperazines buspirone (Riblet et al. 1982), gepirone (Eison et al. 1986) and ipsapirone (Traber and Glaser 1987) have been shown to be anxiolytic in the clinic (Goldberg and Finnerty 1979; Eison et al. 1986; Goa and Ward 1986; Boyer and Feighner 1993), the mechanism of their anxiolytic effects has not been established. It has been suggested that 5-HT 1A receptor partial agonists may exert their actions due to 5-HT 1A receptor agonist or antagonist actions or a combination of both (Dourish et al. 1986; Dourish 1987). The availability of selective 5-HT 1A receptor antagonists such as WAY-100635 (Fletcher et al. 1995; Forster et al. 1995) may help to identify the nature of the anxiolytic action of 5-HT 1A receptor partial agonists. In the present studies, WAY-100635 was used to investigate the role of 5-HT 1A receptors in a preclinical animal model of anxiety, the conditioned emotional response (CER) test. Previous reports of the effects of 5-HT 1A receptor ligands in preclinical animal models of anxiety have been inconsistent. In models involving innate behaviours such as the light/dark box and the elevated plus maze, results have been extremely variable with the demonstration of anxiolytic, anxiogenic or no effects of 5-HT 1A receptor partial agonists (Dourish 1987; Treit 1991; Wilkinson and Dourish 1991). The effects of 5-HT 1A receptor ligands in animal models of anxiety using either conflict procedures (eg., Geller and Seifter 1960; Vogel et al. 1971) or the CER procedure (Estes and Skinner 1941; for a review see Davis 1990) have also proved inconsistent (Barrett and Witkin, 1991; Barrett and Vanover 1993), however, a number of authors have reported that 5-HT 1A receptor partial agonists have anxiolytic effects in such tests (Schefke et al. 1989; Sanger 1990, 1992; Barrett and Witkin, 1991; Charrier et al. 1994). Although Barrett and colleagues (1991, 1993) argue that the most consistent anxiolytic-like effects of 5-HT 1A receptor partial agonists are observed in conflict tests in pigeons, Sanger (1990, 1992), reported robust anxiolytic-like effects with ipsapirone in the CER test in the rat (see also

294 Rittenhouse et al. 1992). Similarly, in another rodent model which employs a CER procedure, the fear potentiated startle paradigm, consistent anxiolytic effects have been reported with buspirone (Davis 1993; see also Mansbach and Geyer 1988). Given that CER procedures appear sensitive to the anxiolytic-like effects of 5-HT 1A receptor partial agonists, a CER procedure was used here to examine the effects of 5-HT 1A receptor ligands with varying degrees of intrinsic activity. As mentioned above, functionally, 5-HT 1A receptor partial agonists could exert their anxiolytic effects due to either agonist (Dourish et al. 1986) or antagonist actions (Dourish 1987). Depending on the assay system that is employed, buspirone, ipsapirone, gepirone, or the aminotetralin 8-hydroxy- 2-(di-n-propylamino)tetralin (8-OH-DPAT) which is generally regarded as a full 5-HT 1A receptor agonist, can display either agonist or antagonist effects (Colino and Halliwell 1987; O Connor et al. 1990; Andrade 1992; Beck et al. 1992). To determine whether agonist or antagonist effects of 5-HT 1A receptor partial agonists induce anxiolytic-like effects, the present series of studies compared the effects of 8-OH-DPAT, ipsapirone, gepirone and buspirone to the effects of the selective and silent 5-HT 1A receptor antagonist, WAY- 100635 (Fletcher et al. 1995; Forster et al. 1995). Before commencing the 5-HT 1A receptor studies we undertook a series of experiments to characterise and validate the CER test, as conducted in our laboratory, as an anxiety model. In the present CER test the conditioned stimulus was occasionally, as opposed to consistently, paired with mild electric footshock. Reducing the predictability of shock has been claimed to change the sensitivity of conflict tests to anxiolytic agents (e.g., Costello et al. 1991). Thus, in order to interpret the effects of the 5-HT 1A receptor ligands, we also examined the effects of two prototypical anxiolytic agents, the benzodiazepine receptor agonists diazepam and chlordiazepoxide. In addition, in order to determine whether false positive effects may be obtained in the present test due to the enhancement of motivation (see Treit 1984), the effects of food deprivation on performance were also examined and compared to the effects of the putative anxiolytic drugs. Finally, to determine whether any cognitive disrupting effects of drugs might induce false positive effects in the present test, the effects of pretreatment with the amnesic agent scopolamine was examined. Materials and methods Subjects Fifty-four male Lister Hooded rats with an average free-feeding weight of 225 g (experiments with chlordiazepoxide, diazepam and scopolamine), 56 male Lister Hooded rats, with a mean free-feeding weight of 264 g (experiment with gepirone) and 56 male Lister Hooded rats with a mean free-feeding weight of 208 g (experiments on food deprivation, buspirone, ipsapirone, WAY-100635, 8-OH- DPAT and WAY-100635 versus ipsapirone) were fed a restricted diet designed to maintain rats at greater than 85% of their initial body weight. Rats were housed in cages of four and fed approximately 55 g of food per cage per day (experiment with chlordiazepoxide, diazepam and scopolamine). In the experiments conducted with the two subsequent groups of rats, the animals were fed approximately 60 g of food per cage per day. A 12:12 light/dark cycle was in effect in the colony room and the rats were trained and tested during the light part of the cycle. Ethics The research described in this manuscript was conducted within the guidelines of the Home Office regulations as outlined in the Animal (Scientific Procedures) Act, 1986, and was approved by an internal ethics committee. Apparatus Eight Colbourn operant chambers with associated Colbourn pellet dispensers and operant levers were used. These chambers were housed in outer sound and light attenuating shells equipped with a ventilation fan which also helped to mask external noise. A single operant lever was positioned on the left side of the front panel of the operant chamber, approximately 2 cm above the grid floor. Colbourn animal test cage grid floor shockers were used to deliver shock (0.4 ma, 0.5 s) to the grid floors (model E-10 10SF). Formula P Noyes food pellets (45 mg) could be delivered to the food magazine centered on the front wall of the chamber approximately 2 cm above the floor. A houselight (4.0 ma, 28 V) was positioned on the front wall of the chamber, above the food magazine, approximately 1.0 cm below the ceiling. The operant chambers were controlled and the number of lever presses recorded by a PDP-11 minicomputer programmed in SKED. The data collected from the PDP-11 were electronically transferred to a VAX computer for statistical analysis using SAS software. Procedure Preliminary training Once the rats had been placed on a restricted feeding regime, magazine training began. The rats received three to four daily 30-min sessions of magazine training during which pellets were delivered on a random time (RT) 30 s schedule. Rats were then given two to four daily sessions of lever press training for food pellets during which each lever press resulted in the delivery of a food pellet (continuous reinforcement or CRF training). These sessions lasted until either 30 min expired or 30 food pellets were delivered. Rats which did not lever press during these initial sessions received additional training. Over the next three or four daily sessions, the session length was increased from 30 to 60 min and the schedule on which the pellets were delivered was changed from a random interval 30 s schedule (RI 30 s) to a RI 90 s schedule. On the RI 90 s schedule pellets could be earned by pressing the lever, on average, once every 90 s. The houselight was off throughout all of the training sessions. Conditioned emotional response training During these sessions, which were usually conducted on 4 days per week, food pellets continued to be available on an RI 90 s schedule as before. However, the rats were now presented with

295 2-min long periods of houselight illumination. The first light presentation occurred approximately 20 min after the start of the session (range 15 25 min) and the second light presentation occurred approximately 20 min after the first light presentation (range 15 25 min). Light presentations were occasionally followed by a 0.5-s 0.4-mA footshock. The session ended 15 min after the second light presentation. In a given session, footshock could follow neither, one, or both of the light presentations. Overall, shock occurred following approximately one in three light presentations. In order to maintain reliable conditioned suppression, at least one light shock pairing occurred in a block of three consecutive sessions. The rate of lever-pressing during each of the light presentations (the during period ) and the rate of lever-pressing in the 2-min interval preceding each light presentation (the pre period ) was recorded. These response rates were used to calculate suppression ratios using the following formula: Response rate during Response rate pre + Response rate during A suppression ratio of 0 indicates that the light has evoked conditioned fear and has completely suppressed lever pressing, whereas a suppression ratio of 0.5 indicates the complete absence of fear: the response rate is unchanged by the light presentation. Once a reliable suppression ratio was observed, occasional test sessions occurred. Test sessions were identical to training sessions except that footshock was never delivered following the first light presentation and treatment with either vehicle or a putative anxiolytic compound occurred before the session began. Testing took place no more frequently than once per week and at least two baseline training sessions occurred before each test session. Prior to test sessions,rats which had suppression ratios of greater than 0.15 during the previous baseline session were excluded from the experiment. The remaining rats were semi-randomly assigned to groups such that the groups were matched for the rate of lever pressing and the level of conditioned suppression displayed during baseline training. Drug treatment in each study was orthogonal to drug treatment which may have been administered to the animals in the previous study. In the food deprivation study the animals were deprived of food for 4, 8, 18 or 23 h prior to testing. Statistical analysis A mixed within-subject (trials) and between-subjects (drug treatment) analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted separately on the pre and during light response rates and on the suppression ratios. Significant interactions were followed by one-way analysis of variance on the data collected for each trial. Significant group effects were analysed using post-hoc Student Newman-Keuls tests. In experiments in which significant Treatment Trial interactions were identified, the data for each trial are illustrated on the appropriate figure. Otherwise, the data for each experiment are presented collapsed across the two test trials. Data are graphically presented only in experiments in which significant effects were detected. For all statistical analyses, alpha was set to 0.05. Suppression ratios could not be calculated if no lever presses occurred during the pre light period. Thus, animals which failed to respond during either the first or the second light presentation were excluded from the analysis of suppression ratios. The size of the groups and the number of animals contributing to each analysis are shown in Table 1. Drugs The centrally and peripherally acting muscarinic receptor antagonist scopolamine HBr (0.015, 0.03, and 0.06), was used. A higher dose of scopolamine could not be examined as results from a pilot experiment found that only two out of seven rats treated with 0.1 mg/kg scopolamine HBr were able to lever press. To control for any peripheral effects of scopolamine, a control group treated with a 0.06 mg/ kg dose of the mainly peripherally acting muscarinic Table 1 Number of subjects (n) allotted to groups and included in ANOVA of Pre and Dur response rates (Pre/ Dur) and suppression ratios (Ratio) Experiment Group Pre/ Dur Ratio Experiment Group Pre/ Dur Ratio (n) (n) (n) (n) CDP Vehicle 10 10 Deprivation 4 h 11 11 1.0 mg/kg 9 9 8 h 11 11 3.0 mg/kg 10 10 16 h 14 14 10 mg/kg 9 9 23 h 14 14 Diazepam Vehicle 8 7 Scopolamine Vehicle 10 10 1.0 mg/kg 8 6 0.015 mg/kg 9 9 3.0 mg/kg 8 7 0.03 mg/kg 9 9 10 mg/kg 9 6 0.06 mg/kg 9 9 30 mg/kg 8 0 MeBr 9 9 Ipsapirone Vehicle 9 9 8-OH-DPAT Vehicle 8 7 1.25 mg/kg 10 10 0.01 mg/kg 10 9 2.5 mg/kg 10 10 0.03 mg/kg 8 8 5.0 mg/kg 10 10 0.1 mg/kg 10 4 10.0 mg/kg 9 9 Ipsapirone 10 9 Gepirone Vehicle 6 6 Buspirone Vehicle 11 11 1.0 mg/kg 6 6 0.1 mg/kg 10 10 3.0 mg/kg 5 5 0.3 mg/kg 11 11 Ipsapirone 7 7 1.0 mg/kg 10 5 Ipsapirone 11 10 WAY-100635 Vehicle 11 11 WAY-100635 Veh/ Veh 9 8 0.03 mg/kg 11 11 versus Veh/ ipsapirone 10 10 0.3 mg/kg 11 11 Ipsapirone 0.03/ ipsapirone 10 9 3.0 mg/kg 12 12 0.3/ ipsapirone 10 10 Ipsapirone 11 7 3.0/ ipsapirone 10 8

296 antagonist, scopolamine MeBr was included. Scopolamine HBr and MeBr (supplied by Sigma) were administered in saline vehicle in a volume of 1 ml/kg. Diazepam (purchased from Becpharm, 1, 3, 10 or 30 mg/kg) was administered in 0.3% HPMC vehicle and a dosing volume of 1 ml/ kg. Chlordiazepoxide HCl (1.0, 3.0 and 10.0 mg/ kg dissolved in saline) obtained from Sigma was administered in a volume of 1 ml/kg. Buspirone (0.1, 0.3 and 1.0 mg/kg administered in saline), ipsapirone (1.25, 2.5, 5.0 and 10.0 mg/ kg administered in 0.3% HPMC), gepirone (1, 3.0 and 10.0 mg/kg administered in 0.3% HPMC), WAY-100635 (0.03, 0.3, 3.0 mg/ kg administered in saline) and 8-OH-DPAT (0.003, 0.01 0.3 and 0.1 mg/kg administered in saline) were made in the Department of Chemistry, Wyeth Research (UK) Ltd and were administered in a dosing volume of 2 ml/kg. A higher dose of 0.3 mg/kg of 8-OH- DPAT could not be used as rats treated with this dose of 8-OH- DPAT in a pilot study did not lever press. All doses are expressed as base weight and all compounds were administered subcutaneously 30 min prior to testing with the exception of 8-OH-DPAT which was administered with a 5-min pretreatment time. The ph of drugs was adjusted to neutral when necessary using NaOH. In order to facilitate between experiment comparisons and to demonstrate the reliability and sensitivity of the present CER test to anxiolytic-like drug effects, a positive control group given ipsapirone (10.0 mg/ kg, 30-min pretreatment time) was included in the experiments evaluating buspirone, gepirone, 8-OH-DPAT and WAY-100635. Due to the difference in pretreatment time between 8-OH-DPAT and ipsapirone, to ensure that both the 8-OH-DPAT and the ipsapirone treated groups had injections at the same time prior to testing (30 min and 5 min), all animals in the experiment assessing the effects of 8-OH-DPAT received two injections: 0.3% HPMC vehicle and saline vehicle; 0.3% HPMC vehicle and 8-OH-DPAT; or ipsapirone and saline vehicle with the first of the two injections occurring 30 min prior to testing and the second injection occurring 5 min prior to testing. Fig. 1A D Effects of chlordiazepoxide (SC) on mean lever press rates before (open bars) and during (shaded bars) the light presentations (top panels) and on suppression ratios (lower panels). Data for trial 1 are presented in panels A and B and data for trial 2 are presented in panels C and D. Mean ± SEM. *Significantly different relative to the vehicle treated control group Results Chlordiazepoxide Chlordiazepoxide did not significantly affect pre CS response rates (see Fig. 1A, C). Two-way ANOVA on these scores found no significant effect of group nor a significant Group Trial interaction, Fs < 1.0. However, chlordiazepoxide did significantly enhance response rates during the CS presentation. Two-way ANOVA on these scores revealed a significant effect of group, F(3,34) = 4.47,and a significant Group Trial interaction, F(3,34) = 3.97. One-way ANOVA of the during CS scores separately on trial 1 and trial 2 found a significant group difference on both trials, F(3,34) = 4.02 and F(3,34) = 4.86, respectively. Post hoc SNK tests revealed that on trial 1 the vehicle treated group differed significantly from the 10.0 mg/ kg treated group. On trial 2, both the 10 and the 3 mg/kg treated group differed significantly from the vehicle treated group. Two-way ANOVA on the suppression ratios revealed a significant effect of group, F(3,34) = 8.75, but the Group Trial interaction failed to reach statistical significance, F(3,34) = 2.83, P = 0.0532 (see Fig. 1B, D). Post hoc SNK tests revealed that both the 3 and the 10.0 mg/kg treated groups had higher suppression ratios than those observed in the vehicle treated group. Fig. 2A D Effects of diazepam and hours of food deprivation on lever press rates before (open bars) and during (shaded bars) the light presentation (panels A and C, respectively) and on suppression ratios (panels B and D, respectively). Mean ± SEM. * Significantly different relative to the vehicle treated control group (panels A and B) or relative to the 4-h food deprived group (panels C and D) Diazepam Due to the sedative effects of the high dose of diazepam, only three out of eight of the rats treated with 30.0 mg/kg diazepam completed at least one response during both the pre and the dur CS periods on both trials. The data for the group treated with this dose of diazepam were therefore excluded from the statistical analysis of suppression ratios. The results for this experiment are displayed in Fig. 2A, B.

297 Two-way ANOVA on the pre CS scores found a significant effect of Group, F(4,36) = 3.92, and the Group Trial interaction failed to reach statistical significance, F(4,36) = 1.61. Post hoc testing on the mean pre scores revealed that the group treated with 30 mg/kg had a lower response rate than did the vehicle treated group. Two-way ANOVA on the during CS scores, however, revealed a significant group effect, F(4,36) = 3.11, but not a Group Trial interaction, F(4,36) = 1.53. Post hoc tests on the group effect revealed that the group treated with 3.0 mg/kg had a higher during CS response rate than did the vehicle treated group (see Fig. 2A). One rat in both the vehicle and the 3.0 mg/kg treated groups, 2 rats in the 1.0 mg/kg treated group, and three rats in the 10.0 mg/kg treated groups did not respond on both the dur and the pre periods on both trials. Due to the empty data cells, the results for these animals were excluded from the analysis of suppression ratios. Two-way ANOVA on the suppression ratios in the remaining animals revealed a significant group effect, F(3,22) = 7.44, but the Group Trial interaction was not statistically significant, F < 1.0. The post hoc SNK tests revealed that the groups treated with both 10 and 3.0 mg/kg of diazepam had higher suppression ratios than the vehicle treated group (see Fig. 2B). Scopolamine Scopolamine had no significant effects on any of the behavioural measures. Mean pre light response rates (± SEM) for the vehicle, 0.015, 0.03, 0.06 mg/kg scopolamine HBr and the 0.06 mg/kg scopolamine MeBr treated groups were 18.0 (± 3.5), 11.4 (± 1.5), 16.8 (± 3.9), 10.7 (± 2.8) and 16.3 (± 2.0), respectively. The mean during light response rates (± SE mean) were 5.6 (± 3.2), 1.8 (± 0.7), 4.2 (± 2.1), 3.6 (± 1.8) and 2.3 (± 0.7) for the vehicle, 0.015, 0.03 and 0.06 mg/kg treated scopolamine HBr treated groups and the 0.06 mg/ kg scopolamine MeBr treated group, respectively. Finally the suppression ratios (± SEM) were 0.19 (± 0.07), 0.12 (± 0.04), 0.11 (± 0.05), 0.16 (± 0.05) and 0.11 (± 0.02) for the vehicle, 0.015, 0.03, 0.06 mg/kg scopolamine HBr and the 0.06 mg/kg scopolamine MeBr treated groups, respectively. Two-way ANOVA on the group effect failed to find a significant difference, F(4,41) = 1.34, or a significant Group Trial interaction, F < 1. Two-way ANOVA on the during scores and the suppression ratios also failed to reveal any group differences, nor a significant Group Trial interaction, Fs 1.08. Food deprivation Two-way analysis of variance on the pre light scores with drug treatment and trials entered as factors found a significant group effect, F(3,46) = 9.37, but not a significant Trial Dose interaction, F < 1. A post hoc SNK test on the mean pre response rates found that both the 23- and the 16-h food deprived groups responded more than did the 8- and 4-h deprived animals (see Fig. 2C). Although increasing deprivation affected pre light response rates this manipulation did not significantly affect responding during the light. Two-way ANOVA on the during light scores revealed no effect of group, F(3,46) = 2.12, nor a reliable Group Trial interaction, F(3,46) = 1.68. Similarly, changing the deprivation state Fig. 3A D Effects of ipsapirone and 8-OH-DPAT on lever press rates before (open bars) and during (shaded bars) the light presentation (panels A and C, respectively) and on suppression ratios (panels B and D, respectively). Mean ± SEM. * Significantly different relative to the vehicle treated control group

298 did not significantly affect suppression ratios (see Fig. 2D). ANOVA on the suppression ratios did not reveal a reliable group effect, F(3,46) = 2.60, nor a reliable Group Trial interaction, F(3,46) = 1.49. Although the group effect approached statistical significance (P = 0.06), the largest group difference was between the 8- and the 23-h deprived groups with the 4- and 23-h groups having the highest suppression ratios. Ipsapirone Ipsapirone did not significantly suppress pre light response rates (see Fig. 3A). The group effect was not statistically significant, F(4,43) = 1.26, nor was the Group Trial interaction statistically significance, F(4,43) = 1.73. The during light response rates, however, did vary among the drug treatment groups, F(4,43) = 8.32, although the Group Trial interaction did not reach statistical significance, F(4,43) = 1.48. Post hoc testing revealed that the group treated with 10.0 mg/kg ipsapirone responded more during the light presentation than did the vehicle treated group. Analysis of the suppression ratios revealed a significant effect of group, F(4,43) = 18.50, and the Group Trial interaction was not statistically significant, F < 1. Post hoc analysis revealed that the group treated with 10.0 mg/kg ipsapirone had a higher suppression ratio than did the vehicle treated group (see Fig. 3B). 8-OH-DPAT Administration of 8-OH-DPAT did not significantly affect pre light response rates (see Fig. 3C). There was no significant group effect nor a significant Group Trial interaction, Fs(4,41) 1.26. There was a significant drug treatment effect on dur CS scores, F(4,41) = 5.01, and the interaction did not reach statistical significance, F(4,41) = 1.18. The ipsapirone treated group responded more during the CS period than did the vehicle treated group. Analysis of variance on the suppression ratios (see Fig. 3D) revealed a significant group effect, F(4,32) = 4.71, but the Group Trial interaction did not reach statistical significance, F(4,32) = 1.85. Post hoc testing revealed that only the group treated with ipsapirone had a higher suppression ratio than did the vehicle treated group. a significant Drug Trial interaction was observed, F(3,20) = 5.09. A subsequent one-way ANOVA on the group effect on the Pre light scores for the first (see Fig. 4A) and second light presentation (see Fig. 4C) did not reveal the source of this interaction, Fs(3,20) = 2.30 and 0.25, respectively, although the interaction appears to due to the larger trend for a reduction in pre light response rates on trial 1 than on trial 2. There was a statistically significant main effect of drug treatment on during light response rates, F(3,20) = 5.58. The Group Trial interaction did not reach statistical significance, F < 1. The post hoc test of the during light scores indicated that both the 3.0 mg/kg treated group and the ipsapirone treated group had significantly higher during light response rates than did the vehicle treated group. Drug treatment significantly affected the suppression ratios, F(3,20) = 18.18, and the Trial Group interaction did not reach statistical significance, F < 1. The ipsapirone and the gepirone (3.0 mg/ kg) treated groups had significantly higher suppression ratios than did the vehicle treated group (see Fig. 4B, D). Buspirone Buspirone did not induce an anxiolytic-like profile in this test (see Fig. 5). Two-way ANOVA on the pre light rates of lever pressing revealed a significant main effect of drug treatment, F(4,48) = 4.42, and the Drug Trial interaction was also statistically significant, F(4,48) = 3.21. One-way ANOVA on both trial 1 and trial 2 (see Fig. 5A, C) revealed significant group effects, Fs(4,48) = 5.02 and 3.53, respectively. Post hoc tests revealed Gepirone The animals treated with the highest dose of gepirone (10.0 mg/kg) showed complete abolition of lever pressing. Therefore, the results for this group were excluded from the statistical analyses. In the remaining groups, drug treatment did not induce a significant overall suppression in pre light response rates, F(3,20) = 1.06, but Fig. 4A D Effects of gepirone on mean lever press rates before (open bars) and during (shaded bars) the light presentations (top panels) and on suppression ratios (lower panels). Data for trial 1 are presented in panels A and B and data for trial 2 are presented in panels C and D. Mean ± SEM. *Significantly different relative to the vehicle treated control group

299 Fig. 5A D Effects of buspirone (SC) on mean lever press rates before (open bars) and during (shaded bars) the light presentations (top panels) and on suppression ratios (lower panels). Data for trial 1 are presented in panels A and B and data for trial 2 are presented in panels C and D. Mean ± SEM. * Significantly different relative to the vehicle treated control group that on trial 1 both the ipsapirone and the 1.0 mg/kg buspirone treated groups displayed a lower rate of lever pressing than did the vehicle treated animals. On trial 2, only the group treated with 1.0 mg/kg buspirone was suppressed during pre light period relative to the vehicle treated group. Two-way ANOVA on the during light response rates revealed a significant group effect on trial 1, F(4,48) = 4.78, although the Group Trial interaction was not statistically significant, F(4,48) = 1.36. Post hoc testing showed that only the rats in the ipsapirone group responded with a higher rate of lever pressing than the vehicle treated rats during the light presentation. Two-way ANOVA on the suppression ratios revealed a significant effect of group, F(4,42) = 17.50. The Group Trial interaction was also statistically significant, F(4,42) = 3.05. A significant group difference was observed on both trial 1 and 2, Fs(4,42) = 8.70, and 20.05(see Fig. 5B, D, respectively). The suppression ratio in the ipsapirone treated rats was significantly higher than for the vehicle treated rats on trial 1 and trial 2. The animals treated with the highest dose of buspirone also had a suppression ratio that was significantly higher than the vehicle treated group on trial 2. However, as indicated by the pre and dur light response rates, this increase in suppression ratio induced by buspirone is due to a decrease in pre light scores as opposed to an increase in during light response rates and should not be misinterpreted as an anxiolytic-like effect. WAY-100635 At the doses tested, WAY-100635 had no significant effects on the performance of lever pressing in the present test. Two-way ANOVA on pre CS scores did not reveal a group effect, F(4,51) = 1.54; however, the Group Trial interaction did achieve statistical significance, F(4,51) = 3.20. One-way ANOVA on each trial revealed a significant group difference on trial 1, F(4,51) = 2.89, but not on trial 2, F < 1 (see Fig. 6A and C, respectively). On trial 1 the rats treated with ipsapirone had a lower pre CS response rate than did the vehicle treated rats. Two-way ANOVA on the during CS scores revealed a reliable group effect, F(4,51) = 3.49. The Group Trial interaction failed to reach statistical significance, Fig. 6A D Effects of WAY-100635 (SC) on mean lever press rates before (open bars) and during (shaded bars) the light presentations (top panels) and on suppression ratios (lower panels). Data for trial 1 are presented in panels A and B and data for trial 2 are presented in panels C and D. Mean ± SEM. *Significantly different relative to the vehicle treated control group

300 F(4,51) = 1.89. Post hoc testing on the group effect revealed that the ipsapirone group had a higher during CS response rate than did the vehicle treated group. WAY-100635 did not affect suppression ratios (see Fig. 6B, D). A significant main effect of group, F(4,47) = 16.9, but not a significant Group Trial interaction, F < 1, was observed when a two-way ANOVA was conducted on the suppression ratios. Post hoc testing revealed that only the group treated with ipsapirone had significantly higher suppression ratio than did the vehicle treated group. Two-way ANOVA on the suppression ratios revealed a significant effect of group, F(4,40) = 18.47, and the Group Trial interaction failed to reach statistical significance, F(4,40) = 2.52. Post hoc testing on the group effect revealed that the group treated with ipsapirone alone had a suppression ratio significantly higher than the ipsapirone +0.03 mg/ kg WAY-100635 group which in turn had a suppression ratio significantly higher than all the other treatment groups (see Fig. 7B). WAY-100635 versus ipsapirone The anxiolytic-like effects of ipsapirone (10.0 mg/ kg) were dose dependently reversed by WAY-100635. Twoway ANOVA on the pre CS scores revealed no significant group effect, F(4,44) = 1.75, nor a significant Group Trial interaction, F(4,44) = 1.69. Two-way ANOVA on the during CS scores did reveal a significant group effect, F(4,44) = 10.47, but the Group Trial interaction failed to reach statistical significance, F(4,44) = 2.15. Post hoc testing revealed that the group treated with ipsapirone alone or ipsapirone +0.03 mg/ kg WAY-100635 differed significantly from all the other treatment groups. Thus, 0.3 and 3.0 mg/ kg WAY- 100635 completely reversed the ipsapirone induced increase in dur CS responding (see Fig. 7A). Fig. 7A,B Effects of WAY-100635 and ipsapirone (SC) on lever press rates before (open bars) and during (shaded bars) the light presentation (panel A) and on suppression ratios (panel B). Mean ± SEM. * Significantly different relative to the vehicle/ vehicle treated control group. + Significantly different relative to the ipsapirone/ vehicle treated group Discussion The benzodiazepine receptor agonists, chlordiazepoxide (3.0 mg/kg) and diazepam (3.0 mg/kg), and the 5-HT 1A receptor partial agonists, ipsapirone (10.0 mg/kg) and gepirone (3.0 mg/kg), induced anxiolytic-like effects in the CER test. However, within the range of doses tested, 8-OH-DPAT, buspirone and WAY-100635 failed to induce an anxiolytic-like effect in this test. At the highest doses, 8-OH-DPAT and buspirone significantly suppressed responding for food pellets, presumably due to induction of the behavioural 5-HT syndrome (Tricklebank 1987) although the suppression of lever pressing induced by buspirone may also be due to its D 2 receptor antagonist effects (see Ryan et al. 1993). Clearly, the anxiolytic-like actions of drugs which disrupt lever pressing are difficult to assess in this model. By contrast, WAY-100635 had no effect on any of the behavioural measures in the CER test. The failure to observe a behavioural effect of WAY- 100635 cannot be due to an insufficient dose: WAY- 100635 was administered at doses which are know to be sufficient to block the 5-HT behavioural syndrome, hypothermia, hyperphagia and the disruption of lever pressing for food induced by 8-OH-DPAT (Fletcher et al. 1995). Moreover, WAY-100635 was found to block the anxiolytic action of ipsapirone. It therefore appears that a silent 5-HT 1A receptor antagonist, in contrast to the partial agonists, ipsapirone and gepirone, is inactive in the CER test. The present failure to observe an anxiolytic-like effect with WAY-100635 contrasts with previous reports of anxiolytic-like actions of the 5-HT 1A receptor antagonists WAY-100135, WAY-100635 and (S)-UH-301 in the light/dark box in mice, and elevated maze test in rats and mice (Moreau et al. 1992; Bill and Fletcher 1994a,b; Rodgers and Cole 1994). In contrast to the data obtained in these ethological models of anxiety, Charrier et al. (1994) failed to observe an anxiolyticlike action of WAY-100135 in the safety signal withdrawal conflict procedure in the rat. Perhaps the most straightforward explanation for the discrepancy between the results from these various studies is due to differences in the models used. Positive effects were obtained with 5-HT 1A receptor antagonists in

301 ethological models measuring innate or unlearned fear whereas negative effects were observed in conditioning models employing learned fear. It has previously been suggested that the type of model used to measure responses to 5-HT 1A receptor ligands appears critical to the determination of their anxiolytic effects. Treit et al. (1993), for example, reported that equivalent doses of 8-OH-DPAT induced an anxiogenic effect in the rat elevated plus maze test and an anxiolytic effect in the rat shock-probe burying test in which a probe is associated with electric shock. Although we have consistently found anxiolytic-like effects with ipsapirone in the CER test, unpublished work conducted by Savraj Grewal in our laboratory has found that acute or chronic treatment with ipsapirone produces either anxiogenic or sedative effects in the rat elevated maze test. The clinical implications of the positive effects obtained in the ethological tests and the negative results obtained with 5-HT 1A receptor antagonists in learned anxiety tests is unknown. It is clear, however, that determination of the effects of 5-HT 1A receptor antagonists in the clinic will have important implications for the predictive validity of both ethological and conditioned fear models of anxiety in animals. Interestingly, Charrier et al. (1994) reported that the 5-HT 1A receptor antagonist WAY-100135 failed to block the anxiolytic action of buspirone in the safety signal withdrawal model, questioning the pharmacological specificity of the anxiolytic effects of buspirone in their test. However, the 5-HT 1A receptor antagonist, WAY-100635, which is 100-fold selective relative to binding at other major neurotransmitter receptors (see Forster et al. 1995), completely and dose-dependently reversed the anxiolytic-like effects of ipsapirone in the present test. This is consistent with previous results from studies using non-selective 5-HT 1A receptor antagonists (Chojnacka-Wojcik and Przegalinski 1991) and provides compelling evidence that the anxiolytic-like actions of ipsapirone are due to its agonist effects at the 5-HT 1A receptor. Given that both ipsapirone and gepirone induced an anxiolytic-like effect, it is curious that 8-OH-DPAT and buspirone were inactive in the present test. As mentioned above, the failure to observe an anxiolytic-like effect with buspirone could be due to its D 2 receptor antagonist properties which may disrupt lever pressing and thereby mask any anxiolytic-like effect due to its 5-HT 1A receptor agonist actions. However, the disruption of lever pressing induced by 8-OH-DPAT is almost certainly due to the induction of the 5-HT behavioural syndrome, an effect which is also mediated by an agonist action at the 5-HT 1A receptor (Tricklebank 1987). It is unclear why ipsapirone and gepirone, but not 8- OH-DPAT, appear able to alleviate conditioned suppression of responding at doses lower than those which induce the 5-HT behavioural syndrome and thereby impair the animal s ability to perform the test. Our results are, however, similar to those obtained by Sanger who observed robust anxiolytic-like effects with ipsapirone, but no evidence of an anxiolytic-like effect of 8-OH-DPAT in a CER test (Sanger 1990) and a punishment procedure (Sanger 1992). The CER test as conducted here has certain advantages over other CER tests in which the conditioned stimulus is consistently paired with shock and over conflict tests such as the Geller-Seifter (see Treit 1984), as it allows for the anxiolytic activity of drugs to be examined in the absence of any potential confounding analgesic effects. This is because on drug test days the behavioural data are collected before the animal is exposed to the aversive footshock. However, another potential confound in most shock-induced response suppression tests that are used to screen for anxiolytic drugs is the effect of drugs on appetite or satiety mechanisms. As pointed out by Treit (1984), conflict tests, by definition, involve crossed motivations (e.g., the attainment of food or water and the avoidance of shock (p. 209). Therefore, compounds such as benzodiazepines and 5-HT 1A receptor partial agonists which are know to increase feeding in rodents (e.g. Dourish et al., 1985a, b; Cooper, 1989), may induce false positives in a conflict or a conditioned suppression model by enhancing the animal motivation to lever press for food (e.g., Leslie, 1977). Due to ceiling effects in baseline response rates, enhanced motivation could lead to an increase in the rate of responding during the signal for shock with no effect on response rates when the signal is off. This pattern of results could be misinterpreted as an anxiolytic effect. It seems unlikely that enhanced motivation to respond for food can account for the anxiolytic-like effects of ipsapirone observed in the present studies. Ipsapirone (10.0 mg/kg) did not increase pre light rates of lever pressing in any of the present experiments including those experiments in which the baseline response rates were clearly submaximal (e.g., see Figs. 3C and 7A). By contrast, enhancing the level of food deprivation from 4 to 23 h resulted in a significant increase in pre, but not during light rates of lever pressing. Thus, the ipspairone induced increase in lever pressing during the light signal for shock in the present paradigm appears to be independent of any effects of the drug on motivation to respond for food. It is also possible that memory disrupting drugs may induce false positive anxiolytic-like effects in the CER test. Both benzodiazepine and 5-HT 1A receptor agonists have been claimed to have cognitive disrupting effects (e.g., Hunter and Roberts 1987; Winter and Petti 1987; Preston et al. 1989) and could induce the forgetting of conditioned fear. This explanation seems unlikely given that the CER procedure involves simple associative learning that is well established prior to the commencement of drug testing (see Hoffman et al. 1963; Hendersen 1985). Moreover, the amnesic agent scopolamine has not been found to produce a false positive anxiolytic-like effects in a CER test (e.g.,

302 Miczek 1973). However, the unpredictable nature of shock in the present CER test could make it more sensitive to the amnesic effects of drugs than traditional CER tests such as that conducted by Miczek (1973). To determine whether an amnesic agent would produce an anxiolytic-like effect in the present studies, we examined the effects of the muscarinic receptor antagonist, scopolamine HBr, which has well established cognitive disrupting effects (Weingartner 1985; e.g., Preston et al. 1988). When administered at doses up to 0.06 mg/kg, scopolamine HBr had no effects on conditioned suppression of responding. However, in cognitive experiments conducted in our laboratory doses as low as 0.025 mg/kg scopolamine has been shown to disrupt the performance of working memory in a delayed matching to position task (see Stanhope et al. 1995). By contrast, unpublished results from the delayed matching-to-position task conducted in our laboratory have failed to find any memory impairing effects of ipsapirone when administered up to a dose of 10 mg/kg (for a similar result see Cole et al. 1994). Given that scopolamine appears to have a more pronounced cognitive disruptive effect than ipsapirone, yet ipsapirone, but not scopolamine induced an anxiolyticlike effect in the present studies, it seems unlikely that this anxiolytic-like effect is due to the forgetting of conditioned fear. In conclusion, the present studies show that 5-HT 1A receptor partial agonists but not a full 5-HT 1A receptor agonist or a silent 5-HT 1A receptor antagonist have anxiolytic effects in the present CER model. These anxiolytic-like effects are not secondary to changes in the sensitivity of the animals to the aversive stimulus, changes in motivation to respond for food, or the induction of amnesia resulting in the forgetting of conditioned fear. Most importantly, the anxiolytic action of the 5-HT 1A receptor partial agonist ipsapirone appears to be mediated by its agonist action at 5-HT 1A receptors as it was dose-dependently reversed by pretreatment with the selective 5-HT 1A antagonist WAY-100635. WAY-100635 does not display an anxiolytic-like effect in the CER test and this result questions the generality of the positive results obtained with 5-HT 1A receptor antagonists in ethological models of anxiety (Moreau et al. 1992, Bill and Fletcher 1994a,b; Rodgers and Cole 1994). References Andrade R (1992) Electrophysiology of 5-HT 1A receptors in the rat hippocampus and cortex. Drug Dev Res 26: 275 286 Barrett JE, Vanover K (1993) 5-HT receptors as targets for the development of novel anxiolytic drugs: models, mechansism and future directions. Psychopharmacology 112: 1 12 Barrett JE, Witkin JM (1991) Buspirone in animal models of anxiety. In: Tunnicliff G, Eison AS, Taylor DP (eds) Buspirone mechanisms and clinical aspects. Academic Press, London, pp 37 39 Beck SG, Choi KC, List TJ (1992) Comparison of 5-hydroxytryptamine1A-mediated hyperpolarization in CA1 and CA3 hippocampal pyramidal cells. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 263: 350 359 Bill DJ, Fletcher A (1994a) Correlation of in vivo functional and anxiolytic effects of 5-HT 1A receptor ligands in the mouse. Br J Pharmacol 111: 151P Bill DJ, Fletcher A (1994b) A comparison of the potencies of the novel silent 5-HT 1A antagonist, WAY-100635, in functional and anxiety models. Behav Pharmacol 5 [Supplement 1]:77 Boyer WF, Feighner JP (1993) A placebo-controlled doubleblind multicenter trial of 2 doses of ipsapirone versus diazepam in generalized anxiety disorder. Int Clin Psychopharm 8: 173 176 Charrier D, Dangoumau L, Hamon M, Puech AJ, Thiebot MH (1994) Effects of 5-HT 1A receptor ligands on a safety signal withdrawal procedure of conflict in the rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 48: 281 289 Chojnacka-Wojcik E, Przegalinski E (1991) Evidence for the involvement of 5-HT 1A receptors in the anticonflict effect of ipsapirone in rats. Neuropharmacology 30: 703 709 Cole BJ, Jones GH, Turner JD (1994) 5-HT 1A receptor agonists improve the performance of normal and scopolamine-impaired rats in an operant delayed matching to position task. Psychopharmacology 116: 135 142 Colino A, Halliwell JV (1987) Differential modulation of three separate K-conductances in hippocampal CA1 neurons by serotonin. Nature 328: 73 77 Cooper SJ (1989) Benzodiazepine receptor-mediated enhancement and inhibition of taste reactivity, food choice and intake. Ann NY Acad Sci 575:321 337 Costello HL, Carlson JN, Glick SD (1991). Acute administration of diazepam and buspirone in rats trained on conflict schedules having different degrees of predictability. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 40: 787 794 Davis M (1990) Animal models of anxiety based on classical conditioning: the conditioned emotional response (CER) and the fear-potentiated startle effect. Pharmac Ther 47: 147 165 Davis M (1993) Pharmacological analysis of fear-potentiated startle. Brazilian J Med Biol Res 26:235 260 Dourish CT (1987) Brain 5-HT 1A receptors and anxiety. In: Dourish CT, Ahlenius S, Hutson PH (eds) Brain 5-HT 1A receptors. Ellis Horwood, Chichester, England, pp 261 277 Dourish CT, Hutson PH, Curzon G (1985a) Low doses of the putative serotonin agonist 8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino)tetralin (8-OH-DPAT) elicit feeding in the rat. Psychopharmacology 86: 197 204 Dourish CT, Hutson PH, Curzon G (1985b) Characteristics of feeding induced by the serotonin agonist 8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino)tetralin (8-OH-DPAT). Brain Res Bull 15: 377 384 Dourish CT, Hutson PH, Curzon G (1986) Putative anxiolytics 8- OH-DPAT, buspirone and TVXQ7821 are agonists at 5-HT 1A autoreceptors in the raphé nuclei. Trends Pharmacol Sci 7: 212 214 Eison AS, Eison MS, Stanley M, Riblet LA (1986) Serotonergic mechanisms in the behavioural effects of buspirone and gepirone. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 24: 701 707 Estes WK, Skinner BF (1941) Some quantitative properties of anxiety. J Exp Psychol 29:390 400 Fletcher A. Forster EA, Bill DJ, Brown G, Cliffe IA, Hartley JE, Jones DE, McLenachan A, Stanhope KJ, Critchley DJ, Childs KJ, Middlefell VC, Lanfumey L, Corradetti R, Laporte, A-M, Gozlan H, Hamon M, Dourish CT (1995) Electrophysiological, biochemical, neurohormonal and behavioural studies with WAY-100635, a potent, selective and silent 5-HT 1A receptor antagonist. Behav Brain Res 73: 337 353 Forster EA, Cliffe IA, Bill DJ, Dover GM, Jones DE, Reilly Y, Fletcher A (1995) A pharmacological profile of the selective silent 5-HT 1A antagonist, WAY-100635. Eur J Pharmacol 281: 81 88

303 Geller I, Seifter J (1960) The effects of meprobamate, barbiturates, d-amphetamine and promazine on experimentally-induced conflict in the rat. Psychopharmacologica 1: 482 492 Goa KL, Ward A (1986) Buspirone: A preliminary review of its pharmacological properties and therapeutic efficacy as an anxiolytic. Drugs 32: 114 129 Goldberg HL, Finnerty RJ (1979) The comparative efficacy of buspirone and diazepam in the treatment of anxiety. Am J Psychiatry 136: 1184 1187 Hendersen RW (1985) Fearful memories: the motivational significance of forgetting. In: Brush FR, Overmier JB (eds) Affect, conditioning and cognition: essays on the determinants of behavior. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J., pp 43 53 Hoffman HS, Fleshler M, Jensen P (1963) Stimulus aspects of aversive controls: The retention of conditioned suppression. J Exp Anal Behav 6: 575 583 Hunter AJ, Roberts FF (1987) The effects of 8-hydroxy-2-(di-npropylamino)tetralin (8-OH-DPAT) on spatial learning in the Morris water maze task. In: Dourish CT, Ahlenius S, Hutson PH (eds) Brain 5-HT 1A receptors. Ellis Horwood Chichester, England, pp 278 285 Leslie JC (1977) Effects of food deprivation and reinforcement magnitude on conditioned suppression. J Exp Anal Behav 28: 107 115 Mansbach RS, Geyer MA (1988) Blockage of potentiated startle responding in rats by 5-hydroxytryptamine 1A receptor ligands. Eur J Pharmacol 156: 375 383 Miczek KA (1973) Effect of scopolamine, amphetamine and benzodiazepines on conditioned suppression. Phamacol Biochem Behav 1:401 411 Moreau JL, Griebel G, Jenck F, Martin JR, Widmer U, Haefely WE (1992) Behavioral profile of the 5HT 1A receptor antagonist (S)-UH-301 in rodents and monkeys. Brain Res Bull 29:901 904 O Connor JJ, Rowan MJ, Anwyl R (1990) Actions of 5-HT 1 ligands on excitatory synaptic transmission in the hippocampus of alert rats. Br J Pharmacol 101:171 177 Preston GC, Brazell C, Ward C, Broks P, Traub M, Stahl SM (1988) The scopolamine model of dementia: determination of cental cholinomimetic effects of physostigmine on cognitive and biochemical markers in man. J Psychopharmacol 2: 67 79 Preston GC, Ward C, Lines CR, Poppleton P, Haigh JRM, Traub M (1989) Scopolamine and benzodiazepine models of dementia: cross-reversals by Ro 15-1788 and physostigmine. Psychopharmacology 98: 487 494 Riblet LA, Taylor DP, Eison MS, Stanton H (1982) Pharmacology and neurochemistry of buspirone. J Clin Psychiatry 43: 11 16 Rittenhouse PA, Bakkum EA, O Connor PA, Carnes M, Bethea CL, Van De Kar LD (1992) Comparison of neuroendocrine and behavioral effects of ipsapirone, a 5-HT 1A agonist, in three stress paradigms: immobilization, forced swim and conditioned fear. Brain Res 580: 205 214 Rodgers RJ, Cole JC (1994) Anxiolytic like effect of (S)-WAY- 100135 a 5-HT 1A receptor antagonist in the murine elevated plus-maze test. Eur J Pharmacol 261: 321 325 Ryan CN, Evenden JL, Petterson M (1993) Effects of buspirone and ipsapirone on schedule induced polydipsia: comparison with 8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino)tetralin. Psychopharmacology 112:34 44 Sanger DJ (1990) Effects of buspirone and related compounds on suppressed operant responding in rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 254: 420 426 Sanger DJ (1992) Increased rates of punished responding produced by buspirone-like compounds in rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 261: 513 517 Schefke DM, Fontana DJ, Commissaris RL (1989) Anti-conflict efficacy of buspirone following acute versus chronic treatment. Psychopharmacology 99: 427 429 Stanhope KJ, McLenachan A, Dourish CT (1995) Dissociation between cognitive and motor/motivation deficits in the delayed matching to position test: effects of scopolamine, 8-OH-DPAT and EAA antagonists. Psychopharmacology 122: 268 280 Traber J, Glaser T (1987) 5-HT 1A receptor-related anxiolytics. Trends Pharmacol Sci 8: 432 437 Treit D (1984) Animal models for the study of anti-anxiety agents: a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 9: 203 222 Treit D (1991) Anxiolytic effects of benzodiazepines, 5-HT 1A agonists: animal models. In: Rodgers RJ, Cooper SJ (eds) 5-HT 1A agonists, 5-HT 3 antagonists and benzodiazepines: their comparative behavioural pharmacology. Wiley, Chichester, England, pp 107 131 Treit D, Robinson A, Rotzinger S, Pesold C (1993) Anxiolytic effects of serotonergic interventions in the shock-probe burying test and the elevated plus-maze test. Behav Brain Res 54: 23 34 Tricklebank MD (1987) The motor and discriminative stimulus properties of 8-OH-DPAT and their relationship to activation of the putative 5-HT 1A receptor. In: Dourish CT, Ahlenius S, Hutson PH (eds) Brain 5-HT 1A receptors. Ellis Horwood, Chichester, England, pp 140 151 Vogel JR, Beer B, Clody DE (1971) A simple and reliable conflict procedure for testing antianxiety agents. Psychopharmacologia 21: 1 7 Weingartner H (1985) Models of memory dysfunctions. Ann NY Acad Sci 444: 359 369 Wilkinson LO, Dourish CT (1991) Serotonin and animal behavior. In: Peroutka SJ (ed) Serotonin receptor subtypes: basis and clinical aspects. Wiley-Liss, New York, pp 147 210 Winter JC, Petti DT (1987) The effects of 8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino)tetralin and other serotonergic agonists on performance in a radial maze: a possible role for 5-HT 1A receptors in memory. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 27: 625 628