Value From Regulatory Fit E. Tory Higgins

Similar documents
How Regulatory Fit Affects Value in Consumer Choices and Opinions

November 2000 o American Psychologist 1217

Regulatory Fit and Persuasion: Transfer From Feeling Right

Distinguishing Gains from Nonlosses and Losses from Nongains: A Regulatory Focus Perspective on Hedonic Intensity

Strategies of self-regulation in goal attainment versus goal maintenance

Motivational Differences in Aiming for Wide or Narrow Goals. Zhaoxia Xu

The Mediating Effect of Processing Fluency between Message Framing and Persuasion

Making Message Recipients Feel Right How Nonverbal Cues Can Increase Persuasion

How Advertising Claims Affect Brand Preferences and Category-Brand Associations: The Role of Regulatory Fit. Arnd Florack

BehaviorLens Behavior Pyramid

Lesson 1: Making and Continuing Change: A Personal Investment

Reflect on the Types of Organizational Structures. Hierarch of Needs Abraham Maslow (1970) Hierarchy of Needs

PROFESSIONALISM THE ABC FOR SUCCESS

Progress Induced Goal Shifting. Grainne M. Fitzsimons & Justin Friesen, University of Waterloo

RUNNING HEAD: Regulatory focus and political decision making

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration

Regulatory Fit and Reaction to Opinion Deviance in Small Groups. Kira Marie Alexander. B.A., University of Colorado, 2002

Counterfactual thinking and regulatory fit

PST-PC Appendix. Introducing PST-PC to the Patient in Session 1. Checklist

Apply Your knowledge of the Psychology of Learning

reward based power have ability to give you what you want. coercive have power to punish

A FRAMEWORK FOR MESSAGE FRAMING 1. A Self-Regulatory Framework for Message Framing. Joseph Cesario. Michigan State University. Katherine S.

The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect. Abstract

This Story Is Right On : The Impact of Regulatory Fit on Narrative Engagement and Persuasion

Rewards & Education 1

Self Motivation Inventory Find out whether you are naturally self-motivated and disciplined

When Risk Seeking Becomes a Motivational Necessity

The Influence of Framing Effects and Regret on Health Decision-Making

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Promotion and Prevention Fit Are Different but Lead to Equal Performance: Examining Fit. Sensitivity and Task Performance. Jessica A.

Investigations of consumer decision making suggest that

Character Education Framework

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

Instructional Strategies! &! Classroom Management! The student-centered classroom & Choice Theory!

Learning Objectives. Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior

Choosing versus Rejecting: The Impact of Goal-Task Compatibility on Decision Confidence

Intern. J. of Research in Marketing

Achievement orientations from subjective histories of success: promotion pride versus prevention pride

The Role of Anticipated Emotions in the Endowment Effect

Experimental Testing of Intrinsic Preferences for NonInstrumental Information

Measuring Psychological Wealth: Your Well-Being Balance Sheet

Mitigating Operator-Induced Vehicle Mishaps

Giving Students a Reality Check. Help Students use Reality Therapy in their everyday lives.

COACH WORKPLACE REPORT. Jane Doe. Sample Report July 18, Copyright 2011 Multi-Health Systems Inc. All rights reserved.

Anthony Robbins' book on success

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

LIVE YOUR BEST LIFE: HELP GUIDE # 21 Helping students be Effective Learners Program LIVE YOUR BEST LIFE

Imagine it is early in January, and you are just beginning

EUNICE Y. KIM June 2009

VALUE CARD SORT Step 1: Step 2: Step 3: Step 4:

All Types of Mortality Salience Are Not Equal: The Effect of Contemplating Natural versus Unnatural Death on. Materialism Behavior

YOUR ESSENTIAL EMOTIONAL NEEDS. Needs that need to be met in balance

Value Differences Between Scientists and Practitioners: A Survey of SIOP Members

Ask the Expert Educator Edition

Rethinking Regulatory Engagement Theory. Michel Tuan Pham. Graduate School of Business, Columbia University. Tamar Avnet

LANGUAGES OF APPRECIATION IN THE WORKPLACE

CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE

See It Through My Eyes: Restaurant Attribute Evaluation Based On Patron Emotions

HOW TO DEAL WITH WORK RELATED STRESS

Discovering Your Primary Motivational Gift

Personal Growth Strategies

Resilience in the RTW Context

ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH

PSY The Psychology Major: Academic and Professional Issues. Module 8: Critical Thinking. Study Guide Notes

Liking the Same Things, but Doing Things Differently: Outcome versus Strategic Compatibility in Partner Preferences for Joint Tasks. Vanessa K.

Choosing Life: Empowerment, Action, Results! CLEAR Menu Sessions. Substance Use Risk 2: What Are My External Drug and Alcohol Triggers?

Self-report measures of individual differences in regulatory focus: A cautionary note

Contents. Chapter. A Closer Look at You. Section 17.1 Your Personality Section 17.2 Develop a Positive Attitude. Chapter 17 A Closer Look at You

Why Do You Study? Complex Answers to a Simple Question. Mimi Bong. Department of Education Brain & Motivation Research Institute Korea University

Consumer Motivation. Week 4

Subjective Well-Being and Adjustment

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological needs & implicit motives. Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6

PERSON PERCEPTION AND INTERPERSONAL ATTRACTION

THE PSYCHOLOGY OF HAPPINESS D A Y 1

Motivation CHAPTER FIFTEEN INTRODUCTION DETAILED LECTURE OUTLINE

Motivational correlates of need for cognition

c) Redraw the model and place on it relevant attributions for each of the four boxes.

DAY 2 RESULTS WORKSHOP 7 KEYS TO C HANGING A NYTHING IN Y OUR LIFE TODAY!

The learning outcomes are colour coded to illustrate where the aspects of PSHE and Citizenship and SEAL are covered within the themes:

ORIENTATION SAN FRANCISCO STOP SMOKING PROGRAM

Field Testing of Regulatory Focus Theory

The Effects of Task Difficulty and Reward Opportunity on Motivation

PSYCHOLOGIST-PATIENT SERVICES

Good Communication Starts at Home

The Instrumentality Heuristic

Foundational tests of the need-support model: A framework for bridging regulatory focus theory and self-determination theory

Optimal Health Questionnaire

YOUR CHILD S BODY IMAGE: WHAT S A PARENT TO DO?

Pain Self-Management Strategies Wheel

PREPARING FOR THE ELEVENTH TRADITION

Private Intensive Therapy Retreats Information for Therapists

Regulatory Focus and product character in consumers' response to advertising

Happiness, Pleasure, and Satisfaction

Evolve Your Relationships Transform Your World

Chapter 8: Consumer Attitude Formation and Change

Creating Motivating Environments: Part II Robert Brooks, Ph.D.

You probably don t spend a lot of time here, but if you do, you are reacting to the most basic needs a human has survival and protection.

Transcription:

CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Value From Regulatory Fit E. Tory Higgins Columbia University ABSTRACT Where does value come from? I propose a new answer to this classic question. People experience regulatory fit when the manner of their engagement in an activity sustains their goal orientation or interests regarding that activity. When there is fit, people engage more strongly in what they are doing and feel right about it. Fit influences the strength of value experiences how good or how bad one feels about something independently of the pleasure and pain experiences that are associated with outcomes. It uniquely contributes to people s experience of the value of things. Fit is shown to influence judgments and decision making, attitude and behavior change, and task performance. KEYWORDS value; evaluation; activity engagement; decision making; attitude change; performance What makes people value something? A classic answer is that values are socialized shared beliefs about ideal objectives and allowable procedures for attaining them (e.g., Merton, 1957). A second answer relates value to usefulness and emphasizes need satisfaction rather than beliefs. For example, Adam Smith s (1994/1776) prime example of an object with high value was water. From the time of the Ancient Greeks, a third answer has emphasized value as experience, especially hedonic experiences of pleasure and pain. In this paper, I present evidence for a type of value experience separate from hedonic experiences: the regulatory fit that people experience when the manner of their engagement in an activity sustains their goal orientation or interests regarding that activity. I propose that all value is an experience of the attraction toward or repulsion from something whose intensity is a function of the strength of two factors the strength of hedonic pleasure or pain and the strength of engagement (see Higgins, 2005). Regulatory fit contributes to value through increasing strength of engagement. It makes people feel right about whatever they are doing, and thus has a broad influence on judgments and decision making, attitude and behavior change, and task performance. Address correspondence to E. Tory Higgins, Department of Psychology, Schermerhorn Hall, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027; e-mail: tory@psych.columbia.edu. REGULATORY FIT When people pursue a goal, they begin with some motivational orientation, some concerns or interests that direct the goal pursuit. They pursue the goal in some manner, some method or way of executing the goal pursuit. Finally, they experience or anticipate experiencing some desirable or undesirable outcomes of successful or unsuccessful goal pursuit. In traditional motivational models, the means or manner in which a goal is pursued can be valued because, as a way to attain the desired outcomes, it is either socially prescribed (e.g., having value from fulfilling social norms of fairness), effective (i.e., having instrumental value), or efficient (i.e., having low costs). What is valued about the means per se is their contribution to attaining desirable outcomes (high benefits and low costs). This value of the manner of goal pursuit as a function of its influence on outcomes has received substantial attention in the literature. In contrast, the value of the manner of goal pursuit as a function of its influence on the motivational orientation of the actor has received relatively little attention. In an earlier paper (Higgins, 2000), I proposed that people experience regulatory fit when the manner of their engagement in an activity sustains (rather than disrupts) their current motivational orientation or interests. Fit makes people engage more strongly in what they are doing and feel right about it. Individuals, for example, can pursue the same goal with different orientations and in different ways. Consider, for instance, students in the same course who are working to attain an A. Some students have a promotion-focus orientation toward an A; that is, the goal is experienced as a hope and an ideal, as something that satisfies the need for accomplishment. Others have a preventionfocus orientation toward an A; the goal in this case is experienced as a responsibility or an ought, as something that satisfies the need for security. To pursue their goal, some students read material beyond the assigned readings an eager way to attain an A whereas others are careful to fulfill all course requirements a vigilant way to attain an A. Previous studies have found that an eager manner fits a promotion focus better than it fits a prevention focus, whereas the reverse is true for a vigilant manner (Higgins, 2000). For all students, receiving an A in a course has certain outcome benefits regardless of the orientation and manner in which Volume 14 Number 4 Copyright r 2005 American Psychological Society 209

Value From Regulatory Fit they pursue their goal. Independent of this outcome value, however, there is an additional experience from regulatory fit. Specifically, when people pursue a goal in a manner that fits their orientation (e.g., eagerly if they have a promotion focus; vigilantly if they have a prevention focus), they experience their engagement in that goal pursuit more strongly than they do when pursuing the goal in a way that is at odds with their orientation (e.g., pursuing a goal eagerly if their orientation is more preventative). When the manner of their goal pursuit fits their orientation they also experience a stronger evaluative reaction to the activity (Higgins, 2000). Regulatory fit makes them feel right about both their positive reactions to things and their negative reactions to things, such as feeling right about one s positive response to some advertisement or feeling right about one s negative response to some other advertisement (see Cesario, Grant, & Higgins, 2004). In one set of studies, for example, my colleagues and I (Higgins, Idson, Freitas, Spiegel, & Molden, 2003) first measured participants chronic or habitual orientations to pursuing goals (i.e., promotion or prevention). Participants were then told that, over and above their usual payment for participating, they could choose between a coffee mug and a pen as a gift. (Pretesting indicated that people liked both objects but the mug was clearly preferred.) The means of making the decision was manipulated through framing: Half of the participants were told to think about what they would gain by choosing the mug or the pen (an eager strategy), and the other half were told to think about what they would lose by not choosing the mug or the pen (a vigilant strategy). As expected, almost all participants chose the coffee mug. These participants were then asked either to assess the price of the chosen mug or to offer a price to buy it. Participants whose habitual orientations fit the way they were encouraged to make the decision (promotion eager; prevention vigilant) gave a 40 to 60% higher price for the mug than participants in the non-fit conditions (promotion vigilant; prevention eager). Consistent with the proposal that fit strengthens evaluative reactions to something independent of hedonic experiences per se, the fit effect was found to be independent of the participants reports of how positive or negative they felt when making their decision. The effect was also independent of participants perception of the efficiency (ease) and effectiveness (instrumentality) of the means they used to make their choice. Fit effects can be found for other orientations as well. For example, we (Avnet and Higgins, 2003) experimentally induced either a locomotion orientation, which is concerned with movement from state to state, or an assessment orientation, which is concerned with making comparisons. The participants chose one book light from a set of book lights using either a progressive-elimination strategy (i.e., eliminate the worst alternative at each phase until only one alternative remains, which fits a locomotion orientation) or a full-evaluation strategy (i.e., make comparisons among all of the alternatives for all of the attributes and then choose the one with the best attributes MONEY OFFERED $8.00 $7.00 $6.00 $5.00 $4.00 $3.00 $2.00 $4.60 $6.55 Assessment $5.89 ORIENTATION Progressive Elimination Full Comparison $4.00 Locomotion Fig. 1. Amount of money offered to buy a book light as a function of participants regulatory mode orientation (assessment or locomotion) and the type of strategy (progressive elimination or full comparison) used to decide which light to purchase. After Avnet & Higgins, 2003. overall, which fits an assessment orientation). As shown in Figure 1, the participants offered more of their own money to buy the same chosen book light in the fit conditions (assessment full evaluation; locomotion progressive elimination) than in the non-fit conditions. In addition, this fit effect was independent of the participants positive or negative feelings at the time that they offered to buy the book light. JUDGMENT AND DECISION MAKING How does fit create value? When fit makes people feel right about what they are doing such as feeling right while choosing between the mug and the pen this experience may transfer to feeling right about something else they do later, such as feeling right when positively evaluating the monetary worth of the chosen mug. The feeling-right experience from an earlier source would be mistakenly transferred to a later evaluative response. If so, then drawing attention to the true source of the feeling-right experience before making the later evaluation should reduce or eliminate the fit effect. In another mug study (see Higgins et al., 2003), participants were reminded of the strategy they had used to choose the mug and were told that sometimes using certain strategies to pursue goals can make people feel right about their goal pursuit. They were asked, How much do you feel right about your goal pursuit? ; then they priced the mug. As predicted, the fit effect was eliminated (see also Cesario et al., 2004). There is evidence that regulatory fit influences past (retrospective) or future (prospective) evaluations as well. Camacho, Higgins, and Luger (2003), for example, had participants think back to a time in their lives when they had a conflict with an authority figure (e.g., a parent) and it was that authority figure 210 Volume 14 Number 4

E. Tory Higgins who determined the manner of conflict resolution. The participants were asked to recall different kinds of resolution. Some participants recalled a resolution where the authority figure encouraged them to succeed (the pleasant eager condition), whereas other participants remembered a resolution where the authority figure safeguarded them against anything that might go wrong (the pleasant vigilant condition). Independent of whether the manner of resolution was itself pleasant or painful, and independent of their own pleasant or painful mood while making their judgments, participants judged the resolution to be more morally right in the fit conditions (promotion participants eager conflict resolution; prevention participants vigilant conflict resolution). In making decisions, people imagine the pleasure or pain of the outcomes that particular choices will produce. Imagining making a desirable choice has higher fit for people in a promotion focus than it does for those in a prevention focus (because success maintains eagerness but reduces vigilance); the opposite is true for imagining making an undesirable choice (because failure maintains vigilance but reduces eagerness). In a study by Idson, Liberman, and Higgins (2004), participants were asked to imagine how good or bad they would feel either paying in cash or with a credit card to buy a book for school where the book costs less if you pay in cash. As shown in Figure 2, participants felt better when they imagined paying less for the book. In addition, when imagining paying less they felt even better if they were in a promotion focus than if they were in a prevention focus; and when imagining paying more, they felt even worse if they were in a prevention focus than if they were in a promotion focus. ATTITUDE AND BEHAVIOR CHANGE A standard method to change people s attitudes and behavior is to provide information about the positive outcomes or benefits of such a change. Regulatory fit permits another method for change. If people respond positively to a message, creating fit should make them feel right about their response and increase message effectiveness. Spiegel, Grant-Pillow, and Higgins (2004), for example, had participants read either a promotion-framed or preventionframed health message that contained the same information urging them to eat more fruits and vegetables. The consequences were framed as either the benefits of complying or the costs of not complying. Participants in the fit conditions (promotion benefits; prevention costs) ate about 20% more fruits and vegetables over the following week than those in the non-fit conditions. In a study by Cesario et al. (2003), the fit experience was induced separately from the message itself so that all participants could be given exactly the same message. The participants were told that they were performing two unrelated tasks. In the first task, fit or non-fit was induced by having participants list either eager or vigilant strategies to attain either promotion or prevention goals. In the second task, all of the participants were given exactly the same persuasive policy message. Cesario et al. (2003) found that fit induced in the first task increased persuasion in the second task for participants who had positive thoughts about the message but decreased persuasion for participants who had negative thoughts (see Fig. 3). Thus, when fit makes people feel right about their response to an object or event, this increases the strength of that response, whether the response is positive or negative. 7 5 6.7 4.8 PROMOTION PREVENTION TASK PERFORMANCE According to the theory of regulatory fit (Higgins, 2000), regulatory fit increases strength of engagement. Consistent with this 7 GOOD/BAD FEELINGS 3 1 1 3 3.1 OVERALL OPINION 6.5 6 5.5 5 4.5 4 5.6 4.7 4.1 5.4 FIT NON-FIT 5 7 PAY LESS 5.1 PAY MORE 3.5 3 POSITIVE NEGATIVE THOUGHT VALENCE Fig. 2. Good/bad feelings about a textbook purchase as a function of participants regulatory focus (promotion or prevention) and the valence of prospective outcome (paying less or more for the book). After Idson, Liberman, and Higgins (2004). Fig. 3. Overall opinion of an advocated policy (higher numbers indicate higher agreement) as a function of positive vs. negative thoughts about the policy message and fit versus non-fit in a prior, unrelated task. After Cesario et al., 2003. Volume 14 Number 4 211

Value From Regulatory Fit prediction, Förster, Higgins, and Idson (1998) found that engagement, as measured by either persistence on an anagram task or arm-pressure intensity while doing the task, was stronger in the fit conditions (promotion eager; prevention vigilant) whether the regulatory focus of the participants varied chronically or had been induced experimentally. If regulatory fit strengthens engagement, then it has the potential to improve performance. Indeed, Förster et al. (1998) found that fit enhanced task performance (see also Bianco, Higgins, and Klem, 2003). Spiegel et al. (2004) also found that fit helps to bridge the gap between intentions and action. Participants were asked to write a report on how they were going to spend their upcoming Saturday and to turn it in by a certain deadline to receive an extra payment for participating in the study. The participants were also asked to imagine how they were going to write the report, and were asked to make either eager plans or vigilant plans for writing the report. Performance was measured by whether or not the report was handed in on time. Promotion participants performed better when they prepared eager plans of when, where, and how to do the report rather than vigilant plans. The reverse was true for prevention participants. Indeed, participants in the fit conditions (promotion eager steps; prevention vigilant steps) were almost 50% more likely to turn in their reports than participants in the non-fit conditions. CONCLUSION This article describes a source of value that has received little prior attention: regulatory fit. When people make decisions or pursue goals, they experience the value of actual or anticipated outcomes that they find attractive or repulsive, but this is not the only way that value is created. Making decisions and pursuing goals are themselves activities and, as with any activity, people can be more or less engaged in the activity. When engagement is strong it intensifies people s feelings about an activity, whether those feelings are positive or negative. Regulatory fit creates value by increasing strength of engagement. People experience regulatory fit when the manner in which they engage in an activity sustains their current orientation. The increased strength of engagement produced by fit is experienced as feeling right about what one is doing, including feeling right about one s evaluative reactions to objects and events in the world. Fit makes people feel right about both their positive responses to things and their negative responses to things. Moreover, it does this not only for the activity that itself produced the fit, but also for later, separate activities. My colleagues and I (Higgins et al., 2003), for example, first had participants think about strategies for pursuing their personal goals that either fit or did not fit their goal orientation. A few minutes later we had participants rate nicelooking, photographed dogs. Participants in the prior fit conditions rated the dogs as more good-natured overall than those in the prior non-fit conditions did. These results suggest that fit induced in the first phase of the study made participants feel right about their later positive reactions to the dogs, thereby increasing the intensity of their positive reactions. Fit influences the strength of value experiences, how good or how bad one feels about something, independent of the pleasure and pain experiences that are associated with outcomes. Other process properties, such as the efficiency, effectiveness, or the hedonic quality of a process, can have value beyond goal outcomes, but fit has been shown to have effects on value that are independent of these other process properties. It is precisely because fit affects value through increasing strength of engagement rather than producing pleasure or pain itself that its effects are likely to go unnoticed. Failure to recognize its effects could be problematic when fit from one source is unknowingly transferred to the monetary value of something else or, even worse, to the ethical value of something else, such as a public policy program that is judged to be morally right simply because how it is executed feels right. Fit has significant implications for improving quality of life. In interpersonal and intergroup conflicts, for example, it is well recognized that the negotiation process needs to be fair, just, and equitable. But fit also needs to be considered. By ensuring that all parties experience fit in how they carry out a negotiation, satisfaction with and commitment to the agreement will increase, independent of outcomes. That is the good news. The bad news is that other parties not directly involved in the negotiation, who miss the fit experience, are unlikely to value the agreement as much and might not support it. It is also important for parents and teachers to allow children their own fit experiences in goal pursuit and decision making and not simply provide them with answers and positive outcomes. More generally, for people to value their lives fully they need to go beyond pleasant outcomes and feel right about what they are doing. Recommended Reading Freitas, A.L., & Higgins, E.T. (2002). Enjoying goal-directed action: The role of regulatory fit. Psychological Science, 13, 1 6. Higgins, E.T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from fit. American Psychologist, 55, 1217 1230. Higgins, E.T., Idson, L.C., Freitas, A.L., Spiegel, S., & Molden, D.C. (2003). Transfer of value from fit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1140 1153. Acknowledgments The research reported in this article was supported by Grant 39429 from the National Institute of Mental Health to E. Tory Higgins. REFERENCES Avnet, T., & Higgins, E.T. (2003). Locomotion, assessment, and regulatory fit: Value transfer from how to what. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 525 530. 212 Volume 14 Number 4

E. Tory Higgins Bianco, A.T., Higgins, E.T., & Klem, A. (2003). How fun/importance fit impacts performance: Relating implicit theories to instructions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1091 1103. Camacho, C.J., Higgins, E.T., & Luger, L. (2003). Moral value transfer from regulatory fit: What feels right is right and what feels wrong is wrong. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 498 510. Cesario, J., Grant, H., & Higgins, E.T. (2004). Regulatory fit and persuasion: Transfer from feeling right. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 388 404. Förster, J., Higgins, E.T., & Idson, C.L. (1998). Approach and avoidance strength as a function of regulatory focus: Revisiting the goal looms larger effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1115 1131. Higgins, E.T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from fit. American Psychologist, 55, 1217 1230. Higgins, E.T. (2005). Value from hedonic experience and engagement. Unpublished manuscript, Columbia University. Higgins, E.T., Idson, L.C., Freitas, A.L., Spiegel, S., & Molden, D.C. (2003). Transfer of value from fit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1140 1153. Idson, L.C., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E.T. (2004). Imagining how you d feel: The role of motivational experiences from regulatory fit. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 926 937. Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science. New York: Harper. Merton, R.K. (1957). Social theory and social structure. Glencoe, IL: The Free Press. Smith, A. (1994). The wealth of nations. New York: Random House. (Original work published in 1776). Spiegel, S., Grant-Pillow, H., & Higgins, E.T. (2004). How regulatory fit enhances motivational strength during goal pursuit. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 39 54. Volume 14 Number 4 213