The anatomy of conscious vision: an fmri study of visual hallucinations

Similar documents
The functional anatomy of the McCollough contingent colour aftereffect

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544

Reproducibility of Visual Activation During Checkerboard Stimulation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging at 4 Tesla

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1

Selective Attention to Face Identity and Color Studied With fmri

Supplementary Note Psychophysics:

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention

Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 6: Vision

fmri: What Does It Measure?

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J.

25/09/2012. Capgras Syndrome. Chapter 2. Capgras Syndrome - 2. The Neural Basis of Cognition

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)

Biological Bases of Behavior. 6: Vision

Are face-responsive regions selective only for faces?

ASSUMPTION OF COGNITIVE UNIFORMITY

Brain activity related to integrative processes in visual object recognition: bottom-up integration and the modulatory influence of stored knowledge

V1 (Chap 3, part II) Lecture 8. Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017

Parallel streams of visual processing

Group-Wise FMRI Activation Detection on Corresponding Cortical Landmarks

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods

LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition

Pathway from the eye to the cortex

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Neuroscience Tutorial

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri

Statistical parametric mapping

Required Slide. Session Objectives

Functional Elements and Networks in fmri

Key questions about attention

Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications.

Visual Selection and Attention

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation

Visualization strategies for major white matter tracts identified by diffusion tensor imaging for intraoperative use

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m.

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1

PRIMARY VISUAL CORTEX AND VISUAL AWARENESS

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye:

Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy. (the part that s visible to us!)

Table 1. Summary of PET and fmri Methods. What is imaged PET fmri BOLD (T2*) Regional brain activation. Blood flow ( 15 O) Arterial spin tagging (AST)

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Human Visual Cortex during Face Matching: A Comparison with Positron Emission Tomography

Cerebral Cortex 1. Sarah Heilbronner

Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response

PHY3111 Mid-Semester Test Study. Lecture 2: The hierarchical organisation of vision

fmri and Voxel-based Morphometry in Detection of Early Stages of Alzheimer's Disease

Competing Frameworks in Perception

Competing Frameworks in Perception

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni

5th Mini-Symposium on Cognition, Decision-making and Social Function: In Memory of Kang Cheng

Functional organization of spatial and nonspatial working memory processing within the human lateral frontal cortex

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory

C:\Documents and Settings\sstensaas\Desktop\dental visual 2010\VisualPath dental 2010.docVisualPath dental 2010.doc

Neuroanatomy, Text and Atlas (J. H. Martin), 3 rd Edition Chapter 7, The Visual System, pp ,

FRONTAL LOBE. Central Sulcus. Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Sulcus. Cingulate Sulcus. Lateral Sulcus

Cortical Visual Symptoms

EDGE DETECTION. Edge Detectors. ICS 280: Visual Perception

Perceptual Gain and Perceptual Loss: Distinct Neural Mechanisms of Audiovisual Interactions*

Extrastriate Visual Areas February 27, 2003 A. Roe

P2 Visual - Perception

VIII. 10. Right Temporal-Lobe Contribution to the Retrieval of Family Relationships in Person Identification

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience

HAL author manuscript

Myers Psychology for AP*

HST 583 fmri DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Supporting Online Material for

The Tools: Imaging the Living Brain

The Eye. Cognitive Neuroscience of Language. Today s goals. 5 From eye to brain. Today s reading

Rules of apparent motion: The shortest-path constraint: objects will take the shortest path between flashed positions.

NEUROSCIENCE. Barbora Cimrová

The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan. Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1

Dissociation of Mnemonic and Perceptual Processes During Spatial and Nonspatial Working Memory Using fmri

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice

PHYSICS OF MRI ACQUISITION. Alternatives to BOLD for fmri

Frontal Contributions to Memory Encoding Before and After Unilateral Medial Temporal Lobectomy

Vision II. Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota

Image Processing in the Human Visual System, a Quick Overview

Perception of Faces and Bodies

Neuroimaging. BIE601 Advanced Biological Engineering Dr. Boonserm Kaewkamnerdpong Biological Engineering Program, KMUTT. Human Brain Mapping

CS294-6 (Fall 2004) Recognizing People, Objects and Actions Lecture: January 27, 2004 Human Visual System

Auditory fmri correlates of loudness perception for monaural and diotic stimulation

Supplementary Online Content

fmri and visual brain function Dr. Tessa Dekker UCL Institute of Ophthalmology

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 2

Evidence for a Refractory Period in the Hemodynamic Response to Visual Stimuli as Measured by MRI

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

Experimental Design. Thomas Wolbers Space and Aging Laboratory Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems

Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition

Transcription:

The anatomy of conscious vision: an fmri study of visual hallucinations D. H. ffytche, R. J. Howard, M. J. Brammer, A. David, P. Woodruff and S. Williams Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, UK Correspondence should be addressed to D.H.ff. (d.ffytche@iop.bpmf.ac.uk) Despite recent advances in functional neuroimaging, the apparently simple question of how and where we see the neurobiology of visual consciousness continues to challenge neuroscientists. Without a method to differentiate neural processing specific to consciousness from unconscious afferent sensory signals, the issue has been difficult to resolve experimentally. Here we use functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) to study patients with the Charles Bonnet syndrome, for whom visual perception and sensory input have become dissociated. We found that hallucinations of color, faces, textures and objects correlate with cerebral activity in ventral extrastriate visual cortex, that the content of the hallucinations reflects the functional specializations of the region and that patients who hallucinate have increased ventral extrastriate activity, which persists between hallucinations. Few imaging studies have investigated the conscious pictures of the external environment that we associate with seeing (visual percepts). The problem that confronts the neuroscientist is recognizing the neural correlate of seeing and differentiating it from afferent sensory activity, which is assumed to remain unconscious 1. One solution is to study a visual system in which percepts have become dissociated from sensory input. Such dissociation can follow a sudden deterioration in visual abilities in patients who in other respects are neuropsychiatrically normal 2 4. This syndrome is termed the Charles Bonnet syndrome (named after the Swiss philospher who first described it) 5. The spontaneous visual percepts (visual hallucinations) experienced by these patients are identical to those associated with normal seeing, although they can be recognized because of their bizarre and often amusing character and because, given the patients impaired vision, they are seen in greater detail than real stimuli 6. They differ from visual imagery experiences in that the hallucinations are localized to external space (rather than inside the head), have the vivid qualities of normal seeing and are not under voluntary control. We investigated the neural substrate of visual consciousness in a group of such patients, using two different but complimentary strategies, both of which have proven successful previously 7 9. The first strategy (Experiment 1) was to ask the patients to signal the onset and offset of each hallucination during a fiveminute scan and to then correlate the timing of the hallucinations with the time-course of the fmri signal. A second, indirect strategy, which did not depend on capturing a hallucination during a scan, identified functionally abnormal brain regions by scanning the patients while they viewed a nonspecific visual stimulus and comparing the results to those of a matched control group who had never experienced hallucinations (Experiment 2). Results Visual hallucinations were reported in both experiments. Four patients had spontaneous hallucinations, whereas two others had hallucinations provoked by visual stimulation (Table 1). With the exception of one patient (PP), all hallucinations were in color. Two patients (SH, LC) reported faces, two (FP, PP) reported brickwork, fencing and map textures, and one (AK) reported objects. Unless otherwise stated, all hallucinations occurred in the central visual field. In two patients (AK and FP), Experiment 1 was repeated within the same scanning session to assess response consistency. Three patients (SH, AK, FP) with spontaneous hallucinations were unable to see the stimulus and therefore did not participate in Experiment 2. Therefore, with the exception of one patient (PP), the two experiments had different subjects. SPONTANEOUS HALLUCINATIONS In all four patients with spontaneous hallucinations, the fmri activity that correlated most significantly with the hallucination report was located in the ventral occipital lobe within or around the fusiform gyrus (Fig. 1). Colored hallucinations were associated with activity in the posterior fusiform gyrus (mean x = +28 and 35, y = 81, z = 13), whereas black-andwhite hallucinations were associated with activity behind and above this region (x = 30, y = 84, z = 2). The hallucination of a face was associated with activity in the left middle fusiform gyrus (x = 42, y = 57, z = 7.5), hallucinations of objects were associated with activity in the right middle fusiform gyrus (x = 21, y = 66, z = 18), and hallucinations of textures were associated with activity around the collateral sulcus. In some experiments, additional activity was found outside ventral extrastriate cortex (for example, the frontal activation in patient FP or the activity on the medial occipital lobe in SH, shown in Fig. 1); however, this additional activity was neither consistent between repeated experiments in the same patient nor common among different patients. An increase in fmri signal often preceded a hallucination (for example, the first, fourth and sixth hallucination shown in Fig. 2a). This temporal relationship was found in all patients studied (Fig. 2b). 738 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 8 december 1998

articles Fig. 1. Spontaneous hallucinations. Positive correlations between T2*-weighted MRI signal and hallucination report are superimposed (red) on transverse sections of high-resolution structural images (r max AK, p < 1 10 3 ; PP, FP, p < 1 10 4 ; SH, p < 1 10 5 ). The fusiform gyrus has been shaded in blue to aid anatomical localization. The hallucinations are illustrated next to each image. Ventral occipital activity was consistent in repeated experiments on the same patient. The r max was always in the ventral occipital lobe, but the optimal temporal shift varied between patients. No areas had a negative correlation at the same level of significance. In all figures, the left of each structural image is the right of the brain. RESPONSE TO VISUAL STIMULATION In Experiment 2, in patients with impaired vision who had never hallucinated, the visual stimulus evoked activity along the calcarine fissure (area V1), extending onto the ventral surface of the occipital lobe to include the fusiform gyrus Table 1. The phenomenology and timing of visual hallucinations. (Fig. 3a). In patients with the Charles Bonnet syndrome, this stimulus evoked activity in the striate cortex but failed to do so in the fusiform and lingual gyri (Fig. 3b). We compared the corrected mean level of fmri signal (see Methods) within the active ventral extrastriate regions in the controls with the cor- Subject Diagnosis Spontaneous Number of hallucinations Mean duration Phenomenology hallucination per 5-min scan of hallucinations (s) AK Optic neuritis Yes 9 ± 2 (3 scans) 16 ± 14 Colored, shiny shapes like futuristic cars or objects found in the pyramids. The shapes contained edges within them and did not look like real objects. FP Glaucoma, Yes 13 ± 2 (2 scans) 11 ± 6 Brickwork in left upper quadrant Retinal detachment of visual field. Fencing in right lower quadrant. Dark blue/green colored blob. Geometrical shape (four squares arranged as the corners of a larger square). SH Retinitis pigmentosa Yes 6 25 ± 24 Face, cartoon-like and in outline. Bright greenish/white light. Vague shadows. PP Glaucoma Yes 5 53 ± 41 Map with lines like roads and an edge like a coast, in black and white. Resembles a page from the A to Z. The names and lettering on the map could not be resolved. LC Diabetes mellitus No N/A N/A The faces and upper body of three soldiers in WW2 uniform against a green background (off-phase of noise stimulus). SF Macular degeneration No N/A N/A Purple raindrops like stars (both phases of noise stimulus). Purple blob that explodes (off-phase of noise stimulus). CC Macular degeneration No N/A N/A No hallucination during scan Typical hallucination: balls in a net, shrubbery, branches. PG Macular degeneration No N/A N/A No hallucination during scan Typical hallucination: balls in a net, bumble bee, mouse and large spider moving from left to right. nature neuroscience volume 1 no 8 december 1998 739

a T2* weighted MR signal T2* weighted MR signal 1003 993 983 973 963 1 2 3 4 Time (min) 5 b Corrected MR signal Corrected MR signal 10 8 6 4 2 0-2 -15-12 -9-6 -3 0 +3 +6 +9 +12 Time (sec) (s) Fig. 2. The timing of visual hallucinations. (a) The fmri time series from the fusiform gyrus (circles) and hallucination log (bars) for patient SH. (b) The mean signal intensity in the 12 seconds before and after the report of hallucination onset. Only hallucinations that occurred after a gap of at least 18 seconds have been included to avoid detecting the signal related to the previous hallucination (n = 13). MR signal has been normalized to the 15 s scan. The increase in signal in the 12 seconds preceding a hallucination is significant (F(4,60) = 3.83; p < 0.01). responding silent regions in the patients. Mean signal was increased significantly in the hallucinators relative to the controls across the whole five-minute experiment (t = 2.94, df = 8, p < 0.025). The apparent silence of the region was due to a relatively greater increase in signal between the periods of visual stimulation (OFF) than during stimulation (ON), with a consequent degradation of periodic signal (see Methods). Discussion Our two sets of results converge on a single conclusion, that hallucinations of color, faces, textures and objects result from increased activity in the ventral occipital lobe. A phasic increase in activity causes a discrete hallucination (Experiment 1), whereas a tonic increase in activity decreases the response to external visual stimulation (Experiment 2). An unexpected finding was the rise in fmri signal before the onset of the conscious experience, the opposite of the normal delayed response to visual stimulation found in fmri experiments 10,11. Patients described the appearance and disappearance of their hallucinations as sudden (< 1 s), all-or-nothing phenomena, so the observed reversed delay is not an artifact of uncertainty as to when to report the experience. One explanation for this finding might be that cerebral activity must a b Fig. 3. Generic activation to visual stimulation. Voxels in phase with the stimulus are thresholded at p < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons, and superimposed (red) on transverse slices of a template structural image. Talairach z coordinates in mm above and below the AC PC plane are displayed beneath each column. (a) Control patients. (b) Hallucinators. exceed a certain threshold level to contribute to visual consciousness 12 and that subthreshold neurophysiological activity starting 15 seconds before the hallucination is responsible for the increase in signal found at 12 seconds. We found a striking correspondence between the hallucinatory experiences of each patient and the known functional anatomy of the occipital lobe. In patients who hallucinated in color, activity was found in the fusiform gyrus in an area corresponding to the color center, area V4 (mean x = ±28, y = 79, z = 16, see refs 13, 14), whereas in the patient who hallucinated in black and white, the activity was outside this region (posterior extent of y = 82, max z = 12, see ref 14). The descriptions of featureless colors in the hallucinations are similar to the descriptions given by patients whose ventromedial occipital cortex has been stimulated directly 15. In the patient who hallucinated an unfamiliar face, additional activity was found in the left middle fusiform gyrus, an area that responds to unfamiliar face stimuli (mean x = 35, y = 63, z = 10, see ref 16). In patients who hallucinated brickwork, fences and a map, activity was found around the collateral sulcus, an area that responds to visual textures 11. Finally, in the patient who hallucinated objects, activity was found in the middle fusiform gyrus, an area that responds to visually presented objects 17,18. These results are, to our knowledge, the first evidence of a correlation between the location of activity within specialized cortex and the contents of a hallucination. Visual hallucinations are difficult to dismiss as vivid imagery experiences, as they differ both qualitatively (see Introduction) and, at least for color hallucinations, neurobiologically. (Area V4 was not differentially activated in a color imagery task compared to a spatial-orientation control task 19.) The neural substrate of a color hallucination is thus closer to that of a true (non-hallucinated) percept than that of color imagery. The areas identified are unlikely to be related to the motor signaling response of the patients, as this would occur at twice the hallucination frequency 740 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 8 december 1998

articles (patients signaled both the onset and the offset of each hallucination), and our correlation method would thus be relatively insensitive to it. The complexity of the percepts and the absence of consistent activity in the striate cortex make it unlikely that the ventral occipital lobe is responding to spontaneous discharges in the retina or LGN. Similarly, the absence of consistent activity outside the occipital lobe argues against the hypothesis that activity in the frontal lobe is a prerequisite for conscious vision 1,20 or that visual complexity in hallucinations implies activity in the anteriolateral temporal lobe 15. However, our data fall short of disproving such theories. If the spatial pattern of higher activity is not fixed for a given perceptual experience or is so diffusely distributed that its activity is not reflected in a change in fmri signal, it would not have been detected by our method. Visual consciousness is presumably the result of complex neuronal processes with topdown influences. The results presented above suggest that such top-down complexity may be localized within each specialized area rather than being distributed across the brain. We conclude that in patients who are neuropsychiatrically normal and in the absence of afferent sensory input, conscious percepts of color, texture, faces and objects are associated with activity in the ventral extrastriate cortex reflecting the known functional specializations of the region. Why these particular brains are functionally abnormal and whether the abnormality is common to all patients with visual hallucinations will require further investigation. These results complement previous studies of consciousness for motion 12,21 and support the hypothesis that processing within each specialized visual area makes a direct contribution to conscious vision 22,23. Methods PATIENTS AND CONTROLS. Eight patients with the Charles Bonnet syndrome (seven male, one female) were selected from a questionnairebased study of visual hallucinations at the Institute of Psychiatry. Selection was based on (i) the frequency of stereotyped hallucinations (> 1 per day), (ii) the absence of psychiatric illness, epilepsy or cognitive impairment (MMSE > 25, ref. 24) and (iii) suitability for an MRI scan. Control patients who had never hallucinated (five males), matched for age, acuity and visual field defect, were recruited from Kings College and St Thomas Hospitals. All patients gave informed consent and were given psychiatric, neurological and ophthalmological assessments. SPONTANEOUS HALLUCINATIONS. The room lights were dimmed, and patients were asked to signal the onset and offset of each hallucination, which was recorded on a computer linked to a hand-held keypad in the scanner. One patient logged the events himself; the remaining three raised or lowered a finger while the event was logged by an investigator. Descriptions of the hallucinations were collected after each scan. VISUAL STIMULATION. A visual stimulus, consisting of five one-minute cycles of 30 s of visual noise (ON) followed by 30 s of a black screen (OFF), was back-projected onto a translucent screen placed over the end of the scanner bore (elongated semi-circular field, 13 vertical 27 horizontal). The stimulus contained luminance, color, motion and form across a range of spatial and temporal frequencies. Patients were asked to attend the stimulus and to describe their hallucinations after each scan. No attempt was made to correct refractive errors. IMAGE ACQUISITION. Gradient echo, echoplanar images (EPI) were acquired on a 1.5-Tesla GE Signa System (General Electric, Milwaukee) with an Advanced NMR operating console and quadrature birdcage headcoil for radio frequency transmission and reception. In each experiment, 100 T2*-weighted images depicting blood oxygen leveldependent (BOLD) contrast 25 (TR = 3 s; TE = 40 ms) were obtained at each of 14, non-contiguous 7-mm slices (0.7 mm interslice spacing), parallel to the plane passing through the anterior and posterior commissures (AC PC) and covering the whole brain (in-plane resolution 3 3 mm). A high-contrast, high-resolution inversion recovery EPI image (TE = 74 ms; TI = 180 ms; TR = 1600 ms; NEX = 8; voxel size = 1.5 1.5 3.3 mm) was acquired after the experiments. IMAGE ANALYSIS. In Experiment 1, the time series were motion corrected 26, smoothed in x and y (7-mm full width half maximum (FWHM)) and the coefficient of correlation (r) was calculated at each voxel. The process was repeated after shifting the hallucination log with respect to the fmri time series in steps of one scan (shifts of 9 s, 6 s, 3 s, 0 s, +3 s, +6 s, +9 s) to optimize r for each patient (r max ). Probability maps were calculated from the estimated r max with 100 degrees of freedom and co-registered with the high-resolution structural image. Talairach 27 coordinates were derived from transformed r max images (see below). In Experiment 2, the time series were motion corrected 26, and the observed and randomized (10 permutations) fundamental power quotient (FPQ) at 0.016 Hz was estimated at each voxel 28. FPQ images were transformed into Talairach space using transformation parameters derived from the structural image 29. After smoothing in x and y, (20-mm FWHM), generic activation across patients was computed at each voxel by comparing the median observed FPQ with the median randomized FPQ 29. Corrections for multiple comparisons were based on the number of independent voxels after smoothing. The phase of activity was calculated from sine and cosine terms in the regression model. The location of generic activation in the original, non-transformed T2*-weighted images was calculated using the transformation parameters derived above. Mean signal within the ventral occipital region of interest over (i) the whole five-minute experiment, (ii) the ON periods and (iii) the OFF periods was corrected for global intersubject differences in maximum signal across the whole slice. Acknowledgements We are grateful to Kim Miller, the Buckinghamshire Association for the Blind, T. J. ffytche, D. McHugh and G. Plant for referring their patients and to the staff of the Neuroimaging group at the Institute of Psychiatry. D.H.ff. is a Wellcome Clinician Scientist Fellow. RECEIVED 22 JUNE: ACCEPTED 24 OCTOBER 1998 1. Crick, F. & Koch, C. Consciousness and neuroscience. Cereb. Cortex 8, 97 107 (1998). 2. Kölmel, H. W. Complex visual hallucinations in the hemianopic field. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 48, 29 38 (1985). 3. Lepore, F. E. Spontaneous visual phenomena with visual loss. Neurology 40, 444 447 (1990). 4. Holroyd, S. et al. Visual hallucinations in patients with macular degeneration. Am. J. Psychiatry 149, 1701 1706 (1992). 5. de Morsier, G. Les automatismes visuels. (Hallucinations visuelles rétrochiasmatiques). Schweiz. Med. Woch. 66, 700 703 (1936). 6. Teunisse, R. J. et al. Visual hallucinations in psychologically normal people: Charles Bonnet s syndrome. Lancet 347, 794 797 (1996). 7. Howard, R. J. et al. Cortical responses to exogenous visual stimulation during visual hallucinations. Lancet 345, 70 (1995). 8. Silbersweig, D. A. et al. A functional neuroanatomy of hallucinations in schizophrenia. Nature 378, 176 179 (1995). 9. Woodruff, P. W. R. et al. Auditory hallucinations and the temporal cortical response to speech in schizophrenia: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Am. J. Psychiatry 154, 1676 1682 (1997). 10. Boynton, G. M., Engel, S. A., Glover, G. H. & Heeger, D. J. Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1. J. Neurosci. 16, 4207 4221 (1996). 11. Puce, A. et al. Differential sensitivity of human visual cortex to faces, letterstrings, and textures: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 16, 5205 5215 (1996). 12. Zeki, S. & ffytche, D. H. The Riddoch syndrome: insights into the neurobiology of conscious vision. Brain 121, 25 45 (1998). 13. Zeki, S. et al. A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 11, 641 649 (1991). 14. Mckeefry, D. J. & Zeki, S. The position and topography of the human colour centre as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Brain 120, 2229 2242 (1997). 15. Penfield, W. & Rasmussen, T. The Cerebral Cortex of Man (Macmillan, New York, 1950). 16. Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J. & Chun, M. M. The fusiform face area: a nature neuroscience volume 1 no 8 december 1998 741

module in human extrastriate cortex specialised for face perception. J. Neurosci. 17, 4302 4311 (1997). 17. Sergent, J., Ohta, S. & Macdonald, B. Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing. Brain 115, 15 36 (1992). 18. Kanwisher, N., Chun, M. M., McDermott, J. & Ledden, P. J. Functional imaging of human visual recognition. Cog. Brain Res. 5, 55 67 (1996). 19. Howard, R.J. et al. The functional anatomy of imagining and perceiving colour. Neuroreport 9, 1019 1023 (1998). 20. Crick, F. & Koch, C. Are we aware of neural activity in primary visual cortex? Nature 375, 121 123 (1995). 21. ffytche, D. H., Guy, C. N. & Zeki, S. Motion specific responses from a blind hemifield. Brain 119, 1971 1982 (1996). 22. Zeki, S. A Vision of the Brain (Blackwell, Oxford, 1993). 23. ffytche, D. H. & Zeki, S. Brain activity related to the perception of illusory contours. Neuroimage 3, 104 108 (1996). 24. Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E. & McHugh, P. R. Mini-mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatry Res. 12, 189 198 (1975). 25. Kwong, K. K. et al. Dynamic magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activity during primary sensory stimulation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 89, 5675 5679 (1992). 26. Friston, K. J. et al. Movement-related effects in fmri time-series. Magn. Reson. Med. 35, 346 355 (1996). 27. Talairach, J. & Tournoux, P. Co-planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain (Thieme, Stuttgart, 1988). 28. Bullmore, E. et al. Statistical methods of estimation and inference for functional MR image analysis. Magn. Reson. Med. 35, 261 277 (1996). 29. Brammer, M.J. et al. Generic brain activation mapping in functional magnetic resonance imaging: a nonparametric approach. Magn. Reson. Imaging 15, 763 770 (1997). 742 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 8 december 1998