Memory reconsolidation mediates the strengthening of memories by additional learning Lee, Jonathan

Similar documents
JLC Lee: Memory reconsolidation mediates the strengthening of memories by additional learning

Reconsolidation: maintaining memory relevance Lee, Jonathan

Subjects. Thirty-five adult male Lister Hooded rats (Charles River, Kent, UK),

The EARNEST study : interarm blood pressure differences should also be recorded Moody, William; Ferro, Charles; Townend, Jonathan

Behavioural memory reconsolidation of food and fear memories

Appetitive Pavlovian goal-tracking memories reconsolidate only under specific conditions

The use of random projections for the analysis of mass spectrometry imaging data Palmer, Andrew; Bunch, Josephine; Styles, Iain

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition

Where a licence is displayed above, please note the terms and conditions of the licence govern your use of this document.

Memory Retrieval and the Passage of Time: From Reconsolidation and Strengthening to Extinction

De novo mrna synthesis is required for both consolidation and reconsolidation of fear memories in the amygdala

A different view on immunity Dempsey, Claire; Jones, Nicholas

Memory retention the synaptic stability versus plasticity dilemma

Report. Detection of a Temporal Error Triggers Reconsolidation of Amygdala-Dependent Memories

A single standard for memory: the case for reconsolidation

Where a licence is displayed above, please note the terms and conditions of the licence govern your use of this document.

Brain Region-Specific Gene Expression Activation Required for Reconsolidation and Extinction of Contextual Fear Memory

Research. Travis E. Brown, Brian R. Lee, and Barbara A. Sorg 1

Involvement of the anterior cingulate cortex in formation, consolidation, and reconsolidation of recent and remote contextual fear memory

DNA and Histone Methylation in Learning and Memory

Screening for ovarian cancer Kehoe, Sean

memory Examples: Obama is president, PSYC 2 is in Price Center Theater, my 21st birthday was a disaster

Supporting Online Material for

Declarative memory includes semantic, episodic, and spatial memory, and

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

<student name> Undergraduate Research Grant Proposal

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Cue-Induced Cocaine Seeking and Relapse Are Reduced by Disruption of Drug Memory Reconsolidation

Behavioural Brain Research 171 (2006) Research report. A.J. Yim, C.R.G. Moraes, T.L. Ferreira, M.G.M. Oliveira

Consolidation and Reconsolidation of Incentive Learning in the Amygdala

Molecular mechanisms of memory reconsolidation

Reconsolidation Allows Fear Memory to Be Updated to a Less Aversive Level through the Incorporation of Appetitive Information

Consolidation and Reconsolidation of Memory in Black-Capped Chickadees (Poecile atricapillus)

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Synaptic Plasticity and NO-cGMP-PKG Signaling Regulate Pre- and Postsynaptic Alterations at Rat Lateral Amygdala Synapses Following Fear Conditioning

Double Dissociation of Amygdala and Hippocampal Contributions to Trace and Delay Fear Conditioning

MY APPROACH to the use of NOACs for stroke prevention in patients with atrial fibrillation Lip, Gregory

Infusions of AP5 into the basolateral amygdala impair the formation, but not the expression, of step-down inhibitory avoidance

Supplementary Methods

The Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

Behavioural Brain Research

Acquisition of new information can be stored into at least two

mtorc2 controls actin polymerization required for consolidation of long-term memory

Memory reconsolidation

Without retrieval, memories would be unusable. Retrieval,

Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet

Synap&c Plas&city. long-term plasticity (~30 min to lifetime) Long-term potentiation (LTP) / Long-term depression (LTD)

COGNITIVE SCIENCE 107A. Hippocampus. Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D.

LONG TERM MEMORY. Learning Objective Topics. Retrieval and the Brain. Retrieval Neuroscience of Memory. LTP Brain areas Consolidation Reconsolidation

Nucleus accumbens lesions impair context, but not cue, conditioning in rats

Updating Procedures Can Reorganize the Neural Circuit Supporting a Fear Memory

Brain Mechanisms of Memory and Cognition 5 Neural basis of memory (2): multiple memory systems

The Amygdala. Sainsbury Wellcome Centre

THE ROLE OF NMDA RECEPTORS IN THE DORSAL HIPPOCAMPUS IN COCAINE-PAIRED CONTEXTUAL MEMORY RECONSOLIDATION. Kati L. Healey

Memory: Computation, Genetics, Physiology, and Behavior. James L. McClelland Stanford University

Characteristics of retrograde amnesia for CS preexposure

Memory for context becomes less specific with time

Multiple memory trace theory Duncan, 1949

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Nat Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2006 September 5.

EFFECTS OF NITRIC OXIDE SYNTHASE INHIBITOR N G -NITRO-L-ARGININE METHYL ESTER ON SPATIAL AND CUED LEANING

Cellular and Systems Reconsolidation in the Hippocampus

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Review

Abstract. R. Roesler 1, J. Quevedo 1, C. Rodrigues 1, M. Madruga 1, M.R.M. Vianna 1 and M.B.C. Ferreira 2

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neuroscience. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2006 December 12.

Emotional Memory, PTSD, and Clinical Intervention Updates

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Learning and Memory II

Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory

Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140

A Synaptic Reinforcement-Based Model for Transient Amnesia Following Disruptions of Memory Consolidation and Reconsolidation

Running head: EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL ON OBJECT RECOGNITION. Impairing Effects of Alcohol on Object Recognition. A Senior Honors Thesis

BRAIN MECHANISMS OF REWARD AND ADDICTION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Cellular Imaging of zif268

Stress within a Restricted Time Window Selectively Affects the Persistence of Long-Term Memory

9.01 Introduction to Neuroscience Fall 2007

Memory consolidation of Pavlovian fear conditioning: a cellular and molecular perspective

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

Feedback Education and Neuroscience. Pankaj Sah

Shervin Gholizadeh, Ninglei Sun, Xavier De Jaeger, Melanie Bechard, Lique Coolen, Steven R. Laviolette*

What is the Role of the Amygdala in Long Term Memory? Jack Pemment. University of Mississippi

More dendritic spines, changes in shapes of dendritic spines More NT released by presynaptic membrane

Learning and Memory. The Case of H.M.

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman

ABSTRACT THE AMYGDALA IS CRITICAL FOR TRACE, DELAY, AND CONTEXTUAL FEAR CONDITIONING. by Daniel Kochli

Renewal of Fear Following Immediate Extinction in a Passive Avoidance Paradigm

Is There Savings for Pavlovian Fear Conditioning after Neurotoxic Basolateral Amygdala Lesions in Rats?

Cannabinoid Receptor Activation in the Basolateral Amygdala Blocks the Effects of Stress on the Conditioning and Extinction of Inhibitory Avoidance

Synaptic plasticity. Mark van Rossum. Institute for Adaptive and Neural Computation University of Edinburgh

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

S100B+ Cell Density day Spine Formation (%)

A role for nitric oxide-driven retrograde signaling in the consolidation of a fear memory

Where a licence is displayed above, please note the terms and conditions of the licence govern your use of this document.

CA3 NMDA Receptors are Required for the Rapid Formation of a Salient Contextual Representation

Integrated Memory for Object, Place, and Context in Rats: A Possible Model of Episodic-Like Memory?

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

The Amygdala Modulates Memory Consolidation of Fear-Motivated Inhibitory Avoidance Learning But Not Classical Fear Conditioning

The Effects of Exercise on Memory. Is timing everything?

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Theories of memory. Memory & brain Cellular bases of learning & memory. Epileptic patient Temporal lobectomy Amnesia

Transcription:

Memory reconsolidation mediates the strengthening of memories by additional learning Lee, Jonathan DOI: 10.1038/nn.2205 Document Version Peer reviewed version Citation for published version (Harvard): Lee, J 2008, 'Memory reconsolidation mediates the strengthening of memories by additional learning' Nature Neuroscience, vol 11, no. 11, pp. 1264-6. DOI: 10.1038/nn.2205 Link to publication on Research at Birmingham portal General rights Unless a licence is specified above, all rights (including copyright and moral rights) in this document are retained by the authors and/or the copyright holders. The express permission of the copyright holder must be obtained for any use of this material other than for purposes permitted by law. Users may freely distribute the URL that is used to identify this publication. Users may download and/or print one copy of the publication from the University of Birmingham research portal for the purpose of private study or non-commercial research. User may use extracts from the document in line with the concept of fair dealing under the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988 (?) Users may not further distribute the material nor use it for the purposes of commercial gain. Where a licence is displayed above, please note the terms and conditions of the licence govern your use of this document. When citing, please reference the published version. Take down policy While the University of Birmingham exercises care and attention in making items available there are rare occasions when an item has been uploaded in error or has been deemed to be commercially or otherwise sensitive. If you believe that this is the case for this document, please contact UBIRA@lists.bham.ac.uk providing details and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate. Download date: 10. Feb. 2018

1 Memory reconsolidation mediates the strengthening of memories by additional learning Jonathan L. C. Lee School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham B15 2TT, UK Text: 2018 words Figures: 2 Supplementary figures: 7 Supplementary text: Methods Title: 85 characters Abstract: 66 words References: 14 Correspondence should be addressed to JLCL (j.l.c.lee@bham.ac.uk) Acknowledgements: This work was supported by a grant from the Royal Society and was conducted within the MRC/Wellcome Trust Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, UK. I thank Barry J. Everitt and David Belin for helpful discussions, as well as Yann Pelloux and Melissa Wood for practical cooperation.

2 Memories are dynamic, rather than static, in nature. The reactivation of a memory through re-exposure to salient training stimuli results in its destabilisation, necessitating a restabilisation process known as reconsolidation, a disruption of which leads to amnesia. Here I show that one normal function of hippocampal memory reconsolidation in rats is to modify the strength of a contextual fear memory as a result of further learning. Following their initial acquisition and consolidation, memories can be modified through further experience. For example additional learning strengthens an already-established memory trace. It is, however, not known whether such a change in memory strength depends upon the same cellular mechanisms as initial learning. The phenomenon of memory reconsolidation, as revealed by the demonstration of an experimentally-induced retrograde amnesia for a consolidated memory in a manner that is critically dependent upon the reactivation of that memory 1, has been suggested to enable the updating of a previously-acquired memory 2,3. However, it has not yet been demonstrated that reconsolidation is necessary to update memories in an animal model 2,4, although in some settings memory reconsolidation is only observed under conditions in which memory updating occurs 5,6. Therefore, it remains unclear whether the modification of a memory, in particular its strengthening, depends upon reconsolidation mechanisms. Memory reconsolidation consists of two phases, a reactivation-dependent destabilisation process, followed by the protein synthesis-dependent restabilisation phase 1. Reconsolidation can be isolated from initial memory consolidation using the doubly dissociable mechanisms of hippocampal contextual fear memories 7, the destabilisation of which is also dependent upon synaptic protein degradation 8. Here, a second learning trial (see Supplementary Methods online; all procedures were conducted in accordance with the UK 1986 Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act (Project License PPL 80/1767)) strengthened a contextual fear memory, but only following its

3 destabilisation (Supplementary Fig. 1). Infusion of the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin into the hippocampus immediately after the second trial resulted in subsequent amnesia, consistent with previous findings in an auditory fear conditioning procedure 9. The use of the broad-spectrum translational inhibitor, as well as having important side-effects 10, does not enable isolation of consolidation and reconsolidation mechanisms, and so it remained possible that the amnesia resulted from inhibition of both reconsolidation of the trial 1 memory and consolidation of the new trial 2 memory 9. Hence, updating memory strength might depend upon consolidation, and not reconsolidation mechanisms. Given that there are doubly dissociable cellular mechanisms of hippocampal contextual fear memory consolidation and reconsolidation 7, BDNF (brain-derived neurotrophic factor) being required for consolidation and zif268 (also known as EGR1, NGFI-A and Krox24) being necessary for reconsolidation, it was possible to determine whether the memory strengthening incurred by additional learning involved consolidation or reconsolidation processes. If memory strengthening involved consolidation mechanisms, both BDNF and zif268 would have to be knocked down in order to disrupt the reconsolidation of the existing memory as well as the consolidation of the new memory, and produce amnesia. However, in contrast to this hypothesis, knockdown of BDNF in the dorsal hippocampus during a second learning trial by antisense oligodeoxynucleotide () infusion, as used previously 7 and demonstrated to be functionally active by disrupting contextual fear memory consolidation (Supplementary Fig. 2 online), had no effect upon the subsequent expression of the strengthened contextual fear memory relative to the infusion of control missense sequences (), even when the concentration of oligodeoxynucleotide was doubled (Supplementary Fig. 3 online). Instead, infusion of zif268 alone into the dorsal hippocampus was sufficient to cause a subsequent severe amnesia (Fig. 1a). This amnesia was of the same magnitude as that following anisomycin infusion and both took several hours to emerge, as evidenced by the intact short-term memory (STM) observed 3 hours after the second learning trial, and was long-lasting for at least 7 days (Supplementary Fig. 4 online). These observations are consistent with typical memory reconsolidation deficits 11 and, coupled with

4 the previous demonstration that zif268 infusion selectively impairs memory reconsolidation 7, support the assertion that knockdown of zif268 in the dorsal hippocampus impaired memory strengthening through the blockade of hippocampal memory reconsolidation. While zif268-dependent reconsolidation mechanisms appear to mediate memory strengthening, the amnestic effect of zif268 might be related instead to the increased absolute strength of the contextual fear memory conditioned through two learning trials, relative to that resulting from the single trial learning that functionally recruits BDNF (Supplementary Fig. 1 online). However, when a stronger fear conditioning procedure was used, in which both trials were condensed into a single session, memory consolidation remained dependent upon hippocampal BDNF and not zif268 (Fig. 1b,c and Supplementary Fig. 5 online). Therefore, these data reveal a double dissociation between the cellular mechanisms of initial memory consolidation (BDNF) and memory strengthening through additional learning (zif268). Moreover, the selective dependence of the two processes upon their relative cellular mechanisms is not a result of non-specific or quantitative factors. A further contention might be that zif268 is not required for memory strengthening per se, but instead for synaptic plasticity in neural circuits that have been modified recently by behavioural experience. Thus the selective dependence of additional learning upon zif268 may result not from the fact that an existing memory is being updated and strengthened, but arises because some learning experience, which need not have been related, engaged the dorsal hippocampus on the previous day. To test this hypothesis, rats were first conditioned in a separate context (different operant chambers 12 ), before being returned to the standard fear conditioning apparatus (context 2) for the second day of training. Infusion of zif268 into the dorsal hippocampus prior to this second conditioning trial had no effect on subsequent conditioned freezing in context 2 (Fig. 2a and Supplementary Fig. 6 online). In contrast, knockdown of hippocampal BDNF resulted in severe amnesia, suggesting that even when there has been recent synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus, contextual fear conditioning

5 functionally recruits consolidation, but not reconsolidation, mechanisms. Moreover, these results provide strong evidence that memory strengthening has separable underlying mechanisms to memory acquisition/consolidation. If the mechanisms of reconsolidation are the same as those that strengthen the memory trace, impairing memory destabilisation should prevent the modification of memory strength. It has been shown recently that hippocampal synaptic protein degradation is a critical process in the destabilisation of contextual fear memories 8. Infusion of the proteasome inhibitor lac into the dorsal hippocampus prevented the amnestic action of anisomycin in a standard memory reconsolidation setting. Thus whereas anisomycin infusion alone, immediately after contextual re-exposure, resulted in subsequent amnesia, the co-infusion of lac rendered the memory invulnerable to protein synthesis inhibition 8. Here, anisomycin alone also resulted in amnesia when infused into the dorsal hippocampus immediately after a second learning trial (Fig. 2b and Supplementary Fig. 1 online). However, whereas the co-administration of lac mitigated against the amnestic effect of anisomycin, the resultant levels of contextual freezing remained significantly lower than vehicle-infused controls. Moreover, infusion of lac, regardless of whether it was combined with anisomycin, resulted in the failure of additional learning to strengthen the pre-existing contextual fear memory (see also Supplementary Fig. 7 online). Therefore, preventing memory destabilisation maintained the strength of the previously-acquired memory at a constant level, further supporting the assertion that memory reconsolidation is the mechanism by which memories are strengthened through additional learning. Given that the process of memory destabilisation is an integral step in the strengthening of memories, greater emphasis must be placed upon the mechanisms of destabilisation that are only beginning to be delineated 8,13,14. Another important implication of the present findings is that memory reconsolidation may, in fact, be the predominant process that occurs during learning and memory in situations that involve more than a single training trial. However, given that these findings are limited to certain training parameters in a contextual

6 fear procedure, it remains to be determined to what extent they can be generalised and whether any boundary conditions exist. Nevertheless, it remains likely that the persistence of memories acquired through repeated experience will be understood primarily through the study of memory reconsolidation, rather than initial consolidation, given their dissociable mechanisms. Moreover, these findings demonstrate that memory reconsolidation has an adaptive function in normal learning and memory, by showing that it enables the modification of memory strength. References 1 Nader, K. Memory traces unbound. Trends Neurosci 26, 65-72. (2003). 2 Dudai, Y. Reconsolidation: the advantage of being refocused. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16, 174-178 (2006). 3 Sara, S. J. Retrieval and reconsolidation: Toward a neurobiology of remembering. Learn Mem 7, 73-84 (2000). 4 Tronel, S., Milekic, M. H. & Alberini, C. M. Linking new information to a reactivated memory requires consolidation and not reconsolidation mechanisms. PLoS Biol 3, e293 (2005). 5 Morris, R. G. et al. Memory Reconsolidation: Sensitivity of Spatial Memory to Inhibition of Protein Synthesis in Dorsal Hippocampus during Encoding and Retrieval. Neuron 50, 479-489 (2006). 6 Rodriguez-Ortiz, C. J., Garcia-DeLaTorre, P., Benavidez, E., Ballesteros, M. A. & Bermudez-Rattoni, F. Intrahippocampal anisomycin infusions disrupt previously consolidated spatial memory only when memory is updated. Neurobiol Learn Mem 89, 352-359 (2008). 7 Lee, J. L. C., Everitt, B. J. & Thomas, K. L. Independent cellular processes for hippocampal memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Science 304, 839-843 (2004). 8 Lee, S. H. et al. Synaptic protein degradation underlies destabilization of retrieved fear memory. Science 319, 1253-1256 (2008). 9 Duvarci, S. & Nader, K. Characterization of fear memory reconsolidation. J Neurosci 24, 9269-9275 (2004). 10 Rudy, J. W., Biedenkapp, J. C., Moineau, J. & Bolding, K. Anisomycin and the reconsolidation hypothesis. Learn Mem 13, 1-3 (2006). 11 Dudai, Y. The neurobiology of consolidations, or, how stable is the engram? Annu Rev Psychol 55, 51-86 (2004). 12 Pelloux, Y., Everitt, B. J. & Dickinson, A. Compulsive drug seeking by rats under punishment: effects of drug taking history. Psychopharmacology 194, 127-137, doi:10.1007/s00213-007-0805-0 (2007). 13 Ben Mamou, C., Gamache, K. & Nader, K. NMDA receptors are critical for unleashing consolidated auditory fear memories. Nat Neurosci 9, 1237-1239 (2006). 14 Suzuki, A., Mukawa, T., Tsukagoshi, A., Frankland, P. W. & Kida, S. Activation of LVGCCs and CB1 receptors required for destabilization of reactivated contextual fear memories. Learn Mem 15, 426-433 (2008).

7 Figure legends Figure 1. (a) Knockdown of zif268, but not BDNF, during a second conditioning session results in subsequent amnesia. Rats were fear conditioned on two consecutive days (Cond1 & Cond2), and were tested 24 hr later (Test). ANOVA revealed a significant Session x Gene x interaction (F 2,38 =18.73, P<0.001). Analysis of the BDNF groups alone revealed no main effect of or Session x interaction (F s<1), whereas analysis of the zif268 groups revealed a significant Session x interaction (F 2,20 =38.71, P<0.001), driven by a significant effect of during the test (simple effects one-way ANOVA; P<0.05), with no differences during Cond1 and Cond2 (n=5 6 per group). Knockdown of (b) BDNF, but not (c) zif268, during a single 2-trial conditioning session results in subsequent amnesia. Rats were tested 3 hr (STM), 24 hr (LTM) and 7 d (LTM2) after conditioning. ANOVA revealed an overall significant Session x Gene x interaction (F 2,44 =11.11, P<0.001). Analysis of the zif268 groups alone revealed no main effect of (F 1,12 =1.05, P=0.32) or Session x interaction (F 2,24 =1.23, P=0.31), whereas analysis of the BDNF groups revealed a significant Session x interaction (F 2,20 =19.95, P<0.001), which was driven by a significant effect of during the LTM and LTM2 tests (simple effects one-way ANOVA; P<0.05), with no effect on the STM test (n=6 7 per group). Data presented as mean ± s.e.m. Figure 2. (a) Knockdown of BDNF, but not zif268, during fear conditioning to a changed context results in subsequent amnesia. Rats were fear conditioned first to CX1 (Cond1), on the next day (Cond2) to CX2, and were tested 24 hr later (Test) in CX2. While ANOVA revealed no gene x interaction during Cond1 (F<1), there was an overall significant Session x Gene x interaction in CX2 (F 1,22 =11.31, P=0.003). Analysis of the zif268 groups alone revealed no main effect of or Session x interaction (F s<1), whereas the BDNF groups had a significant Session x interaction (F 1,12 =22.17, P<0.001), driven by a significant effect of

8 during the LTM test (simple effects one-way ANOVA; P<0.05; n=6 7 per group). (b) Proteasome inhibition protects a contextual fear memory against both additional learning and amnesia. Rats were fear conditioned on two consecutive days (Cond1 & Cond2), and were tested 24 hr later (Test). ANOVA revealed a significant Session x ANI x lac interaction (F 2,48 =26.80, P<0.001). There were significant Session x lac interactions for the ANI (F 2,24 =19.24, P<0.001) and VEH (F 2,24 =9.16, p=0.001) groups analysed separately, but no differences between the two groups infused with lac (F s<1). Simple effects analysis (one-way ANOVA; P<0.05) revealed that at Test, the lac groups were significantly different from both the VEH and ANI alone groups, and that while the VEH and ANI alone groups changed in freezing levels from Cond2 to Test, the lac groups did not (n=7 per group). Data presented as mean ± s.e.m.

9

Percentage time freezing Percentage time freezing 10 80 60 40 VEH ANI 20 0 Cond1 Cond2 Test Session Supplementary Fig. 1. Protein synthesis inhibition after a second contextual fear conditioning session results in subsequent amnesia. Rats were fear conditioned on two consecutive days (Cond1 & Cond2), immediately after the second of which they were infused with anisomycin into the dorsal hippocampus, and then were tested 24 hr later (Test). Repeated measures ANOVA revealed an overall significant Session x ANI interaction (F 2,20 =56.67, P<0.001), which was driven by a significant effect of ANI during the Test (simple effects one-way ANOVA; P<0.05), with no differences during Cond1 and Cond2. VEH controls showed a statistically significant 46.9% increase in freezing from Cond2 to Test (F 1,6 =25.9, P<0.001). Data presented as mean ± s.e.m. (n=7 per group). 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 LTM Supplementary Fig. 2. Verification of BDNF efficacy. Infusion of the standard dose of BDNF (1 nmol/ l) into the dorsal hippocampus 90 min before contextual fear conditioning results in severe amnesia 24 hr later (one-way ANOVA: F 1,13 =20.88, P=0.001). Data presented as mean + s.e.m. (n=6 per group).

Percentage time freezing Percentage time freezing 11 a b 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 BDNF- BDNF- Zif268- Zif268- Group c 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 BDNF- BDNF- Zif268- Zif268- Group Supplementary Fig. 3. Knockdown of zif268, but not BDNF, during a second conditioning session results in subsequent amnesia (supplement to Figure 1A). (a) cannula placements for the rats included in the statistical analysis ( =BDNF ; X=BDNF ; =zif268 ; =zif268 ; Bregma 3.00, 3.24, 3.48, 3.72 & 3.96 mm). (b) no amnesia was observed 3 hr after Cond2 (one-way ANOVA: F<1). (c) rats infused with zif268 remained impaired 7 d later (zif268: one-way ANOVA: F 1,10 =91.02, P<0.001; BDNF: one-way ANOVA: F<1). Data presented as mean + s.e.m. (n=5 6 per group).

Percentage time freezing 12 a b 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 Cond1 Cond2 Test Session Supplementary Fig. 4. Infusion of a high dose of BDNF before a second conditioning session has no effect on subsequent conditioned freezing. (a) cannula placements for the rats included in the statistical analysis ( =BDNF ; X=BDNF ; Bregma 3.00, 3.24, 3.48, 3.72 & 3.96 mm). (b) rats were fear conditioned on two consecutive days (Cond1 & Cond2), 90 min before the second of which they were infused with BDNF or (2 nmol/ l), and then were tested 24 hr later (Test). Repeated measures ANOVA revealed no main effect of, nor a significant Session x interaction (F s<1). Data presented as mean ± s.e.m. (n=5 6 per group).

Supplementary Fig. 5. Cannula placements for the rats included in the statistical analysis of Fig. 1b,c ( =BDNF ; X=BDNF ; =zif268 ; =zif268 ; Bregma 3.00, 3.24, 3.48, 3.72 & 3.96 mm). 13

Percentage time freezing Percentage time freezing 14 a b 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 BDNF- BDNF- Zif268- Zif268- Group c 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 BDNF- BDNF- Zif268- Zif268- Group Supplementary Fig. 6. Knockdown of BDNF, but not zif268, during a fear conditioning to a changed context results in subsequent amnesia (supplement to Fig. 2a). (a) cannula placements for the rats included in the statistical analysis ( =BDNF ; X=BDNF ; =zif268 ; =zif268 ; Bregma 3.00, 3.24, 3.48, 3.72 & 3.96 mm). (b) no amnesia was observed 3 hr after Cond2 (one-way ANOVA: F<1). (c) rats infused with BDNF remained impaired 7 d later (BDNF: one-way ANOVA: F 1,12 =24.27, P<0.001; zif268: one-way ANOVA: F<1). Data presented as mean + s.e.m. (n=6 7 per group).

Percentage time freezing Percentage time freezing 15 a b c 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 VEH/VEH PBS/PBS VEH/ANI PBS/ANI lac/pbs b-lac/pbs lac/ani b-lac/ani Group VEH/VEH PBS/PBS PBS/ANI VEH/ANI lac/pbs b-lac/pbs lac/ani b-lac/ani Group Supplementary Fig. 7. Proteasome inhibition protects a contextual fear memory against both additional learning and amnesia (supplement to Fig. 2b). (a) cannula placements for the rats included in the statistical analysis ( =PBS; X=ANI; = lac/pbs; = lac/ani; Bregma 3.00, 3.24, 3.48, 3.72 & 3.96 mm). (b) no amnesia was observed 3 hr after Cond2 (one-way ANOVA: F<1). (c) the pattern of long-term amnesia remained the same 7 d later (one-way ANOVA: F 3,24 =14.01, P<0.001; tukey s post-hoc analysis [P<0.05] revealed the lac groups to be different from both other groups, but not to be different from each other). Data presented as mean + s.e.m. (n=7 per group).

16 Supplementary Text Methods Subjects and surgical procedures. 142 adult male Lister Hooded rats, weighing 280-320 g at the time of surgery, were implanted with chronic indwelling cannulae targeting the dorsal hippocampus as described previously 1. At the end of the experiment, rats were perfused, and their brains were sectioned and stained to confirm cannula placements as previously described 1. Cannula placements are shown on reproduced atlas figures 2. 10 rats were excluded from the statistical analysis due to misplaced cannulae or failure to complete the full schedule of testing. Infusions. Infusion procedures were as previously described 1. Rats were habituated to the infusion procedure using the PBS vehicle on 2 occasions prior to behavioural training. Infusions (1 l, 0.5 l/min) were carried out of oligodeoxynucleotides (sequences and concentrations as previously used 1 ), or PBS/D vehicle, anisomycin (80 g/ l), clastolactacystin- -lactone ( lac; 32 ng/ l) and a combination of anisomycin and lac prepared as previously described 3. Behavioural procedures. Training and testing took place in 4 operant chambers as previously described 4. The rats were subjected to a previously described contextual fear conditioning procedure 1. For the memory strengthening experiments, two minutes after being placed singly in the chambers, the rats were exposed to a single unsignalled footshock (0.5 ma, 2 s), 60 s after which the session terminated. On the next day the rats received intra-hippocampal infusions either 90 min before (oligodeoxynucleotides) or immediately after (anisomycin/ lac) an identical training trial, and then were tested repeatedly in 2-min context re-exposure sessions at three subsequent timepoints: 3 hr (STM), 24 hr (LTM test) & 7 d (LTM2). For the stronger conditioning experiment, rats were given intra-hippocampal infusions of oligodeoxynucleotides 90 min prior to an extended 4-min conditioning session in which they received 2 unsignalled footshocks after 2 and 3 mins, and then were tested as

17 before. Behaviour was video recorded for later analysis of conditioned freezing by an experimenter blind to the experimental conditions as previously described 1. 1 J. L. C. Lee, B. J. Everitt, and K. L. Thomas, Science 304 (5672), 839 (2004). 2 G. Paxinos and C. Watson, The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates, 6 ed. (Academic Press, New York, 2007). 3 S. H. Lee, J. H. Choi, N. Lee et al., Science 319 (5867), 1253 (2008). 4 J. L. C. Lee, A. Dickinson, and B. J. Everitt, Behav Brain Res 159 (2), 221 (2005).