Laboratory Exercise in Sensory Physiology Student Lab Manual

Similar documents
Ganglion Cells Blind Spot Cornea Pupil Visual Area of the Bipolar Cells Thalamus Rods and Cones Lens Visual cortex of the occipital lobe

Senses and Sense Organs

Guess: Correct or Incorrect. Trial (perform in random order)

Practice Test Questions

Vision and Audition. This section concerns the anatomy of two important sensory systems, the visual and the auditory systems.

Presentation On SENSATION. Prof- Mrs.Kuldeep Kaur

Definition Slides. Sensation. Perception. Bottom-up processing. Selective attention. Top-down processing 11/3/2013

= add definition here. Definition Slide

Biology. Slide 1 of 49. End Show. Copyright Pearson Prentice Hall

Sensation and Perception. Chapter 6

Copyright 2009 Pearson Education, Inc.

Psychology Chapter 4. Sensation and Perception. Most amazing introduction ever!! Turn to page 77 and prepare to be amazed!

The lowest level of stimulation that a person can detect. absolute threshold. Adapting one's current understandings to incorporate new information.

Chapter 4: Sensation and Perception The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc.

Sensation and Perception. 8.2 The Senses

Unit 4: Sensation and Perception

Biological Psychology. Unit Two AD Mr. Cline Marshall High School Psychology

NERVOUS SYSTEM & SENSES TEACHER COPY

Organs of the Nervous System: brain, spinal cord, and nerves

7. Sharp perception or vision 8. The process of transferring genetic material from one cell to another by a plasmid or bacteriophage

Mr. Silimperi Council Rock High School South Chapter 5 Sensation Sensation II

Sensation and Perception. A. Sensation: awareness of simple characteristics B. Perception: making complex interpretations

2 Sensing the Environment

Chapter 5 Test Review. Try the practice questions in the Study Guide and on line

SENSES: VISION. Chapter 5: Sensation AP Psychology Fall 2014

Physiology of human perception

A. Acuity B. Adaptation C. Awareness D. Reception E. Overload

SENSATION & PERCEPTION

Hearing. istockphoto/thinkstock

Chapter 29 The Senses

The Perceptual Experience

Chapter 18. The Senses SENSORY RECEPTION. Introduction: Superhuman Senses. Introduction: Superhuman Senses

-Detect heat or cold and help maintain body temperature

l3;~~?~~~,'0~'~~t~t:~:~~~~~~~~~~!,1

CHAPTER ONE LEARNING AREA: The World Through Our Senses LEARNING OBJECTIVES:

Dikran J. Martin. Psychology 110. Name: Date: Making Contact with the World around Us. Principal Features

HOW DO HUMAN SENSORS WORK? - UNDERSTANDING HUMAN SENSORS AND COMPARING THEM WITH THOSE IN A ROBOT

Sensation and Perception

Sensory Systems Vision, Audition, Somatosensation, Gustation, & Olfaction

Psy393: Cognitive Neuroscience. Prof. Anderson Department of Psychology Week 3

the human 1 of 3 Lecture 6 chapter 1 Remember to start on your paper prototyping

Sense system. Introduction The visual system Hearing. Introduction to sensory mechanisms

Sensing and Perceiving Our World

Outline 2/19/2013. Please see me after class: Sarah Pagliero Ryan Paul Demetrius Prowell-Reed Ashley Rehm Giovanni Reynel Patricia Rochin

Introduction to Physiological Psychology

Chapter 18 Senses SENSORY RECEPTION 10/21/2011. Sensory Receptors and Sensations. Sensory Receptors and Sensations. Sensory Receptors and Sensations

Nervous System. Made of two parts. Central Peripheral

d). Draw the following neural circuits (using the notation taught in class) and then say what would happen if they were stimulated as specified.

Sensation and Perception

Chapter 2. The Senses

Hearing. PSYCHOLOGY (8th Edition, in Modules) David Myers. Module 14. Hearing. Hearing

The Sense Organs 10/13/2016. The Human Eye. 1. Sclera 2. Choroid 3. Retina. The eye is made up of three layers:

4. Which letter in figure 9.1 points to the fovea centralis? Ans: b

Answer: B difficulty: 2 conceptual Goal 3: Critical Thinking Skills in Psychology

Human Anatomy and Physiology - ANAT 14 Sensory System Lab Goals Activities

Auditory Physiology PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 30. Organ of Corti

Senses Other Than Vision. Hearing (Audition) Transmission of Vibrations

The Senses. Lab Exercise 30. Objectives. Introduction

THE NERVOUS SYSTEM Functions of the Nervous System nervous system stimulus response Neuron structure and function neurons nerve impulses dendrite

Vision Seeing is in the mind

Perception of Sound. To hear sound, your ear has to do three basic things:

Sensation Outline Chapter 5, Psychology, David G Meyers, 7 th Edition

Norwood Science Center

Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception

is the clear, transparent part at the front of the eye. It allows light to enter the eye and it also refracts (focuses) the light onto the retina.

The Senses. senses are almost impossible to describe, and yet we use them every moment of the day.

Chapter 3. Sounds, Signals, and Studio Acoustics

Sensing the Environment

First Exam. Sensation and Perception. Process of Forming Sensations. Sensation and Perception. The Eye. Our perceptual experience. Mean = 57.

Chapter 7: The Nervous System

Intro to Audition & Hearing

Coding of Sensory Information

The olfactory epithelium is located at the roof of the nasal cavity. Nasal conchae cause turbulance of incoming air

1 ahmadanizahscienceteacher14

Ch. 9 Sensory Systems. Steps of sensation and perception

Converting Sound Waves into Neural Signals, Part 1. What happens to initiate neural signals for sound?

Learning Targets. Module 20. Hearing Explain how the ear transforms sound energy into neural messages.

Systems Neuroscience Oct. 16, Auditory system. http:

Sound Waves. Sensation and Perception. Sound Waves. Sound Waves. Sound Waves

ID# Final Exam PS325, Fall 1997

17.4 Sound and Hearing

Stimulus any aspect of or change in the environment to which an organism responds. Sensation what occurs when a stimulus activates a receptor

Sounds Good to Me. Engagement. Next Generation Science Standards (NGSS)

Introduction. Chapter The Perceptual Process

Test Bank Chapter 2: The Beginnings of Perception

Structure of the eye and retina

Sensation and Perception

Receptors / physiology

Review Sheet: Sensation and Perception (6-8%) Sensation. Date Period. 1) sensation. 2) perception. 3) bottom-up processing. 4) top-down processing

PSYC 441 Cognitive Psychology II

Review on Nervous System, Senses and Musculoskeletal System

Unit 4 Practice. PSYCHOLOGY SECTION I Time-- Minutes Questions, Unit 4 Practice/Quiz

Sensory Systems. BIOLOGY OF HUMANS Concepts, Applications, and Issues. Judith Goodenough Betty McGuire

Auditory Physiology PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 29. Hearing

PSY 310: Sensory and Perceptual Processes 1

INTRODUCTION: ****************************************************************************************************

Physiology Unit 2 SENSORY PHYSIOLOGY

SPHSC 462 HEARING DEVELOPMENT. Overview Review of Hearing Science Introduction

The Ear. The ear can be divided into three major parts: the outer ear, the middle ear and the inner ear.

SENSATION AND PERCEPTION

Transcription:

Laboratory Exercise in Sensory Physiology Student Lab Manual Introduction Sensory organs allow us to perceive our environment by converting energy sources in the environment, like light or sound, to nerve impulses which our brains can interpret. The process of converting environmental stimuli into nerve impulses is called transduction. The world, which we perceive, is entirely dependent on the transduction properties of our sense organs. These properties differ between animal species. The world to a human looks, tastes, sounds, smells, etc. completely different from that perceived by another animal. Each sensory organ responds to a particular kind of input, and only to a relatively small range of possible stimuli within that modality. The visual system, for example, responds to light, which is a form of electromagnetic radiation. We can only see a small portion of the possible wavelengths of this radiation, because our eyes can only respond to certain wavelengths (see Table 1). Other insects, and birds, for example, can see ultraviolet light, which is invisible to humans. Boas and pit vipers can detect infrared radiation with special, non-visual, organs. Within the range of stimuli to which a sense organ responds, the organ will respond more readily to some stimuli than others. In other words, sensory organs act as filters -- only allowing us to perceive those stimuli which evolution has determined are critical for survival. In this lab, you will use some of the methods employed by sensory physiologists to objectively quantify human perceptual capabilities. You will see how our perception is influenced and limited by the properties of our sense organs. You will examine three sensory modalities: (1) vision, (2) hearing, and (3) touch. In a number of the following experiments, you will determine thresholds. A threshold is the amplitude of intensity of a stimulus at which it is just barely detectable. I. Vision Vision involves the detection of light, often reflected off other objects. Light is a form of electromagnetic radiation. It travels in waves. Different wavelengths of light produce the sensation of different colors. The range of different wavelengths is called the spectrum (see Table 1). The visible spectrum is the familiar colors of the rainbow. Light is detected by the eyes. Table 1. The Electromagnetic Spectrum Wavelength (nm) Description 700-1000 Infrared (detectable by vipers and boas using special pit organs) 425-700 Visible spectrum (to humans) Colors of the rainbow starting with violet at 425, ending with red at 700 300-425 Ultraviolet (visible to some birds and insects)

A schematic diagram of the human eye is shown in Figure 1. Incoming light passes through the pupil, and is focused by the lens onto the retina. Light is actually detected by special receptor cells called rods and cones (see Figure 2). Rods are responsible for vision under low light conditions. Cones function at higher light levels, and are responsible for color vision (note that the first two letters of cones and color are the same). We will consider first how rods function. Each rod is filled with a pigment called rhodopsin, which absorbs light. When a molecule of rhodopsin absorbs light, it breaks down into two smaller molecules. This reaction is coupled to a change in electrical potential in the cell. This electrical potential is passed on to a cell, called a bipolar cell, which is connected directly to a cell called a ganglion cell. Ganglion cells are nerve cells that make up the optic nerve. If the rod is sufficiently excited (i.e. absorbs enough light), a nerve spike is caused in the ganglion cell. The greater the excitation of the rod (i.e. the more light the pigment rhodopsin absorbs) the more nerve spikes are sent down the ganglion cell to the brain. The ganglion cell carries the message to the brain that light has been detected and the more nerve spikes travel down the ganglion cell, the brighter the perceived light. Not all colors of the visible spectrum are equally good at exciting the rods. Rhodopsin is much better at absorbing some wavelengths than others. As a result, a much smaller intensity of green light, for example, is required to excite a rod, than of red light. Scientists have extracted rhodopsin from rods and measured its ability to absorb different wavelengths of light. The results are shown in Figure 3. Since only rods function under dim light conditions we can study how rods function affects our perception by analyzing our perception in dim light. As you will observe later in the lab, under dim light conditions all the wavelengths appear to lack color. If a colored light excites a rod to a certain degree, the brain cannot tell if the rod is being stimulated by a color to which the rod is very sensitive, or if it is being stimulated by a very intense light of a wavelength to which it is less sensitive. A graph of the minimum stimulus strength required to just be seen versus the wavelength of light is called a spectral sensitivity curve. The fact that rods are differentially sensitive to different wavelengths of light does not, in itself, allow for perception of colors. This is because there is only one type of rod. The response of rods changes in exactly the same way if we increase brightness, or if we change to a wavelength to which the rod is more sensitive. There is an ambiguity between color and brightness. Thus, a spectral sensitivity curve, which is the minimum intensity required to elicit vision versus the wavelength of the stimulus, is not the same as measuring the ability to distinguish different colors.

Cones are very similar to rods but there are some important differences. First, cones are much less sensitive than rods, and function only under bright light conditions. Second, there are three different types of cones. Each has a different type of pigment, with a different spectral absorption function (illustrated in Fig. 4). One cone

type has peak sensitivity to red, one type to blue, and one type to green. There are two main advantages to having three types of cones. First it expands the overall range of wavelengths of light, which are visible. Second it allows us to perceive colors. This is because any given wavelength stimulus excites each of the three different types of cones to a different extent. The brain determines the color of the stimulation by comparing the ratio of the neural output from each of the tree types of cones (cones, like rods, become excited and then excite a ganglion cell). An increase in brightness will increase the neural output from all three cone types in equal amounts and the brain will still see the same color, because the ratio of the outputs will not change. Another interesting aspect of the visual system is that it remains sensitive over a remarkably wide range of light intensities. In a typical day, the brightness from midday sun exceeds the brightness of evening starlight by a factor of 100,000,000. At any one time, each photoreceptor can only respond to a 100-fold change in brightness. Fortunately, large changes in environmental brightness usually occur relatively slowly, and the visual system can shift so that the intensity range of the photoreceptors is centered about the average intensity of light in the background. The visual system has several ways of adjusting to changes in ambient light intensity, two of which we will discuss here. First, as already mentioned, there are two types of photoreceptors, rods and cones. Rods respond only in dim light, cones only in bright light. Second, rods change their sensitivity with time, according to the prevailing light conditions. This phenomenon is known as dark adaptation. If you go from a lighted area to a very dark room, you will not be able to see. After about ten minutes, however, you begin to be able to see again. This occurs because the rhodopsin pigment, which gets broken down when it absorbs light, is continually re-synthesized in the rods. When there is very little light, very little rhodopsin gets broken down and the amount of rhodopsin gradually builds up, making the rod much more sensitive to dim light. Another way in which sensitivity is improved in the rods involves the way in which they are hooked up to the ganglion cells. As illustrated in Figure 2, a typical cone connects to one bipolar cell, which connects to a single ganglion cell. In contrast, a number of rods pool their inputs to a single ganglion cell. By adding their inputs, each single rod needs to be excited by only a very small amount to produce a neural spike in the ganglion cell. The phenomenon is known as receptor convergence. There is a trade-off with this method of improving sensitivity. A ganglion cell, which receives input from a number of rods, spread out over the retina, cannot pinpoint the source of a light stimulus as well as a ganglion cell, which is stimulated by a single photoreceptor. In the case of a ganglion cell receiving the added input of several rods, light hitting any one of the rods can cause a neural spike in the ganglion cell, but the brain has no way to know which rod was excited. It is therefore harder to pinpoint the location of light stimuli on the retina when the stimulus is detected by rods (as compared to cones). The result is that our ability to see detail, or our resolving power, is considerably lower under dim light conditions than under bright light conditions. II. Hearing Sound consists of rapid changes in air pressure. These pressure changes travel through the air as waves. These waves of pressure change travel through the air and into our ears. Figure 5 shows a simplified picture of the human ear. The sound pressure waves enter the ear and cause the eardrum, or tympanum, to vibrate back and forth. The vibrations of the eardrum are passed through a series of three inner ear bones to an organ called the cochlea. The cochlea is a long thin organ, which, in humans, is coiled into a snail shell shape. Two membranes run the length of the cochlea, with tiny hairs suspended between them. One of these membranes, the basilar membrane, vibrates at the same rate as the eardrum, the other, the tectorial membrane, does not vibrate. This causes a shearing force in the hairs. This shearing force is converted to electrical potential, and causes nerve spikes to be sent, via the neurons of the auditory nerve, to the brain. The amplitude of the vibration in the cochlea varies along its length, depending on the frequency of the stimulation. Low frequency vibrations stimulate the far end of the cochlea, while high frequencies stimulate the near end. The brain determines the frequency of a sound by noting how far down the cochlea the nerve cell, which is sending signals, is located. The ear is not equally sensitive to all sound frequencies. A greater amplitude is required to hear some sound frequencies than others, while some cannot be heard at all. In this lab, you will measure the relative sensitivity of the human ear to sounds of different frequency.

III. Touch The sense of touch, as well as hot and cold, is mediated by a variety of sense organs located in the skin. Sense of pain and hot/cold is mediated by free nerve ending s near the surface of the skin. There are a large number of receptors, which consist of nerve endings wrapped around the base of hairs, which protrude from the skin and detect light touch. There are also pressure receptors called Pascinian corpuscles, which are located below the surface of the skin. These resemble tiny onions in shape. The ability to precisely locate a stimulus on the skin depends on the local density of these receptors. You will examine this effect in the lab today. Another phenomenon that can be readily observed with the sense of touch (although it occurs in all sensory modalities) is the phenomenon of adaptation, in which sensitivity to a stimulus declines rapidly with exposure time. This occurs in many sense organs. It allows the brain to direct attention to important stimuli and ignore those that are constantly present. In general, changes in stimulation are more critical for attention that is ongoing stimuli. Thus under constant stimulation, the receptor stops sending messages to the brain. This allows us, for example, to wear clothing without having our brain being constantly aware of the contact with our skin. If, however, someone touches us on the back, the touch receptors detect the change and send messages to the brain that the back is being touched. Quantifying Sensory Stimuli There is a variety of ways to quantify sensory perception. The most common method is to develop a graph of threshold (the magnitude of the stimulus which is barely detectable) versus stimulus quality (e.g. frequency). In many cases, sensory systems respond over an extremely wide range of stimulus values. Often this extreme range is achieved because the sensory systems respond only when a stimulus value increases by a multiple of its previous value. In order to graph such results we make use of graphs with logarithmic axes. In a logarithmic graph, each major division on the graph represents an increase by a factor of 10. Figure 6 illustrates how one reads a logarithmic scale. In some cases, only one axis is logarithmic. Such a graph is called a semi-log plot. When both axes are logarithmic, the graph is called a log-log plot.

The Experiments A set of five different experiments is described below. Each group will spend 30 minutes on each experiment (experiments 4 and 5 will be performed in the same time period). The data from experiments 1, 2, 3, and 4 should be recorded on the data tables at the back of the lab report, and later plotted on the graphs provided. Experiment 1: Spectral Sensitivity In this experiment, you will measure the minimum stimulus intensity required for a light of a given wavelength to be visible. You will test six different wavelengths and plot a spectral sensitivity curve. You will also observe the effects of dark and light adaptation on spectral sensitivity. 1. Turn on the illuminator to the low setting. Figure out how to change the intensity of the beam falling on the screen by sliding the variable density filter back and forth. The number at the arrow indicates the percent of maximum brightness passing through the filter. Figure out how to change the color filters. 2. Choose one person in the group to be the subject. 3. Have the subject face the screen. Move the variable filter to 99% transmission. Place one of the colored filters in the holder in front of the illuminator. Now turn on the illuminator. Turn out the room lights. 4. Gradually darken the image on the screen (by moving the variable filter) until the subject reports that he/she cannot see the projected light on the screen. 5. Turn on the room lights. Record the stimulus intensity at threshold (the value, in %, from the variable filter) in Data Table 1. 6. Return the variable filter position to 99% and choose a new filter. Repeat the steps above for all six filters. Be sure to turn the room lights on between each test (this assures that the subject will remain light adapted). 7. When you have recorded the threshold for each of the six filters, turn the room lights out and keep the room totally dark for at least 15 minutes. Now repeat the entire experiment, keeping the room lights off. Use the small flashlight covered with red cellophane to read the threshold values and change the filters. 8. When you have time (later in the lab for example) plot the data for light and dark room on Graph 1.

Questions: 1. Can you see a difference in the threshold levels for the dark adapted and light adapted states? 2. Is there a difference in the wavelength of peak sensitivity for rod vision (dim light) versus cone vision (bright light)? 3. In World War II, fliers going out on night missions would receive their instructions in a room lit only with red light. Based on your data, can you guess why this was done? Experiment 2: A. The Time Course of Dark Adaptation When the eye remains in the dark for a period of time, the ability to see gradually improves as the quantity of visual pigment builds up in the photoreceptors. In the following experiment you will plot the time course of this change in sensitivity. 1. Turn the room lights on. Choose a subject and have him/her face the screen. Turn the illuminator on and figure out how to use the variable density filter to change the brightness of the image. Once the room lights are turned off, only use the flashlight covered with red cellophane to record data. It is very important that the subject see as little light as possible. For the experiment to work well, the subject should keep his/her eyes closed except when the sensitivity is being tested. 2. Move the variable filter to near 1 (i.e. make the image too dark to see). Start the stopwatch and turn out the room lights. Adjust the brightness until the subject reports the spot of light is just visible. Turn the brightness down until the subject reports it disappears. Record the position of the filter by reading the number off the scale that the pointer points to. 3. Repeat this measurement every 2 minutes for the next 16 minutes. 4. Record the results in Data Table 2. Later, when time is available, plot the results on Graph 2.

Questions: 1. Is your graph smooth all the way, or is there a break or sudden change in the shape of the plot? 2. Can you explain the shape of your plot in terms of what you know about rods, cones, and changes in pigment concentration? B. Resolving Power and Ambient Illumination Turn the room lights on. Stand at the tape and determine the smallest row of letters that you can clearly read. Now turn the lights down (use the lamp with the dimmer switch to achieve relatively low illumination), and observe what happens to your resolving power. Questions: 1. Examine Figure 2 and explain your results based on the different way rods and cones are configured in the retina. 2. What are the implications of these results for the relative safety of night versus daytime driving? Experiment 3: Audition A. Observing Sound A microphone is a device that converts sound (pressure waves) into electrical potentials, which can then be observed on an oscilloscope. Observe the patterns on the oscilloscope as you talk, sing, clap, or whatever, into the microphone. Try singing a pure tone. Change from high to low sounds. Try jangling keys into the microphone. This will produce sounds that are too high for the human ear to detect. Many animals, such as bats, rodents, and many insects, can hear these ultrasounds. Use the tuning fork to observe a pure tone. This waveform is called a sine wave. Sounds that are not pure tones are produced by adding sine waves of different frequencies together. B. Making an Auditory Tuning Curve As you observed above, pure tones have the appearance of sine waves. High sounds have a high frequency and low sounds a low frequency of repetition. The ear does not respond to all frequencies equally well. Here we will measure the ability of one person to detect different frequency sounds. Since all sounds consist of added sine waves, we can test the overall ability of a person to hear by stimulating with sine waves of many different frequencies. A plot of threshold amplitude versus frequency is called a tuning curve. Tuning curves are used by doctors to detect hearing malfunctions. 1. Turn on the function generator. Make sure the sine wave button is pressed in. Note the frequency dial and the frequency multiplier buttons. These two numbers are multiplied together to get the correct frequency. 2 Choose a subject and have her/him put the earphones on. The subject should adjust the amplitude of the signal by himself, to avoid dangerously loud stimulation. Choose a frequency and turn the amplitude knob until the subject can barely hear the tone. Read the threshold amplitude from the digital voltmeter. The amplitude is given in millivolts. Fill out Data Table 3 giving the amplitude at which each of the listed frequencies is just barely audible. Plot the data on Graph 3. Note that the axes of the graph have scales that increase exponentially. This is called a log scale. Such scales are used when the data to be plotted

covers a very large range. It allows all of the data to be presented in a single graph. Pay careful attention to the pattern of the intervals on the scales of the graph. 3. Have each person in the group listen to the sine wave generator at different frequencies. Change from a sine wave to a triangle wave, and then to a square wave. These other waves sound similar to the sine wave, but consist of many higher frequency sine waves added to the original. Can you hear the additional high frequencies? If time remains, complete experiment C below. C. Localizing Sound The ability to localize sound depends on the detection of small differences in arrival time at the two ears. 1. Place the ends of the acoustic pipe over the ears of the subject, who keeps his/her eyes closed. Tap lightly on the pipe at different locations, and ask the subject to report whether the sound comes from the right or the left. See how close to the middle of the pipe you can tap and still have the person be able to detect the side from which it is coming. Try bending the pipe to one side, and tapping directly behind (or in front) of the subject. Which side does the sound appear to come from? Try placing the tuning fork on the pipe. Which is easier to localize: a tapping sound, or an extended pure tone? Many animals have warning cries that are fairly long and pure tone in quality. This makes them difficult to locate. This allows the animal to warn its family members without giving away its location to a potential predator.

Experiment 4: Tactile Sensation A. Two-Point Perception 1. Choose a subject and have her/him place their arm on the table with the palm face up and the skin exposed. Touch the skin with two points close together. Ask the subject if they detect one or two distinct points. Move the points apart until they can clearly detect two points. 2. Starting with the tip of the middle finger, measure the two-point distance (i.e. the distance at which the subject can just distinguish two pints) every few centimeters along the finger and hand, and then every 10cm up the arm. Record the results in Table 4 and plot them in Graph 4. Where are the touch receptors at the greatest density? 3. Try the two-point distance on other locations such as the lips, the scalp, and the face.

Question: How does the minimum two-point distance relate to density of tactile receptors in the skin? B. Adaptation of Hot and Cold Receptors 1. Place one hand in ice water and the other in hot water. 2. After a few minutes, move both hands to tepid water. Question: Relate what you experience to the concept of adaptation. Experiment 5: Color Vision The ability to distinguish different wavelengths as colors, independent of their brightness depends on the presence of three kinds of cones. However, if you choose three different colored lights, such that each stimulates one set of cones, you can cause the output from the different sets of cones to occur in any possible ratio, and thereby fool the brain into seeing nearly any possible color. 1. Turn on each of the three projectors. Note that one shines pure red light, one pure green, and one pure blue. Each of these colors primarily stimulates one set of cones. Point the projectors so that there is a zone of overlap of the three colors. 2. Play with the irises in front of the projectors and create as many different colors as you can. Pick one color, and note the positions of each of the irises. Increase the iris on each projector by the same amount. What happens? Conclusion: By the end of the lab, each group should have graphs 1, 2, 3, and 4 filled out for at least one subject. We will discuss these results together at the end of the lab period.