Effects of Inbreeding on Raven Matrices

Similar documents
A Theoretical Note on Sex Linkage and Race Differences in Spatial Visualization Ability

A Note on the Heritability of Memory Span

Roadmap. Inbreeding How inbred is a population? What are the consequences of inbreeding?

Diallel Analysis and its Applications in Plant Breeding

Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability

Genetic analysis of multiple sclerosis in Shetland

(Received 30 March 1990)

Interaction of Genes and the Environment

Which assessment tool is most useful to diagnose adult autism spectrum disorder?

Testing and Individual Differences

Familial Mental Retardation

Reaction Time, Movement Time, and Intelligence

Pedigree Analysis Why do Pedigrees? Goals of Pedigree Analysis Basic Symbols More Symbols Y-Linked Inheritance

1/6 Bellwork and Objective

EFFECTS OF ABACUS TRAINING ON THE INTELLIGENCE OF SUDANESE CHILDREN

Lecture 7: Introduction to Selection. September 14, 2012

Pedigree Construction Notes

An Introduction to Quantitative Genetics

An Examination of Culture Bias in the Wonderlic Personnel Test*

what the change should be and how it can be effected. of New York at Buffalo

Mendelian Genetics and Beyond Chapter 4 Study Prompts

SEX. Genetic Variation: The genetic substrate for natural selection. Sex: Sources of Genotypic Variation. Genetic Variation

Lecture 17: Human Genetics. I. Types of Genetic Disorders. A. Single gene disorders

Discontinuous Traits. Chapter 22. Quantitative Traits. Types of Quantitative Traits. Few, distinct phenotypes. Also called discrete characters

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

Intelligence in Jordan: Norms for the Standard Progressive Matrices

Estimating genetic variation within families

JENSEN'S THEORY OF INTELLIGENCE: A REPLY

Chapter 9: Intelligence and Psychological Testing

Systems of Mating: Systems of Mating:

Lecture 5 Inbreeding and Crossbreeding. Inbreeding

Interaction of Genes and the Environment

Bio 1M: Evolutionary processes

Chapter 5 INTERACTIONS OF GENES AND THE ENVIRONMENT

Forty-year Secular Trends in Cognitive Abilities

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

Section 8.1 Studying inheritance

HERITABILITY AND ITS GENETIC WORTH FOR PLANT BREEDING

SURVEY OF DUCHENNE TYPE AND CONGENITAL TYPE OF MUSCULAR DYSTROPHY IN SHIMANE, JAPAN 1

Your DNA extractions! 10 kb

Spearman s hypothesis on item-level data from Raven s Standard Progressive Matrices: A replication and extension

Cumulative Deficit in IQ of Blacks in the Rural South

Today s Topics. Cracking the Genetic Code. The Process of Genetic Transmission. The Process of Genetic Transmission. Genes

Quantitative Trait Analysis in Sibling Pairs. Biostatistics 666

Behavioral genetics: The study of differences

The secular increase in test scores is a ``Jensen e ect''

Analysis of single gene effects 1. Quantitative analysis of single gene effects. Gregory Carey, Barbara J. Bowers, Jeanne M.

HEREDITARY INFLUENCES ON DEVELOPMENT

The Association Design and a Continuous Phenotype

Inbreeding and Inbreeding Depression

AP PSYCH Unit 11.2 Assessing Intelligence

Chapter 7: Pedigree Analysis B I O L O G Y

Spearman's hypothesis on item-level data from Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices: A replication and extension

Name Class Date. Review Guide. Genetics. The fundamental principles of genetics were first discovered by. What type of plant did he breed?.

B-4.7 Summarize the chromosome theory of inheritance and relate that theory to Gregor Mendel s principles of genetics

Confidence Intervals On Subsets May Be Misleading

BIOL 364 Population Biology Fairly testing the theory of evolution by natural selection with playing cards

Lecture 18 Basics: Genes and Alleles

Supplementary Online Content

Patterns of Inheritance

Testing and Intelligence. What We Will Cover in This Section. Psychological Testing. Intelligence. Reliability Validity Types of tests.

Codominance. P: H R H R (Red) x H W H W (White) H W H R H W H R H W. F1: All Roan (H R H W x H R H W ) Name: Date: Class:

Analysis of Confidence Rating Pilot Data: Executive Summary for the UKCAT Board

Lecture Outline. Darwin s Theory of Natural Selection. Modern Theory of Natural Selection. Changes in frequencies of alleles

DOCUMENT RESUME. Forward and Backward Digit Span Interaction, with Mace and TO: Predictions from Jensen's Theory. PUB DATE 75 NOTE 23p.

Biology 12. Mendelian Genetics

Ch 9 Assignment. 2. According to the blending theory of inheritance, a white rabbit crossed with a red rabbit would produce what kind of offspring?

Sibling Resemblance in Mental Ability: A Review

The merits of mental age as an additional measure of intellectual ability in the low ability range. Simon Whitaker

ISPUB.COM. S Banik, D Mukherjee INTRODUCTION

Inbreeding: Its Meaning, Uses and Effects on Farm Animals

Estimating the Inbreeding Depression on Cognitive Behavior: A Population Based Study of Child Cohort

Mendelian Genetics & Inheritance Patterns. Practice Questions. Slide 1 / 116. Slide 2 / 116. Slide 3 / 116

Progressive Science Initiative. Click to go to website:

Human beings are members of a whole In creation of one essence and soul. If one member is afflicted with pain Other members uneasy will remain

The Inheritance of Fingerprint Patterns

IB BIO I Genetics Test Madden

Inheritance. Children inherit traits from both parents.

Genes and Inheritance (11-12)

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents

The Inheritance of Complex Traits

On genetic variation in menarche and age at first sexual intercourse A critique of the Belsky Draper hypothesis

Stability or Change?

2017 Version. Key Question types NCEA Science 1.9 Genetic Variation AS 90948

(b) What is the allele frequency of the b allele in the new merged population on the island?

Agro/ANSC/Biol/Gene/Hort 305 Fall, 2017 MENDELIAN INHERITANCE Chapter 2, Genetics by Brooker (Lecture outline) #2

What is the relationship between genes and chromosomes? Is twinning genetic or can a person choose to have twins?

Genetics Honors NOtes 2017 SHORT p2.notebook. May 26, 2017

Maryse Bouchard, PhD

Intelligence, Aptitude, and Cognitive Abilities 01/08/2014

The plant of the day Pinus longaeva Pinus aristata

Lecture 13: May 24, 2004

Resemblance between Relatives (Part 2) Resemblance Between Relatives (Part 2)

New Enhancements: GWAS Workflows with SVS

Are people with Intellectual disabilities getting more or less intelligent II: US data. Simon Whitaker

Convergence Principles: Information in the Answer

HARDY- WEINBERG PRACTICE PROBLEMS

What favorite organism of geneticists is described in the right-hand column?

GENOTYPIC-ENVIRONMENTAL INTERACTIONS FOR VARIOUS TEMPERATURES IN DROSOPHILA MELANOGASTER

Activity 65, Breeding Critters! More Traits! Issues & Life Science: Student Book!!

Transcription:

Behavior Genetics, Vol. 14, No. 6, 1984 Effects of Inbreeding on Raven Matrices Nirupama Agrawal, ~ S. N. Sinha, 1 and Arthur R. Jensen z'3 Received 5 July 1984--Final 15 July 1984 Indian Muslim school boys, ages 13 to 15 years, whose parents are first cousins, were compared with classmates whose parents are genitically unrelated on the Raven Standard Progressive Matrices, a nonverbal test of intelligence. The inbred group (N = 86) scored significantly lower and had significantly greater variance than the noninbred group (N = 100), both on raw scores and on scores statistically adjusted to control for age and socioeconomic status. Genetic theory predicts both of these effects for a polygenic trait with positive directional dominance. KEY WORDS: inbreeding depression; intelligence; Raven matrices; heritability; polygenic theory. INTRODUCTION The effect of inbreeding on the mean of a quantitative trait conditioned by polygenic factors is directly related to the amount of directional dominance deviation involved in the trait and to the coefficient of inbreeding. The coefficient of inbreeding, f, is the average probability over all gene loci that the same allele on both homologous chromosomes comes from the same ancestor (Crow and Kimura, 1970, pp. 64-65). If the alleles which enhance the phenotypic expression of the trait are dominant, the effect of inbreeding is to lower the mean of the trait in the inbred group relative to the mean of a noninbred but otherwise comparable population. This phenomenon is known as inbreeding depression. Also, because inbreeding brings out previously hidden recessive factors which contribute to the phenotypic variance, the variance of the trait is increased by in- Department of Psychology. University of Rajasthan, Jaipur, India. 2 School of Education, University of California, Berkeley, California 94720. 3 To whom correspondence should be addressed. 579 0001-8244/84/1100-0579503.50/0 9 1984 Plenum Pubhshlng Corporahon

580 Agrawal, Sinha, and Jensen breeding. The theory of the genetic mechanism responsible for these statistical effects of inbreeding on polygenic traits has been explicated elsewhere (Crow and Kimura, 1970; Jensen, 1978). The effects of inbreeding in humans have been investigated by studies of the offsprings of incestuous matings (f = 88 and of double first cousins (f = ~), first cousins (f = ~), first cousins once removed (f = ~), and second cousins (f = ~). Effects of inbreeding which are consistent with the theoretical genetic expectations have been reported in 11 studies (Adams and Neel, 1967; Bashi, 1977; B66k, 1957; Carter, 1967; Cohen et al., 1963, Neel, 1970; Reed and Reed, 1965; Schull and Neel, 1965, 1972: Seemanova, 1971; Slatis and Hoene, 1961). These studies have been reviewed in an article by Jensen (1983), which also showed that variation in the degree of inbreeding depression found on the various subtests of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) is directly related to the subtests' loading on the general factor, g, which is common to all the subtests. Performance on tests with the largest and purest saturation of g is subject to the greatest degree of inbreeding depression. This finding is consistent with a polygenic theory of intelligence in which there is positive directional dominance; that is, dominant genes enhance phenotypic intelligence. The fact that inbreeding depresses intelligence is evidence for genetic dominance of intelligence-enhancing genes. The presence of directional dominance also suggests that intelligence is a fitness character which has been subjected to natural selection in the course of human evolution. It is of considerable interest that our present standard psychometric tests are capable of reflecting to some extent this biological aspect of human intelligence. This theory is spelled out in greater detail elsewhere (Jensen, 1983). A problematic feature of a number of studies of inbreeding depression of psychometric intelligence arises from the fact that, in certain populations, inbreeding is negatively correlated with socioeconomic status (SES); that is, consanguineous matings occur more frequently among persons of lower SES than among persons of higher SES. Because SES is usually positively correlated with intelligence measurements, interpretation of the lower mean scores of inbred groups, as compared with noninbred groups, is rendered somewhat ambiguous. Although statistically controlling SES has had little effect on the magnitude of inbreeding depression of the IQ, it has been argued, however unconvincingly, that perhaps not all of the relevant SES factors have been fully taken account of (Kamin, 1980). A study of inbreeding in Israeli Arabs, however, found a significant degree of inbreeding depression of intelligence scores despite the fact that inbreeding was more prevalent in the upper socioeconomic classes than in the lower classes of Arab society (Bashi, 1977).

Effects of Inbreeding on Raven Matrices 581 While the third author was a visiting lecturer at the University of Rajasthan, in Jaipur, India, he learned that marriage between cousins was a not uncommon custom in Jaipur's Muslim community. (However, consanguineous marriages are quite rare among Hindus.) But more important, cousin marriages are also more frequent among the well-to-do Muslim classes, as a means of preserving wealth in family lines. As this seemed an ideal population in which to examine the effects of inbreeding on mental test scores, the present study was initiated. METHOD Subjects. The Ss were 186 male students in two government higher secondary schools in Jaipur, Rajasthan, India. All the Ss were Indian Muslims. With their parents' consent, they volunteered to participate in the study. Consanguinity of the Ss" parents was ascertained by interview and a questionnaire given to the parents. A total of 86 inbred Ss was found. All were offsprings of first-cousin matings. There were no double first cousins. Also, a noninbred control group was secured, consisting of 100 Ss whose parents claimed no genetic kinship. All the Ss were between 13 and 15 years of age. The means and standard deviations of age (in years) of the inbred and noninbred groups are 13.6, SD = 0.60, and 13.7, SD = 0.59, respectively--a nonsignificant difference (t = 1.73, df = 184, P = 0.085). Mental Test. The Standard Progressive Matrices (Raven, 1960) is a nonverbal test of reasoning ability consisting of figural patterns and geometric forms which are found in virtually all cultures. When factor analyzed among other diverse tests of mental ability, the matrices test is highly loaded on the general factor, g, which is common to all complex tests of cognitive ability and is the largest source of variance in all tests of intelligence. The test was administered according to the standard instructions in the test manual (translated into Hindi), with a l-h time limit. Ss were tested in groups of six by the first author. Socioeconomic Status. An index of SES was derived from information obtained in a questionnaire filled out by the Ss' parents. The questionnaire sought information considered most indicative of SES in the Indian Muslim population of Jaipur: father's education (years), mother's education, father's occupation, father's monthly income, number of siblings, home owned vs. rented, number of rooms in house/number of persons in household, monthly food expenditure/number of persons, and ownership of a car, TV, or refrigerator. The items were quantified by rating the responses to each item on a Likert-type scale varying from two

582 Agrawal, Sinha, and Jensen Table I. Correlations Between Variables in the Inbred (Above Diagonal) and Noninbred (Below Diagonal) Groups Variable Age SES Matrices Age 0.219" 0.193" SES 0.076-0.110 Matrices 0.145 0.039 * P < 0.05. to six points. These ratings were transformed to standard scores and the overall SES index is expressed as a T score, with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10 in the combined groups (N = 186). RESULTS Socioeconomic Status. The means and SDs of the T-score index of SES of the inbred and noninbred groups are 50.45, SD = 9.90, and 49.62, SD = 10.12. The SES difference of 0.083cr is nonsignificant (t < I). (All group differences in this study are expressed as ~r units, where cr is the square root of the N-weighted mean of the variances of each of the groups.) Correlation Between Variables. Table I shows the correlations among age (in months), SES, and raw scores on the progressive matrices. Progressive Matrices. Figure 1 shows the relative frequency distribution of raw scores of the inbred and noninbred groups. The shape of the distribution differs significantly (X 2 = 32.41, df = 9, P < 0.001). The significant difference in the shapes of the distributions is mainly the result of the inbred group's disproportionately larger frequency of lower scores. Scores of 25 and below were obtained by 44% of the inbred group and by only 11% of the noninbred group--a significant difference ( = 19.33, df = 1, P < 0.001). To control for the slight group differences in age and SES, the matrices scores were adjusted for these variables by multiple regression, in the combined groups regressing age (in months) and SES out of every subject's Raven score. (Analysis of covariance was also applied and yielded identical results.) Table II shows the statistics, both for raw scores and for adjusted scores. The group means differ significantly, by slightly more than half of a standard deviation, favoring the noninbred group. This finding is in accord with the prediction from genetic theory. Genetic theory also predicts greater variability among inbred than among noninbred individuals. This prediction is tested by the variance

Effects of Inbreeding on Raven Matrices 583 30 20 I0 0 0 I0 20 30 ~' t t... t -~.j I ~' -t 56-60 I L I 51-55 I 41-45 36-40 31-35 16-20 11-15 Fig. 1. 30 20 I0 0 ~ 0 I0 20 30 Inbred (%) Non-Inbred (%) #a 'corg Interval Relative frequency distribuhons of Standard Progressive Matrices raw scores m the inbred and noninbred groups. Table II. Group Statistics on Matrices Raw Scores and Scores Adjusted for Age and SES Noninbred Inbred (3" Score Mean SD Mean SD difference t test Raw 34.02 8.19 28.30 10.96 0,597 3.97* Adjusted 33.75 8.21 28.62 10.66 0.545 3.63* * Two-tailed t test, P < 0.001.

584 Agrawal, Sinha, and Jensen ratio, F (i.e., variance of inbred/variance of noninbred) using the variances adjusted for age and SES. The prediction is borne out significantly (F = 1.69, df = 85/99, P < 0.01). The correlation between inbreeding (quantitized as inbred = l, noninbred = 0) and matrices, with SES and age statistically controlled, is -0.263 (P < 0.001). DISCUSSION Both genetic predictions of the effects of inbreeding on a polygenically conditioned trait with positive directional dominance were significantly borne out: the inbred group (the male offsprings of firsts cousins) shows a lower mean and larger variance than a noninbred group on a highly g- loaded test of mental ability. Statistically controlling the slight group differences in age and SES did not affect the results appreciably. In any case, the lower mean mental test score of the inbred group could not be attributed to SES, because the inbred group was of slightly (but nonsignificantly) higher SES than the noninbred group. A study by Schull and Neel (1965) of the offsprings of consanguineous parents in Japan showed inbreeding depression of 3.7 IQ points on the Wechsler Full Scale IQ. But the inbred Japanese sample comprised offsprings of second cousins and first cousins once removed (with inbreeding coefficients of ~ and ~, respectively) as well as offsprings of first cousins (f = ~). Inbreeding depression in offsprings of first cousins in the Japanese sample amounts to about 5 IQ points, or one-third of a standard deviation (1 cr = 15 IQ points). The inbreeding depression in the present study is 0.55cr, or equivalent to about 8 IQ points. The difference of 3 IQ points between the Japanese and the Indian samples could be due to smaller within-group standard deviations in the Indian samples, making for a relatively larger difference between their means when the difference is expressed as o- units. Or it could be due to the fact that the matrices test is more highly g loaded than the Wechsler IQ, which, in addition to g, also reflects verbal and performance factors. As shown elsewhere (Jensen, 1983), the magnitude of inbreeding depression is directly related to the Wechsler subtests' loading on the g factor and inversely related to their loadings on the performance factor (independent of g). The results of the present study are consistent with those of at least 11 other studies which have reported the genetically predictable effects of inbreeding depression and increased variance in psychometric assessments of intelligence. Not a single empirical study has yet been reported in the literature which contradicts the effects of inbreeding on intelligence predicted by genetic theory.

Effects of Inbreeding on Raven Matrices 585 REFERENCES Adams, M. S., and Neck J. V. (1967). Childred of incest. Pediatrics 40:55-62. Bashi, J. (1977). Effects of inbreeding on cognitive performance. Nature 266:440-442. Book, J. A. (1957). Genehcal investigation in a north Swedish population: The offspring of first-cousin marriages. Ann. Hum. Genet. 21:191-221. Carter, C. O. (1967). Risk to offspring of incest. Lancet 1:436. Cohen, T., Block, N., Flum, Y.. Kadar. M., and Goldschmidt, E. (1963L School attainments in an immigrant village. In Goldschmidt, E. (ed)., The Genetics of Migrant and Isolate Populations, Wilhams & Wilkins, Baltimore. Crow, J. F., and Kimura, M. (1970). An Introduction to Population Theory. Harper & Row, New York. Jensen, A. R. (1978). Genetic and behavioral effects of nonrandom mating. In, Osborne, R. T., Noble, C. E., and Weyl, N. (eds), Haman Variation: The Biopsychology of Age, Race, and Sex. Academic Press. New York. pp. 51-105. Jensen, A. R. (1983). The effects of inbreeding on mental-ability factors. Personal. Indiv. Diff. 4:71-87. Kamin, L. J. (1980). Inbreeding depression and IQ. Psychol. Bull. 87:469-479. Neel, J. V. (1970). The effects of parental consanguinity and inbreeding m Hlrado, Japan. II. Physical development, tapping rate, blood pressure, intelligence quotient, and school performance, Am. J. Hum. Genet. 22:263-286. Raven, J. C. (1960). Guide to the Standard Progressive Matrices, H. K. Lewis. London. Reed, W. E.. and Reed. S. C. (1965). Mental Retardation: A Family Study. Saunders, Philadelphia. Schull, W. J., and Neel, J. V. (1965). The Effects oflnbreeding on Japanese Children, Harper & Row, New York. Schull. W. J., and Neel, J. V. (1972). The effects of parental consanguinity and inbreeding in Hirado, Japan. V. Summary and interpretation. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 24:425-453. Seemanova, E. (1971). A study of children of incestuous matings. Hum. Hered. 21:108-128. Slatis, H. M., and Hoene. R. E. (1961). The effect of consanguinity on the distribution of continuously variable characteristics. Am J. Hum. Genet. 13:28-31. Edited by John C. Loehlin