Demographic Variations in Fundraising Success

Similar documents
Emotional Delivery in Pro-social Crowdfunding Success

arxiv: v1 [cs.cy] 21 May 2017

Recent advances in non-experimental comparison group designs

Is entrepreneur s photo a crucial element in a crowdfunding webpage?

A Practical Guide to Getting Started with Propensity Scores

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

M E M O R A N D U M. Peter D. Hart Research Associates, Inc./ American Viewpoint

Using Big Data To Solve Economic and Social Problems

Identity, Homophily and In-Group Bias: Experimental Evidence

Unconscious Bias and the Hiring Decision

Recent developments for combining evidence within evidence streams: bias-adjusted meta-analysis

Experimental Psychology

The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose. Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX

Those Who Tan and Those Who Don t: A Natural Experiment of Employment Discrimination

Physician-Patient Race-Match & Patient Outcomes

TRIPLL Webinar: Propensity score methods in chronic pain research

CHAPTER 4 THE QUESTIONNAIRE DESIGN /SOLUTION DESIGN. This chapter contains explanations that become a basic knowledge to create a good

Summary Report: The Effectiveness of Online Ads: A Field Experiment

Econometric analysis and counterfactual studies in the context of IA practices

What is Multilevel Modelling Vs Fixed Effects. Will Cook Social Statistics

Sentiment Analysis of Reviews: Should we analyze writer intentions or reader perceptions?

Identifying Peer Influence Effects in Observational Social Network Data: An Evaluation of Propensity Score Methods

Addendum: Multiple Regression Analysis (DRAFT 8/2/07)

Color Difference Equations and Their Assessment

Class 1: Introduction, Causality, Self-selection Bias, Regression

Research Dissemination and Impact: Evidence from Web Site Downloads

Modelling Research Productivity Using a Generalization of the Ordered Logistic Regression Model

Propensity score methods to adjust for confounding in assessing treatment effects: bias and precision

Empirical Tools of Public Finance. 131 Undergraduate Public Economics Emmanuel Saez UC Berkeley

9 research designs likely for PSYC 2100

Race-of-Virtual-Interviewer Effects

The effect of entrepreneurial passion on funding at crowdfunding platforms

Myers Psychology for AP* David G. Myers PowerPoint Presentation Slides by Kent Korek Germantown High School Worth Publishers, 2010

Analyzing Personality through Social Media Profile Picture Choice

Feldexperimente in der Soziologie

Dr Angela Ballantyne Unconscious bias in law

Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology

The Social Norms Review

2019 Judging Form Suicide Prevention Category

MS&E 226: Small Data

Second Thoughts About Second Acts: Gender Differences in Serial Founding Rates

Online Appendix A. A1 Ability

This is a repository copy of Measuring the effect of public health campaigns on Twitter: the case of World Autism Awareness Day.

EMPIRICAL STRATEGIES IN LABOUR ECONOMICS

Preliminary Draft. The Effect of Exercise on Earnings: Evidence from the NLSY

Theory. = an explanation using an integrated set of principles that organizes observations and predicts behaviors or events.

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS VERSION 1 - REVIEW. Ball State University

An Application of Propensity Modeling: Comparing Unweighted and Weighted Logistic Regression Models for Nonresponse Adjustments

The Effects of Maternal Alcohol Use and Smoking on Children s Mental Health: Evidence from the National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth

Mission and Vision Statements

Methods for Addressing Selection Bias in Observational Studies

How to assess the strength of relationships

PERCEIVED TRUSTWORTHINESS OF KNOWLEDGE SOURCES: THE MODERATING IMPACT OF RELATIONSHIP LENGTH

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

Objectives. Positive First Impressions. Outline. Problem

CALL FOR PROPOSALS OCTOBER Proposal Submission Deadline: April 12, 2013

Experimental Research in HCI. Alma Leora Culén University of Oslo, Department of Informatics, Design

Psych 1Chapter 2 Overview

Introduction to Agent-Based Modeling

Lab 2: The Scientific Method. Summary

CHAPTER 2: PERCEPTION, SELF, AND COMMUNICATION

Online Appendix to: Online Gambling Behavior: The Impacts of. Cumulative Outcomes, Recent Outcomes, and Prior Use

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT

Technical Specifications

Bias in the Portrayal of Sex and Race in Photographs from Undergraduate Sport Psychology Texts

The Effects of Voice Pitch on Perceptions of Attractiveness: Do You Sound Hot or Not?

RUNNING HEAD: RAPID EVALUATION OF BIG FIVE 1

Management Science Letters

Temporal Context and the Recognition of Emotion from Facial Expression

ONLINE BRANDING GUIDE

WHY NEW MEMBERSHIP OFFERINGS AND WHO DECIDED ON THE CHANGE

Emotion Regulation: Cognitive and Affective Consequences for Fund-Raising Advertisements

Supporting Online Material for

DAZED AND CONFUSED: THE CHARACTERISTICS AND BEHAVIOROF TITLE CONFUSED READERS

Understanding and Applying Multilevel Models in Maternal and Child Health Epidemiology and Public Health

State-of-the-art Strategies for Addressing Selection Bias When Comparing Two or More Treatment Groups. Beth Ann Griffin Daniel McCaffrey

BIG IDEAS. The communicative context determines how we express ourselves. Learning Standards

GfK Verein. Detecting Emotions from Voice

Module 14: Missing Data Concepts

Jami Moiz Assistant Professor Department of Marketing & Institute of Business Administration IBA Karachi February 23rd, 2013

Evaluation of Linguistic Labels Used in Applications

Chapter 1. Introduction

PDRF About Propensity Weighting emma in Australia Adam Hodgson & Andrey Ponomarev Ipsos Connect Australia

Who Needs Cheeks? Eyes and Mouths are Enough for Emotion Identification. and. Evidence for a Face Superiority Effect. Nila K Leigh

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Psychological testing

The influence of the psychological factors upon the entrepreneurial tendency in crisis environments

Do Your Online Friends Make You Pay? A Randomized Field Experiment on Peer Influence in Online Social Networks Online Appendix

An Understanding of Role of Heuristic on Investment Decisions

Toward Vitality Research Project. Statistical Report. June December 2012

Lecture II: Difference in Difference and Regression Discontinuity

TRACER STUDIES ASSESSMENTS AND EVALUATIONS

Hiram Clarke Community Festival October 22, 2016 Noon 6pm Townwood Park, Houston

Dr. Jeanett Castellanos

The degree to which a measure is free from error. (See page 65) Accuracy

The Credibility Process

C/S/E/L :2008. On Analytical Rigor A Study of How Professional Intelligence Analysts Assess Rigor. innovations at the intersection of people,

An Empirical Study on Causal Relationships between Perceived Enjoyment and Perceived Ease of Use

Mackenzie Davis NATS

UNDERSTANDING GIVING: ACROSS GENERATIONS

Transcription:

Who Gets Started on Kickstarter? Demographic Variations in Fundraising Success Research-in-Progress Lauren Rhue Wake Forest University School of Business 1834 Wake Forest Rd, Winston-Salem NC 27106 rhuela@wfu.edu Abstract Crowdfunding platforms like Kickstarter are expected to democratize funding by increasing the availability of capital to traditionally underrepresented groups, but there is conflicting evidence about racial disparities in success rates. This paper contributes to the information systems literature on crowdfunding by examining the racial dynamics in the crowdfunding platform Kickstarter. The race of subjects in project and user photos are determined with facial recognition software for 138,778 fundraising projects, and matched sample techniques are used to control for observable differences in project categories among racial groups. Even controlling for these observable differences, this study finds that projects with African-American photo subjects achieve lower success rates. African-American fundraisers also achieve significantly lower success than other groups, and this effect is larger than the effect from project photos. This study has practical implications for individuals seeking capital in these markets as well as design implications for the platforms themselves. Keywords: Crowdfunding, empirical analysis, diversity Thirty Sixth International Conference on Information Systems, Fort Worth 2015 1

Introduction Many factors can influence individuals successes in seeking and raising capital, including demographics such as gender (Greene et al. 2001, Becker-Blease and Sohlb, 2007). The landscape of fundraising opportunities has shifted with the introduction of crowd-funding platforms, which enable entrepreneurs to broadcast their appeal for investments and potentially attract sufficient capital from a large number of smaller investments. Peer-to-peer crowd-funding platforms like Kickstarter are expected to be an easier source of capital for fledging ventures than the venture capital or financial institutions and are thus expected to democratize funding (Marom et al., 2015) by increasing the availability of capital to traditionally underrepresented groups. Access to these platforms is not synonymous with success on these platforms, and there is conflicting evidence regarding whether crowd-funding is actually easier for traditionally underrepresented groups (Barasinska and Schäfer, 2014) or whether the real-world fundraising challenges for traditionally underrepresented groups are also present in crowd-funding platforms (Greenberg and Mollick, 2014). This paper presents evidence that African-Americans, a traditionally underrepresented group in entrepreneurship (Fairlie and Robb, 2008), experience lower success rates with their projects than Whites in crowd-funding platforms. Holding the creator s race constant, projects with photos of African-Americans also achieve lower fundraising success, although that effect is overshadowed by the disparities in fundraising success rates according to the fundraiser s race. This paper contributes to the literature on crowdfunding by examining racial disparities in fundraising success. Prior research on underrepresented groups has focused on how gender influences funding efforts, and there is little research into the effects of race in attracting crowdfunding. The finding that there are racial differences in fundraising success has practical implications for individuals seeking capital in these markets as well as design implications for the platforms themselves. Fundraisers may engage in activities to obscure their race to increase their fundraising success, and platform designers may consider how to obscure race to encourage backers to focus on the content of the project and not its creators. Theoretical Foundations and Research Questions Crowdfunding, a relatively recent phenomenon, is defined by the use of technology to broadcast a fundraising message. Success on crowdfunding platforms, defined as achieving the pre-defined fundraising goal, is influenced by many factors including culture (Burtch et al. 2013) and demographics (Marom et al., 2015, Radford, 2015). This study s central conjecture is that there will be racial disparities in the success rates of crowd-funding projects. The ability to broadcast a funding request in a crowd-funding market enables traditionally underrepresented groups to bypass institutional difficulties in funding; however, discrimination can still emerge as potential contributors receive indications of the fundraiser s gender and/or race. These indications can emerge in multiple forms textual cues like the fundraiser s title, e.g., Mr. or Ms., (Radford, 2015) or visual cues such as the fundraiser s picture (Edelman and Luca, 2013; Chan and Wang, 2014). When this information about the fundraiser s identity is available, market participants may alter their financial decisions, which would lead to racial disparities in financial outcomes (Radnor, 2015; Edelmen and Luca, 2013). Crowd-funding platforms often do not explicitly include race as an attribute in the user profile but financial backers could infer race from the fundraiser s photograph, which contains visual cues of the fundraiser s race. Crowd-funding platforms often have separate profiles for projects and users so visual cues related to identity can emerge at two levels. First, user profiles often contain the fundraiser s photo, yielding visual cues about the fundraiser s identity. Second, pictures associated with the project may contain people and thus contribute racial cues related to the project. These cues are more difficult to interpret because the project s photo may serve a variety of purposes, e.g., to highlight the team behind the project or to showcase the project s beneficiaries. The two levels of visual cues provide an identification strategy to differentiate between effects of the user-related racial cues and the project-related racial cues on fundraising success. There are multiple potential mechanisms for racial disparities in success rates on crowd-funding platforms, and those mechanisms have different implications for project-level photos and creator-level photos. Thirty Sixth International Conference on Information Systems, Fort Worth 2015 2

Racial cues in the user photos In general, project and fundraiser pictures are likely to influence fundraising success as potential backers may feel a reduced social distance with less anonymity (Charness and Gneezy, 2008). There are multiple potential mechanisms that would lead to racial differences in success rates based on the fundraiser s race. First, the composition of the contributors may lead to bias in crowd-sourced knowledge (Reagle and Rhue, 2011), and a similar mechanism may lead to biases in crowd-funding. A preference for racial or gender similarity between the backer and the fundraiser homophily may influence the fundraising success of underrepresented groups (Ruef et al., 2003). Thus, if the demographics of potential contributors skew towards a particular race, then the demographics of successful fundraisers would reflect that skew. Second, there may be bias against African-Americans entrepreneurs, similar to the bias against female entrepreneurs (Brooks et al., 2014) and against minorities in charitable fundraising (List and Price, 2009). Racial bias is present in a number of economic outcomes, including interview call-backs (Bertrand and Mullainathan, 2004) and responses to research requests (Milkman et al., 2015). A bias in crowdfunding opportunities reduces the available capital source for African-American entrepreneurs, who are half as likely as White Americans to own their own business (Fairlie and Robb, 2008). Multiple mechanisms both predict that the fundraiser s race will shape differences in success rates. Therefore, it is hypothesized that H1. African-American fundraisers experience a lower success rate than other fundraisers. Although an African-Americans fundraiser may include a picture of him/herself as the project creator, the photo associated with project might not indicate his/her racial identity. The theory that connects the project photos with the identity characteristics is discussed in the next subsection. Racial cues in the project photos The photos associated with crowd-funding projects may serve a variety of purposes, e.g., to showcase the team behind the project, to highlight the target customers, to display the project logo, etc. Thus, any racial cues in the project photos do not necessarily reflect the racial category of the fundraiser. For example, a White fundraiser may propose starting a charity that primarily benefits African-American students and may include an image of those beneficiaries in the project photo. In that instance, there is a dissonance between the racial cues in the project photos and in the fundraiser profile, and this dissonance enables the comparison of success rates by fundraiser and by project photos. There are multiple mechanisms that would result in racial disparities in the success rates based on the racial cues of the projects, discussed below. Racial disparities based on project photos would suggest that elements of the project photo shape backers financial contribution decisions and not necessarily the fundraiser s race. Still, it is expected that visual cues of race, similar to text cues of gender (Radford, 2015), will decrease success in a crowdfunding setting. Investors may have preferences for projects associated with in-group members, similar to the homophily for fundraisers (Ruef et al., 2003). Alternatively, there may be a bias against projects that benefit minorities (Jenq et al., 2015), which would also result in racial disparities of success as the races represented in the project photos would reflect the skewed demographics of the backers. It is hypothesized that H2. African-American subjects in projects photos will be associated with decreased fundraising success rates. Identification The presence of project and user photos creates an identification strategy to determine the effect of projectrelated racial cues as compared to creator-related racial cues. The relative magnitude of the differences in success rate can be considered by investigating the projects in which the race(s) of the individuals in the project photo do not align with the race of the fundraiser. This study hypothesizes that H3. There will be racial disparities in success rates for project photos and user profile photos, and projects with African-American fundraisers and African-African project photos will experience the lowest success rates. Thirty Sixth International Conference on Information Systems, Fort Worth 2015 3

By identifying projects with misalignment between the fundraiser s race and the race of the project photo s subjects, we can distinguish between bias towards the fundraisers and bias towards projects that promote or benefit a racial category. Empirical Analysis Kickstarter, a popular crowd-funding platform, is used to investigate the racial disparities in success rates outlined in the above hypotheses. Kickstarter enables entrepreneurs to connect with small investors to achieve a pre-defined capital goal. Background and Data The dataset, downloaded from WeRobots.io, contains over 193,000 records of the entire project list on Kickstarter in December 2014. It contains extensive background information on the projects and their fundraisers. Each project has a name, text description, fundraising goal amount, actual amount of capital raised, project status at the time of the web crawl, project-related picture, category, and location. Each fundraiser s profile has a name, brief description, and biographical photo. Both types of data project and fundraiser are employed in the analysis. Variable Construction For each project, several readily available variables are used in the analysis: Project Category, Goal, Backers Count, Success, Launched Year, and Launched Day of the Week. Project Category classifies the project into one of fifteen high-level categories, and indicator variables for each category are used in the regressions. Goal is the amount of capital that the fundraisers were trying to raise, and Success is a binary variable that indicates whether the fundraiser achieved his/her goal. Backers Count is the number of backers who contribute to the project. Launched Year and Launched Day of the Week are the year and the day-of-theweek (e.g., Monday) respectively that the project was launched, and these variables are included to control for changes in project success rate over time. The two pictures associated with each project, the fundraiser photo and the project photo, are categorized using the FacePlusPlus application to identify 1) whether the picture contains faces, 2) if the subjects(s) are smiling, 3) the subjects gender(s), 4) the subjects races, and 5) the estimated age of the subjects. Thus, the following variables are constructed with the FacePlusPlus application: NumberOfFaces the number of faces in the pictures, Smiling the degree to which the subjects were smiling, Average Age the average estimated age of the subjects in the photo, Gender indicator variables for whether a male or female is in the picture, and Race indicator variables for whether there is a Black, White or Asian subject in the picture. These categories are not mutually exclusive. Thus, the fundraiser s race is operationalized as the perceived race identified using the facial recognition software, which recognizes White, Black, and Asian faces. With this operationalization, a gap may exist between a subject s perceived race and his/her self-identified race; however, this study focuses on fundraising success based on racial perception. Facial recognition software mimics the process of racial categorization that is based on visual cues; therefore, it is consistent with the theoretical constructs on race. There were a few important restrictions placed on the dataset. First, because this study examines the outcome of the projects, only projects with known outcomes either success or failure are included; projects that were still soliciting donations at the time of the web crawl were omitted. Second, some projects and/or the associated fundraiser profiles did not allow for their pictures to be accessed and/or categorized based on privacy settings and those projects are excluded from the analysis. The dataset comprised 138,778 projects with those two exclusions. Summary Statistics The summary statistics shown in Table 1 indicate a few important characteristics about the data. About 40% of projects are successful so there is a relatively high success rate overall. Roughly 50% of the projects have pictures with identifiable faces and the majority of those faces are identified as White. Of all projects, 2.4% of project photos and 3.1% of fundraiser photos have identifiably African-American faces. Thirty Sixth International Conference on Information Systems, Fort Worth 2015 4

Variable Mean Goal ($) 28106 710601 Success (%) 0.40 0.49 Backers (#) 113 889 Number of Faces in Fundraiser Photos 0.52 0.65 Smiling Index for Fundraiser Photos 27.62 46.10 Standard Deviation Variable Mean Number of Faces in Project Photos 0.38 1.2 Standard Deviation Smiling Index for Project Photos 13.78 62.72 Male Fundraisers (%) 0.31 0.46 Males in Project Photos (%) 0.20 0.73 Female Fundraisers (%) 0.18 0.38 Female in Project Photos (%) 0.16 0.67 White Fundraisers (%) 0.39 0.49 White in Project Photos (%) 0.16 0.37 Black Fundraisers (%) 0.032 0.175 Black in Project Photos (%) 0.023 0.15 Asian Fundraisers (%) 0.045 0.21 Asian in Project Photos (%) 0.038 0.19 N = 138,778 N = 138,778 Table 1. Summary Statistics for the Dataset In addition, as expected, there are racial differences in the project distribution. Figure 1 shows the breakdown of percentage of all projects by project category for African-American and non-african- American fundraisers. Film and Video is the largest category for African-American and non-african- American fundraisers, although the category comprises nearly 30% of the Kickstarter projects for African- American fundraisers. Music, publishing, and fashion are also more popular with African-American fundraisers than non-african-american fundraisers. Conversely, three categories art, games, and technology are more popular with non-african-american fundraisers. This observation of the racial differences in project distribution supports the future investigation into the intersection of racial prescriptions, fundraising efforts, and success rates similar to that of Greenberg and Mollick (2014). Figure 1. Distribution of Project Categories by Creator Race The following section describes the methodology used to examine the effects of race both the fundraiser s race and the racial cues of the project. Thirty Sixth International Conference on Information Systems, Fort Worth 2015 5

Methodology The goal of this research is to understand the effect of race in crowdfunding efforts. In particular, this research examines how the inclusion of African-American subjects in the project photos and/or fundraiser photos shapes success rates. Because race is invariant, the challenge is to control for the systematic differences between projects with African-American and without African-American subjects. The identification strategy is to create a matched sample of the projects with photos of at least one African- American subject with projects that have no African-American subjects. This identification strategy eliminates the alternative explanation that projects with African-American subjects systematically differ on observable characteristics. Two techniques are used to create a matched dataset: Coarsened Exact Matching (CEM) and Propensity Score Matching (PSM). Coarsened Exact Matching techniques match the covariates of the treated group with that of the untreated group using bins for the variables (Blackwell et al., 2009). Propensity Score Matching (PSM) techniques would match the projects based on the propensity to be in the treatment group, i.e., the propensity to be projects with African-American photo subject, based on the matching covariates (Sekhon, 2011). The following dimensions were the covariates used to create the matched dataset: Goal, Project Category, Launched Year, Launched Day of the Week, Number of Faces, Smiling, Average Age, and Gender. Plus, there were controls for whether the project photo s contained White or Asian subjects. The CEM and PSM matched datasets are both used to estimate the success rate of projects and evaluate the average treatment. The coefficient estimates for the matched datasets are compared to the estimates for the logistic regression model on unmatched dataset: Success = Treatment + Project Covariates Where Success is an indicator of project success, the Treatment is whether the project photo has an African- American subject, and the covariates are the variables described above: Goal, Project Category, Launched Year, Launched Day of the Week, Number of Faces, Smiling, Average Age, and Gender. Results Success Rates by Subjects of the Project Photo The treatment effects of African-American subjects in project photos were estimated using the matched datasets to evaluate H2. Across multiple methods of controlling the observable characteristics, the results consistently suggest that the projects with African-American subjects in the photos are consistently less successful than projects without African-American subjects. Treatment African-American Subjects in Project Photos Unmatched (I) Outcome = Success CEM (II) PSM (III) -0.496 (0.0534)*** -0.500*** -0.209 (0.0122)*** N 138,778 13,719 138,778 Table 2. Treatment Effect for African-Americans in Project Photos *** denotes p-values < 0.001 The estimates suggest that the inclusion of a Black subject in the project photo is associated with a 60% to 82% decrease in the odds of success. Success Rates by Subjects in the Fundraiser and Project Photos Next, projects with dissonance between the race of the two photos, project and fundraiser, are examined to better understand whether this disparity in success outcomes is driven by a bias against African-American Thirty Sixth International Conference on Information Systems, Fort Worth 2015 6

fundraisers or a bias against projects with references to African-Americans. Projects are placed into one of four categories by the visual cues in their project and creator photos, as shown in Table 3. Creator Photos Non-African-American African-American Project Photos Non-African-American Group 1 Group 2 African-American Group 3 Group 4 Table 3. Project Categories The success rates are calculated for each group, and Groups 2 and 3 are particularly interesting because there is not alignment between the racial categories of the fundraiser and the subjects of the projects photo. It is expected that Group 1 will have the highest success rate, Group 4 will have the lowest success rate, and the success rates for Groups 2 and 3 will fall in between those values. Figure 2 shows the success rate by those four groups with error bars for the standard error. Figure 2. Average Success Rates A simple comparison across categories indicates that the success rates are highest for Group 1. Non- African-American who sponsor projects with African-American subjects in the photos (Group 3) experience a lower success rate than those without African-American subjects (Group 1). African-American fundraisers have significantly lower funding rates, whether the project photo contains at least one identifiable African- American faces (Group 4) or not (Group 2), and their success rates are significantly lower than those of non- African-Americans (Groups 1 and 3). This comparison indicates the lower success rates are driven by race of the fundraiser, not the races of the subjects in the project photos. The simple graphical comparison above does not control for systematic variations of project category and other project attributes that might explain the variation in success rate among these groups. A logistic regression is estimated to examine the differences in success rates among these categories while controlling for the fundraiser and project characteristics. The logistic regression uses the following model: Success = Group + Fundraiser covariates + Project covariates Thirty Sixth International Conference on Information Systems, Fort Worth 2015 7

Where Group is the collection of indicator variables for Group1, Group2, Group3, and Group4; Fundraiser covariates are the Race, Gender, and Average of the fundraiser; Project covariates include the Goal, Project Category, and Launched Year of the project. In addition to the success rate, racial categories may influence the ability of creators to attract interest from multiple backers. Therefore, a similar model investigates how these categories affect the backers by estimating the linear model: Backers Count = Group + Fundraiser covariates + Project covariates Where Backers Count is the number of contributors to the project, and fundraiser and project covariates are the same as in the previous model. The estimated coefficients, shown in Table 4, provide support for both H1 and H3. In both models African- American fundraisers experience decreased success and produce fewer backers (H1), even controlling for the fundraiser and project covariates described above. The negative coefficient estimates for Groups 2-4 indicate that projects with an African-American either in the picture or as the fundraiser all experience a lower success rate than Group 1 and a reduced ability to attract backers, even controlling for observable characteristics like project category (H3). Dependent Variable Success Indicator Backers Count Variable (IV) Estimate (V) Estimate Group 2-0.934 (0.048)*** -0.190 (0.010) *** Group 3-0.403(0.053)*** -0.092 (0.011) *** Group 4-1.008 (0.032)*** -0.203 (0.022) *** Fundraiser Covariates Yes Yes Project Covariates Yes Yes AIC 171,323 187,990 *** denotes p-values < 0.001 Table 4. Coefficient Estimates for Racial Alignment in Project / Fundraiser Photos Group 2 projects maintain success rates on par with those in Group 4, suggesting that the race of the fundraiser drives the lower success rate, not the race of the subjects in the project photo. This result provides evidence that the success in attaining capital is lower for identifiably African-American fundraisers on crowdfunding platforms, despite any observable racial differences in the distribution of project categories. Discussion This research-in-progress paper begins the examination of racial disparities in successful fundraising on crowdfunding platform by investigating Kickstarter, one of the largest and most well-known platforms. The research hypotheses investigate how race affects both the success rate and the ability to attract backers. There is initial support for the conjecture that African-American fundraisers experience lower success in capital-raising efforts in crowdfunding platforms, and this research employs multiple techniques to isolate fundraiser races from other confounding factors. This research-in-progress will continue by exploring several different avenues. First, it will continue to examine racial disparities in fundraising success by performing a similar analysis on Asian fundraisers to gauge if there is a similar decrease in success rates for Asians. In addition, the study will examine whether diversity, measured as the Gini coefficient (Oestreicher-Singer and Sundararajan, 2012), influences success metrics. Second, this study will examine how the societal expectations related to race affect the success rate of various projects. Research about gender in online platforms indicate that women experience higher success in non-stereotypical fields (Greenberg and Mollick, 2014) but lower success in other fields (Radford 2015). Perhaps the alignment between racial prescriptions (Akerlof and Kranton, 2001) and fundraising project category accounts for some of the lower success. Third, the racial disparities in fundraising success may be explained by systematic racial disparities in the projects such as linguistic differences (Gorbatai and Nelson, 2015) or project categories based on racial prescriptions (Benjamin et al., 2010). A more robust Thirty Sixth International Conference on Information Systems, Fort Worth 2015 8

textual analysis of the projects is slated to understand the sentiment of the projects and whether that differs by race. For example, are African-American fundraisers more prone to negative emotions in their project descriptions, and those negative emotions are decreasing the attractiveness of their projects? If the matched sample can also account for differences in the project s tone, then it would strengthen any findings about the effect of race on fundraising success. There are multiple limitations to this study. First, the results rely on the accuracy of the facial recognition software, so a next step is to verify the alignment between the human perceived race and the automatic classification. Second, there are no controls for the alignment between the project category and the racial prescription. Behavioral prescriptions related to identity (Akerlof and Kranton, 2010; Akerlof and Kraton, 2000) may influence backers preference for projects by particular entrepreneurs in particular demographics (Mollick, 2013). Crowdfunding platforms are supposed to democratize funding; however, fundraisers may encounter similar bias in the online world as in the offline world as crowdsourcing platforms become more visual and provide more context. If visual cues about race reduce the probability of success, some fundraisers may choose to omit their pictures to broaden their appeal and increase their chance of success. Also, crowdfunding platforms could choose to omit certain identifying characteristics such as gender and race, like the platform Prosper, so that potential backers are forced to judge the project s worthiness on its characteristics. Thus, the dynamics of race in crowdfunding platforms have implications for the fundraisers and for the platforms. This research contributes to the information systems literature by beginning this investigation. Thirty Sixth International Conference on Information Systems, Fort Worth 2015 9

References Akerlof, G.A., and Kraton, R. 2001. Economics and Identity. Quarterly Journal of Economics 115 (3) 715-753. Akerlof, G.A., and Kraton, R. 2010. Identity Economics: How Our Identities Shape Our Work, Wages, and Well-Being. Princeton University Press, 2010. Barasinska, N., and Schaefer, D. 2014. Is Crowdfunding Different? Evidence on the Relation between Gender and Funding Success from a German Peer-to-Peer Lending Platform. German Economic Review 15(4) 436-452. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/geer.12052 Becker-Blease, J.R., and Sohlb, J.E. 2007. Do Women-owned Businesses Have Equal Access to Angel Capital? Journal of Business Venturing, 22(4) 503 521. Benjamin, D.J., Choi, J.J., and Strickland, A.J. 2010. Social Identity and Preferences. American Economic Review, 100(4) 1913-28. Bertrand, M., and Mullainathan, S., 2004. Are Emily and Greg More Employable than Lakisha and Jamal? A Field Experiment on Labor Market Discrimination. American Economic Review 94(4) 991-1013. Blackwell, M., Iacus, S.M., King, G., Porro, G. (2009) Cem: Coarsened Exact Matching in Stata. Stata Journal. 9(4) 524-546. Brooks, A.W., Huang, L., Wood Kearney, S., and Murray, F.E. 2014. Investors prefer entrepreneurial ventures pitched by attractive men. PNAS 2014 111 (12) 4427-4431, doi:10.1073/pnas.1321202111 Burtch, G., Ghose, A., Wattal, S. 2014. Cultural Differences and Geography as Determinants of Online Pro- Social Lending. MIS Quarterly 38(3) 773-794. Chan, J., and Wang, J. Hiring Biases in Online Labor Markets: The Case of Gender Stereotyping. Proceedings of ICIS 2014, Auckland NZ. Available online at: http://aisel.aisnet.org/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1094&context=icis2014. Charness, Gary & Gneezy, Uri, 2008. What's in a name? Anonymity and social distance in dictator and ultimatum games. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 68(1) 29-35. Edelman, B.G., and Luca, M. 2013. Digital Discrimination: The Case of Airbnb. Working Paper. Available online at: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2377353 Fairlie, R.W., and Robb, A.M. 2008. Race and Entrepreneurial Success: Black-, Asian-, and White-Owned Businesses in the United States. MIT Press Books, 026206281x, June. Gorbatai, A., and Nelson, L. 2015. The Narrative Advantage: Gender and the Language of Crowdfunding. Working paper, available at: http://faculty.haas.berkeley.edu/gorbatai/working%20papers%20and%20word/crowdfunding- GenderGorbataiNelson.pdf. Greene, Patricia G., Brush, C.G., Hart, M.M., Saparito, P. 2001. Patterns of venture capital funding: Is gender a factor? Venture Capital 3 (1) 63-83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13691060118175 Greenberg, Jason and Mollick, Ethan R., Leaning In or Leaning On? Gender, Homophily, and Activism in Crowdfunding (July 3, 2014). Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2462254 Jenq, C., Pan, J., Theseira, W., 2015. Beauty, Weight, and Skin Color in Charitable Giving. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, forthcoming. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2015.06.004 List, John A. & Price, Michael K. 2009. The role of social connections in charitable fundraising: Evidence from a natural field experiment. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 69(2)160-169. Marom, D., Robb, A., and Sade, O. 2015. Gender Dynamics in Crowdfunding (Kickstarter): Evidence on Entrepreneurs, Investors, Deals and Taste Based Discrimination. http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2442954 Thirty Sixth International Conference on Information Systems, Fort Worth 2015 10

Milkman, K.L., Akinola, M., and Chugh, D. 2015. What Happens Before? A Field Experiment Exploring How Pay and Representation Differentially Shape Bias on the Pathway Into Organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, forthcoming. Mollick, Ethan R. 2013. Swept away by the crowd? Crowdfunding, Venture Capital, and the Selection of Entrepreneurs. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2345872 Oestreicher-Singer, G., and Sundararajan, A. 2012. Recommendation networks and the long tail of electronic commerce. MIS Quarterly. 36(1) (March 2012), 65-84. Radford, J. 2015. The Mechanisms and Structure of Gender Discrimination in a Crowdfunding Market. Working Paper. Available at: http://home.uchicago.edu/~jsradford/downloads/radford%20- %20Mechanisms%20and%20Structure.pdf Reagle, J., and Rhue, L. 2011. Gender bias in Wikipedia and Britannica. International Journal of Communication 5(21). Ruef, M., Aldrich, H.E., and Carter, N.M. 2003. The Structure of Founding Teams: Homophily, Strong Ties, and Isolation among U.S. Entrepreneurs. American Sociological Review, 68(2) pp. 195-222. Sekhon, J. S. (2011). Multivariate and propensity score matching software with automated balance optimization: the matching package for R. Journal of Statistical Software, 42(7). Stone, J., Perry W., Darley, J.M. 1997. White Men Can't Jump : Evidence for the Perceptual Confirmation of Racial Stereotypes Following a Basketball Game. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 19 (3). WeRobots.io Available at: http://werobots.io/kickstarter-datasets/ Thirty Sixth International Conference on Information Systems, Fort Worth 2015 11