FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

Similar documents
postreinforcement pause for a minute or two at the beginning of the session. No reduction

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS'

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a

ANNA STATE HOSPITAL. peated measurement of the attack. A mechanical

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT'

Subjects and apparatus

Classical and Instrumental Conditioning. Lecture 8

AUTOSHAPING OF KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

Instrumental Conditioning I

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING

REPEATED MEASUREMENTS OF REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE EFFECTS ON GRADIENTS OF STIMULUS CONTROL' MICHAEL D. ZEILER

Schedules of Reinforcement

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour

Effects of Discriminated Stimuli on Temporal Responding in a Free-Operant Discriminated Avoidance Procedure

Operant Conditioning B.F. SKINNER

INDUCED POLYDIPSIA1 JOHN L. FALK UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN. a large number of these small meals. Under. CRF, inter-reinforcement time is short and

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

Chapter 6/9: Learning

Schedules of Reinforcement 11/11/11

Behavioural Processes

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1

acquisition associative learning behaviorism B. F. Skinner biofeedback

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning

Initial Tests of the Learned Helplessness Explanation Innoculation against helplessness If dogs are given escapable pre-preshock, then subsequent expo

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

ASSOCIATED WITH NALORPHINE IN

The Application of the Species Specific Defense Reaction Hypothesis to Free Operant Avoidance

Chapter 6: Learning The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc.

CHAPTER 15 SKINNER'S OPERANT ANALYSIS 4/18/2008. Operant Conditioning

Unit 06 - Overview. Click on the any of the above hyperlinks to go to that section in the presentation.

REINFORCEMENT AT CONSTANT RELATIVE IMMEDIACY OF REINFORCEMENT A THESIS. Presented to. The Faculty of the Division of Graduate. Studies and Research

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN

Operant Conditioning

BEHAVIOR IN THE ALBINO RAT'

IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA. because no special deprivation or home-cage. of other independent variables on operant behavior.

acquisition associative learning behaviorism A type of learning in which one learns to link two or more stimuli and anticipate events

Lecture 5: Learning II. Major Phenomenon of Classical Conditioning. Contents

Operant Conditioning

STIMULATION1. of clear plastic. The floor consisted of steel. rods, %2 in. in diameter and spaced 0.5 in.

J. E. R. STADDON DUKE UNIVERSITY. The relative inability of the usual differential. to ask whether performance under DRL schedules

Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events.

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

A concurrent assessment of the positive and negative properties of a signaled shock schedule*

OBSERVING RESPONSES AND SERIAL STIMULI: SEARCHING FOR THE REINFORCING PROPERTIES OF THE S2 ROGELIO ESCOBAR AND CARLOS A. BRUNER

Objectives. 1. Operationally define terms relevant to theories of learning. 2. Examine learning theories that are currently important.

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Some Effects of Discrimination Training on a Line Length Dimension

an ability that has been acquired by training (process) acquisition aversive conditioning behavior modification biological preparedness

THESES SIS/LIBRARY TELEPHONE:

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is

Myers PSYCHOLOGY. (7th Ed) Chapter 8. Learning. James A. McCubbin, PhD Clemson University. Worth Publishers

Variability as an Operant?

Extinction. n Operant Extinction: n Ideally combined with DRO (esp. DRI) n No longer reinforcing operant behavioral response

Theories of Learning

Supporting Online Material for

Response-cost punishment with pigeons: Further evidence of response suppression via token loss

Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2009, 92, NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) AMERICAN UNIVERSITY

Learning. AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 4

STUDY GUIDE ANSWERS 6: Learning Introduction and How Do We Learn? Operant Conditioning Classical Conditioning

Unit 6 Learning.

MCMASTER UNIVERSITY. more values independently of food delivery. In any case, the well known result of Skinner's

Effects of a Novel Fentanyl Derivative on Drug Discrimination and Learning in Rhesus Monkeys

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

1. A type of learning in which behavior is strengthened if followed by a reinforcer or diminished if followed by a punisher.

Learning. Learning. Learning

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience.

What is Learned? Lecture 9

The organism s problem: Avoid predation and injury so you can reproduce

that simple discrimination training to compound given a set to react -to one aspect of a stimulus a "set", a discrimination is "pretrained" along

Some Variables in Conditioned Suppression with Humans

Learning Habituation Associative learning Classical conditioning Operant conditioning Observational learning. Classical Conditioning Introduction

BACB Fourth Edition Task List Assessment Form

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus.

Learning. Association. Association. Unit 6: Learning. Learning. Classical or Pavlovian Conditioning. Different Types of Learning

I. Classical Conditioning

CO DITIO ED LEARNING I PREGNA CY

Computational Versus Associative Models of Simple Conditioning i

Classical Conditioning. Learning. Classical conditioning terms. Classical Conditioning Procedure. Procedure, cont. Important concepts

Chapter 5 Study Guide

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience or practice.

3/7/2010. Theoretical Perspectives

Extinction-Induced Aggression in Laboratory Rats

Study of the FR-TO effect

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats

Functionality. A Case For Teaching Functional Skills 4/8/17. Teaching skills that make sense

Transcription:

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 6, NUMBER 2 APRIL, 1963 FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 Responses were maintained by a variable-interval schedule of food reinforcement. At the same time, punishment was delivered following every nth response (fixed-ratio punishment). The introduction of fixed-ratio punishment produced an initial phase during which the emission of responses was positively accelerated between punishments. Eventually, the degree of positive acceleration was reduced and a uniform but reduced rate of responding emerged. Large changes in the over-all level of responding were produced by the intensity of punishment, the value of the punishment ratio, and the level of food deprivation. The uniformity of response rate between punishments was invariant in spite of these changes in over-all rate and contrary to some plausible a priori theoretical considerations. Fixed-ratio punishment also produced phenomena previously observed under continuous punishment: warm-up effect and a compensatory increase. This type of intermittent punishment produced less rapid and less complete suppression than did continuous punishment. During continuous reinforcement, the reinforcing stimulus is delivered for each response. Alternatively, during intermittent reinforcement, the reinforcing stimulus is delivered for some of the responses; the others go unreinforced. Similarly, every response may be punished (continuous punishment) or only some may be punished (intermittent punishment). At this time, little is known of the changes produced by different schedules of intermittent punishment. The present study investigated the use of fixed-ratio schedules of punishment wherein only every nth response was punished. METHOD Subjects Seven White Carneaux pigeons were maintained at about 85% of free-feeding weight. Because of the repeated use of the same subjects, body weight was redetermined before each experiment. 'This investigation was supported by a grant from the Psychiatric Training and Research Fund of the Illinois Department of Public Welfare, NSF grant G16357 and NIMH grant 4925. 'Anna State Hospital, Anna, Illinois. 'Southern Illinois University. Apparatus The experimental chamber measured 14 by 14 by 20 in. high and contained a translucent response key mounted 13 in. from the floor. A response consisted of a peck against this key. Responses were reinforced according to a variable-interval schedule for at least one month prior to the initial introduction of punishment. The reinforcing stimulus was the presentation of grain for 3 sec. The duration of each daily session was constant for a given subject, but was as brief as 1.5 hr and as long as 8 hr for different subjects. Similarly, the mean interval between reinforcements was constant for a given subject but was 1, 2 or 3 min for different subjects. (These differences in the reinforcement frequency were found to be relevant only to differences in the absolute level of response). Punishment was a brief (100 msec) electric shock that was delivered through a 10,000 ohm series resistor to electrodes implanted around the pubis bone in the tail region of the pigeon (Azrin, 1959). Every 10 volts was equivalent to approximately 1 ma., e.g. 80 volts equalled 8 ma. The food reinforcement schedule was in effect at all times. Figure 1 illustrates the main features of the fixed-ratio schedule of punishment. Great care was necessary when introducing punishment to the subject for the first time. Previous study of continuous punishment 141

142 N. H. AZRIN, W. C. HOLZ, and D. F. HAKE SCHEMA T/C T M E Fig. 1. Fixed-ratio punishment procedure. The responses are maintained by a variable-interval schedule of food reinforcement. Every nth response was punished. (Azrin, 1959) had shown that if a high intensity (80 volts or more) was used initially, the responses were likely to be completely and usually irreversibly, suppressed. If the initial introduction to punishment involved lower intensities (60 volts or less), performance was easily maintained even when the punishment was later increased to intensities as great as 130 volts (Azrin, 1960). The present study of fixed-ratio punishment also required that a low intensity be used initially. Thus, the subjects in this investigation all received their initial exposure to punishment at intensities less than 50 volts. Once the subject was exposed to progressively higher shock values, intensities as high as 300 volts could be introduced at a later time without causing complete disruption of the performance. Five of the seven subjects received their initial exposure to punishment at FR-1 punishment (continuous punishment). The fixed-ratio value was then increased progressively to 2, 5, 10, 20, 50, 100, 200, 300, 500, 1000 in that order. The other two subjects received their initial exposure to punishment at an FR-50 punishment and the FR value was increased from that point. The fixed-ratio value was decreased as well as increased for all subjects during the 24 month period over which the subjects were studied. Performance under a given fixed-ratio (punishment) was usually recoverable. Partial irreversibility of performance appeared to result primarily from the sudden introduction of high intensities of punishment following a period of several weeks in which no punishment had been programmed. RESULTS Introduction of Fixed-Ratio Punishment The introduction of fixed-ratio punishment produced a characteristic sequence of changes in the temporal pattern of responses of all subjects. Figure 2 illustrates these changes for a subject that had not been exposed to fixedratio punishment for 15 experimental sessions. (The same changes were observed with the two subjects that had not been exposed to punishment previously). In the absence of punishment (top curve), the responses were maintained at a high and uniform level by a 3 min VI schedule of food reinforcement. Punishment (240 volts) was then delivered for every 300th response (middle curve). The first punishment delivery (arrow) produced a complete cessation of responding for about 10 min, after which the responses gradually accelerated until a second punishment was delivered after 300 more responses. Again, the response rate decreased sharply following the delivery of punishment; again the responses were positively accelerated. This cycle was repeated. After each of the punishment deliveries, the responses were suppressed but gradually increased in frequency until another punishment was delivered. It can be seen that with successive punishment deliveries, there was a progressive reduction of the suppression which followed each punishment. Concurrently, there was a reduction in the degree of positive acceleration. The end result of these changes was a reduced level of responding showing no systematic acceleration or deceleration between the deliveries of punishment. When the same intensity of punishment (240 volts) was introduced at a later date for every response, the reduction in responding was immediate. Whereas the FR-300 punishment had suppressed the responses only partially, FR-1 punishment suppressed the responses almost completely. Fixed-Ratio Requirement for Panishment Figure 3 shows segments of the cumulative response records for one subject under various fixed-ratio requirements for punishment. The

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT 143 AO WJiwIr, a-ye w w I-- 4 -J M D FR-300 FR-I PUVISA fwn 0 HOURS Fig. 2. Introduction of fixed-ratio punishment. The oblique lines on the response curve indicate the delivery of a punishment (240V) after every response (FR-I) or after every 300 responses (FR-300). The food reinforcements (not indicated) are being delivered according to a 3 min variable interval schedule. No punishments had been delivered prior to the arrow. shock intensity was 240 volts and a 3 min VI schedule of food reinforcement was in effect. At least 100 hr of exposure had been given at each of the fixed-ratio values. This figure shows that the suppression was a direct function of the proportion of responses that were punished. When every response was punished (FR-1), suppression was almost complete; when every 100th response was punished, the responses were reduced to approximately 20% of the unpunished rate; when every 1000th response was punished, the responses were reduced to approximately 40% of the unpunished rate. All values of fixed-ratio punishment produced a uniform reduction in the rate of responding between the successive deliveries of punishment. The size of the fixed-ratio requirement determined the extent of the reduction but the uniform pattern of responding remained the same. Intensity of Punishment Variations in the intensity of punishment did not alter the uniformity of response rate during fixed-ratio punishment. Figure 4 presents a segment of the cumulative response record from the start of different experimental sessions. A 1 min VI schedule of food reinforcement was used and the punishment was delivered from every 50th response. It can be seen that a shock intensity of 160 volts produced a uniform, but reduced level, of responses. At a lower intensity of 120 volts, the responses were less suppressed but still occurred at a uniform rate between successive presentations of punishment. At a still lower

144 N. H. AZRIN, W. C. HOLZ, and D. F. HAKE intensity of 90 volts, suppression was restricted to the initial portion of the session. Within this initial period of suppression, the response rate generally remained uniform within the period between any two successive presentations of punishment. Occasionally, negative acceleration of responses was observed between successive presentations of punishment. For example, slight negative acceleration was observed between the first and second shock delivery at 90 volts and between the second and third presentation at 120 volts. Figure 4 illustrates a characteristic feature of the performance under fixed-ratio punishment: the "warm-up period" at the start of the session. It can be seen that the response rate was suppressed initially at a punishment intensity of 90 volts but recovered completely within 15 min after the start of the session. At 120 volts and 160 volts, no warm-up period appeared. This initial warm-up period characterized the performance at lower punishment intensities but did not appear at the higher intensities. Food Deprivation In a previous study (Azrin, 1960), slight changes in food deprivation produced unusually large changes in the degree of suppression. Figure 5 shows the effects of food deprivation during fixed-ratio punishment (FR-100). A 3 min VI schedule of food reinforcement was used throughout. The punishment intensity was 160 volts. A typical segment of the response record is shown for different levels of food deprivation. The body weight of the subject had been gradually reduced over a NO PUNISHMENT B-7V RATI/O FIXE'D,PUNISHM6NT HOUIRS _ Fig. 3. Stable performance during fixed-ratio punishment at several fixed-ratio values from FR-1 to FR-1000. The oblique lines on the response curve indicate the delivery of a punishment (240V). The food reinforcements (not indicated) are being delivered according to a 3 min variable interval schedule.

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT 145 Ig PUNISHMENT INTENSITY (Volts) 0 30 60 Minutes Fig. 4. Effect of punishment intensity during fixedratio punishment. Every 50th response is being punished at the moment indicated by the oblique lines on the response curves. Each response curve represents the performance during the first 60 min of different sessions. The food reinforcements (not indicated) are being delivered according to a 1 min variable interval. period of one month from 85% to 60% by limiting the usual feeding after each session. The body weight was then gradually returned to 85% over a period of about one month by increasing the amount of food given after each session. It can be seen that the responses were almost completely suppressed by the fixedratio punishment at 85% of body weight. At 60% of body weight, about 3000 responses per hour were emitted. Changes of only 5% in body weight produced several-fold changes of the response rate. In spite of these large changes in the over-all level of response, the extent of food deprivation did not alter the uniformity of response rate between successive deliveries of punishment. Termination of Fixed-Ratio Punishment Figure 6 shows the changes in response rate when a fixed-ratio schedule of punishment was terminated. A 2 min VI schedule of reinforcement was used throughout. A long history of fixed-ratio punishment (FR-100) had been in effect prior to the 6 hr period depicted in Fig. 6. The punishment intensity was 80 volts. When the fixed-ratio punishment was terminated (dotted line), the response rate gradually increased from approximately 20 responses per min to more than 60 per min during a period of 2 hr. During the third hour, the response rate rose still higher, achieving a level (130 responses per min) that exceeded the usual unpunished level of responding. By the fourth hour, the response rate had returned to the usual unpunished level of about 105 responses per min. The above results demonstrated the complete recovery of responding after fixed-ratio punishment was terminated. Figure 7 of the same subject shows that a period of response suppression reappeared at the beginning of each session in spite of the complete recovery during the preceding session. It can be seen that the initial period of suppression became progressively briefer during consecutive days without punishment. This initial period of suppression persisted for many days. The responses were still greatly suppressed during the first 5 min of the sixth day even after five daily sessions during which time there were over 150,000 unpunished responses. During the third day, the session was interrupted for a period of 5 min by blacking out the experimental chamber (see arrow "A"). This brief interruption reinstated the period of suppression that was seen previously at the start of the session. DISCUSSION The introduction of fixed-ratio punishment produced an initial phase in which the responses were positively accelerated between successive deliveries of punishment. Under maintained fixed-ratio punishment, the degree of positive acceleration progressively diminished until a reduced, but uniform, rate of response emerged. Instances of negative acceleration between punishments were relatively rare and did not persist regardless of the particular subject, the punishment intensity, the value of the punishment ratio, the duration of exposure to a given ratio value (up to 300 hr), the level of food deprivation, or the underlying frequency of food reinforcement. Two considerations might have led one to expect negative acceleration to have been the rule rather than the exception. The first of these considerations is the approach-avoidance conflict postulated by Miller (1944) in which approach behavior in a runway decreased as the subject approached the spatial locus of aversive stimulation. Similarly, under fixed-

146 N. H. AZRIN, W. C. HOLZ, and D. F. HAKE ratio punishment the emission of successive responses might be viewed as bringing the subject progressively closer to the punishment. Fixed-ratio punishment might be expected to produce a gradient along the behavioral dimension analogous to the gradient hypothesized for the spatial dimension. Hence, negative acceleration of the responses might have been expected between successive punishments. Negative acceleration might also be expected on the basis of a second consideration. During FR-200 punishment, for example, 199 unpunished responses occur between the successive deliveries of punishment. The delivery of a punishment was the occasion upon which the immediately succeeding responses were not punished. A discrimintation could have been formed on this basis and resulted in a decreased level of response immediately preceding the next delivery of punishment. This result might be expected on the basis of a previous procedure in which punishments were delivered according to a fixed-interval schedule (Azrin, 1956). Negative acceleration of the responses would appear likely, then, on the basis of two theoretical considerations; yet negative acceleration was absent. An explanation of the % OF FREE-FEEDING BODY weight 60% FR-100 PAVISVENrT 65% B-76 0 Minute s Fig. 5. Effect of food deprivation during fixed-ratio punishment of food reinforcement responses. Every 100th response is being punished (160V) at the moment indicated by the short oblique lines on the response curves. The food reinforcements (not shown) are being delivered according to a 3 min variable schedule.

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT 147 uniform rate actually obtained under fixedratio punishment might be that punishment produced a generalized suppression which prevented any precise discrimination. Yet previous findings have shown that precise discriminations may be formed through punishment (Azrin, 1956; Holz and Azrin, 1961). Apparently, the fixed-ratio schedule of punishment itself operates in some manner to attenuate any response fluctuation attributable to behavioral proximity to the punishment. 140 Responses per Minute ^1 0 ' FR-100 * PU& I _.. I. NO PUNISHMENT / 0 1 2 B-93 Hours Fig. 6. Gradual recovery and compensatory increase of responses following a long history of fixed-ratio 100 punishment (80V). The changes in rate occurred during a single 6 hr session. A 2 min VI schedule of food reinforcement was used. The observed effects of fixed-ratio punishment may be analyzed by comparison with the effects of fixed-ratio positive reinforcement. Under fixed-ratio reinforcement, the higher the response rate, the greater is the frequency of reinforcement. The increased frequency of reinforcement in turn would be expected to produce a still higher rate of responses. Presumably, because of this mutually facilitating relation, the response rate reaches a maximum level of occurrence (Ferster and Skinner, 1957). Fixed-ratio punishment may be analyzed in the same terms. Under fixed-ratio punishment, the higher the response rate, the greater is the frequency of punishment. However, the increased frequency of punishment would be expected to produce a lower rate of response. Any tendency for the response rate to increase would be counteracted, therefore, by the resulting increase in punishment. Conversely, any temporary tendency to decrease the response rate would reduce the frequency of punishment and allow the response rate to recover. The result appears to be a state of dynamic equilibrium in which any changes in the response rate would be opposed by the resulting changes in the frequency of punishment. The present results revealed several differences between continuous punishment and intermittent punishment. First, the rapidity of suppression following the introduction of punishment was greater for continuous than for intermittent punishment. Secondly, continuous punishment produced greater suppression for as long as the punishment was maintained. With respect to the termination of punishment, recovery occurred very suddenly after continuous punishment, but only after a few unpunished responses were emitted (Azrin, 1960). After intermittent punishment was terminated, recovery occurred very gradually and appeared to begin only after a few omissions of the scheduled punishment. Previous studies of continuous punishment (Azrin, 1960) have revealed a temporary but immediate "compensatory" increase of responding when the continuous punishment was terminated. The temporary increase of 6thDDy / DOry 2nd Day. DLay Days Ate Rmval of InterrnWMent FP4ISMWnt' Fig. 7. Reappearance of suppression at the start of successive sessions following the termination of fixedratio 100 punishment (80V). Each response curve is taken from the start of a session of 7 hr duration. At the moment indicated by arrow "A", the session was interrupted by a "black-out" of the experimental chamber for a period of 5 min during which time the recording paper did not move. A 2 min VI schedule of food reinforcement was in effect.

148 N. H. AZRIN, W. C. HOLZ, and D. F. HAKE unpunished responses appeared to result from the contrast with the preceding period of continuous punishment. This punishment contrast effect has been obtained when different types of food reinforcement schedules were used to maintain the punished responses (Azrin, 1961). The present results appear to extend the generality of this punishment contrast effect. The major difference was that after the fixed-ratio punishment was eliminated, the compensatory increase did not occur immediately. It would appear that no contrast existed until several of the normally scheduled punishments had been omitted. It was found that mild intensities of fixedratio punishment produced temporary suppression at the start of each experimental session. This warm-up phenomenon had been noted previously under continuous punishment (Azrin, 1960). One possible interpretation of this warm-up period is that sensory adaptation had resulted from the frequent administration of shock. Under intermittent punishment, however, the shocks were necessarily separated in time and little sensory adaptation could be expected. Also, the warm-up period persisted for several days after the punishment was discontinued. It appears, therefore, that the warm-up or recovery phenomenon is not attributable to sensory adaptation. Rather this phenomenon appears to be a general reaction to a situation involving aversive stimulation. The stimuli associated with the beginning of an experimental session appeared to exert a persistent effect upon responding. It was found that suppression gradually disappeared during an experimental session when the intermittent punishment was discontinued. However, suppression reappeared at the start of the following session. This reappearance of suppression at the start of each session appears to be comparable to the spontaneous recovery phenomenon (Pavlov, 1927) in which responses reappear at the start of successive extinction sessions (see also Skinner, 1938). This initial period of suppression became briefer with succeeding sessions in much the same way as does the phenomenon of spontaneous recovery. When the experimental session was interrupted and restarted, this brief period of suppression reappeared. Hence, it seems that the initial period of suppression is under the control of the stimuli associated with the start of each session. The persistence of this initial suppression necessitated sessions of long duration in the present investigation. Failure to provide long durations often resulted in the maintenance of behavior that was primarily under the control of the stimuli associated with the start of each session rather than the stimuli being programmed during the session. REFERENCES Azrin, N. H. Effects of two intermittent schedules of immediate and non-immediate punishment. 1. Psychol., 1956, 42, 3-21. Azrin, N. H. A technique for delivering shock to pigeons. J. exp. Anal. Behav., 1959, 2, 161-163. Azrin, N. H. Effects of punishment intensity during variable-interval reinforcement. J. exp. Anal. Behav., 1960, 3, 123-142. Azrin, N. H. and Holz, W. C. Punishment during fixed-interval reinforcement. J. exp. Anal. Behav., 1961, 4, 343-347. Ferster, C. B. and Skinner, B. F. Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1957. Holz, W. C. and Azrin, N. H. Discriminative properties of punishment. J. exp. Anal. Behav., 1961, 4, 225-232. Miller, N. E. Experimental studies in conflict. In J. McV. Hunt, ed. Personality and the behavior disorders. New York: Ronald Press, 1944, 431-465. Pavlov, I. P. Conditioned reflexes. (Translated by G. V. Anrep) London: Oxford University Press, 1927. Skinner, B. F. The behavior of organisms; an experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1938. Received April 10, 1962