The case for quantum entanglement in the brain Charles R. Legéndy September 26, 2017

Similar documents
Evaluating the Effect of Spiking Network Parameters on Polychronization

Why do we have a hippocampus? Short-term memory and consolidation

Axon initial segment position changes CA1 pyramidal neuron excitability

Part 11: Mechanisms of Learning

Structural basis for the role of inhibition in facilitating adult brain plasticity

Memory retention the synaptic stability versus plasticity dilemma

The storage and recall of memories in the hippocampo-cortical system. Supplementary material. Edmund T Rolls

Synapses and synaptic plasticity. Lubica Benuskova Lecture 8 How neurons communicate How do we learn and remember

Basics of Computational Neuroscience: Neurons and Synapses to Networks

You submitted this quiz on Sun 19 May :32 PM IST (UTC +0530). You got a score of out of

Synaptic plasticityhippocampus. Neur 8790 Topics in Neuroscience: Neuroplasticity. Outline. Synaptic plasticity hypothesis

Exploring the Functional Significance of Dendritic Inhibition In Cortical Pyramidal Cells

Memory: Computation, Genetics, Physiology, and Behavior. James L. McClelland Stanford University

Synaptic Plasticity and the NMDA Receptor

Memory Systems II How Stored: Engram and LTP. Reading: BCP Chapter 25

COGNITIVE SCIENCE 107A. Hippocampus. Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D.

Cell body, axon, dendrite, synapse

to Cues Present at Test

Shadowing and Blocking as Learning Interference Models

Synaptic Transmission: Ionic and Metabotropic

THE HISTORY OF NEUROSCIENCE

The synaptic Basis for Learning and Memory: a Theoretical approach

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Neurons: Structure and communication

THE HISTORY OF NEUROSCIENCE

Nature Methods: doi: /nmeth Supplementary Figure 1. Activity in turtle dorsal cortex is sparse.

TNS Journal Club: Interneurons of the Hippocampus, Freund and Buzsaki

LESSON 3.3 WORKBOOK. Why does applying pressure relieve pain?

Biomimetic Cortical Nanocircuits: The BioRC Project. Alice C. Parker NSF Emerging Models of Technology Meeting July 24, 2008

How Synapses Integrate Information and Change

Temporally asymmetric Hebbian learning and neuronal response variability

11/2/2011. Basic circuit anatomy (the circuit is the same in all parts of the cerebellum)

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

International Journal of Scientific & Engineering Research Volume 4, Issue 2, February ISSN THINKING CIRCUIT

The Ever-Changing Brain. Dr. Julie Haas Biological Sciences

Motor systems III: Cerebellum April 16, 2007 Mu-ming Poo

2Lesson. Outline 3.3. Lesson Plan. The OVERVIEW. Lesson 3.3 Why does applying pressure relieve pain? LESSON. Unit1.2

Introduction. Charles R. Legéndy* On the data stirring role of the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus

The Contribution of Neuroscience to Understanding Human Behaviour

LONG TERM MEMORY. Learning Objective Topics. Retrieval and the Brain. Retrieval Neuroscience of Memory. LTP Brain areas Consolidation Reconsolidation

IIE 269: Cognitive Psychology

The Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

Unit III. Biological Bases of Behavior

Abstract. 1 Introduction

Modeling of Hippocampal Behavior

Applied Neuroscience. Conclusion of Science Honors Program Spring 2017

The Nervous System and the Endocrine System

Ch 8. Learning and Memory

Ch 8. Learning and Memory

Anatomy Review Graphics are used with permission of: Pearson Education Inc., publishing as Benjamin Cummings (

Na + K + pump. The beauty of the Na + K + pump. Cotransport. The setup Cotransport the result. Found along the plasma membrane of all cells.

Chapter 6 subtitles postsynaptic integration

LEARNING ARBITRARY FUNCTIONS WITH SPIKE-TIMING DEPENDENT PLASTICITY LEARNING RULE

Modeling a Neuron. 1 Introduction. 2 Grade Levels and Topics. 3 Objectives. 4 Materials

Cellular Bioelectricity

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Review

Supplementary figure 1: LII/III GIN-cells show morphological characteristics of MC

COGS 107B Week 1. Hyun Ji Friday 4:00-4:50pm

Cognitive Neuroscience History of Neural Networks in Artificial Intelligence The concept of neural network in artificial intelligence

Structure and Function of Neurons

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

The issue (1) Some spikes. The issue (2) Am I spikes over time?

LESSON 3.3 WORKBOOK. Why does applying pressure relieve pain? Workbook. Postsynaptic potentials

Neurons. Pyramidal neurons in mouse cerebral cortex expressing green fluorescent protein. The red staining indicates GABAergic interneurons.

NEURONS COMMUNICATE WITH OTHER CELLS AT SYNAPSES 34.3

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Supplementary Figure 1

Rolls,E.T. (2016) Cerebral Cortex: Principles of Operation. Oxford University Press.

Theoretical Neuroscience: The Binding Problem Jan Scholz, , University of Osnabrück

Modeling a Neuron. Grade Levels. Introduction. Objectives. Materials and Resources

Modeling the brain Christ Chris er t Sv S e v nsson e

Synfire chains with conductance-based neurons: internal timing and coordination with timed input

Neuroscience Flythrough. Lukas Schott und Letita Parcalabescu

Synap&c Plas&city. long-term plasticity (~30 min to lifetime) Long-term potentiation (LTP) / Long-term depression (LTD)

Nerve Cell Communication

VS : Systemische Physiologie - Animalische Physiologie für Bioinformatiker. Neuronenmodelle III. Modelle synaptischer Kurz- und Langzeitplastizität

When cells are already maximally potentiated LTP is occluded.

Brain and Cognitive Sciences 9.96 Experimental Methods of Tetrode Array Neurophysiology IAP 2001

Observational Learning Based on Models of Overlapping Pathways

Cellular mechanisms of information transfer: neuronal and synaptic plasticity

Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003

STRUCTURAL ELEMENTS OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM

Supplementary Figure 1. Basic properties of compound EPSPs at

Lecture 22: A little Neurobiology

WHAT DOES 1 PETABYTE EQUAL? On average, one synapse can hold about 4.7 bits of information. This means that the human brain has a capacity of one

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010

Activity-Dependent Development II April 25, 2007 Mu-ming Poo

συν together απτειν to clasp 2h Neuroscience with Pharmacology Functions and Mechanisms of Reflexes Cogito, ergo sum ( I think therefore I am ) Down

What is Anatomy and Physiology?

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

Modulators of Spike Timing-Dependent Plasticity

Spiking Inputs to a Winner-take-all Network

Investigation of Physiological Mechanism For Linking Field Synapses

VISUAL CORTICAL PLASTICITY

Activity 2 The Brain and Drugs

MEMORY MODELS. CHAPTER 5: Memory models Practice questions - text book pages TOPIC 23

Neurobiology and Information Processing Theory: the science behind education

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. ACx plasticity is required for fear conditioning.

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems

Transcription:

The case for quantum entanglement in the brain Charles R. Legéndy September 26, 2017 Introduction Many-neuron cooperative events The challenge of reviving a cell assembly after it has been overwritten by other cell assemblies The argument for quantum entanglement

Meaningful inputs enter neurons on sets of synapses, linked by shared function and certain contexts call for direct links to unite these synapses sets 1. Quantum entanglement is a possible candidate to provide them.

The dendritic tree from the last slide: a pyramidal cell from the hippocampus of a rat 2. Scale bar 100µm (Megías et al, 2001)

Dendrites, covered with synaptic spines, and some axons from a nearby neuron (Valverde and Ruiz- Marcos, 1969)

The appearance of dendrites and their synaptic spines at electron microscope magnification. This is a 7 µm piece of a dendrite, with 25 synaptic spines on it (Harris, 1999)

The output of cortical neurons is a sequence of spikes (of firing ), usually at irregular time intervals

2. Many-neuron cooperative events

The cell assembly theory of the brain 3 Mental entities, as well as components of mental entities, are represented by groups of neurons ( cell assemblies ; Hebb, 1949) The groups are structureless; and they can overlap; existing assemblies do not constrain their membership. Once assigned, the membership remains fixed. Functional unity of the groups is provided by reinforcement of all synapses between their neurons. When the parameters are right, cell assemblies are capable of chain-reaction-like cascades of firing ( ignitions Legéndy, 1967), whereby firing spreads to all group members but not beyond, and is then silenced (by a combination of refractoriness and inhibition). Cell assemblies must contain enough neurons to exceed critical size for ignition, and to send out enough axonal branches with synapses to be recognizable to all neurons in their region. Structure is added to mental entities through linkages between different cell assemblies, resulting in reproducible timing of their ignitions, thereby conveying syntactic relations (Legéndy, 2009)

Ignitions appear to the rest of the brain as volleys well detectable over the usual random firing of neurons Intense volleys like this induce long-term potentiation (LTP). The LTP-inducing machinery is in effect a detector of lowprobability events ( surprising events) in spike trains. LTP increases the synaptic weights on its synapses (A).

Multisynaptic volleys (from ignitions) have great combinatorial diversity but the data tell us that their effect on the neurons ignores most of it it is solely determined by the sum of synaptic weights at the inputs

Learning via LTP consists of increasing the synaptic weights at the synapses excited, and normalization by decreasing the rest of the weights, when positive (longterm depression, LTD) 4

3. The challenge of reviving a cell assembly after it has been overwritten by other cell assemblies

The trouble is: if learning can change nothing other than the synaptic weights, those changes will be buried after a while. 5 Only the newest cell assemblies will be ignitable; the rest will be lost It follows that actual brains have a backup system able to revive lost cell assemblies (and the backup utilizes the full diversity of inputs!) Reviving a cell assembly means making its connecting synapses stronger than the rest, like when the cell assembly was newly formed. Reaching the right synapses (which are mixed in with other irrelevant ones), from the agent that calls for reviving the cell assembly, requires action at a distance possibly making use of some information-rich molecular messengers.

Proposed molecules for use in targeted remote action: Random-sequenced RNA/DNA segments, arbitrarily assigned to mental entities, and serving as code words for the entities thereafter (Legéndy, 2016)

To enable the bird synapses to be revived later, it is necessary to place permanent bird markers near them. Later, when remotely sent bird messengers recognize these, they can initiate synapse growth 6

4. The argument for quantum entanglement

The disadvantage of pure molecular messaging as opposed to its combination with quantum communication Molecular messaging can, in principle, achieve targeted action at a distance but the messengers needed are too large (at least 10 nm). Therefore they move too slowly. Also, they take up too much space; they ll fill up the brain. Alternatively, if molecular messaging is only used at the time of initial memory formation, and used for handcarrying pairs of entangled fragments into their desired places, the entanglement (which is automatically a targeted affair) makes further molecular recognition unnecessary. Quantum communication is fast; and complexes of qubits can be super-small. In the desired final arrangement, a chemical restore the assembly process initiates a state transition at the Alice location (at the central decision element); and the resulting state transition at the Bob location (at the synapse) initiates the chemical process causing the required growth. 7

However, in the final configuration: Many entangled pairs must be able to communicate simultaneously, without crosstalk The quantum entanglement must endure for years it must be effectively decoherencefree

NOTES 1. There are two other such contexts I can think of, in addition to the one I chose for the purposes of this presentation (all of them having to do with long-term memory; Legéndy, 2016); but I left those out to shorten the talk. I think, the one I chose (about reconstructing the lost synaptic patterns in mental images when they are being recalled; see below) was the most clear-cut of the three, and seemed best suited to make my point. In the same vein as Alice and Bob are sometimes invoked in connection with quantum communication, I sometimes use bird, leaves, coffee, and others, in connection with the brain (as opposed to for instance a 1, a 2,,a n ). 2. The neuron shown has about 30,000 excitatory synapses (the barely visible small hairs ). The drawing shows, for each class of dendritic segment, the number of synapses per micron (the small rectangles list the densities for excitatory and inhibitory synapses and the ratio of the two). This slide, and the next two, serve to illustrate the extraordinary number of synaptic spines on pyramidal cells and the non-trivial communication task involved in addressing a preselected set of them, to the exclusion of all the others (see later slides). 3. The conclusion reached below (that all synaptic patterns are overwritten many times over a lifetime, see next slides) does not depend on the details of the cell assembly theory; it only depends on the assumptions that mental images are represented by sets of interconnected neurons, that the sets can overlap, that they are consistent between different occurrences of the same image, and that storage involves synaptic changes of some sort, between set members. 4. Dots are placed next to the synapses modified by LTP (and kept there); showing that in successive episodes of signal interaction more and more of the synaptic weights become modified until eventually all of them do, upward and downward, many times, and all old patterns are thoroughly overwritten. 5. The overwhelming evidence indicating that synaptic changes alone are insufficient for explaining long-term memory, or the vast capacity of human brains, is surveyed in the aforementioned paper (Legéndy, 2016). 6. The small framed portion in the left-hand drawings is magnified, then magnified again (top center), to show that, as time goes by, more and more tags are planted inside the dendrite (some irrelevant ones are also included), each of them uniquely responsive to the messenger specific to its underlying entity. In the upper right-hand sketch the messengers of one entity ( bird ; leaves ; coffee ) flood the dendrite, combine with their corresponding tags (receptors) when they recognize them, and wake up the synapses near them. Because of the placement of the tags in the middle drawing, the patterns of woken-up synapses (red circles) match the red arrows in the left-hand drawings (which describe events that occurred much earlier). 7. If the last step were a measurement, as in a quantum computer paradigm, the entangled system would undergo irreversible quantum collapse. But in the system I envision the corresponding step is not strictly speaking a measurement, where the quantum system would interact with a macroscopic classical system; rather, here the entangled quantum system interacts with another quantum system (a molecule). My hope is that (maybe) in such an interaction the state of the entangled system is not ruined to the same degree, and (maybe through a multi-step process) the original situation can be reinstated well enough that the same communication can take place again, whenever needed.

REFERENCES Harris K. M. (1999) Structure, development, and plasticity of dendritic spines. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 9, 343-348. Hebb D. O. (1949) The organization of behavior (New York: Wiley). Legéndy C. R. (1967) On the scheme by which the human brain stores information. Math. Biosci. 1, 555-591. Legéndy C. R. (2009) Circuits in the Brain: A model of shape processing in the primary visual cortex. (New York: Springer) Legéndy C. R. (2016) Synaptic and extrasynaptic traces of long-term memory: the ID molecule theory. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 575-598. Megías M., Emri Z.S., Freund T.F., and Gulyás A.I. (2001). Total number and distribution of inhibitory and excitatory synapses on hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells. Neuroscience 102, 527-540. Valverde and Ruiz-Marcos A. (1969) Dendritic spines in the visual cortex of the mouse: Introduction to a mathematical model. Exp. Brain Res. 8, 269-283.