Studying social cognition in patients with schizophrenia and patients with frontotemporal dementia: Theory of mind and the perception of sarcasm

Similar documents
Age-related quantitative and qualitative changes in decision making ability

Dr Fiona Kumfor University of Sydney

Neurodegenerative diseases that degrade regions of the brain will eventually become

Piano playing skills in a patient with frontotemporal dementia: A longitudinal case study

Frontal Behavioural Inventory (FBI)

Baseline Characteristics of Patients Attending the Memory Clinic Serving the South Shore of Boston

Michael L. Kimbarow, Ph.D. Wendy Quach, Ph.D. Marion D. Meyerson, Ph.D. San Jose State University San Jose, CA

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Right and Left Medial Orbitofrontal Volumes Show an Opposite Relationship to Agreeableness in FTD

After the Diagnosis: Rehabilitation & Support Options for Mild Dementia

EARLY ONSET FRONTOTERMPORAL DEMENTIA AND ALZHEIMERS DISEASE: DIAGNOSIS, TREATMENT AND CARE

Valence and Gender Effects on Emotion Recognition Following TBI. Cassie Brown Arizona State University

Social Cognition and Metacognition in Alzheimer s Disease

CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE

Clinical Study First- and Third-Person Perspectives in Psychotic Disorders and Mood Disorders with Psychotic Features

Pharmacologyonline 3: (2010)

F rontotemporal dementia (FTD) is the term currently used

doi: /brain/awn314 Brain 2009: 132;

Frontotemporal Degeneration

Theory of mind following traumatic brain injury: The role of emotion recognition and executive dysfunction

Peer Perception in Autism. Kathryn McVicar, MD Assistant Professor Clinical Pediatrics and Neurology Albert Einstien College of Medicine

Recognition memory in psychotic patients

Geri R Hall, PhD, GCNS, FAAN Advanced Practice Nurse Emeritus Banner Alzheimer s Institute

CHAPTER 2 CRITERION VALIDITY OF AN ATTENTION- DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER (ADHD) SCREENING LIST FOR SCREENING ADHD IN OLDER ADULTS AGED YEARS

Running head: SOCIAL COGNITION IN NEURODEGENERATIVE DISEASES 1

Mental Health Disorders Civil Commitment UNC School of Government

Storage and processing working memory functions in Alzheimer-type dementia

AVAILABLE COURSES FOR THE ACADEMIC YEAR (GREEK COURSES)

Executive Dysfunction as a Barrier to Authenticity in Decision Making

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children

Form D1: Clinician Diagnosis

Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams

DSM-5 MAJOR AND MILD NEUROCOGNITIVE DISORDERS (PAGE 602)

Adult Attachment Interview

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni

How preserved is episodic memory in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia?

Brooke DePoorter M.Cl.Sc. (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Differentiation of semantic dementia and Alzheimer s disease using the Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination (ACE)

Commentary: The Forensic Psychiatry of Frontotemporal Dementia

Number perseveration in healthy subjects: Does prolonged stimulus exposure influence performance on a serial addition task?

Contextual bias and inferencing in adults with right hemisphere brain damage It is widely accepted that damage to the right cerebral hemisphere (RHD)

Cognitive and neural basis of emotion regulation in bipolar disorder

Asperger's Syndrome WHAT IS ASPERGER'S? Article QUICK LINKS :

What is. frontotemporal. address? dementia?

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective

Emotion comprehension in the temporal variant of frontotemporal dementia

Do not copy or distribute without permission. S. Weintraub, CNADC, NUFSM, 2009

Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics August 2014, Volume 16, Number 8:

Frontotemporal Dementia: Towards better diagnosis. Frontotemporal Dementia. John Hodges, NeuRA & University of New South Wales, Sydney.

Pacific Assessment of Confabulation. Confabulation has been defined as unintentional verbal distortions or misinterpretations

Perspectives on Frontotemporal Dementia and Primary Progressive Aphasia

Other Disorders Myers for AP Module 69

Exploration of social rule violation in patients with focal prefrontal neurosurgical lesions

What is Autism? Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA.

Alzheimer s Disease. Clinical characteristics of late-onset Alzheimer s disease (LOAD) A/Prof David Darby

Cognitive Ability (Decline) & Social Isolation

Buy full version here - for $ 7.00

Intro to Executive Functioning Across the Lifespan. Agenda. The Construct of Executive Functioning. Construct of Executive Functioning

DISCLAIMER: ECHO Nevada emphasizes patient privacy and asks participants to not share ANY Protected Health Information during ECHO clinics.

Screening for Cognitive Dysfunction in Corticobasal Syndrome: Utility of Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination

CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Course Syllabus, Spring 2018 Columbia University

Executive function in progressive and nonprogressive behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia

Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs

Psychiatric Diagnoses In Developmentally Disabled Persons

Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury. Seth Warschausky, PhD Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation University of Michigan

NATIONAL INSTITUTE FOR HEALTH AND CLINICAL EXCELLENCE SCOPE. Personality Disorder: the clinical management of borderline personality disorder

Action and Emotion Understanding

Dynamic functional integration of distinct neural empathy systems

William W. Hale III, 1 Quinten A. W. Raaijmakers, 1 Anne van Hoof, 2 and Wim H. J. Meeus 1,3. 1. Introduction

Alzheimer's Disease Brain Failure, Stopping the Momentum. Katherine E. Galluzzi, DO, CMD, FACOFP dist.

Agoraphobia. An anxiety disorder marked by fear of being in situations in which escape may be difficult or impossible.

A CONVERSATION ABOUT NEURODEVELOPMENT: LOST IN TRANSLATION

Language Disorders in Greek 5, May 2014

I n the past three decades various cognitive screening

Module 2: Diagnosing frontotemporal dementia Important Notice: this work may not be a Commonwealth publication or product.

Psychological Disorders. Schizophrenia Spectrum & Other Psychotic Disorders. Schizophrenia. Neurodevelopmental Disorders 4/12/2018

منتخب بهترین مجلات در حوزه روانپزشکی تهیه و تنظیم:مهشید تسلیمی کارشناس ارزشیابی مرکز تحقیقات روان تنی

Perspective-taking deficits in people with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a prospective investigation

What APS Workers Need to Know about Frontotemporal, Lewy Body and Vascular Dementias

Affect Recognition and Empathy. Barry Willer, PhD

Non Alzheimer Dementias

TBI Irritability, Aggression & Anger. A New Perspective on Anger and Aggression after TBI. Disclosures 9/13/2018. Grant support:

ALS, Cognitive Impairment (CI) and Frontotemporal Lobar Dementia (FTLD): A Professional s Guide

Mild cognitive impairment A view on grey areas of a grey area diagnosis

Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients

The brain and behaviour

Epidemiology Old Age

Laboratoire sur le Langage, le Cerveau et la Cognition (L2C2), Institut des Sciences

Overview. Case #1 4/20/2012. Neuropsychological assessment of older adults: what, when and why?

MY RIGHT I: DECEPTION DETECTION AND HEMISPHERIC DIFFERENCES IN SELF-AWARENESS

Oxford University Press Brain Science Collection

Cognition and Psychopathology Fall 2009

FTD/PPA Caregiver Education Conference March 11, 2011

Gender Sensitive Factors in Girls Delinquency

Time perception, cognitive correlates, age and emotions

Lone Star College-Tomball Community Library Tomball Parkway Tomball, TX

The ABCs of Dementia Diagnosis

Dementia Diagnosis Guidelines Primary Care

An estimated half a million

Empathy is a complex psychological response in which observation, memory,

Transcription:

Behavioural Neurology 19 (2008) 65 69 65 IOS Press Studying social cognition in patients with schizophrenia and patients with frontotemporal dementia: Theory of mind and the perception of sarcasm Mary H. Kosmidis a,, Eleni Aretouli a, Vassilis P. Bozikas b, Maria Giannakou a and Panayiotis Ioannidis c a School of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki, Greece b 2 nd Psychiatry Department, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki, Greece c 2 nd Neurology Department, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki, Greece Abstract. We investigated social cognition and theory of mind in patients with schizophrenia and in patients with frontotemporal dementia in order to elucidate the cognitive mechanisms involved in the breakdown of these skills in psychiatric and neurological patients. Our tasks included videotaped scenarios of social interactions depicting sincere, sarcastic and paradoxical remarks, as well as lies. We found impaired performance of the schizophrenia group on all theory of mind conditions despite their intact understanding of sincere statements. In contrast, the FTD group performed poorly only when they had to rely on paralinguistic cues indicating sarcasm or lies, and not on paradoxical remarks or sarcasm when given additional verbal cues. Our findings suggest that, while current deficits in social and interpersonal functioning in patients with FTD may reflect a decrement in previously acquired skills, similar deficits in patients with schizophrenia may reflect an altogether inadequately learned process. Keywords: Social cognition, theory of mind, schizophrenia, frontotemporal dementia 1. Introduction Social cognition is an umbrella term that comprises a variety of skills which make successful social interaction possible. One of these skills, theory of mind (ToM), is the ability to perceive the intentions and beliefs of others. This skill has received considerable research attention recently, particularly with respect to schizophrenia [1,5,10,11], as it may help to explain these patients poor social and interpersonal functioning. In fact, some theorists have suggested that positive symptoms are the result of erroneous attribution of mental representations in schizophrenia [10,11] or the Corresponding author: Mary H. Kosmidis, Ph.D., School of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, 54124 Thessaloniki, Greece. Tel.: +30 2310 997 308; Fax: +30 2310 997 384; E-mail: Kosmidis@psy.auth.gr. inability to inhibit personal beliefs and subsequent attribution of one s own beliefs to others. Other researchers have suggested that patients with schizophrenia do not lack ToM but tend to overuse or misuse it [1], interpreting others intentions beyond what the data support [5]. Regardless of symptom subtype (positive or negative), patients with schizophrenia appear to have a poor understanding of the subtle cues typically used in a conversation [7]. Individuals with frontotemporal dementia (FTD) also demonstrate unusual and inappropriate social behaviour in the early stages of the disorder, even before the diagnosis is confirmed. They exhibit deficits in empathy and inhibition of behavior, impulsivity, stereotypical behaviors and changes in appetite [4] with little cognitive decline [13]. In fact, their behavioral changes often lead to an erroneous diagnosis of schizophrenia or bipolar disorder, rather than dementia [6,12]. Sever- ISSN 0953-4180/08/$17.00 2008 IOS Press and the authors. All rights reserved

66 M.H. Kosmidis et al. / Social cognition schizophrenia frontotemporal dementia al studies have reported deficits in the perception and processing of emotions in patients with FTD [4,19,20], implicating the ventromedial frontal lobes [14], an area thought to be involved in social functioning [3,9]. Interestingly, ToM and social cognition were found to be independent of overall neuropsychological status [15]. Therefore, changes in social behavior may reflect difficulties in the perception of emotional cues in FTD. We sought to investigate the cognitive mechanisms involved in one aspect of social cognition, namely, ToM. More specifically, we were interested in comparing two patient groups with known frontal and temporal dysfunction on tasks tapping into ToM, particularly the ability to infer the meaning of a conversational interaction in which the speaker s true opinions are not literal, as often occurs in daily discourse. 2. Method 2.1. Participants Our sample comprised 28 patients diagnosed with schizophrenia (according to DSM-IV criteria [2] and confirmed with the Greek version (translationadaptation to the Greek language by S. Beratis) of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (4.4) (MINI) [20]) and their age- and education-matched healthy control group (n =26). We also examined nine FTD patients [all fulfilled the Lund-Manchester consensus criteria for frontal variant frontotemporal dementia [18] and were at an early stage of the disorder] and their age- and education-matched healthy control group (n =10). Table 1 lists demographic characteristics of the groups. Exclusion criteria for the patient groups included other psychiatric or neurological diagnoses, a diagnosis of a developmental disorder, a history of head injury with loss of consciousness, alcohol or drug abuse during the six-month period prior to testing, non-native speakers of Greek, and any physical illness that may have affected cognitive performance. The same exclusion criteria, plus a history of any psychiatric or neurological diagnosis, applied to the healthy participants. 2.2. Procedure In order to test the ability to perceive sarcasm, we used two subtests of a broader test of ToM, the Perception of Social Inference Test (PESIT), which we developed for the Greek population (based on the Awareness of Social Inference Test [16,17]). The items on these subtests were videotaped scenarios played by two actors. In the Social Inference-Minimal (SI-M) subtest, we asked participants to view a series of brief scenarios of a social interaction in which one of the actors made a sincere, sarcastic or paradoxical conversational remark. Comprehension was assessed by three questions following each dialogue, assessing the speaker s emotional state, the meaning of his/her remarks, his/her beliefs and intentions and the message he/she thought that the other person got from his/her remarks. Table 2a lists examples of a sarcastic and a paradoxical remark. The Social Inference Enriched (SI-E) subtest differed from the previous in that contextual cues (in the form of a prologue or dialogue, which revealed the speaker s true thoughts) were provided in addition to the paralinguistic features (namely, prosody, facial expressions and any subtle upper body movement that came naturally as the speaker made his/her comment). Participants were required to assess the speaker s emotional state, the meaning of his/her conversational remarks, his/her beliefs and intentions, and the message he/she thought that the other person got from his/her remarks. Through this subtest, we examined the ability to use contextual knowledge to understand a scenario, in which a false remark is made either sarcastically (meant to imply the opposite meaning, thus amplifying the inferred truth) or as a lie (meant to conceal the truth). Table 2b lists examples of items on this subtest showing a sarcastic remark and a lie. The aforementioned subtests were administered in the same order for all participants (first SI-M, then SI- E). Within each subtest, the items were also presented in the same order to all participants, regardless of their specific nature (SI-M: first sincere items, then sarcastic, and finally paradoxical; SI-E: first sarcastic, then lies). 3. Results 3.1. Schizophrenia Patients with schizophrenia performed more poorly than their healthy control group on both conditions of sarcasm (minimal: U =70.5, p<0.001; enriched: U = 156.0, p < 0.001), on the paradoxical statements (U =36.5, p<0.001) and on lies (U =69.5, p< 0.001), despite their unimpaired performance on the sincere statements condition (U = 259.0, p > 0.05). Table 1 lists means and standard deviations for the performance of all groups on all tests.

M.H. Kosmidis et al. / Social cognition schizophrenia frontotemporal dementia 67 Table 1 Means (SDs) of demographic characteristics and test performance by group Variable Group Schizophrenia Healthy Frontotemporal Healthy (n =28) controls (n =26) dementia (n =9) controls (n =10) Median Mean (SD) Median Mean (SD) Median Mean (SD) Median Mean (SD) Age (years) 36.3 (7.5) 33.8 (9.8) 64.6 (7.8) 64.0 (7.2) Education (years) 11.9 (2.3) 12.9 (2.5) 7.8 (4.5) 9.6 (2.8) Duration of illness (years) 11.4 (7.8) 2.7 (1.6) Social Inference-Minimal Sincere 16.0 14.0 (4.1) 16.0 16.0 (2.2) 14.0 12.9 (4.6) 16.5 15.6 (2.8) Sarcastic 12.0 10.2 (5.7)** 18.0 17.3 (1.3) 4.0 4.7 (4.3)** 13.0 12.3 (4.5) Paradoxical 12.0 11.4 (5.6)** 24.0 22.1 (3.7) 14.0 13.5 (7.0) 19.0 17.7 (5.9) Social Inference-Enriched Sarcastic 9.5 9.0 (3.7)** 14.0 14.1 (1.6) 10.0 7.9 (4.2) 13.5 12.9 (2.2) Lies 10.0 9.6 (4.8)** 15.0 13.9 (7.0) 7.0 7.9 (6.4)* 11.0 10.8 (3.8) *p <0.005. *p <0.001. Table 2 Examples of items from (a) the Social Inference-Minimal (SI-M) and (b) the Social Inference-Enriched subtests (a) Social Inference-Minimal subtest Sarcastic remark Nick: Did you have a good time yesterday? Mary: What can I say, it was fantastic... (b) Social Inference-Enriched subtest Sarcastic remark Mary (speaking on the phone): Nick and I were looking at the photographs yesterday and laughing. Even he thought we looked awful. Nick (enters): Hi, Mary. How s it going? Mary: I was just saying how great your photographs were. Paradoxical remark Mary: Do you like my new haircut? Nick: Did you pay for that? Lie Mary (speaking on the phone): Did you see Nick s new haircut? It looks awful. Nick (enters): Mary, how do you like my haircut? Mary: It looks terrific on you! 3.2. FTD Patients with FTD performed more poorly than their healthy control group on comprehension of sarcastic remarks only on the minimal subtest (U =8.5, p< 0.001) and on lies (U =5.5, p<0.005); there was no group difference on sincere (U =27.0, p>0.05) and paradoxical (U = 27.0, p > 0.05) conditions, nor on the enriched scenarios of sarcasm (U =25.5, p>0.05). 4. Discussion We examined social cognition in two groups of patients with known frontal and temporal dysfunction in an attempt to elucidate the cognitive mechanisms involved in the difficulties these patient groups have with social interactions. More specifically, we expected that any differences in the pattern of deficits between these two groups might help to elucidate the point in the process of ToM in which there is a breakdown. We found both similarities and differences in the response patterns of patients with schizophrenia and those with FTD. Specifically, neither group demonstrated deficits in the perception of sincere or literal interactions; thus, we ruled out the possibility that any difficulties on ToM subtests could be attributed to a generalized problem with comprehension. Interestingly, performance patterns diverged on the ToM tasks. While patients with schizophrenia performed poorly on the perception of sarcastic and paradoxical remarks, as well as of lies, regardless of whether or not the statements were enriched with additional information, we found circumscribed deficits in patients with FTD. The latter groups difficulties were limited to the perception of sarcasm when only paralinguistic cues (prosody and/or the facial expressions) of sarcasm were available. Their difficulty perceiving lies may reflect problems reconciling contradictory yet apparently literal information, leading them to interpret the most recent information (i.e., the actual lie) literally. In contrast, these patients were able to perceive the intention of the speaker on the sarcasm-enriched items, perhaps because they were able to rely on additional verbal information indicat-

68 M.H. Kosmidis et al. / Social cognition schizophrenia frontotemporal dementia ing the speaker s true beliefs and intentions. A similar pattern emerged on the paradoxical items. One potential limitation to the study design, namely, the possibility of an order effect on the present results, warrants mention. Ideally, we would have presented the conditions in counterbalanced order across participants in each group. Despite the fact that we did not do this, however, we believe that we can safely rule out an order effect. This is certainly the case for the schizophrenia group, as they performed better on the first (sincere or literal) condition than on any of the subsequent conditions. In a similar fashion, the FTD group performed better on the first and third (paradoxical) conditions than on either of the sarcastic conditions (presented second and fourth in order) or the lies (last) condition. Based on the aforementioned findings, we suggest that the differential pattern of performance of the FTD group on the conditions of the ToM tasks was due to the amount and type of information available. When given additional verbal information, such as a prologue, or paradoxical information, patients with FTD were able to benefit from this and respond correctly. Without this information, they were unable to use paralinguistic cues effectively. The differential pattern of performance of our two patient groups highlights the broad range of difficulties with interpersonal interactions in schizophrenia and a partially preserved ability in FTD. These discrepant patterns may reflect group differences in illness duration, as well as in level of severity of their respective disorders. Clearly, in light of the relatively young age of onset of schizophrenia, our schizophrenia group had a much longer duration of illness; in fact, some studies have suggested less than normal interpersonal functioning even before symptom onset in schizophrenia [8]. Another interpretation involves the fundamental nature of the two disorders. The neurodevelopmental nature of schizophrenia may impede the process of mastering subtle and effective social skills such as ToM. In contrast, the late onset of FTD, a neurodegenerative disorder, may have allowed these patients to apply previously overlearned skills in perceiving subtle cues in a social interaction, despite their present diagnosis. Consequently, while impaired interpersonal functioning in patients with FTD may reflect a decrement in previously acquired skills, similar deficits in patients with schizophrenia may reflect an altogether inadequately learned process. Further investigations of social cognition in patients with schizophrenia and those with FTD may help elucidate the cognitive mechanisms involved in these disorders and the reasons for the observed difficulties in social functioning and interpersonal relationships. Shedding light on the pathoaetiology of patients social difficulties, as well as on the cognitive mechanisms of ToM, may help guide psychotherapeutic efforts to improve social functioning, and, consequently, their quality of life. Acknowledgements This research was supported by a Pythagoras II Grant for the Reinforcement of Research Teams in Universities (Ministry of Education and Religion of the Hellenic Republic) to the first author. References [1] A. Abu-Akel, Impaired theory of mind in schizophrenia, Pragmatics and Cognition 7 (1999), 247 282. [2] A. Bechara, H. Damasio and A.R. Damasio, Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex, Cerebral Cortex 10 (2000), 295 307. [3] S. Bozeat, C.A. Gregory, M.A. Ralph and J.R. Hodges, Which neuropsychiatric and behavioral features distinguish frontal and temporal variants of frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer s disease? Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 69 (2000), 178 186. [4] M. Brüne, Social cognition and behavior in schizophrenia, in: The Social Brain: Evolution and Psychopathology, M. Brüne, H. Ribbert and W. Schiefenhövel, eds, Wiley & Sons, Chichester, UK, 2003, pp. 277 313. [5] H.A. Comstock, Establishing the association for frontotemporal dementia, Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 17 (2004), 368 369. [6] R. Corcoran and C.D. Frith, Conversational conduct and the symptoms of schizophrenia, Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1 (1996), 305 318. [7] B.A. Cornblatt, The New York high risk project to the Hillside recognition and prevention (RAP) program, American Journal of Medical Genetics 114 (2002), 956 966. [8] A.R. Damasio, The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London/Biological Sciences 351 (1996), 1413 1420. [9] C.D. Frith, The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1992. [10] C.D. Frith, Theory of mind in schizophrenia, in: The Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia, A.S. David and J.C. Cutting, eds, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hove, 1994, pp. 147 161. [11] C.A. Gregory and J.R. Hodges, Clinical features of frontal lobe dementia in comparison to Alzheimer s disease, Journal of Neural Transm (Suppl.) 47 (1996), 103 123. [12] C.A. Gregory, J. Serra-Mestres and J R. Hodges, Early diagnosis of the frontal variant of frontotemporal dementia: How sensitive are standard neuroimaging and neuropsychologic tests? Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsycholology and Behavioral Neurology 12 (1999), 128 135.

M.H. Kosmidis et al. / Social cognition schizophrenia frontotemporal dementia 69 [13] J. Keane, A.J. Calder, J.R. Hodges and A.W. Young, Face and emotion processing in frontal variant frontotemporal dementia, Neuropsychologia 40 (2002), 655 665. [14] S. Lough, C. Gregory and J.R. Hodges, Dissociation of social cognition and executive function in frontal variant frontotemporal dementia, Neurocase 7 (2001), 123 130. [15] S. McDonald, C. Bornhofen, D. Shum, E. Long, C. Saunders and K. Neulinger, Reliability and validity of The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT): A clinical test of social perception, Disability Rehabilitation 28 (2006), 1529 1542. [16] S. McDonald, S. Flanagan, J. Rollins and J. Kinch, TASIT: A new clinical tool for assessing social perception after traumatic brain injury, Head Trauma Rehabilitation 8 (2003), 219 238. [17] D. Neary, J.S. Snowden, L. Gustafson, U. Passant, D. Stuss, S. Black, M. Freedman, A. Kertesz, P.H. Robert, M. Albert, K. Boone, B.L. Miller, J. Cummings and D.F. Benson, Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: A consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria, Neurology 51 (1998), 1546 1554. [18] H.J. Rosen, K. Pace-Savitsky, R.J. Perry, J.H. Kramer, B.L. Miller and R.W. Levenson, Recognition of emotion in the frontal and temporal variants of frontotemporal dementia, Dementia in Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 17 (2004), 277 281. [19] D.V. Sheehan, Y. Lecrubier, K.H. Sheehan, P. Amorim, J. Janavs, E. Weiller, T. Herguetta, R. Baker and G.C. Dunbar, The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric (M.I.N.I): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10, Clinical Psychiatry 59(Suppl. 20) (1998), 22 33. [20] J.S. Snowden, D. Bathgate, A. Varma, A. Blackshaw, Z.C. Gibbons and D. Neary, Distinct behavioural profiles in frontotemporal dementia and semantic dementia, Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 70 (2001), 323 332.

MEDIATORS of INFLAMMATION The Scientific World Journal Gastroenterology Research and Practice Diabetes Research International Endocrinology Immunology Research Disease Markers Submit your manuscripts at BioMed Research International PPAR Research Obesity Ophthalmology Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine Stem Cells International Oncology Parkinson s Disease Computational and Mathematical Methods in Medicine AIDS Behavioural Neurology Research and Treatment Oxidative Medicine and Cellular Longevity