THE COGNITIVE NEUROPSY CHOLOGY OF FALSE MEMORIES: INTRODUCTION

Similar documents
Inducing and reducing false memories: A Swedish version of the Deese Roediger McDermott paradigm

The effect of retrieval instructions on false recognition: exploring the nature of the gist memory impairment in amnesia

Recognizing Identical Versus Similar Categorically Related Common Objects: Further Evidence for Degraded Gist Representations in Amnesia

Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective

Suppression of false recognition in Alzheimer's disease and in patients with frontal lobe lesions

Impaired implicit memory for gist information in amnesia

Misattribution Errors in Alzheimer s Disease: The Illusory Truth Effect

A cognitive neuropsychological approach to false memory: Korsakoff patients and the DRM paradigm

Metacognition and False Recognition in Alzheimer s Disease: Further Exploration of the Distinctiveness Heuristic

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

Metacognition and false recognition in patients with frontal lobe lesions: the distinctiveness heuristic

Importance of Deficits

Running Head: MODALITY EFFECTS IN FALSE RECALL 1. Modality Effects in False Recall: Dissociations Between Short- and Long-Term Memory

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 1999, 16 (3/4/5), Rahul M. Dodhia and Janet Metcalfe. Columbia University, New York, USA

Memory part I. Memory Distortions Eyewitness Testimony Lineup Studies

Retrieval Failure Contributes to Gist-Based False Recognition

Memory II. Reconstructive Memory Forgetting

C242 Comparing Decay Rates 1. Running head: COMPARING DECAY RATES FOR FALSE MEMORIES. Comparing Decay Rates for Accurate and False Memories

Memory: sins and virtues. The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters.

Pacific Assessment of Confabulation. Confabulation has been defined as unintentional verbal distortions or misinterpretations

Pupillary Response to False Memories in the DRM Paradigm

Retrieval Monitoring and Anosognosia in Alzheimer s Disease

Increasing the amount of information that can be held in short-term memory by grouping related items together into a single unit, or chunk.

Imagination and memory: Does imagining implausible events lead to false autobiographical memories?

Use of a false recognition paradigm in an Alzheimer s disease clinical trial: A pilot study

Brain Potentials Reflect Behavioral Differences in True and False Recognition. Tim Curran Department of Psychology, Case Western Reserve University

Why misinformation is more likely to be recognised over time: A source monitoring account

Memory: sins and virtues

Confabulation behavior and false memories in Korsakoff s syndrome: Role of source memory and executive functioning

BRIEF REPORTS Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of retrieval

The Development of Recency and Frequency Memory: Is There a Developmental Shift from Reliance on Trace-Strength to Episodic Recall?

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

The effects of a levels-of-processing manipulation on false recall

PSYCHOLOGY SCHACTER PDF

The effect of testing on the vulnerability to misinformation in adolescents and adults

WSC 2018 SCIENCE. Science of Memory

Overdependence on Degraded Gist Memory in Alzheimer s Disease

False memories and confabulation

Memory for Actions of an Event: Older and Younger Adults Compared

Mechanisms of Memory: Can we distinguish true from false memories?

Telling It Like It Isn t: The Cognitive Neuroscience of Confabulation

Technical accuracy vs. content accuracy. Is this good or bad? Advantages/Disadvantages

Suppressing False Recognition in Younger and Older Adults: The Distinctiveness Heuristic

What Effect Do Schemas Have On The Recall Of

False Memory: P300 Amplitude, Topography, and Latency. Antoinette R. Miller, Christopher Baratta, Christine Wynveen, and J.

Learning Facts From Fiction: Effects of Healthy Aging and Early-Stage Dementia of the Alzheimer Type

Reality Monitoring in Adults Reporting Repressed, Recovered, or Continuous Memories of Childhood Sexual Abuse

3/20/2014. Seven Sins of Memory. Seven Sins of Memory. Seven Sins of Memory

The Seven Sins of Memory: An Update. Daniel L. Schacter Harvard University

MEASURING CONSCIOUS MEMORY 0

Running head: False memories for emotional DRM lists. Original title:

The Suggestibility of Older Witnesses

The Effects of Frontal Lobe Functioning and Age on Veridical and False Recall

Presurgical Language Mapping Kathleen B. McDermott, 1 Jason M. Watson, 1 and Jeffrey G. Ojemann 2

Brain Imaging Applied to Memory & Learning


Discovering Statistics: Experimental Project

3/31/2017. Seven Sins of Memory. Seven Sins of Memory. Seven Sins of Memory

HBEV: Non-Print Items

Investigating the Role of Episodic Gist and False Memory

The contribution of recollection and familiarity to yes±no and forced-choice recognition tests in healthy subjects and amnesics

Age-Related Deficits in Reality Monitoring of Action Memories

Attributions of Familiarity in Amnesia: Evidence From a Fame Judgment Task

False Memories: Young and Older Adults Think of Semantic Associates at the Same Rate, but Young Adults Are More Successful at Source Monitoring

False Memory? False Memory: DRM Errors Are Unrelated to the Misinformation Effect

Neuropsychologia 48 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Semantic, Phonological, and Hybrid Veridical and False Memories in Healthy Older Adults and in Individuals With Dementia of the Alzheimer Type

Misattribution of Memory misattribution of memory Misattribution of Memory Components of Misattribution Cryptomnesia Cryptomnesia

Age-related Neural Changes during Memory Conjunction Errors

Frontal lobe contributions to recognition and recall: Linking basic research with clinical evaluation and remediation

Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

The neural correlates of gistbased true and false recognition

Memory in Everyday Life. Lesson 5

Blending of Non-Similar Episodes as a Result of Analogical Mapping with a Third One

Serial model. Amnesia. Amnesia. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. Prof. Stephan Anagnostaras. Lecture 3: HM, the medial temporal lobe, and amnesia

The Fate of Memory: Comment on McCloskey and Zaragoza

10/24/2017. Medial Temporal Lobes. Autobiographical Memory. Episodic and Semantic Memory. Arlo Clark-Foos, Ph.D.

Arlo Clark-Foos, Ph.D.

False Memories in Schizophrenia

Announcements. Returning to Memory. V. Stage of processing. V. Stage of Processing Model. What do you recall? 4/9/2014

Memory distortion: an adaptive perspective

False memory and importance: Can we prioritize encoding without consequence?

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report

Interfering with remembering and knowing: Effects of divided attention at retrieval

Announcements. Grade Query Tool Updated with. Exam Scores Aplia Scores Cumulative scores and comparison to class

THE ROLE OF THE MEDIAL TEMPORAL LOBES IN OLDER ADULTS ASSOCIATIVE MEMORY DEFICIT: A BEHAVIORAL STUDY. Molly Bisbee

Distinguishing between Category-based and Similarity-based Induction

The Influence of Pop-Culture on Misattribution of Memory

MEMORY STORAGE. There are three major kinds of storage:

Journal of Memory and Language

The Ontogeny and Durability of True and False Memories: A Fuzzy Trace Account

Not All False Memories Are Created Equal: The Neural Basis of False Recognition

/ The Unbidden Past: Involuntary Autobiographical Memories as a Basic Mode of. Remembering.

2. Source Monitoring. False Memories, Psychology of

False recognition in younger and older adults: Exploring the characteristics of illusory memories. Memory & Cognition, 25,

Script memory for typical and atypical actions: controls versus patients with severe closed-head injury

Strong memories obscure weak memories in associative recognition

Forced confabulation affects memory sensitivity as well as response bias

Misleading Postevent Information and the Memory Impairment Hypothesis: Comment on Belli and Reply to Tversky and Tuchin

Transcription:

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 1999, 16 (3/4/5), 193 195 THE COGNITIVE NEUROPSY CHOLOGY OF FALSE MEMORIES: INTRODUCTION This special issue of Cognitive Neuropsychology is devoted to one of the most intensively investigated topics in recent memory research: the analysis of false memories. Experimental and theoretical studies of memory have long been informed by analyses of errors, distortions, and illusions in remembering (for reviews, see Estes, 1997; Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993; Roediger, 1996; Schacter, 1995). Within cognitive psychology, Sir Frederic Bartlett s monograph, Remembering, is generally recognised as a key source of modernresearch on memory distortions (Bartlett, 1932). Bartlett pioneered the use of errors and distortions as a tool for probing reconstructive aspect of memory. His approach enjoyed a revival during the 1970s, when a number of researchers developednew techniques for exploring memory distortions, and conceptualised them in the context of schema theories that were heavily influenced by Bartlett s thinking (Alba & Hasher, 1983). During the 1990s, there has been renewed interest in memory errors and distortions, sparked in part by a heated debate concerning the accuracy of traumatic memories recovered in psychotherapy (e.g., Loftus, 1993; Read & Lindsay, 1997). It is in the context of this debate that the term false memory has come into common usage in psychological research. False memories refer to recollections that are in some way distorted or, in extreme cases, involve remembering events that never happened at all. Cognitive neuropsychologists, like cognitive psychologists, have long been aware of, and interested in, the occurrence of false memories. Until Daniel L. Schacter Harvard University, Cambridge, USA recently, relevant research was comprised mainly of descriptive case studies of patients who produced confabulations about past experiences. During the past few years, however, there has been a marked increase in experimental research and theorising concerning the cognitive neuropsychology of false memories. New methods for systematically eliciting and analysing confabulation have been developed, and research has extended beyond the domain of confabulation to investigate experimentally different kinds of memory distortions and illusions in a variety of populations. In addition, electrophysiological and neuroimaging techniques have been applied to the analysis of false memories. This recent surge of research has much in the spirit of Bartlett attempted to use the occurrence of memory distortions as a window into the nature of basic memory processes (for review, see Schacter, Norman, & Koutstaal, 1998). The papers in this special issue are characterised by a variety of methodological and conceptual approaches. Kopelman provides an opening overview of cognitive neuropsychological research on false memories, and relates this work to theoretical, clinical, and legal issues. The next four papers involve detailed case studies of brain-damaged patients. Dab, Claes, Morais, and Shallice report a series of experiments indicating impaired search and intact monitoring or verification processes in a case of confabulation. Dab et al. discuss the observed pattern of preserved and impaired abilities in the context of several recent theoretical accounts of confabulation and related memory distortions. Requests for reprints should be addressed to Daniel L. Schacter, Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA. Supported by grant AG08441 from the National Institute on Aging. Ó 1999 Psychology Press Ltd 193

SCHACTER Parkin, Ward, Bindschaedler, Squires, and Powell describe a patient with left frontal lobe damage who exhibits exceptionally high levels of false recognition. Their experiments indicate that encoding deficits play an important role in this patient s heightened false recognition. Rapcsak, Reminger, Glisky, Kaszniak, and Comer describe two patients with frontal lobe damage who exhibit increased false recognition of faces on both anterograde and retrograde memory tests. They conclude that these patients fail to invoke effortful strategies that can normally be used to suppress false recognition. Ward, Parkin, Powell, Squires, Townshend, and Bradley describe a patient who shows elevated false recognition of faces and names of unfamiliar people. Ward et al. establish the specificity of the deficit, relate the impairment to component processes that have been postulated in theoretical models of person recognition, and conclude that inflated false recognition results from a failure in top-down regulation of the person recognition system. Four papers focus on groups of brain-damaged patients. The articles by Koutstaal, Schacter, Verfaellie, Brenner, and Jackson, and by Melo, Winocur, and Moscovitch examine false recognition and recall in amnesic patients. Previous research delineated conditions in which amnesic patients are more or less susceptible than controls to false memory effects, and related these findings to theoretical distinctions between memory for general or gist information on the one hand and memory for detailed, specific information on the other. The papers by Koutstaal et al. and Melo et al. extend and refine the earlier findings, point toward possible differences among subgroups of amnesic patients, and also help to specify the conditions under which amnesic patients show greater or lesser susceptibility to false memory effects. Balota, Cortese, Duchek, Adams, Roediger, McDermott, and Yerys examine false recall and recognition in patients with Alzheimer s disease. They use a converging semantic associates procedure initially developed by Deese (1959), recently revived and modified by Roediger and McDermott (1995), that produces high levels of false recall and recognition in healthy people (this procedure was also used by Melo et al.). Balota et al. report evidence that, relative to their own level of veridical recall and recognition, Alzheimer s patients are more susceptible to false recall and recognition than are normal older adults. However, in absolute terms, Alzheimer s patients showed reduced levels of false recognition and similar levels of false recall compared to healthy elderly controls. Dalla Barba, Nedjam, and Dubois examined relations among confabulation, executive functions, and source memory in Alzheimer s patients. They found that confabulation arises with respect to the future as well as the past, and conclude that it results from a disorder of what they term temporality. Four papers focus on false memories in normal ageing. One of the more intriguing findings from recent research is that older adults are sometimes more susceptible to false memories than are younger adults, a finding that is replicated in the normal elderly controls studied by Balota et al. Using a variant of the Roediger and McDermott false recognition procedure that involves repeated study and testing, Kensinger and Schacter report that young adults reduce false recognition with repeated study/test trials whereas older adults do not. They relate this finding to the theoretical distinction between memory for gist and specific information. Jacoby also provides evidence that older adults can be more susceptible to false memories than younger adults, using a novel procedure in which subjects are biased to produce false memories by misinformation that is presented after an initial learning trial. Jacoby discusses his results and related findings in terms of an accessibility bias that influences performance independently of recollective processes. The articles by Kensinger and Schacter and by Jacoby both consider the contribution of impaired recollection of source information to age-related increases in false memories. Mather, Johnson, and De Leonardis extend research on source monitoring and ageing into a novel domain: stereotype reliance. They show that older individuals, who are characterised by poor memory for the exact source of information, tend to rely more on pre-existing stereotypes when trying to remember who said what than do younger adults. They also report evidence that these effects are related to performance 194 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 1999, 16 (3/4/5)

FALSE MEMORIES on tasks sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction. Similarly, Rubin, Van Petten, Glisky, and Newberg found that older adults susceptibility to illusory memory conjunctions, where people falsely recognise as old re-combinations of previously studied items, is related to performance on frontal-sensitive tasks. They also report a separate study using event-related potentials that shows that brain activity during true recognition differs from that observed during false recognition of illusory conjunctions. Dodhia and Metcalfe investigate two types of false memories one involving misleading postevent information (Loftus, Miller, & Burns, 1978), the other involving false recognition of semantic associates in a variant of the Roediger and McDermott procedure in the context of a formal computational model (CHARM). They demonstrate how the model can help to explain some of the effects they observe, but also present data concerning the suppression of false memories that are difficult for the model to handle. In discussing how the model needs to be altered to account for the observed patterns of data, Dodhia and Metcalfe provide a further illustration of how the investigation of false memory phenomena can be revealing at a theoretical level. Taken together, the findings and ideas from the papers in the special issue constitute a compelling case that the study of memory errors, distortions, and illusions from a cognitive neuropsychological perspective can provide useful insights into basic memory processes (for further discussion and illustration, see Schacter, in press). They thus suggest that further development of the cognitive neuropsychology of false memories will be an informative and enlightening enterprise. REFERENCES Alba, J.W., & Hasher, L. (1983). Is memory schematic? Psychological Bulletin, 93, 203 231. Bartlett, F.C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17 22. Estes, W.K. (1997). Processes of memory loss, recovery, and distortion. Psychological Review, 104, 148 169. Johnson, M.K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D.S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3 28. Loftus, E.F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist, 48, 518 537. Loftus, E.F., Miller, D.G., & Burns, H.J. (1978). Integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 19 31. Read, J.D., & Lindsay, D.S. (Eds.) (1997). Recollections of trauma: Scientific research and clinical practice. New York: Plenum Press. Roediger, H.L., III. (1996). Memory illusions. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 76 100. Roediger, H.L., III, & McDermott, K.B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21, 803 814. Schacter, D.L. (1995). Memory distortion: History and current status. In D.L. Schacter (Ed.) Memory distortion: How minds, brains, and societies reconstruct the past. (pp. 1 43). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Schacter, D.L. (in press). The seven sins of memory: Insights from psychology and cognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist. Schacter, D.L., Norman, K.A., & Koutstaal, W. (1998). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 289 318. COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 1999, 16 (3/4/5) 195

Copyright of Cognitive Neuropsychology is the property of Psychology Press (UK) and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.