REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

Similar documents
INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING

Transfer of discriminative control during stimulus fading conducted without reinforcement

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus.

REPEATED MEASUREMENTS OF REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE EFFECTS ON GRADIENTS OF STIMULUS CONTROL' MICHAEL D. ZEILER

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

Instrumental Conditioning I

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS'

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY

Stimulus control of topographically tagged responding

Predictive Accuracy and the Effects of Partial Reinforcement on Serial Autoshaping

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

AUTOSHAPING OF KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

ESTABLISHING FUNCTIONAL CLASSES IN A CHIMPANZEE (PAN TROGLODYTES) WITH A TWO-ITEM SEQUENTIAL-RESPONDING PROCEDURE MASAKI TOMONAGA

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Functionality. A Case For Teaching Functional Skills 4/8/17. Teaching skills that make sense

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues

Computational Versus Associative Models of Simple Conditioning i

postreinforcement pause for a minute or two at the beginning of the session. No reduction

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

between successive DMTS choice phases.

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association

DESCRIPTIVE VERSUS FUNCTIONAL ACCOUNTS OF PSYCHOLOGICAL EVENTS: CYBERRAT AS A CASE IN POINT

that simple discrimination training to compound given a set to react -to one aspect of a stimulus a "set", a discrimination is "pretrained" along

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons

OBSERVING RESPONSES AND SERIAL STIMULI: SEARCHING FOR THE REINFORCING PROPERTIES OF THE S2 ROGELIO ESCOBAR AND CARLOS A. BRUNER

Learning new response sequences

The effects of reinforcement upon the prepecking behaviors of pigeons in the autoshaping experiment.

Some Effects of Discrimination Training on a Line Length Dimension

Effects of a Novel Fentanyl Derivative on Drug Discrimination and Learning in Rhesus Monkeys

SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT'

BACB Fourth Edition Task List Assessment Form

OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS. Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

Resistance to Change Within Heterogeneous Response Sequences

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a

Publications Blough, D. S. Dark adaptation in the pigeon. Doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, Ratliff, F., & Blough, D. S.

Contrast and the justification of effort

Variability as an Operant?

Behavior Shaping. Shaping. Shaping is defined as. Differential Reinforcement. Jesús Rosales-Ruiz UNT. TxABA 2018

Behavioural Processes

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

Eye fixations to figures in a four-choice situation with luminance balanced areas: Evaluating practice effects

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli

A PRACTICAL VARIATION OF A MULTIPLE-SCHEDULE PROCEDURE: BRIEF SCHEDULE-CORRELATED STIMULI JEFFREY H. TIGER GREGORY P. HANLEY KYLIE M.

Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action

Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

GENERALIZATION GRADIENTS AS INDICANTS OF LEARNING AND RETENTION OF A RECOGNITION TASK 1

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2005 November 14.

Schedules of Reinforcement

ANNA STATE HOSPITAL. peated measurement of the attack. A mechanical

Analysis of autoshaping in goldfish

Transfer of Serial Reversal Learning in the Pigeon

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information

Supporting Online Material for

TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES SN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION

1/20/2015. Maximizing Stimulus Control: Best Practice Guidelines for Receptive Language Instruction. Importance of Effective Teaching

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals

Concurrent Identity Training is not Necessary for Associative Symmetry in Successive Matching

MicrOfilms International

Common Coding in Pigeons Assessed Through Partial Versus Total Reversals of Many-to-One Conditional and Simple Discriminations

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, &

J. E. R. STADDON DUKE UNIVERSITY. The relative inability of the usual differential. to ask whether performance under DRL schedules

behavioral contrast and inhibitory stimulus control.

Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio on Pigeons' Choice

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN

EMERGENCE OF COMPLEX CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS BY JOINT CONTROL OF COMPOUND SAMPLES

E. K. SHRIVER CENTER, UNIVERSITY OF MASSACHUSETTS MEDICAL SCHOOL, PROTESTANT GUILD LEARNING CENTER, AND SHRIVER CLINICAL SERVICES CORPORATION

Discrimination Trials to Influence Self-Awareness

Stimulus control of behavior during the postreinforcement pause of FI schedules

NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience

GRAHAM C. L. DAVEY, GARY G. CLELAND, and DAVID A. OAKLEY The City University, London, England

Variability Is an Operant

REINFORCEMENT AT CONSTANT RELATIVE IMMEDIACY OF REINFORCEMENT A THESIS. Presented to. The Faculty of the Division of Graduate. Studies and Research

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2009, 92, NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) AMERICAN UNIVERSITY

Confounding variables in the discriminated Irt procedure.

SERIAL CONDITIONING AS A FUNCTION OF STIMULUS, RESPONSE, AND TEMPORAL DEPENDENCIES

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

Relative numerosity as a dimension of stimulus control: The peak shift

ASSOCIATIVE FACTORS UNDERLYING THE PIGEON'S KEY PECKING IN AUTO-SHAPING PROCEDURES'

Transcription:

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1985, 439 235-241 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES LANNY FIELDS THE COLLEGE OF STATEN ISLAND/CUNY Stimulus control of pigeons' key pecking was transferred from colors to lines by the method of stimulus fading. Fading was conducted with the addition of probes consisting of the line stimuli presented alone at each fading level. Probe responding was used to measure stimulus-control acquisition by the lines. Effects of reinforcement and nonreinforcement of probe responding upon acquisition of stimulus control were assessed using a singleorganism repeated-acquisition design in which three fades were conducted serially. Probe responding was not reinforced in the first and third fade but was in the second. Reinforcement of probe responding substantially reduced the number of fading levels needed to complete fading. The outcome of a control experiment ruled out the possibility of accounting for these results in terms of the specific stimuli used in each fade or in terms of the sequential exposure to the three discriminations. Although probes permitted measurement of stimulus-control acquisition in fading, a measurement/acquisition interaction was also present. Key words: stimulus fading, probe stimuli, measurement/acquisition interaction,. repeated acquisition procedure, reinforcement of probe responding, discrimination learning, stimulus control transfer, key pecking, pigeons Stimulus fading refers to a group of procedures for establishing stimulus control through use of two sets of stimuli: originals that control responding at the beginning of fading and new stimuli that do not. The new stimuli gain control of responding by being presented concurrently with the originals while the originals are gradually attenuated. During this process, the new stimuli frequently gain control of responding without the occurrence of errors. Under these conditions, the course of acquisition cannot be measured because error reduction, the typical means of assessing acquisition of stimulus control, does not occur (Sidman, 1977). To measure that acquisition, fading procedures have been modified by presenting the new stimuli alone at various points This research was supported by PSC/BHE Faculty Research Awards 12232, 1313, and 1349, granted by the City University of New York.The author thanks Joseph F. Dempsky, Carol Tait, and Michael Rivera for their technical assistance in conducting the research and preparing the manuscript. Reprints may be obtained from Lanny Fields, Department of Psychology, The College of Staten Island/CUNY, 13 Stuyvesant Place, Staten Island, New York 131. in fading. These stimuli have been referred to as probes. Thus, responding to the probes has been used to assess the control acquired by the new stimuli (Doran & Holland, 1979; Fields, 1978, 1979; Fields, Bruno, & Keller, 1976; Laar, 1977; Moore & Goldiamond, 1964; Schusterman, 1967; Sidman & Stoddard, 1967; Touchette, 1971). Although probes have been introduced to measure stimulus control, their presentation could also influence acquisition itself. Indeed, this possibility was confirmed when Fields (1979, 198) demonstrated that earlier introduction of probes reduced the number of fading levels needed for new stimuli to acquire control of responding. Another aspect of probe stimuli that might influence acquisition is the contingency of reinforcement that prevails when probes are presented. In some experiments, S+ and S- probe responding have been differentially reinforced (Doran & Holland, 1979; Fields, 198, 1981; Moore & Goldiamond, 1964; Sidman & Stoddard, 1967; Touchette, 1971). Alternatively, S+ and S- probe responding have been unreinforced (Fields, 1978, 1979; Fields et al., 1976; Laar, 235

236 1979). Finally, S+ and S- probe responding have been reinforced nondifferentially (Huguenin & Touchette, 198; Schusterman, 1967). Although different sets of probe contingencies have been used, none of the studies mentioned compared the effects of those contingencies. The present experiment was designed to assess the effects of different probe reinforcement contingencies upon acquisition of stimulus control in fading. This was done using a within-subjects repeated-acquisition technique within the context of a mixed experimental design (Boren & Devine, 1968). Subjects METHOD Sixteen pigeons, naive at the beginning of the experiment, were used as subjects. They were maintained throughout the experiment at 8% of their free-feeding weights. LANNY FIELDS Apparatus Subjects were studied in single-key Scientific Prototype chambers. The operant was defined as depression of the 25-mm diameter response key with a force of at least.2 N. Reinforcement consisted of 2.5-s access to mixed grain. Stimuli transilluminating the response key were generated from an IEE inline projector, and consisted of red and green fields as well as white lines having, 45, 9, or 135-degree angular orientations. The intensities of the colors and lines were controlled by changing the value of precision resistors wired in series with the projector bulbs.thus, ohms corresponded to full intensity, and higher resistance was inversely related to stimulus intensity (Fields, 1978, 1979; Fields et al., 1976; Karpicke & Hearst, 1975; Terrace, 1963). A D.E.C. PDP-8/L minicomputer and Super- SKED software were used to control all experimental contingencies and data recording. Procedure Subjects were trained to discriminate between successively presented full intensity (-ohm) red S+ and green S- stimuli by means of differential autoshaping (Brown & Jenkins, 1968). The two stimuli were presented in pseudorandom order. Once responding occurred on red and not on green, pecking at the red key (S+) was maintained on a variableinterval (VI) 15-s schedule of reinforcement, in which S+ trials lasted from 5 to 3 s and each terminated upon the presentation of the first reinforcer, while S- occasioned extinction for 2-s periods. All stimuli were followed by 1-s intertrial intervals (ITIs) during which the response key was darkened. The experiment was continued until all presentations of S+ terminated with food delivery, and no pecks at S- occurred. Thereafter, full intensity -degree and 9-degree lines were introduced, presented against dark backgrounds. On every third trial, one of the two line probes was presented with a probability of.5 after any color trial in which appropriate responding or nonresponding occurred. If inappropriate responding occurred on a color trial, presentation of the next probe was postponed until the color-trial criterion was satisfied. Each of the lines was presented for an average of 2 s under extinction conditions. Once no responding occurred in the presence of either line for five consecutive presentations, their intensity was reduced to 6 ohms, and each was superimposed on red or green, respectively, forming compound stimuli. The intensity of the lines was then increased in 4-ohm steps until full intensity ( ohms) was reached. Each increment occurred following the completion of at least four reinforcer-terminated S+ trials and two successive S- trials with no responses. Once lines reached full intensity, colors in both compounds were attenuated in small steps following every fourth S+ presentation, on the average, in which at least two reinforcer-terminated S+ trials and at least two consecutive response-free S- trials occurred. Attentuation in 2-ohm steps followed, until responding occurred in the presence of either probe. Thereafter, attenuation progressed in 1-ohm steps. Prior to each attenuation, the S+ line element and the S- line element were presented alone, once each, as probes. Pecking at the probe stimuli was used to assess degree of control acquired by the lines. The procedure stopped once S+ probe responding exceeded 9% of the prevailing compound S+ rate, and no

REINFORCING PROBE RESPONSES IN FADING Table 1 Experimental Conditions and Stimuli Used Line Tilt (degrees) Groups Fade S+ S- Experimental Control 1 9 N N 2 45 135 R N 3 9 45 N N S- probe responding occurred in two consecutive fading levels. After completion of the first fade, the procedure was repeated by reexposing each subject to the original red-green discrimination for one or two sessions. Thereafter, new lines (45 degrees and 135 degrees) were presented as described above until no responding occurred in their presence. They were then superimposed on the red and green stimuli, respectively. Finally, fading was conducted as described above. Once completed, the procedure was repeated a third time but with 9-degree and 45-degree lines superimposed on the red and green stimuli. Thus, three fades (Fl, F2, and F3) were conducted. The stimuli used in each fade and their functions are indicated in Columns 2 and 3 of Table 1. To assess the effects of probe reinforcement contingencies, an experimental group of 8 pigeons was studied in which S+ probe responding was not reinforced in Fades 1 and 3, but was reinforced in Fade 2. Responding during each S+ probe was reinforced using the same contingencies that prevailed in the presence of a compound S+. The effect of reinforcement and nonreinforcement of S+ probe responding was assessed by comparing acquisition of stimulus control across adjacent fades within subjects. Using this procedure, reinforcement and nonreinforcement of S+ probe responding was also correlated with line orientations that were unique to each discrimination. Additionally, the three fades were conducted serially. Because both factors covaried with the reinforcement and nonreinforcement of probe responding, either or both might be responsible for any differences observed across fades in the experimental group. To determine whether they were, the remaining 8 subjects were assigned to a control group for which three fades were conducted; 237 the stimuli used in each were the same as those used in the corresponding fade in the experimental group. In addition, all three fades were conducted with nonreinforcement of probe responding. The reinforcement contingencies that prevailed for S+ probe responding for each fade in each group are summaried in Columns 4 and 5 of Table 1. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The differential effects of nonreinforcement and reinforcement of probe responding can be manifested only after probe responding begins to occur at a level that would produce reinforcers. To assess the effects of reinforcement and nonreinforcement of probe responding, the first fading level at which S+ probe responding would have qualified for reinforcement was identified in each of the three fades. This was accomplished by determining if at least one response occurred after the reinforcer would have become available, whether or not S+ probe responding was slated for reinforcement. The number of fading levels from that point to the completion of a fade was determined and will be referred to as the 'corrected number of fading levels." The effects of reinforcement and nonreinforcement of probe responding upon stimulus control acquisition then were assessed by comparing the number of corrected fading levels needed to complete adjacent fades for each subject in the experimental group. Figure 1 illustrates the number of corrected fading levels needed to complete each discrimination for each subject. Data for subjects in the experimental group are displayed on the left side of the figure. When the first and second fades were compared, fewer corrected fading levels were needed for stimuli to acquire control in the second discrimination than in the first, for 7 of 8 subjects. Likewise, when the second and third fades were compared, 7 of 8 subjects learned the second discrimination in fewer corrected fading levels than the third. The likelihood of at least 7 of 8 subjects learning the second fade faster than the first and third by chance was.35, based upon a binomial test. Using number of trials as a

238 2 - I 4. 2 EXP. 4-222 45 -j 2 \ / 45 224 a/ 4 25 UL. wc: m a w cc LU 4 2 45 so 25 -,. it 8 6 4 2 85 65 25 I ' 6 4 1 6 4 1 o 4 177 6 2-6 - 4. Ṅ R N 41 12 1 8 4 45,9 25 45 CONTROL N N N Fig. 1. Number of corrected fading levels (CFL 1) needed to complete the learning of each discrimination for each subject. Data for subjects in the Experimental group are plotted on the left; those for Control group subjects are plotted on the right. N = nonreinforced S+ probe responding; R = reinforced S+ probe responding. LANNY FIELDS dependent variable yields equivalent results. In addition, although the average difference between number of corrected levels needed to learn adjacent discriminations was statistically significant for Fades 1 and 2, and 3, there was no significant difference when the first and third discriminations were compared. The significance of these differences, determined at p <.5 using Welsch's (1977) test for pairwise comparisons, is summaried in Table 2. The statistical analyses of the within-subject comparisons just presented show that reinforcement of S+ probe responding accelerated stimulus-control acquisition in fading when compared to nonreinforcement of S+ probe responding. Such statistical analyses, however, do not specify the magnitude of the effect. That was assessed by determining the number of corrected fading levels needed to complete the second fade relative to adjacent fades. To compare the first two fades, the corrected number of levels needed to complete the first fade (CFL 1) was subtracted from the corrected number of levels to complete the second fade (CFL 2), and the difference was divided by the number of corrected levels to complete the first fade (CFL 1): (CFL 2 - CFL 1)/CFL 1. To compare the second and third fade, the corrected number of fading levels to complete the third fade (CFL 3) was subtracted from the corrected number of fading levels needed to complete the second (CFL 2), and the difference was divided by the corrected number of fading levels needed to complete the third fade (CFL 3): (CFL 2 - CFL 3)/CFL 3. Both proportions had the same characteristics. When the same number of corrected fading levels were needed to complete adjacent fades, the proportions equaled ero. When fewer corrected fading levels were needed in the second fade than in adjacent fades, the proportions were negative. When more corrected fading levels occurred in the second fade than in the adjacent fades, the proportions were positive. As the relative difference in number of levels needed to complete adjacent fades increased, the absolute value of the proportions increased. Figure 2 shows how reinforcement of probe responding enhanced learning for each subject in the experimental group. Reinforce-

REINFORCING PROBE RESPONSES IN FADING 239 Table 2 Statistical analysis of the differences in the average number of corrected fading levels for pairs of fades, using Welsch's test for pairwise comparisons. Experimntal Group CFL 2= CFL 1= CFL 3= Critical 15.875 33. 34.875 Value P< CFL 2= 15.875 17.125* 19.* 16.543.5 CFL 1 = 33. 1.875 12.438.5 Control Group CFL 2= CFL 1= CFL 3= Critical 38.875 4.375 48.625 Value P< CFL 2= 38.875 1.5 9.75 38.955.5 CFL 1= 4.375 8.25 29.289.5 * = statistically significant differences; CFL i= average number of corrected fading levels for the i-th fade. ment of probe responding reduced the corrected number of fading levels by an average of 52% when comparing the first and second discriminations, and by an average of 46% when comparing the second and third fades. Subjects in the control group were studied to determine how stimulus-control acquisition would vary across three fades in which S+ probe responding was never reinforced. Results obtained from these subjects were presented in the right hand column of Figure 1. A comparison of the second discrimination and the first revealed that 3 of 8 subjects learned the second in fewer corrected fading levels than the first. The likelihood of at least 3 of 8 subjects learning the second discrimination faster than the first by chance, was equal to.86 using a binomial test. When the second and third discriminations were compared, 5 of 8 subjects learned the second in fewer corrected fading levels than the third. The likelihood of at least 5 of 8 subjects learning the second discrimination faster than the third by chance, was equal to.36 using the binomial test. In addition, the differences in the number of corrected fading levels needed to learn the discriminations in Fades 1 and 2, Fades 2 and 3, and Fades 1 and 3 were not statistically significant, using Welsch's test for pairwise comparisons (see Table 2). To summarie, when fading was conducted with no reinforcement of S+ probe responding, there were no systematic differences in the number of corrected fading levels needed by the new stimuli to acquire control in each of the three discriminations, learned serially. Finally, when viewed in terms of the overall rate of stimulus-control acquisition, the results were entirely comparable to the acquisition relationships characteried above. Fields (1978, 1979, 1981) demonstrated that some temporal parameters of probe stimuli influenced acquisition of stimulus control in fading procedures. The present experiment demonstrated that the parameter of reinforcement present during the probe stimuli also influenced acquisition. Thus, the probes used to measure stimulus-control acquisition in fading also influenced the process they were used to measure. To conclude that their use represents a critical shortcoming, however, is neither ineluctable nor appropriate. If stimulus-control acquisition is to be measured in fading, inclusion of probes would appear to be a necessity because other measurement alternatives are not now available. Since various parameters of probe stimuli that produce systematic changes in acquisition of stimulus control in fading have been identified, the influence of probes upon acquisition is measurable and controllable. Rather than viewing probes as being problematic, then, it would be more appropriate to acknowledge the apparently inevitable role they play in the measurement process, and to conceptualie the parameters of the probe stimuli as determinants of acquisition of stimulus control. Indeed, the routine inclusion of probes would provide far more information about such acquisition than is currently available (Rilling, 1977), and might also

24 LANNY FIELDS -J uj.5 FADE 2 & FADE 1 -. (i -.5 CM U. U) n -1. IL 1U 8 FADE 2 & FADE 3 IL UI. LU. Lu -.5-1. 16 18 22 24 48 5 17 49 SUBJECTS Fig. 2. Relative difference in the number of corrected fading levels (CFL 1) needed to learn adjacent discriminations in the Experimental group.the upper graph shows a comparison of Fades 1 and 2, computed with (CFL 2 - CFL 1)/CFL 2. The lower graph shows a comparison of Fades 2 and 3, computed with (CFL 2 - CFL 3)/CFL 3. permit a systematic integration of the effects of traditional discrimination training and stimulus-fading procedures upon discrimination learning. REFERENCES Boren, J. J., & Devine, D. D. (1968). The repeated acquisition of behavioral chains. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 651-66. Brown, P. L., & Jenkins, H. M. (1968). Autoshaping of the pigeon's key-peck. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 1-8. Doran, J., & Holland, J. G. (1979). Control by stimulus features during fading. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 31, 177-187. Fields, L. (1978). Fading and errorless transfer in successive discriminations. Journal of the Experimrental Analysis of Behavior, 3, 123-128. Fields, L. (1979). Acquisition of stimulus control while introducing new stimuli in fading. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 32, 12 1-127. Fields, L. (198). Enhanced learning of new discriminations after stimulus fading. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 15, 327-33. Fields, L. (1981). Early and late introduction of probes and stimulus control acquisition in fading. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36, 363-37. Fields, L., Bruno, V., & Keller, K. (1976). The stages of acquisition in stimulus fading. Journal ofthe Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 26, 295-3. Huguenin, N. H., & Touchette, P. E. (198). Visual attention in retarded adults: Combining stimuli which control incompatible behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 33, 77-86. Karpicke, J., & Hearst, E. (1975). Inhibitory control and errorless discrimination learning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 23, 159-166. Laar, R. (1977). Extending sequence-class membership with matching to sample. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 381-392. Moore, R., & Goldiamond, I. (1964). Errorless establishment of visual discrimination using fading procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 269-272. Rilling, M. (1977). Stimulus control and inhibitory processes. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 432-48). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Schusterman, R. J. (1967). Attention shift and errorless reversal learning by the California sea lion. Science, 156, 833-835. Sidman, M. (1977). Remarks. Behaviorism, 5(1), 111-113. Sidman, M., & Stoddard, L. T. (1967). The effectiveness of fading in programming a simultaneous form discrimination for retarded children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1, 3-15. Skinner, B. F. (1968). The technology of teaching. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Terrace, H. S. (1963). Errorless transfer of a discrimination across two continua. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 223-232. Touchette, P. E. (1971). Transfer of stimulus control: Measuring the moment of transfer. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 15, 347-354.

REINFORCING PROBE RESPONSES IN FADING 241 Welsch, R. E. (1977). Tables for stepwise multiple comparison procedures (pp. 949-977). Working Paper, Sloan School of Management, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. ReceivedJune 8, 1981 Revision received May 3, 1984 Final acceptance December 24, 1984