CHAPTER. The Self as a Point of Contact Between Social Psychology and Motivation. Constantine Sedikides Aiden P. Gregg University of Southampton

Similar documents
The Self as a Point of Contact between Social Psychology and Motivation. Constantine Sedikides. Aiden P. Gregg. University of Southampton

Is Implicit Self-Esteem Really Unconscious?: Implicit Self-Esteem Eludes Conscious Reflection

The Implicit Self. Rutgers University, Piscataway, New Jersey, USA. University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada

THE INFLUENCE OF UNCONSCIOUS NEEDS ON COLLEGE PROGRAM CHOICE

Optimistic Versus Pessimistic Life-Orientation Beliefs among University Teachers

Pride. Jessica L. Tracy. University of British Columbia

A Discussion of the Incongruence Between Optimism and Academic Performance and Its Influence on School Violence

University of Groningen. Mastering (with) a handicap Kunnen, Elske

Implicit Attitude. Brian A. Nosek. University of Virginia. Mahzarin R. Banaji. Harvard University

Self-enhancement: What, where, and why. Lindsay Thibodeau. Algoma University College

Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology The Graduate Center of the City University of New York

Internalized Motivation in the Classroom

Request for Proposals: Junior Investigators in Genetics and Human Agency Letters of Intent Due March 1, 2016

Definitions of Nature of Science and Scientific Inquiry that Guide Project ICAN: A Cheat Sheet

Motivation: Internalized Motivation in the Classroom 155

The more like me, the better : Individual differences in social desirability ratings of personality items

Measuring and Assessing Study Quality

Identity Development. Kate C. McLean Western Washington University. Moin Syed University of Minnesota

Self-Handicapping Variables and Students' Performance

Mnemic neglect and self-threat: Trait modifiability moderates self-protection

Conclusion. The international conflicts related to identity issues are a contemporary concern of societies

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Pearson Education Limited Edinburgh Gate Harlow Essex CM20 2JE England and Associated Companies throughout the world

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2)

Slide 1. Psychology 386b Fundamentals of Cognitive Science. Slide 2 Cognitive Science?

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations

Contextual influences on student motivation in the first year of middle school. Caroline Mansfield. (Ph.D student)

Chapter 3 Perceiving Ourselves and Others in Organizations

Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments

THEORIES IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY PSY834, Fall 2018 Thursdays, 9:10-12:00 210A Berkey Hall

Professor Tatiana Bachkirova Congress of Coaching Psychology University of Aalborg 14 September 2018

Values, Self-Esteem and Ethics

Influence of Mental States on Reflexive Processes in Academic Activity

JOURNAL OF INTERNATIONAL ACADEMIC RESEARCH FOR MULTIDISCIPLINARY Impact Factor 3.114, ISSN: , Volume 5, Issue 4, May 2017

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

THE UNDESIRED SELF AND EMOTIONAL EXPERIENCE: A LATENT VARIABLE ANALYSIS

Do Attitudes Affect Memory? Tests of the Congeniality Hypothesis

DOWNLOAD OR READ : PHENOMENOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY THEORY RESEARCH AND METHOD PDF EBOOK EPUB MOBI

SOCI 323 Social Psychology

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

relationships (i.e., attribution schemal). The I-E Scale Peterborough, Ontario, Canada ROTTER S (1968) Internal-External Locus of Control (I-E) Scale

The Insatiable Quest for Self-Worth

Motivational Affordances: Fundamental Reasons for ICT Design and Use

Locus of Control: Influence of Internality, Externality- Others, Externality-Chance among Management Students

The Rationality of Perception rewrites perception s rational role and its rational standing. This

The Good, the Bad and the Blameworthy: Understanding the Role of Evaluative Reasoning in Folk Psychology. Princeton University

Assessment of Explicit and Implicit Self Esteem among Students of Different Social Categories

both their identity and how it slots into society, ultimately feeling like you serve a purpose. According to Miller and Moran ( 2012 p:18) self concep

INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 5. CLINICAL APPROACH TO INTERVIEWING PART 1

A Study of Life Satisfaction and Optimism in Relation to Psychological Well-Being Among Working and Non Working Women

The Components of Mental Health and Test Anxiety in Talented and Normal Schools Students

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK

Defining Psychology Behaviorism: Social Psychology: Milgram s Obedience Studies Bystander Non-intervention Cognitive Psychology:

Models of Information Retrieval

Activity 19 Great Ideas in the History of Psychology. Purpose

1.1 FEATURES OF THOUGHT

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE COLLEGE AMONG TEACHERS IN COIMBATORE

The Association between Free Will Beliefs and Stereotypes: People's Belief in Fatalism Promotes Gender Stereotypes

Chapter 1 Applications and Consequences of Psychological Testing

Stephen Madigan PhD madigan.ca Vancouver School for Narrative Therapy

AUGMENTATION AND DISCOUNTING IN IMPRESSIONS OF TARGETS DESCRIBED BY THIRD PARTIES WITH ULTERIOR MOTIVES

The Yin and Yang of OD

PSYCHOLOGY (413) Chairperson: Sharon Claffey, Ph.D.

Self enhancement and voting intentions: A proposal for questions to be included in the

Motivational Research

Book Information Jakob Hohwy, The Predictive Mind, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2013, ix+288, 60.00,

Implicit motives and hemispheric processing differences are critical for understanding. personality disorders: A Commentary on Hopwood

SITUATIONAL STRUCTURE AND INDIVIDUAL SELF-ESTEEM AS DETERMINANTS OF THREAT-ORIENTED REACTIONS TO POWER. Arthur R» Cohen

CASE SEMINAR: THE DEVELOPMENTAL LENS AND ADULT ANALYTIC WORK. (one credit) Kate Oram, Ph.D.

B849:C91. gender stereotype. gender counter-stereotype ~461 Advances in Psychological Science

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

The Attribute Index - Leadership

Organizational Behaviour

FEEDBACK TUTORIAL LETTER

VISUAL ANALYSIS AS A QUALITATIVE RESEARCH CONCEPT

Impression Management in the workplace individual and organizational influences

Culture, Self-construal and Social Cognition: Evidence from Cross-Cultural and Priming Studies

AU TQF 2 Doctoral Degree. Course Description

UC Davis UC Davis Previously Published Works

ʻThe concept of Deaf identity in Sloveniaʼ

Nursing Research Series. Nursing Research Series Essentials of Science: Methods, Appraisal and Utilization

Running Head: COGNITIVE VULNERABILITY AND ATTACHMENT. Cognitive Vulnerability and Attachment. Nathan L. Williams University of Arkansas

Impression order effects as a function of the personal relevance of the object of description*

Unconscious motivation

Colorado Resilience Collaborative: Research for Preventing Identity-Based Violence

Unconscious motivation

Social Change in the 21st Century

Leadership Beyond Reason

Investigating the Appraisal Patterns of Regret and Disappointment 1

COURSE DESCRIPTIONS 科目簡介

Chapter 4: The self. What s it about?

8/17/2012. Self-Concept Video. Cultural Differences in Defining the Self. Chapter 5. The Self: Understanding Ourselves in a Social Context

Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing. Example 1: Compare and contrast

Chapter 3: Perception and the Self in IPC 01/24/2012

Social Fitness Training: Integrated Short-Term Treatment for Chronic Shyness

Research on happiness and economics

Positive Mood and Future-Oriented Self-Evaluation

Post-Hoc Rationalism in Science. Eric Luis Uhlmann. HEC Paris. (Commentary on Jones and Love, "Bayesian Fundamentalism or Enlightenment?

How has attribution theory been studied in the past? How might it be studied in the future? Psychology 1

Transcription:

CHAPTER 34 The Self as a Point of Contact Between Social Psychology and Motivation Constantine Sedikides Aiden P. Gregg University of Southampton I nterdisciplinarity is a mixed blessing. An attempted union between two disciplines can cook up a couscous of conflicting assumptions and theories, misaligned methodological and data analytic strategies, and disconcertingly divergent validation philosophies. The chimerical offspring of this union will be amorphous, uncompelling, and unusable. Yet sometimes such a union can turn out to be a marriage made in heaven. The union of social psychology and motivation is a case in point, with the self as master of ceremonies. The self acts as a methodological point of contact between contemporary social psychological and traditional motivational approaches. The former approach is nomothetic, experimental, and laboratory-based, whereas the latter is idiographic, naturalistic, and questionnaire-based. More importantly, the self acts as a theoretical point of contact between the two approaches. The role f the self as the facilitator of this doubly harmonious union is illustrated though a brief exposition to the self-evaluation literature. SELF-EVALUATION From Needs to Self-Evaluation Motives. Classic motivation research has graced the field of social psychology with such constructs as the need for uncertainty reduction (Weineretal., 1971), the need for control (Rotter, 1966), the 233

2 3 4 s^sj SEDIKIDES AND GREGG need to experience the self as an origin of action (decharms, 1968), and the need for self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965). These needs have been investigated by motivation researchers with the aid of targeted questionnaires. Their theoretical and empirical forays have had a profound impact on the self-evaluation literature, particularly in regard to the interplay between the individual and the social context. For example, what do people want to know about themselves? What kind of information are they most likely to solicit? What sort of feedback will they accept from others and how will they treat the person supplying it? How will they process and remember that feedback? Will that feedback influence their goal-setting and behavior? Inspired by early motivation research on human needs, social psychologists set about addressing these questions in a generative manner. In the process, the concept of needs gave way to the concept of self-evaluation motives. Such motives were assumed to influence the way in which self-relevant information is selected, processed, remembered, or acted upon. Thus, the need for uncertainty reduction was reconceptualized in terms of the self-assessment motive; the need for control, in terms of the self-verification motive; the need to experience the self as an origin of action, in terms of the self-improvement motive; and the need for self-esteem, in terms of the self-enhancement motive (Sedikides & Strube, 1997). Importantly, these reconceptualizations were complementary rather than antagonistic: The motives were assumed to result in need satisfaction. More specifically, the self-assessment motive was proposed to guide the processing of self-relevant information so that the content of the self-concept (e.g., memories, judgments, behavioral sequelae) would be more accurate when judged by consensual or objective standards (Trope, 1986); the self-verification motive was proposed to direct self-relevant information processing in favor of the confirmation and validation of existing self-beliefs (Swann, 1983); the self-improvement motive was proposed to steer self-relevant information processing so as to raise levels of ability or performance and maximize potential for personality growth (Taylor, Neter, & Wayment, 1995); and the self-enhancement motive was proposed to tilt self-relevant information processing in favor of a positive self-concept (Brown & Dutton, 1995). Social psychology's appropriation of these theoretical constructs was a breath of fresh air. The motives were construed as dynamic, intraindividual variables that could readily lend themselves to laboratory experimentation. Accordingly, three research vistas opened up, each revolving around a key question. First, is each motive influential in its own right? This question was answered in the affirmative: Each of the four motives was shown to affect selfrelevant information processing in substantive ways. Second, although the motives could and did coexist, what happened when they are in an antagonistic relation? In several programs of research, one motive was pitted against the other. Although we do not claim to be utterly dispassionate reviewers of the available literature (no doubt, being swayed by self-motives of our own!), we believe that the available evidence suggests that, all things considered, the

34. THE SELF AS A POINT OF CONTACT * ^ 235 self-enhancement motive is preeminent (Sedikides & Gregg, 2003). Third, and perhaps most importantly, what are the circumstances under which one motive prevails over another? A plethora of research has addressed this question. It has been shown, for example, that despite the general primacy of self-enhancement, (a) self-assessment prevails over self-enhancement when the self is evaluated on well-defined and verifiable attributes as opposed to ill-defined and unverifiable ones, (b) self-improvement prevails over self-enhancement on personality dimensions viewed as modifiable as opposed to fixed, and (c) self-verification prevails over self-enhancement when cognitive resources are plentiful as opposed to scarce (Baumeister, 1998; Sedikides & Strube, 1997). FROM APPROACH/AVOIDANCE TO SELF-ENHANCEMENT/SELF-PROTECTION The uxorious union of classic motivation and contemporary self-evaluation approaches has given birth to another bouncing baby. Classic motivation research introduced the idea of the approach/avoidance dimension, inspired by the fact that behavior in achievement settings is oriented either toward the pursuit of success (approach) or the flight from failure (avoidance) (McClelland, Atkinson, Clark, & Lowell, 1953; Murray, 1938). Modern selfevaluation research has reconceptualized this dimension in terms of self-promotion versus self-protection, the yin and yang of self-enhancement. The former refers to the proactive attempt to positively affirm the self, and the latter to the defensive attempt to prevent devaluation of the self. This reconceptualization provided impetus for investigating the self-protection motive. An impressive amount of research has now documented that humans are motivated to protect the self against threat. Humans treat the self as priceless possession, guard it vigilantly, protect it with zeal and vigor. For example, they deny their shortcomings, displace blame for their failures, react angrily to unfavorable feedback and recruit compensatory qualities to offset it, change the meaning of their negative self-aspects to give them a more positive spin, and strategically compare themselves to less fortunate others (Sedikides & Gregg, 2003). In fact, humans are averse to negative self-relevant feedback even when it is accurate (Sedikides, 1993), and remember such feedback poorly (Sedikides & Green, 2000) even when the feedback is consistent with their negative characteristics. That is, in direct tests, the self-protection motive has trumped the self-assessment and self-verification motives. Classic motivation research has concerned itself with the relative potency f the approach versus avoidance motive, treated as an individual-difference variable (McClelland et al., 1953). In contrast, self-evaluation research has conceptualized the self-promotion and self-protection motives as intraindiv 'dual variables (but also as an individual-difference variable; Tice, 1991) while asking the same question about relative potency. The ensuing research revealed that self-protection is, on the whole, more powerful than self-pro-

2 3 6 " ^ SEDIKIDES AND GREGG motion: Humans are more strongly motivated to protect the self against threatening feedback than to use feedback to boost the positivity of the self (Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Finkenauer, & Vohs, 2001). FROM EXPLICIT TO IMPLICIT SELF-EVALUATION Classic motivation research has distinguished between implicit and explicit motives (McClelland etal., 1953; McClelland, Koestner, & Weinberger, 1989). Explicit motives were considered cognitive representations of one's values, developed through verbally transmitted socialization experiences, and predictive of deliberate choices. Implicit motives, on the other hand, were thought to be affective associative structures, developed through preverbal socialization experiences, and predictive of spontaneous or habitual responses. This distinction has once again been transmuted in self-evaluation research. Explicit self-evaluation motives are accessible to conscious awareness and are assessed through questionnaires. However, implicit motives are inaccessible to conscious awareness and are assessed through indirect or unobtrusive tests such as the Implicit Association Test (indexing automatic associations between self and valence; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) or the Name-Letter Effect (estimating participants' preference for letters in their own name; Nuttin, 1985). Implicit measures are credited for some of the most exciting, if controversial, developments in self-evaluation research. For example, use of these measures has documented the universalism of self-enhancement: This motive is equally prevalent in Western and Eastern culture (Hetts, Sakuma, & Pelham, 1999). CONCLUSIONS We began this chapter by claiming that the union of two disciplines is not necessarily a happy one. In fact, we are prone to believe that interdisciplinarity, like marriage, has only a 50% chance of succeeding. We have chosen, however, to tell a success story: the influence of classic motivation research on contemporary self-evaluation research. We highlighted three domains of such influence. The first involves the reconceptualization of crucial human needs in terms of self-evaluation motives. The second involves the reconceptualization of the approach/avoidance dimension in terms of the self-promotion/self-protection dimension. The final one involves the reconceptualization of implicit versus explicit motives in terms of implicit versus explicit self-evaluations. In all three domains, substantial theoretical developments have resulted in methodological innovation, just as happy marriages produce healthy children. Of course, these three domains are not the only instances of interdisciplinary fertilization. The field of self-evaluation has been influenced by philosophical theorizing, and advances in cognitive, developmental and health psychology, as well as trends in personality and cultural psychology (Sedikides

34. THE SELF AS A POINT OF CONTACT <**& 2 3 7 & Gregg, 2003; Sedikides & Strube, 1997). Nevertheless, we believe that the interdisciplinary success of classic motivation and self-evaluation research can serve as a concrete example for additional bridge-building between motivation and social psychology. Also, we hope that this scholarly influence will increasingly become bidirectional in nature, as the maturing of self-evaluation research will have theoretical and methodological implications for motivation research. REFERENCES Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 680-740). New York: Oxford University Press. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323-370. Brown, J. D., & Dutton, K. A. (1995). Truth and consequences: The cost and benefits of accurate self-knowledge. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1288-1296. decharms, R. (1968). Personal causation: The internal affective determinants of behavior. New York: Academic Press. Greenwald, A. G.,McGhee,D.E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,1464-1480. Hetts.J.J., Sakuma, M., & Pelham, B. W. (1999). Two roads to positive self-regard: Implicit and explicit self-evaluation and culture. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35,512-559. McClelland, D. C, Atkinson, J.W., Clark, R. A., & Lowell, E. L. (1953). The achievement motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. McClelland, D. C, Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ? Psychological Review, 96, 690-702. Murray, H. (1938). Explorations in personality. New York: Oxford University Press. Nuttin, J. M. (1985). Narcissism beyond gestalt and awareness: The name letter effect. European Journal ofsocial Psychology, 15, 353-361. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80(1, Whole No. 609). Sedikides, C. (1993). Assessment, enhancement, and verification determinants of the self-evaluation process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 317-338. Sedikides, C, & Green, J. D. (2000). On the self-protective nature of inconsistency/negativity management: Using the person memory paradigm to examine self-referent memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 906-922. Sedikides, C, & Gregg, A. P. (2003). Portraits of the self. In M. A. Hogg &J. Cooper (Eds.), Sage handbook of social psychology (pp. 110-138). London: Sage. Sedikides, C, & Strube, M. J. (1997). Self-evaluation: To thine own self be good, to thine own self be sure, to thine own self be true, and to thine own self be better. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 209-269). New York: Academic Press.

238 SEDIKIDES AND GREGG Swann, W. B.,Jr. (1983). Self-verification: Bringing social reality into harmony with the self. In J. Suls & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on the se (Vol. 2, pp. 33-66). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Taylor, S. E., Neter, E., & Wayment, H. A. (1995). Self-evaluation processes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1278-1287. Tice, D. M. (1991). Esteem protection or enhancement? Self-handicapping motives and attributions differ by trait self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 711-725. Trope, Y. (1986). Self-enhancement and self-assessment in achievement behaviour. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 1, pp. 350-378). New York: Guilford Press. Weiner, B., Frieze, I., Kukla, A., Read, C, Rest, S., & Rosenbaum, R. M. (1971). Perceiving the causes of success and failure. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.