Auditory nerve model for predicting performance limits of normal and impaired listeners

Similar documents
I. INTRODUCTION. for Sensory Research, 621 Skytop Road, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY

Representation of sound in the auditory nerve

The Structure and Function of the Auditory Nerve

Acoustics, signals & systems for audiology. Psychoacoustics of hearing impairment

A truly remarkable aspect of human hearing is the vast

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA)

Chapter 40 Effects of Peripheral Tuning on the Auditory Nerve s Representation of Speech Envelope and Temporal Fine Structure Cues

A computer model of medial efferent suppression in the mammalian auditory system

Stimulus Coding in the Auditory Nerve. Neural Coding and Perception of Sound 1

Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Week 5. The peripheral auditory system: The ear as a signal processor

I. INTRODUCTION. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 111 (1), Pt. 1, Jan /2002/111(1)/271/14/$ Acoustical Society of America

Auditory nerve. Amanda M. Lauer, Ph.D. Dept. of Otolaryngology-HNS

Lecture 3: Perception

Auditory Phase Opponency: A Temporal Model for Masked Detection at Low Frequencies

Modeling auditory-nerve responses for high sound pressure levels in the normal and impaired auditory periphery

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Pitch & Binaural listening

Cochlear anatomy, function and pathology II. Professor Dave Furness Keele University

HST 721 Efferent Control Lecture October 2004

Temporal Adaptation. In a Silicon Auditory Nerve. John Lazzaro. CS Division UC Berkeley 571 Evans Hall Berkeley, CA

9/29/14. Amanda M. Lauer, Dept. of Otolaryngology- HNS. From Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics, Green & Swets (1966)

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

Healthy Organ of Corti. Loss of OHCs. How to use and interpret the TEN(HL) test for diagnosis of Dead Regions in the cochlea

Perceptual pitch shift for sounds with similar waveform autocorrelation

Linguistic Phonetics. Basic Audition. Diagram of the inner ear removed due to copyright restrictions.

Issues faced by people with a Sensorineural Hearing Loss

Acoustics Research Institute

Processing in The Cochlear Nucleus

Christopher J. Plack Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Wivenhoe Park, Colchester CO4 3SQ, England

Lauer et al Olivocochlear efferents. Amanda M. Lauer, Ph.D. Dept. of Otolaryngology-HNS

JARO. Estimates of Human Cochlear Tuning at Low Levels Using Forward and Simultaneous Masking ANDREW J. OXENHAM, 1 AND CHRISTOPHER A.

Level dependence of auditory filters in nonsimultaneous masking as a function of frequency

Evaluating Adaptation and Olivocochlear Efferent Feedback as Potential Explanations of Psychophysical Overshoot

ClaroTM Digital Perception ProcessingTM

Frequency refers to how often something happens. Period refers to the time it takes something to happen.

Effects of Remaining Hair Cells on Cochlear Implant Function

Linguistic Phonetics Fall 2005

Pitfalls in behavioral estimates of basilar-membrane compression in humans a)

Can components in distortion-product otoacoustic emissions be separated?

Coding of Sounds in the Auditory System and Its Relevance to Signal Processing and Coding in Cochlear Implants

Spectrograms (revisited)

An active cochlear model showing sharp tuning and high sensitivity

Biological Basis of Hearing-Aid Design

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

The mammalian cochlea possesses two classes of afferent neurons and two classes of efferent neurons.

A cochlear AGC model, proposing a new type of cochlear non-linearity

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Otoacoustic Estimates of Cochlear Tuning: Testing Predictions in Macaque

Physiological assessment of contrast-enhancing frequency shaping and multiband compression in hearing aids

Brad May, PhD Johns Hopkins University

Half-Octave Shift in Mammalian Hearing Is an Epiphenomenon of the Cochlear Amplifier

Christopher J. Plack Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, E. Sussex BN1 9QG, England

David A. Nelson. Anna C. Schroder. and. Magdalena Wojtczak

Mechanical Properties of the Cochlea. Reading: Yost Ch. 7

Estimates of compression at low and high frequencies using masking additivity in normal and impaired ears

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency

J Jeffress model, 3, 66ff

Synaptopathy Research Uwe Andreas Hermann

64 IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON BIOMEDICAL ENGINEERING, VOL. 61, NO. 1, JANUARY 2014

Noise Induced Hearing Loss

HST 721 Lecture 4: Mechanics, electromotility and the cochlear amplifier

Christopher J. Plack and Ray Meddis Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Wivenhoe Park, Colchester CO4 3SQ, United Kingdom

Cochlear compression in listeners with moderate sensorineural hearing loss

Springer. Springer Handbook of Auditory Research. Series Editors: Richard R. Fay and Arthur N. Popper

EEL 6586, Project - Hearing Aids algorithms

Effects of background noise level on behavioral estimates of basilar-membrane compression

sensitivity (db) frequency (khz) I: MONGOLIAN GERBIL FREQUENCY HEARING CHARACTERISTICS

ABSTRACT EQUALIZING NOISE (TEN) TEST. Christine Gmitter Doctor of Audiology, 2008

Precursor effects on behavioral estimates of frequency selectivity and gain in forward masking

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Innervation of the Cochlea. Reading: Yost Ch. 8

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency. (Some slides are adapted from Zhiyao Duan s course slides on Computer Audition and Its Applications in Music)

Trajectory of the Aging Cochlea

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System

Neural correlates of the perception of sound source separation

Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception

Processing of sounds in the inner ear

functions grow at a higher rate than in normal{hearing subjects. In this chapter, the correlation

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Improving the diagnostic power of otoacoustic emissions. Arturo Moleti Physics Department University of Roma Tor Vergata

Isolating mechanisms that influence measures of the precedence effect: Theoretical predictions and behavioral tests

Hearing Lectures. Acoustics of Speech and Hearing. Auditory Lighthouse. Facts about Timbre. Analysis of Complex Sounds

I. INTRODUCTION. A. Extracellular physiological responses to AM stimuli.

How is the stimulus represented in the nervous system?

Detection of high-frequency spectral notches as a function of level a)

Theme 2: Cellular mechanisms in the Cochlear Nucleus

Required Slide. Session Objectives

Modeling auditory evoked brainstem responses to transient stimuli. Filip Munch Rønne and Torsten Dau. Centre for Applied Hearing Research,

Basilar membrane nonlinearity determines auditory nerve rate-intensity functions and cochlear dynamic range

Binaurally-coherent jitter improves neural and perceptual ITD sensitivity in normal and electric hearing

Frequency tuning of the contralateral medial olivocochlear reflex in humans

SOLUTIONS Homework #3. Introduction to Engineering in Medicine and Biology ECEN 1001 Due Tues. 9/30/03

Deafness and hearing impairment

An Auditory-Model-Based Electrical Stimulation Strategy Incorporating Tonal Information for Cochlear Implant

FREQUENCY COMPRESSION AND FREQUENCY SHIFTING FOR THE HEARING IMPAIRED

Auditory Physiology PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 30. Organ of Corti

Topics in Linguistic Theory: Laboratory Phonology Spring 2007

Physiological measures of the precedence effect and spatial release from masking in the cat inferior colliculus.

Thresholds for different mammals

Tuning curves and pitch matches in a listener with a unilateral, low-frequency hearing loss Florentine, Mary; Houtsma, A.J.M.

A variant temporal-masking-curve method for inferring peripheral auditory compression a)

Transcription:

Heinz et al.: Acoustics Research Letters Online [DOI 1.1121/1.1387155] Published Online 12 June 21 Auditory nerve model for predicting performance limits of normal and impaired listeners Michael G. Heinz Speech and Hearing Sciences Program, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts 2139 and Hearing Research Center, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts 2215 mgheinz@alum.mit.edu Xuedong Zhang Hearing Research Center and Biomedical Engineering, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts 2215 zxd@bu.edu Ian C. Bruce Department of Biomedical Engineering, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland 2125 ibruce@bme.jhu.edu Laurel H. Carney Hearing Research Center and Biomedical Engineering, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts 2215 carney@bu.edu Abstract: A computational auditory nerve (AN) model was developed for use in modeling psychophysical experiments with normal and impaired human listeners. In this phenomenological model, many physiologically vulnerable response properties associated with the cochlear amplifier are represented by a single nonlinear control mechanism, including the effects of level-dependent tuning, compression, level-dependent phase, suppression, and fast nonlinear dynamics on the responses of high, medium, and low spontaneous-rate (SR) AN fibers. Several model versions are described that can be used to evaluate the relative effects of these nonlinear properties. c 21 Acoustical Society of America PACS numbers: 43.64Bt Date Received: February 9, 21 Date Accepted: May 19, 21 1. Introduction Zhang et al. 1 have developed a physiologically based auditory nerve (AN) model (Fig. 1) that uses a nonlinear control path to account for broadened tuning with increases in level, including the associated compressive-magnitude and nonlinear-phase responses, suppression, and the fast dynamics of these properties. The use of a single control path to account for all of these physiologically vulnerable nonlinear response properties represents the finding that these properties are concomitantly absent in many forms of sensorineural hearing loss that are thought to be related to outer hair cell (OHC) dysfunction. 2 This phenomenological implementation was used here to develop an AN model for evaluating normal and impaired human psychophysical performance. The present model includes five versions, differing in the control-path implementation, which allow evaluation of the relative importance of sensitivity, spectral resolution, magnitude compression and level-dependent phase responses, and suppression. The present model has a modified description of frequency resolution as a function of characteristic frequency (CF: most sensitive frequency of an AN fiber) that is derived from estimates of human rather than cat tuning. The present model extends the Zhang et al. 1 model by including a simple implementation of all three spontaneous-rate (SR) groups of AN fibers. 3 Low SR (LSR), high threshold fibers are important for psychophysical predictions because of their potential role in encoding high sound levels, 4,5 for which the majority of AN fibers are saturated. 91 ARLO 2(3), July 21 1529-7853/1/2(3)/91/6/$18. (c)21 Acoustical Society of America 91

Heinz et al.: Acoustics Research Letters Online [DOI 1.1121/1.1387155] Published Online 12 June 21 Input: P(t) Signal Path Time-Varying Narrowband Filter Acoustic Stimulus (in Pascals) K Nonlinear Wideband Filter Control Path Output: r(t) Inner Hair Cell and Synapse τ(t) Outer Hair Cells Time-Varying Discharge Rate (in spikes/sec) Fig. 1. Schematic block diagram of the phenomenological nonlinear auditory-nerve model for a single CF. Finally, the present model is specifically designed to describe the time-varying discharge rate of the AN fiber, r(t), rather than producing discharge times with a modified Poisson renewal process, 1 which is more computationally intensive. Psychophysical performance limits based on the Poisson variability of AN discharges can be calculated directly from r(t) for either average rate or temporal information. 6,7 2. Model overview The implementation of the present model is identical to the Zhang et al. 1 model (Fig. 1), except as described in Section 3. Level-dependent tuning is represented in the signal path by a nonlinear, third-order, time-varying narrowband filter followed by a linear, first-order, broadband filter (not shown in Fig. 1). The gain and bandwidth of the nonlinear filter are controlled via the time constant τ(t), which is varied according to the control-path output. The control path has a nonlinear wideband filter followed by an asymmetric saturating nonlinearity. The saturating nonlinearity, which represents OHC transduction properties, is followed by a third-order lowpass filter with an 8-Hz cutoff frequency. The lowpass filter determines the dynamics of the cochlear amplifier (time constant of roughly 2 µs). 1 The lowpass-filter output is mapped into τ(t) according to a nonlinearity that equals τ narrow for zero input and τ wide for the maximum dc output of the OHC nonlinearity, where τ narrow > τ wide. The low-level time constant τ narrow is specified from data describing low-level tuning, and the high-level time constant τ wide is specified as a fraction of τ narrow according to the cochlear-amplifier gain for the given CF. The linear broadband filter following the nonlinear signal-path filter has a time constant of τ wide. Thus, the control path broadens the nonlinear signal filter [i.e., decreases τ(t)] as the level of the stimulus that passes through the control-path filter increases. This implementation produces the desired decrease in gain and level-dependent phase shifts near CF associated with broadened tuning. Two-tone suppression 8,9 is accounted for by making the control-path filter wider than the excitatory filter (Fig. 1), thus allowing nonexcitatory stimuli to reduce the gain of the cochlear amplifier. The control path can be modified to allow the relative effects of different nonlinear properties to be evaluated separately. Five useful versions of the AN model are described. (1) The standard AN model (nonlinear with compression and suppression) represents a normalhearing subject. (2) The effects of suppression can be removed by implementing the controlpath filter with the same bandwidth and CF as the nonlinear signal-path filter. This AN model version (nonlinear with compression and without suppression) is unrealistic physiologically, but is useful for isolating suppression effects. (3) The linear sharp AN model corresponds to τ(t) =τ narrow for the narrowband signal-path filter and retains full cochlear-amplifier gain. This model version has the same low thresholds and low-level tuning as the nonlinear AN models, but has sharp tuning at all levels, similar to many classic auditory models. (4) The effects of reduced frequency selectivity can be isolated from sensitivity effects with the linear broad, low threshold version, which corresponds to τ(t) =τ wide and full cochlear-amplifier gain, independent of stimulus level. (5) The linear broad, impaired AN model corresponds to τ(t) =τ wide with no cochlear-amplifier gain and represents a cochlea that has completely 92 ARLO 2(3), July 21 1529-7853/1/2(3)/91/6/$18. (c)21 Acoustical Society of America 92

Heinz et al.: Acoustics Research Letters Online [DOI 1.1121/1.1387155] Published Online 12 June 21 dysfunctional OHCs but fully functioning inner hair cells (IHCs). The time-varying AN discharge rate, r(t), is calculated by passing the output of the signal-path filter through an asymmetric saturating nonlinearity, a lowpass filter, and a synapse model. The saturating nonlinearity and lowpass filter produce response properties associated with IHC transduction, whereas the synapse model includes adaptation effects such as the extended dynamic range at onset relative to the steady-state response. 1 3. Implementation details This section describes the implementation details of the present model that differ from the Zhang et al. 1 model (see http://earlab.bu.edu/ for the source code for both models). The cochlear map [i.e., the CF distribution along the basilar membrane (BM), used for AN population studies] in the present model was based on a human cochlear map 1 (Table 1 in ref 6), rather than cat. Psychophysical estimates of human frequency selectivity at low levels 11 were used to specify the low-level tuning of the model filters (Eq. 4 in ref 1) as a function of CF. The low-level time constant of the nonlinear signal-path filter was specified by τ narrow (CF)=1.2{2π[1.19][24.7(4.37CF/1 + 1)]} 1, (1) where the expression 24.7(4.37CF/1 + 1) describes mid-level psychophysical estimates of equivalent rectangular bandwidth (ERB), the factor of 1.19 converts between gammatone-filter bandwidth and ERB, 12 and the factor of 1.2 increase in τ is a simplified conversion from midto low-level tuning that is consistent with the ratio of low- to mid-level human ERBs. 11 The inclusion of different SR groups and the SR dependence of AN thresholds required modifying the transformation from IHC-lowpass-filter output to immediate permeability in the synapse model. The slope parameter, V sat, 1 was adjusted so that the rate-level curves had shapes and relative thresholds that correspond well with those reported from physiological studies, 5,13 and varies with SR as well as with CF. Thus, Eq. A17 in ref 1 was modified to V sat = 6 ( ) 1 + SR P 6 + SR Imax K CF ; ( ) CF K CF = max[1.5,2 + 1.3log 1 ], (2) 1 where P Imax was unchanged. This modification changed the value of the parameters p 1 and p 2 given in Table I and Eq. 18 of ref 1, which were simplified based on the previous SRindependent V sat. The appendix in ref 1 describes the dependence of p 1 and p 2 on SR. The parameter PT S (peak-to-steady-state discharge ratio) in Table I of ref 1 depends on SR as PT S = 1 + 9SR/(9 + SR), consistent with AN data. 14 In addition, the parameter A SS was changed from 35 to 13 spikes/sec to maintain reasonable saturated rates in r(t) without refractoriness. The cutoff frequency of the IHC lowpass filter, cut ihc, was changed from 3.8 to 4.5 khz to maintain a proper rolloff in phase locking with CF for r(t). 15 A 5-kHz sampling rate was often necessary due to strong nonlinearities in the model; 1 lower sampling rates may suffice in some conditions. 4. Model properties Broadened tuning with increases in stimulus level is illustrated in Fig. 2. Normalized magnitude responses of the signal-path filter (Fig. 2A) are qualitatively consistent with BM magnitude responses to tones. 16 Compressive magnitude responses occur near CF (Figs. 2A,B), with maximum compression occurring at CF and more linear responses well away from CF. The compressive response begins to occur at 2 db SPL and is fully compressive by 4 db SPL (Fig. 2B), consistent with physiological data. 16 Model compression begins to decrease above 8 db SPL; however, filter responses are still slightly compressive up to 12 db SPL. The cochlear amplifier boosts the response to low level stimuli (by 3 db in Fig. 2B), and compression represents the reduction in amplification as stimulus level increases. The amount of compression (or cochlear-amplifier gain) in the model is largest for high CFs (Figs. 2C,D); there is 55 db 93 ARLO 2(3), July 21 1529-7853/1/2(3)/91/6/$18. (c)21 Acoustical Society of America 93

Heinz et al.: Acoustics Research Letters Online [DOI 1.1121/1.1387155] Published Online 12 June 21 Normalized Filter Output (rms db) Filter Output (db SPL) 1 2 3 4 12 1 8 6 4 A 15 2 25 Tone Frequency (Hz) B Nonlinear Impaired 1 db 3 5 7 9 2 Gain=3 db 2 4 6 8 1 12 Filter Output (db SPL) Gain at CF (db) 12 1 8 6 4 2 C 2 4 6 8 1 12 6 5 4 3 2 Linear 5 Hz 1 Hz 2 Hz 4 Hz 8 Hz 1 D.1 1 1 Characteristic Frequency (khz) Avg. Discharge Rate (sp/sec) Phase re: 9 db SPL (π) 3 25 2 15 1 5 1.5.5 1 E F 15 2 25 1 db 3 5 7 9 11 15 2 25 Tone Frequency (Hz) Fig. 2. Model response properties associated with nonlinear tuning. (A) Nonlinear signal-pathfilter responses for a 2-kHz CF for various levels. RMS output re: 1-dB-SPL response. (B) Cochlear amplification at 2-kHz CF. (C) Magnitude compression for various CFs. (D) Cochlearamplifier gain vs. CF, defined as the reduction in filter response at 12 db SPL relative to a linear response. (E,F) AN response areas for 2-kHz CF. Phase plotted re: 9-dB-SPL response for same levels as (E). Any difference from zero represents a phase response that changes with level. of gain at 8 khz and less than 2 db of gain below 1 khz, consistent with data from the basal 16 and apical 17 turns of the chinchilla cochlea. Model compression (in db/db from 4 to 8 db SPL, Fig. 2C) increases with frequency by a factor of 1.9 over 3 octaves, similar to psychophysical estimates of human compression (2.1 factor over 3-octaves). 18 Nonlinear cochlear tuning influences AN-fiber response areas in both average rate and phase (Figs. 2E,F). The level-dependent phase changes below and above CF (Fig. 2F) are consistent with nonlinear BM 16 and AN 19 phase responses, but are not included in most AN models. Two-tone suppression tuning curves (Fig. 3) illustrate frequency and level combinations of a suppressor tone that reduce the response to a CF tone. 8,9 Suppression occurs below and above CF, consistent with physiological data. 9 Growth of suppression with level in the present model matches Zhang et al. (Fig. 13 in ref 1). The asymmetric growth is qualitatively consistent with physiological AN data for which suppression grows more rapidly below than above CF; however, the degree of model asymmetry is less than in the physiological data. 1,9 Threshold tuning curves and rate-level curves for five versions of the AN model are compared in Fig. 4. The effects of the external and middle ear are not considered in this model; thus all AN-model versions except the impaired version have CF thresholds near db SPL. Loss of the cochlear-amplifier gain (Fig. 2D) in the impaired version results in a sloping highfrequency hearing loss, with less than 2-dB loss at low frequencies and roughly 6-dB loss at Threshold (db SPL) 8 6 4 Excitatory Suppression 2 CF=F =2 khz 1 1 2 1 3 1 4 Frequency (Hz) A Threshold (db SPL) 8 6 4 2 CF=F =8 khz 1 1 2 1 3 1 4 Frequency (Hz) Fig. 3. Two-tone-suppression tuning curves for the nonlinear AN model (A: CF=2 khz; B: CF=8 khz). Solid line: excitatory tuning (threshold: 1 sp/sec > SR). Stars: suppression thresholds (-1 sp/sec criterion) for a second tone presented in addition to 15-dB-SPL CF-tone (as in ref 9). B 94 ARLO 2(3), July 21 1529-7853/1/2(3)/91/6/$18. (c)21 Acoustical Society of America 94

Heinz et al.: Acoustics Research Letters Online [DOI 1.1121/1.1387155] Published Online 12 June 21 1) NL w/ Compression 2) NL w/ Compression w/ Suppression w/o Suppression 3) Linear Sharp 4) Linear Broad Low Threshold 5) Linear Broad Impaired Threshold (db SPL) Rate (sp/sec) Rate (sp/sec) 8 6 4 2 3 2 1.1 1 1 3 2 1 5 1 8 6 4 2 3 2 1.1 1 1 3 2 1 5 1 8 6 4 2.1 1 1 Frequency (khz) 3 2 1 1 8 6 4 2 3 2 1.1 1 1 5 1 5 1 3 3 2 2 8 6 4 2 1.1 1 1 3 HSR MSR 2 LSR 5 1 5 1 5 1 5 1 5 1 5 1 Fig. 4. Five model versions (five columns, see text) used to evaluate nonlinear properties associated with cochlear amplifier. (Row 1) Schematic diagrams. (Row 2) Excitatory tuning curves for HSR (6 sp/sec) fibers. (Rows 3,4) Rate-level curves at CF for 1- and 8-kHz CFs. Three SR groups (HSR; MSR: 5 sp/sec; LSR: 1 sp/sec). Rate over 4-24 ms for 25-ms tones. 1 3 2 1 high frequencies. Rate-level curves for high-sr (HSR) fibers have thresholds near db SPL, and a dynamic range of about 3 db SPL. Medium SR (MSR) and LSR fibers have higher thresholds and larger dynamic ranges than the HSR fibers. The LSR rate-level curves for the nonlinear version of the AN model have sloping-saturation shapes at 1 khz, and straight shapes at 8 khz, consistent with guinea pig data. 5 The linear versions of the AN model have reduced dynamic ranges for MSR and LSR fibers, especially at high CFs, consistent with physiological data from impaired animals. 2 The dependence of saturated rate on SR in Fig. 4 varies with the stimulus parameters used to calculate average rate; however, the SR effects on threshold, dynamic range, and rate-level shape are consistent. 5. Discussion The cochlear-amplifier gain as a function of CF produced straight rate-level curves for LSR fibers at high frequencies but not at low frequencies, consistent with guinea pig data; 5 however, straight rate-level curves have not been observed in cat. 4,13 Further studies directly relating physiological properties to psychophysical performance are necessary to verify the appropriate strength of compression as a function of CF for the human AN model. The implementation of human tuning (Eq. 1) was an intuitive choice based on psychophysical estimates of auditory filters; 11 however, these ERBs may be influenced by suppression. 7 More accurate descriptions can be easily included when they become available. The traveling wave delay correction used in the cat AN model 1 may not be appropriate for humans, and thus only the CF-dependent delay associated with the gammatone filters is present; a more accurate description could be derived from evoked-response and otoacoustic-emission studies. The simplified synapse model used to generate appropriate rate-level curves and PTS ratios for different SR groups may not capture 95 ARLO 2(3), July 21 1529-7853/1/2(3)/91/6/$18. (c)21 Acoustical Society of America 95

Heinz et al.: Acoustics Research Letters Online [DOI 1.1121/1.1387155] Published Online 12 June 21 other effects as a function of SR, such as recovery from prior stimulation. 21 The present model does not include the effects of refractoriness, which could be incorporated as in ref 1 if desired. Despite these limitations, this AN model provides a useful tool for quantitatively relating physiological properties associated with the cochlear amplifier to human psychophysical performance. Performance limits based on rate or temporal information in the AN population (i.e., with physiological distributions of SR 3 and CF 1 ) can be calculated directly from r(t) using signal detection theory. 6,7 This approach has been used to demonstrate that compression and nonlinear phase cues extend the dynamic range for level encoding within a narrow CF range, and that suppression can produce psychophysical ERBs that overestimate peripheral tuning. 7 Acknowledgments This model was developed as part of a graduate dissertation in the Speech and Hearing Sciences Program of the Harvard-MIT Division of Health Sciences and Technology. 7 This work was supported by ONR-MURI, NIH-NIDCD, and by NSF-IBN. References and links 1 X. Zhang, M.G. Heinz, I.C. Bruce and L.H. Carney, A phenomenological model for the responses of auditorynerve fibers: I. Nonlinear tuning with compression and suppression, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 19, 648-67 (21). 2 M.A. Ruggero, Physiology and coding of sound in the auditory nerve, in The Mammalian Auditory Pathway: Neurophysiology, edited by A.N. Popper and R.R. Fay (Springer-Verlag, New York, 1992) pp. 34-93. 3 M.C. Liberman, Auditory-nerve response from cats raised in a low-noise chamber, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 63, 442-455 (1978). 4 R.L. Winslow and M.B. Sachs, Single-tone intensity discrimination based on auditory-nerve rate responses in backgrounds of quiet, noise, and with stimulation of the crossed olivocochlear bundle, Hear. Res. 35, 165-19 (1988). 5 I.M. Winter and A.R. Palmer, Intensity coding in low-frequency auditory-nerve fibers of the guinea pig, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 9, 1958-1967 (1991). 6 M.G. Heinz, H.S. Colburn and L.H. Carney, Evaluating auditory performance limits: I. One-parameter discrimination using a computational model for the auditory nerve, Neural Computation, in press (21). 7 M.G. Heinz, Quantifying the effects of the cochlear amplifier on temporal and average-rate information in the auditory nerve, Ph.D. diss., Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA (2). 8 M.B. Sachs and N.Y.S. Kiang, Two-tone inhibition in auditory-nerve fibers, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 43, 112-1128 (1968). 9 B. Delgutte, Two-tone rate suppression in auditory-nerve fibers: Dependence on suppressor frequency and level, Hear. Res. 49, 225-246 (199). 1 D.D. Greenwood, A cochlear frequency-position function for several species - 29 years later, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 87, 2592-265 (199). 11 B.R. Glasberg and B.C.J. Moore, Derivation of auditory filter shapes from notched-noise data, Hear. Res. 47, 13-138 (199). 12 R.D. Patterson, I. Nimmo-Smith, J. Holdsworth and P. Rice, An efficient auditory filterbank based on the gammatone function, paper presented at a meeting of the IOC Speech Group on Auditory Modeling at RSRE, December 14-15 (1987). 13 M.B. Sachs and P.J. Abbas, Rate versus level functions for auditory nerve fibers in cats: Tone burst stimuli, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 81, 68-691 (1974). 14 W.S. Rhode and P.H. Smith, Characteristics of tone-pip response patterns in relationship to spontaneous rate in cat auditory nerve fibers, Hear. Res. 18, 159-168 (1985). 15 D.H. Johnson, The relationship between spike rate and synchrony in responses of auditory-nerve fibers to single tones, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 68, 1115-1122 (198). 16 M.A. Ruggero, N.C. Rich, A. Recio, S.S. Narayan and L. Robles, Basilar-membrane responses to tones at the base of the chinchilla cochlea, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 11, 2151-2163 (1997). 17 N.P. Cooper and W.S. Rhode, Mechanical responses to two-tone distortion products in the apical and basal turns of the mammalian cochlea, J. Neurophysiol. 78, 261-27 (1997). 18 M.L. Hicks and S.P. Bacon, Psychophysical measures of auditory nonlinearities as a function of frequency in individuals with normal hearing, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 15, 326-338 (1999). 19 D.J. Anderson, J.E. Rose, J.E. Hind and J.F. Brugge, Temporal position of discharges in single auditory nerve fibers within the cycle of a sinewave stimulus: Frequency and intensity effects, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 49, 1131-1139 (1971). 2 R.V. Harrison, Rate-versus-intensity function and related AP responses in normal and pathological guinea pig and human cochleas, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 7, 136-144 (1981). 21 E.M. Relkin and J.R. Doucet, Recovery from prior stimulation. I: Relationship to spontaneous firing rates of primary auditory neurons, Hear. Res. 55, 215-222 (1991). 96 ARLO 2(3), July 21 1529-7853/1/2(3)/91/6/$18. (c)21 Acoustical Society of America 96