Depressed mood effects on processing of highand low content structure text in American and Australian college women

Similar documents
Commentary: The role of intrapersonal psychological variables in academic school learning

Content Similarities and Differences in Cattell s Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire, Eight State Questionnaire, and Motivation Analysis Test

Comparison of higher stratum motivational factors across sexes using the Children's Motivation Analysis Test

Prediction of academic achievement using the School Motivation Analysis Test.

References. Note: Image credits are in the slide notes

DEPARTMENT OF APPLIED PSYCHOLOGY GURU JAMBHESHWAR UNIVERSITY OF SC.& TECHNOLOGY, HISAR

An Affective Aspect of Computer-Mediated Communication : Analysis of Communications by

Memory for emotional faces in naturally occurring dysphoria and

PÀ ÁðlPÀ gádå G À Áå ÀPÀgÀ CºÀðvÁ ÀjÃPÉë (PÉ- Émï) KARNATAKA STATE ELIGIBILITY TEST (K-SET) FOR LECTUERSHIP

Maharashtra State Eligibility Test for Lectureship SYLLABUS AND SAMPLE QUESTIONS. Conducted by University of Pune

Research Questions and Survey Development

Use of change scores in redundancy analyses of multivariate psychological inventories

The power of positive thinking: the effects of selfesteem, explanatory style, and trait hope on emotional wellbeing

Cognitive-Behavioral Assessment of Depression: Clinical Validation of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire

THE EFFECTS OF READER PERSPECTIVE AND COGNITIVE STYLE ON REMEMBERING IMPORTANT INFORMATION FROM TEXTS

The Stability of Undergraduate Students Cognitive Test Anxiety Levels

Increasing the amount of information that can be held in short-term memory by grouping related items together into a single unit, or chunk.

The Effects of Societal Versus Professor Stereotype Threats on Female Math Performance

Valence and Gender Effects on Emotion Recognition Following TBI. Cassie Brown Arizona State University

The Short NART: Cross-validation, relationship to IQ and some practical considerations

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Mindful Emotion Coaching

Nature of emotion: Five perennial questions. Motivation & Emotion Nature of emotion. Five questions. Outline Nature of emotion. Remaining chapters

SECONDARY SCHOOL STUDENTS TEST ANXIETY AND ACHIEVEMENT IN ENGLISH

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT S A R A H R A S K I N, P H D, A B P P S A R A H B U L L A R D, P H D, A B P P

The IE-ACCME test. The IE-ACCME test. MetaEmotional Intelligence: Antonella D'Amico. Meta-Emotional Intelligence in adolescents

Nature of emotion: Six perennial questions

Emotion Coaching. A tool to help you to work successfully with young people with SEN&D

Emotion Lecture 26 1

Nature of emotion: Six perennial questions

Emotional Development

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention

U3A PSYCHOLOGY. How Memory works January 2019

Chapter 16: Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing?

Mood and Recall of Autobiographical Memory: The Effect of Focus of Self-Knowledge

GRADE LEVEL AND SUBJECT: ADVANCED PLACEMENT PSYCHOLOGY (11 TH AND 12 TH )

Effects on academic learning of manipulating emotional states and motivational dynamics

Prerequisites: (Recommended) none, although concurrent enrolment in Biology 11 is helpful

Coding. The format in which information is stored in the various memory stores.

Effects of self-complexity on mood-incongruent recall 1

It takes 2 to Tango: The role of emotion in service interactions

Retrieval of Concepts in Script-Based Texts and Narratives: The Influence of General World Knowledge

Sex differences in emotion recognition by observing body movements: A case of American students

What Effect Do Schemas Have On The Recall Of

to Cues Present at Test

Memory Development. Cognitive Development

Computational models of emotion

PSYCHOLOGY ADVANCED LEVEL

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

BarOn Emotional Quotient Inventory. Resource Report. John Morris. Name: ID: Admin. Date: December 15, 2010 (Online) 17 Minutes 22 Seconds

Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction. Table of Contents

Human cogition. Human Cognition. Optical Illusions. Human cognition. Optical Illusions. Optical Illusions

Key words: State-Trait Anger, Anger Expression, Anger Control, FSTAXI-2, reliability, validity.

A Behavioral Attention Task for Investigating Rumination in Borderline Personality Disorder: Final Report

Fear of faces: a psychophysiological investigation of facial affect processing in social phobia

University of Warwick institutional repository:

Optical Illusions 4/5. Optical Illusions 2/5. Optical Illusions 5/5 Optical Illusions 1/5. Reading. Reading. Fang Chen Spring 2004

GOLDSMITHS Research Online Article (refereed)

Language Shapes Emotion Experience and Perception

What does the neuropsychological Category Test measure?

PS3021, PS3022, PS4040

PSYC 222 Motivation and Emotions

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations

Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information:

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources

Academic achievement and its relation to family background and locus of control

Tutorial: Cognition See Tutorials on Attention, Memory, Retrieval, Organization, Problem Solving, Reasoning, Self- Regulation/Executive Functions

Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information:

VARIABLES AND MEASUREMENT

Testing the Multiple Intelligences Theory in Oman

Emotions (Chap 10 second half) Learning (Chap 6) Memory (Chap 7) Developmental/lifespan Psychology (Chap 11)

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

and women Lauren Jayne Hall, BA-Psych (Honours) Murdoch University

Emotions and Moods. Robbins & Judge Organizational Behavior 13th Edition. Bob Stretch Southwestern College

The Importance of the Mind for Understanding How Emotions Are

coping strategies, involving a critical cognitive set, should be associated with reduced

Emotional Intelligence Assessment Technical Report

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

The Relationship between the Attachment Patterns and the Coping Skills with Drug Abuse

Emotion Coaching. A tool to help you to work successfully with young people with SEN&D

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE The key to harmonious relationships Lisa Tenzin-Dolma

The Learning Process. Learning is a Process. Behavioral Learning Theories. Chapter 3 Learning and Memory. How many of these do you remind?

Syllabus AP Psychology

Span Theory: An overview

Memory: Storage and Retrieval. Lecture 19

A STUDY ON EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND ITS CONNOTATIONS FOR PERSONAL, SOCIAL AND WORK PLACE SUCCESS

DRAI 10 November 1992 Display Rule Assessment Inventory (DRAI): Norms for Emotion Displays in Four Social Settings

Relationship between Social Cognition and Depressive Symptomatology, Anxiety and Social Functioning in Individuals with Multiple Sclerosis

ADMS Sampling Technique and Survey Studies

Where do emotions come from? What are they made of? Why do we have them? ADAPTIVE - Survival. Emotion. AP Psych Myers Ch. 13

Sample Exam Questions Psychology 3201 Exam 1

3/23/2017 ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT NEEDS OF THE INDIVIDUAL WITH A TRAUMATIC BRAIN INJURY: A SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGIST S PERSPECTIVE

Mindful Emotion Coaching

A comparison of the PAD scale, Premo and checklist

Outline. Emotion. Emotions According to Darwin. Emotions: Information Processing 10/8/2012

24. PSYCHOLOGY (Code No. 037)

ʻThe concept of Deaf identity in Sloveniaʼ

Trait Emotions and Affective Modulation of the Startle Eyeblink: On the Unique Relationship of Trait Anger

Transcription:

Bond University From the SelectedWorks of Gregory J. Boyle 1986 Depressed mood effects on processing of highand low content structure text in American and Australian college women Gregory J. Boyle, University of Melbourne Available at: https://works.bepress.com/greg_boyle/149/

Depressed Mood Effects on Processing of High- and Low content Structure Text in American and Australian College Women Gregory J. Boyle 123 University of Delaware 1 The author (currently an academic faculty member at the University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia) is indebted to his PhD co-advisers for their constructive advice - Professor Jack Pikulski and Professor Sylvia Farnham-Diggory, College of Education, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19716, USA. 2 Professor Carrol Izard, Department of Psychology, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19716, USA allowed use of his DES-III instrument and was a member of the author s dissertation committee. 3 Professor Jum Nunnelly, Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37235, USA, during a visit to the University of Delaware, provided advice on the use of the repeated-measures application of the multiple discriminant function analysis on the DES-III items.

Abstract Depressed mood at encoding and retrieval of high- (HCS) and low-content structure (LCS) text was studied in samples of American and Australian college women. Within each sample, undergraduate students were allocated to one of three subgroups: 1) an Acquisition Group - received Velten depressogenic statements before reading a passage of text. 2) a Retrieval Group - received the same treatment immediately after reading the passage, while 3) a Neutral group - received neutral statements only. Depressed mood at encoding but not at retrieval was associated with a decrement in LCS cued recall scores only, consistent with a limited-capacity model, wherein interference due to depressed mood reduced task relevant processing capacity in short-term memory (STM).

An important aspect of text is the structural organization of content. Highcontent structure information (HCS) represents gist, whereas low-content structure material (LCS) involves textual detail. Meyer (1975) showed that HCS is better retained and recalled than LCS. Meyer's analysis of textual structure is based on accepted linguistic work; it specifies logical relationships within text and it facilitates analysis of text comprehension processes (cf. Kintsch & Young, 1984). Mood effects on learning and memory (Boyle, 1983) might be localized in either storage or retrieval mechanisms. According to Ellis et al. (1984, p. 480). "The small amount of information on this question suggests that mood effects on retrieval may be limited or negligible. HCS schemata (Rumelhan, 1980) should facilitate storage in STM thereby making little demand on cognitive capacity, whereas lack of appropriate schemata (LCS) should overload STM. This limitedcapacity model has received much support (e.g., Britton et al., 1982). At encoding, depressed mood might reduce storage of LCS but not of HCS (for which the demand on STM capacity is not critical). At retrieval, depressed mood might hinder recall of information not stored in superordinate positions in the memory structure (LCS), thereby increasing the disparity in HCS and LCS recall. This paper examines these possibilities. Method Subjects The 154 women in Sample 1 were all University of Delaware undergraduates whose mean age was 20.93 years (S.D. = 4.36 years). The 124 undergraduate women in the second sample were from the same population (mean

age 21.65 years, S.D. = 2.65 years). Sample 3 comprised 74 undergraduate women at the Melbourne College of Advanced Education, Australia (mean age 21.74 years, S.D. = 2.59 years). Design and Procedure Each sample comprised three subgroups. An Acquisition group received 60 Velten (1968) depressogenic statements, responded to Izard s Differential Emotions Scale (DES-III - see Boyle. 1984, for information concerning the reliability and validity of the DES-III), read a passage about parakeets (from Meyer. 1975), received 60 neutral Velten statements and answered a cued recall test of 18 items (cf. Tyler & Voss. 1982) involving HCS and LCS textual idea units. The DES-III is a 30-item self-report inventory with each item scored on a 5-point Likert-type scale, measuring 10 fundamental emotions labeled: Interest, Joy, Surprise, Sadness, Fear, Shame/Shyness, Guilt, Anger, Disgust, and Contempt. Izard et al. (1974, p. 32) reported a test-retest stability coefficient of 0.77 for the trait version of the DES-III scale over a time interval of several months. Izard et al. (1974, p. 1) stated that, The DES focuses on 10 discrete emotions, each of which was defined theoretically as having neural, neuromuscular-expressive, and experiential components. The components are interactive, but in the fundamental emotion process the common order of occurrence is: (a) efferent neural activity in the facial nerve and consequent patterned facial expression; (b) sensory feedback from the expression via the trigeminal nerve; (c) cortical integration of the sensory data and consequent subjective experience.

Validity of the Velten statements as a mood-induction procedure is well documented (e.g., Snyder & White. 1982; Clark. 1983). The DES measured those aspects of the depressive pattern of fundamental emotions affected by the Velten depressogenic treatment (cf. Boyle. 1985). The 630 word passage was analyzed previously for HCS and LCS idea units. In the cued recall test (cues comprised up to four words from the passage central to the respective idea units), students wrote down everything they could remember. Discrepancies in scoring were resolved by raters who were required to reach a consensus. Results and Discussion In all samples, multivariate ANOVA's (Finn. 1977) on cued recall scores indicated significant differences across the three groups. Mean LCS scores were highly significant (F (3,151) = 8.85. p <.004; F (3,121) = 3.75, p <.03; F (3,68) = 5.07, p <.01), in each sample. No significant effects occurred for HCS scores, however. Within each sample, the mean LCS score for the Acquisition group was significantly lower than that for the Retrieval and Neutral groups combined (F (2,151) = 11.15, p <.001; F (2,121) = 5.24. p <.02; F (2,68) = 7.61. p <.01). Means and standard deviations are shown in Table 1.

Sample and Treatment Group Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations for HCS and LCS Cued Recall Memory Scores across Treatments Cued Recall Scores HCS LCS N = 154 Acquisition 6.03 (1.40) 2.54 (1.44) Retrieval 6;12 (1.27) 3.67 (1.38) Neutral 6.00 (1.55) 3.05 (1.33) N = 124 Acquisition 4.84 (1.95) 2.98 (1.73) Retrieval 5.32 (1.68) 4.00 (1.54) Neutral 5.21 (1.85) 3.44 (1.87) N = 74 Acquisition 4.92 (1.80) 2.40 (1.29) Retrieval 4.69 (1.41) 3.12 (1.51) Neutral 5.61 (1.64) 3.70 (1.55) Note. Standard deviations shown in parentheses. The multivariate between groups effect for DES scores was significant in all three samples (F (24,274) = 3.60, p <.0001; F (20,224) = 1.58. p <.05; F (20.118) = 2.51.. p <.001). Only the contrast between the Acquisition vs Retrieval and Neutral groups combined was significant (F (12,137) = 4.50, p <.0001; F (10,112) = 2.40, p <.01; F(10,59) = 3.88, p <.0005). Of the DES depressive subscales (Sadness. Shame. Anger, Fear and Guilt), the main subscale (Sadness) was very significantly elevated for the Acquisition group in all three samples. Moreover, Anger and Guilt were also significantly elevated in all samples (Table 2). All three samples exhibited a significant lowering of LCS cued recall scores for the Acquisition group only but not for the Retrieval group. As the Velten treatment was effective in all three samples for the Acquisition group, there is no reason to question their validity for the Retrieval group.

DES Subscale Table 2 Mean DES S ubscale Scores for the Second American and Australian Samples Velten Condition Depressive Neutral Neutral p less than Interest 6.14 (6.76) 6.07 (7.54) 5.49 (6.87) n.s. (n.s.) Joy 6.95 (7.80) 8.51 (9.12) 8.59 (8.57).03 (n.s.) Surprise 4.77 (5.40) 4.61 (5.54) 4.95 (5.74) n.s. (n.s.) Sadness 7.68 (8.40) 5.54 (5.38) 6.44 (5.74).01 (.0001) Anger 6.39 (5.28) 4.85 (4.88) 5.44 (3.78).05 (0.3) Disgust 5.73 (5.00) 4.34 (4.85) 4.56 (3.43).02 (.003) Concempt 5.80 (6.08) 5.73 (4.92) 5.87 (4.13) n.s. (.002) Fear 5.27 (5.04) 3.71 (4.35) 4.54 (3.70).01 (n.s.) Shame/Shyness 4.73 (5.60) 4.00 (4.19) 4.49 (3.61) n.s. (.004) Guilt 5.68 (6.08) 4.27 (4.81) 5.26 (4.17).04 (.02) Notes. Data for Australian sample in parentheses. Following the crossvalidational principle, only those subscales which were significant in both samples can be regarded as showing real effects due to presentation of the depressive Velten statements. Conclusions Listening to negative Velten statements produced two effects. First, depressed mood had a differential impact on textual detail as opposed to gist, such that LCS was less readily recalled than under neutral conditions, whereas no deficit was apparent for recall of HCS. Second, depressed mood failed to interfere significantly with retrieval. In none of the three samples was the mean LCS cued recall score for the Retrieval group significantly lowered as com pared with the Neutral group. Despite findings of Britton et al. (1982), the present findings suggest that retrieval-deficit is less relevant to text processing than is encodingdeficit. This supports Ellis et al. (1984) that debilitating effects of depressed mood operate primarily at encoding rather than at retrieval, consistent with a limitedcapacity model of STM.

References Boyle, G. J. Effects on academic learning of manipulating emotional states and motivational dynamics. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 1983, 53, 347-357. Boyle, G. J. Reliability and validity of Izard's Differential Emotions Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 1984, 5, 747-750. Boyle, G. J. Self-report measures of depression: Some psychometric considerations. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1985, 24, 45-59. Britton, B. K., Glynn. S. M. Meyer. B.I. F. & Penland. M. J. Effects of text structure on use of cognitive capacity during reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1982, 74, 51-61. Izard, C. E., Dougherty, F. E. Boxom, B. M. & Kotsch, N. E. (1974). The Differential Emotions Scale: A method of measuring the subjective experience of discrete emotions. Vanderbilt University Press, Nashville Tennessee. Qark, D. M. On the induction of depressed mood in the laboratory: Evaluation and comparison of the Velten and musical procedures. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1983, 5, 27-49. Ellis, H. C., Thomas. R. L. & Rodriguez, I. A. Emotional mood states and memory: Elaborative encoding, semantic processing, and cognitive effort. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition.1984. 10. 470-482. Finn, J. D. Mulivariance: Univariate and mulivariate analysis of variance, covariance, regression and repeated measures: A Fortran IV program. Version VI. National Educational Resources, 1977.

Kintsch, W. & Young, S. R. Selective recall of decision-relevant information from texts. Memory and Cognition, 1984, 12, 112-117. Meyer, B. J. F. The organization of prose and its effects on memory. Amsterdam: North-Holland. 1975. Rumelhan, D. E. Schemata: The building blocks of cognition. In R. J. Spiro, B. C. Bruce & W. F. Brewer (Eds.), Theoretical issues in reading comprehension: Perspectives and cognitive psychology, linguistics, artificial intelligence, and education. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 1980. Snyder, M. & White. P. Moods and memories: Elation, depression, and the remembering of the events of one's life. Journal of Personality, 1982, 50, 149-167. Tyler, S. W. & Voss, J. F. Attitude and knowledge effects in prose processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1982, 21, 524-538. Velten, E. A laboratory task for induction of mood states. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1968, 6, 473-482.