Modeling of Mechanoelectrical Transduction of Hair Cells to Action Potentials in the Auditory Nerve

Similar documents
Representation of sound in the auditory nerve

Mechanical Properties of the Cochlea. Reading: Yost Ch. 7

Deafness and hearing impairment

Required Slide. Session Objectives

Systems Neuroscience Oct. 16, Auditory system. http:

SOLUTIONS Homework #3. Introduction to Engineering in Medicine and Biology ECEN 1001 Due Tues. 9/30/03

Intro to Audition & Hearing

Auditory System. Barb Rohrer (SEI )

ENT 318 Artificial Organs Physiology of Ear

SPHSC 462 HEARING DEVELOPMENT. Overview Review of Hearing Science Introduction

Hearing. istockphoto/thinkstock

Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception

Spectrograms (revisited)

Before we talk about the auditory system we will talk about the sound and waves

Sound and its characteristics. The decibel scale. Structure and function of the ear. Békésy s theory. Molecular basis of hair cell function.

Auditory System Feedback

Acoustics Research Institute

The mammalian cochlea possesses two classes of afferent neurons and two classes of efferent neurons.

Hearing: Physiology and Psychoacoustics

Auditory Physiology Richard M. Costanzo, Ph.D.

Lecture 6 Hearing 1. Raghav Rajan Bio 354 Neurobiology 2 January 28th All lecture material from the following links unless otherwise mentioned:

College of Medicine Dept. of Medical physics Physics of ear and hearing /CH

Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Week 5. The peripheral auditory system: The ear as a signal processor

What is the effect on the hair cell if the stereocilia are bent away from the kinocilium?

Unit VIII Problem 9 Physiology: Hearing

Sound and Hearing. Decibels. Frequency Coding & Localization 1. Everything is vibration. The universe is made of waves.

Structure, Energy Transmission and Function. Gross Anatomy. Structure, Function & Process. External Auditory Meatus or Canal (EAM, EAC) Outer Ear

Hearing Sound. The Human Auditory System. The Outer Ear. Music 170: The Ear

Music 170: The Ear. Tamara Smyth, Department of Music, University of California, San Diego (UCSD) November 17, 2016

The Structure and Function of the Auditory Nerve

Lecture 3: Perception

Chapter 3: Anatomy and physiology of the sensory auditory mechanism

Sound Waves. Sensation and Perception. Sound Waves. Sound Waves. Sound Waves

Auditory Physiology PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 30. Organ of Corti

Temporal Adaptation. In a Silicon Auditory Nerve. John Lazzaro. CS Division UC Berkeley 571 Evans Hall Berkeley, CA

HEARING. Structure and Function

Sound Waves. Sound and Sensa3on. Chapter 9. Sound waves are composed of compression and rarefac3on of air molecules. Domain

SPECIAL SENSES: THE AUDITORY SYSTEM

Chapter 17, Part 2! The Special Senses! Hearing and Equilibrium!

Chapter 17, Part 2! Chapter 17 Part 2 Special Senses! The Special Senses! Hearing and Equilibrium!

PSY 214 Lecture # (11/9/2011) (Sound, Auditory & Speech Perception) Dr. Achtman PSY 214

Can You Hear Me Now?

Receptors / physiology

BCS 221: Auditory Perception BCS 521 & PSY 221

Auditory Physiology PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 29. Hearing

PSY 310: Sensory and Perceptual Processes 1

Educational Module Tympanometry. Germany D Germering

A truly remarkable aspect of human hearing is the vast

So now to The Ear. Drawings from Max Brodel, an Austrian artist who came to Johns Hopkins in the 1920s. My point in showing this figure is to

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency

Cellular Bioelectricity

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency. (Some slides are adapted from Zhiyao Duan s course slides on Computer Audition and Its Applications in Music)

Human Acoustic Processing

Acoustics, signals & systems for audiology. Psychoacoustics of hearing impairment

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Pitch & Binaural listening

ID# Final Exam PS325, Fall 1997

HEARING AND PSYCHOACOUSTICS

9.01 Introduction to Neuroscience Fall 2007

Chapter 13 Physics of the Ear and Hearing

Hearing. By Jack & Tori

Learning Targets. Module 20. Hearing Explain how the ear transforms sound energy into neural messages.

THE MECHANICS OF HEARING

Hearing. By: Jimmy, Dana, and Karissa

PSY 214 Lecture 16 (11/09/2011) (Sound, auditory system & pitch perception) Dr. Achtman PSY 214

Auditory nerve. Amanda M. Lauer, Ph.D. Dept. of Otolaryngology-HNS

Vision and Audition. This section concerns the anatomy of two important sensory systems, the visual and the auditory systems.

HST 721 Lecture 4: Mechanics, electromotility and the cochlear amplifier

PSY 215 Lecture 10 Topic: Hearing Chapter 7, pages

A Brief History of Auditory Models

Improving the diagnostic power of otoacoustic emissions. Arturo Moleti Physics Department University of Roma Tor Vergata

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA)

The Ear. The ear can be divided into three major parts: the outer ear, the middle ear and the inner ear.

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

THE COCHLEA AND AUDITORY PATHWAY

Synaptopathy Research Uwe Andreas Hermann

A computer model of medial efferent suppression in the mammalian auditory system

Sound. Audition. Physics of Sound. Properties of sound. Perception of sound works the same way as light.

Audition. Sound. Physics of Sound. Perception of sound works the same way as light.

Hearing I: Sound & The Ear

Cochlear anatomy, function and pathology II. Professor Dave Furness Keele University

Hearing I: Sound & The Ear

SENSATION & PERCEPTION

Processing of sounds in the inner ear

The transformation of sound stimuli into electrical signals

2 Background. 2.1 The Hearing Sense

MECHANISM OF HEARING

! Can hear whistle? ! Where are we on course map? ! What we did in lab last week. ! Psychoacoustics

-Detect heat or cold and help maintain body temperature

Linguistic Phonetics Fall 2005

Issues faced by people with a Sensorineural Hearing Loss

Hearing and Balance 1

Who are cochlear implants for?

SUBJECT: Physics TEACHER: Mr. S. Campbell DATE: 15/1/2017 GRADE: DURATION: 1 wk GENERAL TOPIC: The Physics Of Hearing

L2: Speech production and perception Anatomy of the speech organs Models of speech production Anatomy of the ear Auditory psychophysics

Hearing. Juan P Bello

Stimulus Coding in the Auditory Nerve. Neural Coding and Perception of Sound 1

Essential feature. Who are cochlear implants for? People with little or no hearing. substitute for faulty or missing inner hair

Processing in The Cochlear Nucleus

THE EAR AND HEARING Be sure you have read and understand Chapter 16 before beginning this lab. INTRODUCTION: hair cells outer ear tympanic membrane

Frequency refers to how often something happens. Period refers to the time it takes something to happen.

Transcription:

Modeling of Mechanoelectrical Transduction of Hair Cells to Action Potentials in the Auditory Nerve Anthony Au, Yuchen Wang, Sibai Xie A Term Paper for BENG26 University of California, San Diego San Diego, CA 9293 a5au@eng.ucsd.edu wangyuchen81@gmail.com s8xie@eng.ucsd.edu Abstract An auditory nerve model based on Meddis model is test in this project. In this model, the input sound was decomposed into different constituent single frequencies, and was therefore responded by individual hair cells. Mechanical amplification during the transduction process was taken into account. The deflection was translated into the change in membrane potential, which was subsequently used as the input to the Meddis model. The spiking patterns were observed under pure tone and human voice stimulations. Phase-lock phenomenon, adaptation, and spiking rate pattern indicate that this model reflects some of the distinct properties of the hair cell. 1 Introduction 1.1 H earing M echanism of Huma n Ears A human auditory system is one of the many examples of finely engineered functional systems that are fabricated by the nature. The sound wave is collected by an elegantly shaped auricle and introduces the vibration of the tympanum. Such vibration transmits through the three cartilaginous structures, malleus, incus and stapes, to the sound processing center of cochlea (Figure 1). The cochlea is the critical organism where mechanoelectrical transduction takes place [1]. 1.2 Structure and functions of the hu ma n cochl ea The cochlea forms a spiral structure, and if we unfold it, we obtain a strip-like structure. The detailed structure of cochlea is very complicated, so in this project we only focus on the hair cell behavior in the cochlea (Figure 2). There are thousands of hair cells in the cochlea, between the tectorial membrane and the basilar membrane. They are specialized sensory epithelial cells. Two types of hair cells are present: inner hair cells (IHCs) and outer hair cells (OHCs). OHCs are believed to play a role in the active amplification process, and also the decomposition of complex sound [6]. In this project, we focus on the mechanoelectrical transduction that is realized by IHCs. The sound decomposition function of OHC is mimicked by a Fourier transform and threshold filtering process.

1.3 Inne r H a ir cells The hair cells in different positions on the cochlea respond to different frequencies of sound. It is found that the hair cells in the deeper position of the cochlea are stiffer and respond to lower frequencies of sound. The heights of the stereocilia also increase from the base to the apex of the cochlea. This together with graduate change in the thickness of the basilar membrane gives rise to the sensitivity to a broad range of frequencies. The sound wave triggers the movement the vibration of the basilar membrane, and subsequently causes the hair cells to move again the stationary tectorial membrane. The deflection of hair bundle or the bundle of the stereocilia and kinocilia leads to the opening of the potassium channel by a tip link connecting the top of every stereocilium. This unique mechanical gating mechanism ensures the sensitivity of the system to the rapid change in the frequency of the sound. Figure 1: The structure of a human ear. The hair cells sit in the middle of tectorial and basilar membranes. The structure of the hair cell is shown on the right. When hair cell bundles are deflected, the tip links trigger the potassium gates to open up. 2 Model Description 2.1 Frequency analysis In order to know how many hair cells join the encoding of the sound. The original sound is firstly transformed through Fourier transform into the frequency domain. Then a threshold was set based on the Gaussian white noise assumption to filter out the noise and pick out the major frequency components which indicates the number of active hair cells. An assumption was made here that one hair cell only responded to one specific frequency.

Figure 2: Fourier transform of the original signal and threshold setting. (The red line in the lower graph is a demonstration of a threshold and it equals to the mean of the amplitude of the signal plus the three times standard deviation). 2.2 Sound pressure conversion to me mbrane potential The following equations were used for calculating the deflection and the resulting membrane potential changes. The mechanical vibration is sent to cochlea and the force of sound pressure is converted to deflection by equation 1. The denominator of the equation is a combination of the tip link stiffness (Nγ 2 κ) and a position-dependent hair bundle stiffness (K sp ). The deflection is then used to calculate the membrane voltage change by equation 2. This equation was subtracted from experimental results [1]. 1 ΔX = ( ) ΔF Nγ 2 κ+k HB (1) sp V m = 2.2734 6 (2) 5.7578 e ( ΔX 24.73 ) +1 2.3 M eddis mo del for hair cell synapse In 1986, Meddis published his hair cell ribbon synapse model [2]. The model consists of four parts: factory, free transmitter pool, reprocessing store, cleft. The factory is the source of the transmitters and these transmitters will flow to the free transmitter pool with a speed proportional to the amount of the remaining transmitters. When the membrane potential of the presynaptic neuron changes, the neurotransmitters will leak from the free transmitter pool into the Cleft to incite the spiking of postsynaptic neuron and the speed is controlled by the presynaptic neuron s membrane potential and concentration of transmitters in the pool. During this process, some of transmitters will be lost because of the random motion and some will be reuptake by reprocessing store which then replenishes the free transmitter pool after being fixed. In the end, the remaining transmitters will exert influences on the postsynaptic neuron. For the purpose of simplifying the problem, we assume that one

transmitter causes a spike. Then the releasing rate is the spike rate at each moment. Figure 3: Schematic description of the Meddis model [3] The differential equations describing this model are shown below: dq = xw(t) + y(m q(t)) k(t)q(t) dt dc = k(t)q(t) lc(t) rc(t) dt dw = rc(t) xw(t) dt k(t) = g dt[s(t) + A]/[s(t) + A + B] when [s(t) + A] > k(t) = when [s(t) + A] < 3 Auditory Nerve Spiking Rate(spikes/s) rate(spikes/s) 2 1 Pressure Deviation from Atmospheric(Pa) 5 1 15 2 15 1 5 Original Signal 5 1 15 2 time(ms) Figure 4: Meddis Model with a step function input signal We demonstrated the adaptability of the Meddis model by using a step function as the input signal. When the first step in the signal occurs at 2.5ms, the model responds with a spike rate that eventually declines due to adaptation to the constant stimulus. When the stimulus is removed, the model responds with an offset adaptation (the signal dips below the initial zero stimulus point before rising back to the steady state point for the stimulus intensity). At 15ms, the stimulus is increased from a non-zero value and the model responds by showing a short increase in spiking rate before settling into the new steady state. A similar phenomenon is shown again when the stimulus increases at 17.5ms. Thus, this model shows the ability to adapt to louder or softer sounds. 3 Results and discussion 3.1 Pure tones analysis Pure tone was firstly used to test the validity of our model and the phenomenon our model can reflect. In the test we used a 5dB, 1 khz pure tone with a 3dB Gaussian white noise. The waveform and the frequency domain are shown in Figure 5. After processing by our model, the final spiking pattern is shown in Figure 6.

Amplitude of Pressure 5 Original Sound -5 5 1 15 t(ms) Spectrum of Signal Abs(S(jw)).5.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5 5 f/khz Figure 5: Pure tone waveform and its frequency components Figure 6: Spiking pattern of the hair responding to the pure tone. Firstly the figure shows that the model successfully picks up the pure tone signal without being influenced by noise and it also displays two important phenomena phase locking and adaptation. As the red lines indicate, the auditory neuron s spike rate reaches its maximum value at the same portion of each pure tone cycle. In another words, the response (spike pattern) is aligned with the stimulus periods. This is the phase locking which has been tested by the experiment in auditory neurons [4]. The figure also shows that in each stimulus cycle, the auditory neuron reaches the local maximum spiking rate first and then decreases its spiking rate, even though the strength of the stimulus is still increasing. The local maximum value of each cycle also decreases as the stimulus continues until it finally reaches a steady state. The former phenomenon is called short-term adaptation (shown earlier in figure 4) and the second one is long-term adaptation. They are both the basic phenomenon in the auditory experiments which further prove the validity of the model. Comparing with the adaptation, why the model can reflect the phase-locking is easier to understand. As the transforms we used for the sound, pressure and deflection are linear transform, the phase-locking seems to be natural result. The occurrence of adaptation is a little strange at first, but it can be explained by the Meddis model. For each period, the hair cell rapidly consumes transmitters when its membrane potential deviates from the resting value. When the reprocessing store can t replenish the free pool in time, the short-term adaptation occurs and the spiking rate has to decrease. And for the lasting loss of transmitter by the cleft, the neuron s maximum spike rate in each cycle decreases. Moreover, it can be better shown by the solution of the Meddis model s differential equations. In fact, the k in the equation should be a function of the hair cell s membrane potential and the equations are nonlinear. But when the frequency of the sound is very high, k can be treated as a constant [3]. Then assuming the initial conditions for the variables are all zero and applying the Laplace transform to the equation, the results are:

Q(s) = (s + y + k)(s + x)(s + l + r)ym s((s + y + k)(s + k)(s + l + r) xrk) Because the dominator of Q(s)is a forth order polynomial. The inverse Laplace transform of it should be of the form: q(t) = Const + C 1 e + C 2 e 2 + C 3 e 3 t In the solution, the relatively smaller t accounts for the short term adaptation while the larger t causes the long-term adaptation. Moreover, when time goes to infinity, q(t) reaches the steady value. 3.2 Tw o -frequency tone analysis In this section, two-frequency tones are used to test the robustness of the model. Here we use two 5dB tones with frequencies: 1 khz and 5 khz. The analysis result are shown in Figure 7 Figure 7: Two-frequency tone analysis result. (The first and second graph are spiking patterns of two auditory neurons. The third one is the original sound) The figure shows that the model successfully picks out the two major frequency components and the spiking patterns basically reflect the phase-locking and adaptation. Plotting the first local maximum value of two neurons with the frequency spectrum of the original sound together and normalize them results in figure 8. Normalized Spiking rate(spikes/s) 1.8.6.4.2 Spiking rates for different frequencies Spike Pattern Original Frequency Spectrum 1 2 3 4 5 6 Frequency(kHz) Figure 8: The normalized frequency spectrum. (The two red dots represent first local maximum spiking rates of two neurons) As figure 8 shows, the normalized firing rate of neurons can reflect the frequency spectrum of the original sound in certain degree. Actually, this is an important characteristic of auditory neuron encoding and the details will be discussed in the next section

3.2 Co mplex Signal We simulated the model with a complex signal in order to test its ability to respond to the type of sounds commonly detected by human inner hair cells. The complex signal was a short.wav file recording of a group member speaking the phrase Good Morning. Although simple, this phrase still contained many different frequencies along with background noise. rate(spikes/s) rate(spikes/s) Atmospheric Pressure Deviation(Pa) 1 Auditory Nerve Spiking Rate for Frequency =.14kHz 5 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 Auditory Nerve Spiking Rate for Frequency = 2.52kHz 1 5 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 Original Signal 2-2 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 time(ms) Figure 9: Spiking rates for the highest and lowest frequencies detected by our model The spiking rate analysis of the complex signal showed similar results as both the single pure tone and the two pure tones signals. As the signal continues the spiking rates decrease, which demonstrates the adaptation showed earlier. The complex signal contained significantly more frequencies, but they all displayed similar adaptation results. We were unable to determine phase locking due to the difficultly associated with the mass frequencies and background noise of the original signal. Normalized Spiking rate(spikes/s) 1.2 1.8.6.4.2 Spiking rates for different frequencies Spike Pattern Original Frequency Spectrum.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 Frequency(kHz) Figure 1: The Normalized Spiking Rates for the frequency spectrum isolated at the first local minimum of the spiking rates (shown in figure 9 above) As shown in figure 8, the normalized firing rate of neurons can reflect the frequency spectrum. The complex frequency displays similar results. Each point signifies the spiking rate of an individual frequency isolated at the point where the first local maximum occurs. We chose to isolate the rates at the time the first local maximum occurred instead of at ms (start of the signal) because the beginning spiking rate was affected by the initial conditions and did not accurately display the dynamics of the hair cell. The first local maximum offered an easy point to determine

that each spiking rate vs frequency plot had in common. In figure 1, it can be seen that the pattern created by the spiking rates has a similar wave form to the first portion of the normalized frequency spectrum. There are no points after 2 khz because those frequencies were filtered out as noise by the model. The amplitude of the spiking rates is lower than that of the actual frequency spectrum but this may due to only one hair cell responding to each signal and a lack of amplification of the original signal. This reflection replicates a similar physiological phenomenon, where the spiking rates of different hair cells have a similar pattern to the frequency spectrum of the original signal [5]. 4 Conclusions and future steps 4.1 Conclusions The spiking patterns were observed under pure tone and human voice stimulations. Phase-lock phenomenon, adaptation, and spiking rate pattern indicate that this model reflects some of the distinct properties of the hair cell. The pure tone analysis shows that our model can successfully pick up the responding hair cell and transform the original sound to the final spiking patter of the hair cell. More importantly, it can reflect two key physiological phenomena phase locking and adaptation. The complex signal displayed similar adaptation characteristics to both of the pure tone analyses. By using the complex signal, we were also able to demonstrate the strength of the model in replicating the physiological phenomenon of the spiking rates following the frequency spectrum. Overall, this model approximates several distinct properties of hair cells in decoding sound signals and encoding action potentials to reflect these signals. This model may be used to further explore the auditory system and assist in the development of a fully functional ear. 4.2 Future steps Figure 11: Range of frequency responses of 3 different hair cells for varying stimulus intensity As shown in figure 11, each hair cell may respond to multiple frequencies. At the threshold intensity the hair cells may only respond to a single frequency, but as the intensity of the signal is increased (volume), the hair cells respond to a broader range. This phenomenon suggests that multiple hair cells are responsible detecting a certain frequency. As a result, the spike train for that specific frequency may be the spatial summation of several hair cells. In future works, we would like to include this phenomenon in our model. This should result in more spikes per second in the post stimulus time histogram, since more action potentials will be created by the multiple hair cells responding to each frequency. A spatial summation of the action potentials created from the different hair cells would allow for a more physiologically relevant model and offer better insights into how the signals ultimately encoded for interpretation by the auditory cortex.

References 1 James H. Schwartz, et al., Principles of Neural Science. 5th edn (McGraw-Hill, 213). 2 R. Meddis, 'Simulation of Mechanical to Neural Transduction in the Auditory Receptor', Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 79 (1986), 72-11. 3 R. Meddis, M. J. Hewitt, and T. M. Shackleton, 'Implementation Details of a Computation Model of the Inner Hair-Cell Auditory-Nerve Synapse', Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 87 (199), 1813-16. 4 J. E. Rose, J. F. Brugge, D. J. Anderson, and J. E. Hind, 'Phase-Locked Response to Low-Frequency Tones in Single Auditory Nerve Fibers of Squirrel Monkey', Journal of Neurophysiology, 3 (1967), 769-&. 5 M. B. Sachs, and E. D. Young, 'Encoding of Steady-State Vowels in the Auditory-Nerve - Representation in Terms of Discharge Rate', Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 66 (1979), 47-79. 6 A. Stasiunas, et al., 'Compression, Adaptation and Efferent Control in a Revised Outer Hair Cell Functional Model', Medical Engineering & Physics, 27 (25), 78-89.

Appendix: Va lues used for the variables in the mo del Variable Value Variable Value A 5 Basic_Pressure 2*1^(-5) N/m2 B 3 Oval window amplification 22.4 g 2 Nγ 2 κ 6 un/m y 5.5 Ksp 1 un/m l 25 H 5 x 66.3 r 658 m 1.