/typeset1:/sco3/jobs1/elsevier/nsy/week.23/pnsy1244y Thu Jun 28 11:34: Page Th

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "/typeset1:/sco3/jobs1/elsevier/nsy/week.23/pnsy1244y Thu Jun 28 11:34: Page Th"

Transcription

1 Neuropsychologia 00 (2001) Abnormal gaze strategies during problem solving in Parkinson s disease T.L. Hodgson *, B. Tiesman, A.M. Owen, C. Kennard Di ision of Neuroscience and Psychological Medicine, Imperial College School of Medicine, Charing Cross Hospital Campus, 10th Floor East Wing, St Dunstans Road, London W 68RP, UK Received 10 July 2000; received in revised form 19 February 2001; accepted 8 May 2001 Abstract We have taken a novel approach to the study of problem solving involving the detailed analysis of natural scanning eye movements during the one touch Tower of London task. Control subjects and patients with idiopathic Parkinson s disease (PDs) viewed a series of pictures depicting two arrangements of coloured balls in pockets within the upper and lower halves of a computer display. The task was to plan (but not execute) the shortest movement sequence required to rearrange the balls in one half of the display (the Workspace) to match the arrangement in the opposite half (the Goalspace) and indicate the number of moves required for problem solution. As problem complexity increased, control subjects spent proportionally more time fixating the Workspace region. This pattern is found regardless of whether subjects were instructed to solve problems by rearranging balls in the lower or upper visual fields. The distribution of gaze within the Workspace was also found to be problem dependent, with gaze being selectively directed towards the problem critical balls. In contrast, PDs were found to make more errors in the task and failed to show any dissociation in the amount of time fixating the two halves of the display. This pattern suggests that the patients had difficulty in encoding and/or maintaining current goals during problem solving, consistent with a role for fronto striatal circuits in mechanisms of working memory and attention Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Eye movements; Executive; Basal ganglia; Cognitive; Planning; Dopamine 1. Introduction It is well established that eye movements can be influenced by psychological processes and that complex visual tasks use specialised gaze shifting strategies [10,11,24,26,33,50,61]. Detailed analysis of these strategies can provide a richer description of task performance than is afforded by gross measures such as reaction times and error rates. By focussing on this active component of cognition, researchers have been able to shed light on the internal cognitive processes subserving performance of a number of behaviours [6,12,26,27,31,34]. We have recently taken this approach by measuring eye movements while subjects plan solutions to Tower of London (TOL) problems [27]. This task was developed with the aim of testing the subtle deficits in behaviour, which are observed following frontal lobe damage in man [55], but it has also proved sensitive to a number of other neurological conditions including Parkinson s disease [38] and schizophrenia [39]. The test involves the presentation of two different arrangements of coloured discs or balls (Fig. 1). The subject s task is to rearrange the first array of balls (referred to in this paper as the Workspace ) so that it matches the second array of balls (referred to here as the Goalspace ) using the minimum number of moves possible. The positioning of the balls is constrained to the location of three pegs or pockets in each half of the display. Due to this, complex problems demand that the sequence of moves is carefully planned in advance before attempting the first move. Failure to engage in advanced planning of the sequence will result in initial moves blocking subsequent ball moves. * Corresponding author. Tel.: ; fax: address: t.hodgson@ic.ac.uk (T.L. Hodgson) /01/$ - see front matter 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. PII: S (01)

2 2 T.L. Hodgson et al. / Neuropsychologia 000 (2001) Owen et al. [49] have devised a one-touch version of the TOL in which the incentive for individuals to plan solutions internally is enhanced still further. In this variant of the task, subjects are required to inspect the problems visually and then make a single motor response to indicate how many moves would be required to reach an ideal solution. In this way, the one-touch task isolates the cognitive planning component of the test by demanding the internal planning of solutions without actually executing the appropriate moves. Earlier studies have established formally identical patterns of impairment on the onetouch task and classical TOL tasks in patients with frontal-lobe damage and in patients with Parkinson s disease [45,47,49] even when the same patients are performing the two tasks [42]. Our studies in control subjects have shown that during the solution of relatively simple 3 move problems several discrete phases are observed in ocular scanning during the one-touch TOL task. These correspond to an initial problem assessment during which gaze is equally distributed between the Goalspace and Workspace, followed by a solution elaboration phase when subjects bias their gaze towards the Workspace COLOUR Fig. 1. Example X Y plots for different subjects planning solutions to the same five move Tower of London problem. Half the subjects were instructed to solve problems in the Downstairs manner by rearranging the balls in the upper visual field to match the lower. The other group of subjects solved problems in the converse Upstairs manner. Although all subjects were presented with an identical set of problem pictures, the eye movements made by the control subjects differed systematically dependent upon instruction set, with gaze being strongly biased towards the Workspace during solution planning. In contrast, Parkinson s disease patients failed to show this pattern regardless of whether a correct or incorrect response was given. Fixations were classified offline according to where they landed in a 3 2 analysis grid (shown top-left corner) superimposed over each problem picture.

3 T.L. Hodgson et al. / Neuropsychologia 000 (2001) region. As problem complexity increases, the total time spent fixating the Workspace region increases strongly with the total number of moves required for problem solution. Fixations on the Workspace are also distributed in a problem dependent manner, such that gaze is selectively biased towards balls relevant to the problem in hand. One upshot of these observations is that the relative time spent fixating the Workspace provides a useful measure of the time spent planning or elaborating problem solutions as opposed to assessing problem goals. One group of patients who are impaired at performance of the TOL task are those with Idiopathic Parkinson s disease (PD). These individuals suffer a depletion of the neurotransmitter dopamine within the striatum of the basal ganglia, which becomes progressively more severe as the disease takes its course. Although the symptoms of the disease are predominantly motor in nature, it has become apparent in recent years that even in the early stages, subtle cognitive impairments may be observed (e.g. [8,17,29,49,53,57]). These are most likely to arise from the disruption to the reciprocal loops between the striatum and structures in the prefrontal cerebral cortex [1,32]. However, cognitive deficits may also result from dopamine depletion within the frontal cortex itself, due to degeneration of the meso-limbic dopaminergic system [9,43]. A steady degradation in performance of patients in the TOL task has been observed as the disease takes its course. In the early stages, the time taken for patients to plan problem solutions may be significantly increased. In more severely affected patients, PDs make an increased number of response errors in both the standard and one-touch version of the task [47,49]. Here we describe how Parkinson s disease patients move their point of gaze whilst attempting to solve TOL problems. By examining the relative time they spend looking at different components of the problems, we hope to understand better what aspects of planning are impaired in PD. For example, it is possible that PDs possess essentially normal processes of problem solving, but have slowed motor imagery (bradyphrenia) [17], leading to an increase in the time taken to solve problems. This would be expected to exert a disproportional effect on the solution elaboration phase of planning, reflected by an increase in the amount of time fixating the Workspace. In contrast, inefficient problem solving might also arise from an inability to identify and maintain relevant goal information (goal-processing deficit). If the goal-processing hypothesis were correct, then we would expect patients to spend an increased amount of time fixating the Goalspace relative to the Workspace region of the display. 2. Methods 2.1. Subjects All of the participants in the study gave their informed consent and the local ethics committee approved the work. None of the subjects had encountered the TOL task earlier Control group Eight age matched control subjects were tested. The mean age of the control group was 68.4 years with a range of years. All were neurologically normal and had normal or corrected to normal vision. Eye movement data for one control subject failed to record due to lack of disk space and, therefore, could not be analysed further Patient group Ten subjects with idiopathic Parkinson s disease were tested, with five subjects allocated to each instruction condition. Mean age of the patient group was 61 years, with a range of None of the PDs showed any signs of dementia as assessed by the mini-mental state questionnaire. The severity of their Parkinson s disease was assessed using the abbreviated Webster Scale [60]. The Webster scale is essentially a sub-set of the United Parkinson s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) [20], which focuses on the clinical motor signs of PD. It provides a more detailed and objective assessment than the more commonly used Hoen and Yahr scale [30], but is quicker to administer than the full UPDRS. This allowed us to perform patient assessment at the same session as the planning/eye movement test. The duration of the disease and relevant medication was also noted (Table 1) Materials/stimuli Each subject viewed 20 pictures showing two arrays of three coloured balls positioned in pockets. Pictures subtended 17 by 20 degrees of visual arc and were displayed using a Macintosh 2ci computer with a 17- in. colour computer monitor. Balls were coloured either red, green or blue. The two arrays of balls were located in the upper and lower visual fields. The left most location in each array could contain a maximum of three balls, the middle location contained a maximum of two balls and the right location had space for only one ball. The arrangement of balls in the lower field was always fixed, whilst the position of balls in the upper field varied from trial to trial (Fig. 1). At the start of each trial a central fixation cross was displayed for 500 ms. For calibration purposes, the subject was asked to look at this cross,

4 4 T.L. Hodgson et al. / Neuropsychologia 000 (2001) Table 1 Details of individual Parkinson s disease patients who participated in the study Patient Age Webster score Disease duration (years) Medication JA Seleginine, Sinemet CR JR Unmedicated EL Madapor, Pergolide DR Madopor, Seleginine PS Sinemet, Pergolide RH Sinemet CR BH Sinemet CR CB Sinemet EB Sinemet CH Madapor which was extinguished simultaneously with the presentation of problem pictures Procedure Subjects were instructed to plan but not execute the problem solutions (i.e. the one touch TOL task). Once the subject thought that they had worked out the correct solution to each problem they pressed the mouse key and gave a verbal response to indicate the minimum number of moves required to solve the problem. Errors trials were those for which the subject indicated the wrong number of moves. Prior to the start of the experiment, participants were given written instructions concerning the task indicating how the balls could be moved from one pocket to another and could not be placed directly underneath another ball without moving obstructing balls to an alternative location. They were told to plan the entire sequence of moves in their heads before giving their response. The experimenter asked each subject to show them how they would solve a simple example problem in order to confirm that they had understood the task correctly. Both groups of subjects were then given two blocks of practice using a simpler two-ball task [27] prior to the experimental block Instructions Both patients and controls were randomly allocated into two equal groups which received different instructions on how to solve the task. Upstairs subjects were told to plan the sequence of moves required to rearrange the balls in the top part of the display to match the bottom half (i.e. Workspace in the upper visual field, Goalspace in the lower visual field). In contrast, the Downstairs group were required to rearrange the balls in the bottom half of the display to match the top half (i.e. Workspace in the lower visual field, Goalspace in the upper visual field) (Table 1) Eye tracking and analysis Eye movements were recorded using the EyeLink system (Sensorimotoric systems GMbH), a video based pupil tracker, with head movement compensation system sampling at 250 Hz. Subjects were seated at a comfortable viewing distance in front of the display monitor approximately 60 cm from the computer screen. Pupil position was monitored via two miniature infra-red CCD video cameras mounted on an adjustable head-band. Subjects were instructed to keep head movements to a minimum and no active restraint of head movements was required to obtain sufficiently accurate gaze position recordings. Eye movements were analysed offline using custom software written in C on the Macintosh. Fixations were categorised according to where they landed on a 3 2 grid, which divided the pictures into six sectors of equal area (Fig. 1). Eye movement traces were visualised by the experimenter and played back at slowed speed superimposed over the picture that was being viewed during that trial. Any obvious offset in eye position due to slippage of the head-band or gross head movements were corrected for at this stage. The experimenter could also reject any fixations, which were contaminated by eye blink or eye-lid clipping artefacts. Individual saccades were then identified using a semi-automated procedure, as periods in the eye position signal where the instantaneous, absolute velocity rose above 30 per s for more than two data samples. Fixations were identified as pauses between saccades longer than 50 ms in duration (in order to exclude short fixations preceding corrective saccades). Fixation duration, horizontal and vertical position and grid location were then outputted to text files for statistical analysis (Fig. 1).

5 T.L. Hodgson et al. / Neuropsychologia 000 (2001) Table 2 Mean total time to solve problems for problems with different minimum moves to solution for patients and control groups Minimum moves to solution One Two Three Four Five Controls PD Results 3.1. Response times A two-way ANOVA with subject group (Control/ PD) and problem difficulty (one to five moves) as factors confirmed that response times increased strongly with the minimum number of moves required to solve each problem (main effect of difficulty: F(4,60) =26.61, P ). However, there was no significant main effect or interaction effect of subject group on response times in the task (Table 2) Errors A two way ANOVA with subject group and problem difficulty as factors showed that error rates also increased strongly with sequence length (main effect of difficulty F(4,72)=18.59, P ). There was also a difference in the total number of errors between the control and patient groups although this was found to be significant only for the most difficult problems (interaction group difficulty: F(1,18)=6.19, P 0.05)(Table 3). Earlier reports have suggested that error rates were not significantly increased for mildly affected patients and that the deficit in performance becomes more pronounced as the disease progresses. We, therefore, examined the effect of disease severity by correlating the total number of errors made in the task with each patient s Webster Score. This analysis revealed a significant correlation between the Webster rating and error rates in the planning task (R 2 =0.80; F(1,7)=27.80, P 0.005)(Fig. 2). In contrast, no significant correlation was found between response times and disease severity, as indexed by the Webster score (Fig. 2) Basic saccadic measures Basic saccadic measures were analysed using two-way ANOVAs with subject group and problem difficulty as factors, with either fixation duration, number of eye movements or amplitude of saccades as the dependent variables. Both the total number of eye movements made and the duration of individual fixations increased strongly with problem difficulty for both subject groups (main effects of problem difficulty: F(4,60) =20.02, P ; F(4,60)=9.45, P , respectively). However, there was no significant difference between patients and controls in the total number of eye movements made or the duration of each individual fixation (F(1,15) =0.36; F(1,15) =0.007). Under some conditions eye movement amplitudes are found to be hypometric in PD [29]. However, no significant difference in saccade amplitude was observed between the two subject groups (F(1,15) =0.19) Gaze times Further analysis of the eye movement data was based upon the distribution of gaze times within each segment of a 3 2 analysis grid superimposed on top of each problem picture (Fig. 1). Data for control subjects were entered into a two-way ANOVA with visual field (Goalspace/Workspace), and problem difficulty as factors. Consistent with our earlier investigations [27], the total time spent fixating the Workspace increased more strongly with problem difficulty than did the total time fixating the Goalspace (interaction visual field problem difficulty: F(4,20)=6.57, P 0.002). This was found to be the case regardless of whether the Goalspace was in the upper or lower visual field, such that a three-way ANOVA with instruction condition (Upstairs/Downstairs), visual field (upper/lower) and problem difficulty as factors produced a significant interaction between instruction condition and visual field (F(4,20)=6.57, P 0.002)(Table 4 Fig. 3). Further inspection of the eye movement data revealed that this increase in total fixation time reflected an increase in both the number of eye movements, as well as the duration of individual fixations (Table 4). Table 3 Mean number of response errors per block at each sequence length for controls and PDs (four trials were presented at each sequence length) Minimum moves to solution One Two Three Four Five Controls PD

6 6 T.L. Hodgson et al. / Neuropsychologia 000 (2001) Fig. 2. Graph showing correlation between the total number of errors in the TOL task and motoric symptoms of Parkinson s disease as indexed by the Webster score rating (least squares linear fit line shown). In contrast, the identical analysis for the PD patients showed no significant difference in the amount of time spent fixating the two parts of the display at any level of problem difficulty (main effect of visual field F(4,32) = 0.49). Even at the most difficult problem level, there was no dissociation in the average time spent fixating the Goal and Workspace (Table 4 Fig. 3). This difference in gaze strategy was confirmed using a three way two within, one between groups ANOVA with subject group, visual field (Goalspace/Workspace) and problem difficulty as factors. A significant three way interaction was detected, confirming that there was a significant dissociation between the two groups for the most difficult problems (group field difficulty interaction: F(4,60)=4.08, P 0.01). The abnormal gaze strategy used by PD could be explained simply by the fact that patients made more errors than controls. Similarly, any tendency for the patients to press the response key impulsively before problems had been solved could also result the observed dissociation in gaze times. In order to investigate the effect further data from all subjects were subjected to a three way ANOVA with group, response type (Error/No error) and visual field (Goalspace/Workspace) as factors. No significant interaction was found between response type and any other factor, indicating that PDs non-selectivity of gaze did not depend upon whether an error or correct response had been given (group response visual field F(1,64) =0.056). Likewise no correlation was found between total response times and the relative difference in gaze times between the two halves of the display (R 2 =0.41, F(1,5)=0.71, P=0.04), suggesting that there was no direct relationship between the two variables (Fig. 3). Table 4 Mean total gaze time per trial spent in upper and lower visual fields for each subject group and instruction condition Downstairs Upper Lower Upstairs Upper Lower Controls PD Fig. 3. Total gaze duration per trial in the Goalspace and Workspace regions of TOL problems. Control subjects show a strong increase in the time spent fixating the Workspace on more complex problems, reflecting elaboration of problem solutions. In contrast, PDs do not show any difference in the time spent fixating the two parts of the display even for the most demanding problems.

7 T.L. Hodgson et al. / Neuropsychologia 000 (2001) Table 5 Mean probability of a given saccade resulting in a lateral shift between zones in the analysis grid in each half of the display Minimum moves to solution One Two Three Four Five PDs Goalspace Workspace Controls Goalspace Workspace Saccadic shifts between grid locations Next we examined the likelihood of particular transformations in gaze position occurring between different regions of the display. For example, the probability of a given fixation being followed by another fixation at a grid location horizontally adjacent to it was calculated and analysed according to whether the two fixations occurred in the subject s Goalspace or Workspace. For control subjects, the probability of making a lateral shift between grid locations was significantly greater in their respective Workspace. This difference was most apparent for the most difficult problems (visual field problem difficulty interaction: F(1,6)=6.68, P 0.05)(Table 5). However, as with the analysis of total gaze times, the identical ANOVA for the PD group failed to show this dissociation in lateral saccadic shifts between the two halves of the display (main effect of visual field: F(1,9) =0.02) (Table 5). A similar analysis of the probability of making a vertical shift in fixation between adjacent grid locations did not show any significant changes dependent upon problem difficulty or subject group Problem dependent gaze shifts Our earlier studies showed that gaze is not distributed evenly across all the locations within the Workspace region, but is systematically biased towards problem relevant items [27]. We carried out the same analysis on the present data by comparing trials on which the left-sided balls were the most important items for problem solution, with those for which the centrally located ball was the critical move to make. For the Downstairs group, the central ball in the Workspace is always the blue ball, and consequently, these problems are often termed blue-ball problems. For four move problems, blue-ball trials always require a shunting maneuver in which the central ball is moved to a temporary location whilst one or more intervening moves are made using the other balls. Failure to realise that this is the critical maneuver leads to an impasse in which the problem cannot be solved without undoing earlier moves. A two-way ANOVA with problem type (blue/nonblue) and grid location (left/centre) as factors confirmed that the elderly control subjects displayed the expected interaction between problem type and gaze time per trial spent looking at the left and central locations within the Workspace (F(2,12)=10.69, P 0.005). Gaze was biased towards the lateral balls on non-blue ball problems and the central location on blue-ball trials. In contrast, PDs failed to show this selectivity in gaze for problem critical items. There was no significant difference in the time PDs spent fixating the different locations in the Workspace dependent upon problem type (F(2,18)=1.81, P 0.1). Once again, in order to understand this effect further we carried out a trial by trial analysis on the data from the PD group, in which correct and error response trials were separated. A three-way ANOVA with problem type, response type (error/no error) and location revealed that eye movement patterns made on correct trials differed systematically from those on incorrect response trials (problem type response type location interaction F(4,32)=3.11, P 0.05). Correct trials showed the expected dissociation between blue and non-blue ball problems, but for error trials there was no dissociation in the distribution of gaze times between the two types of problem (Fig. 4). 4. Discussion The performance of control subjects in the current study replicates our earlier findings with younger controls [27]. Efficient solution of TOL problems is associated with a characteristic pattern of eye movements, which cannot be explained purely by the features of the visual stimulus the subject is looking at. Specifically, subjects always bias their gaze towards the Workspace part of the display when planning/elaborating solutions, regardless of whether they are instructed to manipulate the balls in the upper (Upstairs group) or lower (Downstairs group) part of the picture. Clearly, it

8 8 T.L. Hodgson et al. / Neuropsychologia 000 (2001) is difficult to accommodate these results within accounts of eye movement control which emphasise the salient features of the visual image as the most important variable controlling the distribution of fixations [41,56]. Instead, particular tasks are associated with dedicated attentional sets or strategies, which direct gaze to different regions of space or stimulus features dependent upon current task goals [5,10,11,24,33,61]. As well as having implications for models of eye movement control, our observations also bear on the nature of problem solving itself. For example, it is often assumed that cognitive planning involves the construction of a detailed internal program for the entire movement sequence, which has to be encoded into memory and then later recalled to control execution of the correct solution (e.g. [21]). But recent research has suggested that this classical view of planning may be incorrect [12,52,58] and our findings also support an alternative view in which only the key features of problems are encoded during planning. For example, we did not find evidence for stereotyped sequences of eye movements corresponding directly to the rehearsal of a fully formed action sequence. However, we do find that normal subjects selectively bias gaze towards one or two problem critical locations and this selectivity is crucial for efficient performance of the task [27]. It has also been suggested that point of gaze may constitute a parsimonious deictic code for cognition [6]. Rather than constructing detailed representations of the external world, spatial co-ordinates may be used to guide re-foveation of parts of the visual scene whenever information about the location s contents are required. Similarly, for the TOL task a major component of the planning phase may involve determining the location of one or two problem critical balls. The full sequence of moves does not need to be memorised in detail during solution rehearsal because once the key location has been foveated other action systems would be directly cued into executing the appropriate behavioural sequence to solve the problem. In not so many words, we may solve problems with our eyes rather than in our heads. Although the data from control subjects have interesting implications on their own, the primary motivation of this particular study was to compare the eye movements made by older controls and Parkinson s disease patients. This comparison revealed a striking dissociation between the two groups. In contrast to control subjects, PDs show no dissociation in gaze times between the two ball arrays. Total time fixating the Workspace and Goalspace was equally distributed for all problem difficulties. Error rates were also significantly increased, but unlike earlier reports, no significant difference in response time was found between the two groups. The behavioural differences between the two subject groups are unlikely to reflect purely visual or oculomotor abnormalities in PD, as they were modulated by the complexity of the task. Simple problems were completed accurately by both groups, suggesting that patients understood the task and were quite capable of making a simple comparison between the upper and Fig. 4. Mean total gaze time spent by PDs looking in left and middle ball locations within the Workspace for different problem types on correct and error trials (four move problems only). On correct trials, more time is spent fixating balls, which are critical for solution of the current problem. Gaze is selectively biased towards the centrally located blue ball on blue ball problems and towards the left-sided balls on non-blue ball problems. In contrast, error trials fail to show any problem dependent bias in gaze duration.

9 T.L. Hodgson et al. / Neuropsychologia 000 (2001) Table 6 Relative times fixating Goalspace and Workspace for two unmedicated PD patients Goalspace Workspace Controls Elderly controls Young Controls Unmedicated PDs Patient JR Patient AN Data for elderly controls and patient JR shown for four move problems taken from the present study. Data for young controls and patient AN shown for three move problems in simpler version of task described elsewhere [28]. Both unmedicated patients show a selective increase in fixation times on the Goalspace relative to their peer group. lower arrays of balls. Neither was there a significant dissociation between groups in the relative time fixating the Goal and Workspace arrangements on simple problems. It was only for more complex problems that an abnormal distribution of eye movements became apparent. The number of errors made by PDs correlated strongly with disease severity, confirming the existence of a steady decline in cognitive ability as the disease progresses [57,40]. But what is the exact nature of this cognitive dysfunction and can eye movement data be used to throw light on this question? We originally reasoned that eye movement measures would allow us to discriminate between two potential explanations for abnormal problem solving in PD; a general slowing of mental imagery (bradyphrenia) and defective encoding/ maintenance of current goals on-line (goal-processing deficit). The data came out clearly in favour of the goal-processing hypothesis. No dissociation was observed between Goal and Workspace fixation times suggesting that PDs have difficulty encoding and/or maintaining task goals leading to increased response errors. More detailed analysis suggests that this abnormal gaze strategy, although accompanied by increased error rates, did not exclude correct planning on a subset of trials. Patients correctly directed their gaze towards the problem critical balls on correct but not incorrect response trials. In contrast to earlier investigations of cognitive planning in PD (e.g. [49]), no significant increase in total response time was observed in our PD group. Heterogeneity of cognitive deficits in the general population of PDs may have contributed to this discrepancy with earlier work, but it also raises the possibility that our patients were impulsively pressing the response key before they had completed the correct solution. This in itself would lead to both response errors and a distribution of gaze times more similar to that seen for control subjects on simpler problems (Fig. 3). However, several aspects of the data make it unlikely that errors and impulsivity directly explain abnormal gaze patterns. For example, we have recently found that a sub-set of control subjects make a large proportion of errors during the TOL [27]. However, unlike PDs, these individuals bias their gaze towards the Workspace in the same manner as efficient planners do. We also found that patients relative increase in Goalspace gaze durations did not vary significantly between error and nonerror trials. Similarly, correlation of response time with the difference in gaze times between the two halves of the display also failed to yield a significant relationship. These analyses imply that the lack of selectivity between the two display regions cannot be directly attributed to impulsivity and errors. On the contrary, it suggests that the patients have a consistent problem in encoding and maintaining task goals, which leads to premature responses and errors on some (but not all) trials. Another factor that complicates our understanding of cognitive dysfunction in PD is the potential role of medication. It is possible that some of the effects observed may be attributable to various aspects of medication as both L-Dopa [23], and scopolomine [18] have been shown to detrimentally affect performance in PD patients. While it is not possible to resolve this issue entirely, the fact that the majority of studies show improved performance in patients on L-Dopa, either compared with the off L-Dopa condition [35], or when compared with matched groups of patients who are not on L-Dopa (e.g. [44,47,49]), suggests that the deficits observed in the current study are not a direct result of dopaminergic medication. Further, all our patients showed clinical signs of Parkinson s disease (e.g. rigidity, bradykinesia) indicative of a hypo-dopaminergic state, rather than a drug induced excess of the chemical. A single patient (JR) in this study was unmedicated but also showed a lack of selectivity between fixation times between the two halves of the display (Table 6). We have also tested a young (31 years) unmedicated Parkinson s individual (patient AN) using a simpler but formally identical version of the task in which problem difficulty varies between one and three moves. Once again, in contrast to his peers, this patient showed no bias in fixation towards the Workspace region of the display (Table 6). Taken together, the evidence from earlier studies and our own investigations suggest that medication effects could not have influenced the major finding of the current study. Abnormal goal-processing in PD could itself be due to dysfunction in a number of sub-processes which are often subsumed into the concept of an executive or supervisory attentional mechanism [3,55]. This poorly defined process has been invoked to explain the control of non-routine aspects of behaviour, which are not

10 10 T.L. Hodgson et al. / Neuropsychologia 000 (2001) clearly specified by an external sensory context or earlier learnt action plans. The current findings are consistent with the view that prefrontal striatal networks play a critical role in cognitive planning and executive processes [15,37,4], but the exact components of executive control which are impaired in PD are more difficult to discriminate using the current data set. However, a number of different explanations for the patients deficit present themselves for further investigation. The simplest account of the present findings is that trans-saccadic working memory for the colour and location of balls is impaired in PD. Put simply, patients keep forgetting the arrangement of balls in the Goalspace every time they look away. Due to this, they spend proportionately less time fixating the Workspace and more time fixating the Goalspace. Alternatively, it is also possible that normal subjects use covert attention to continuously monitor the goals in peripheral vision whilst fixating the Workspace. If this were the case then the present results imply an impairment in the control of covert attention in PD. Although we cannot discriminate between these two accounts using the current data set, recent studies of a closely related block-copying task indicate that covert attention is not used to monitor goals in this manner. Instead, the colour and location of relevant items are acquired during separate fixations and are held in working memory across gaze shifts between different regions of the display [26]. More speculatively, our results are also consistent with another type of impairment in PD. Rather than a deficit in visual working memory, PDs may have difficulty with the attentional selection of different behavioural sets or schema which would normally be activated at different stages of the task. Patients may be unable to disengage from a scanning set in which the location and colour of balls are acquired, in order to switch to solution elaboration mode in which imagined representations come to the fore and gaze is directed to the Workspace [38,54,22]. PDs remain locked into a scanning mode in which goals are repeatedly assessed and proper rehearsal of problem solutions is consequently impeded. Interestingly, the ability to rapidly switch between gaze control sets would seem to be necessary for the performance of a range of complex tasks ranging from solving math problems [19] to making a cup of tea [34]. Yet to date only a very few studies have explicitly examined this aspect of eye movement control in normals and neurologically impaired populations [25,59,28]. Ultimately, only further experimentation will permit a discrimination between different explanations for the goal-processing deficit in PD. For example, the use of gaze contingent display changes could allow us to probe how much information is retained in memory between successive fixations. However, one recent theory suggests that it may not always be useful to conceive of covert attention, working memory and cognitive set as reflecting the operation of discrete and entirely dissociable sub-processes. An interesting alternative view is that they represent aspects of the same generic function subserved by prefrontal cortex, namely the top down modulation of activity within posterior cortical regions. According to this model, the prefrontal cortex maintains a constellation of synaptic facilitations, biases, associations and inhibitions which modulate an organisms behaviour by changing the neural context within which posterior sensory and motor regions operate [13,14,2,36,51,16]. Interestingly, a crucial role in this framework is hypothesised for extra-cellular dopamine in the modulation of prefrontal neuronal activity. Recent neural network simulation studies also suggest that different degrees of dopaminergic disruption within prefrontal cortex can lead to quite different behavioural symptoms, which would conventionally be interpreted as reflecting deficits in discrete cognitive functions [7]. This account predicts that the goal-processing deficit in PD occurs due to dysfunction in a range of cognitive operations, all of which have their origin in dopaminergic depletion within fronto striatal networks. 5. Summary and conclusions In summary, we have shown that as well as making increased response errors, Parkinsons patients use abnormal gaze strategies during the TOL task. Rather than directing their eyes towards the Workspace during planning as controls do, PDs divide their attention equally between the Goalspace and Workspace region of the display. The pattern is not consistent with slowed motor imagery but suggests that the planning deficit in PD is due to abnormal encoding and maintenance of current goals. This goal-processing abnormality may reflect disruption to trans-saccadic working memory or attentional control due to dopaminergic depletion within prefrontal striatal networks. 6. Uncited references [46,48]. Acknowledgements This study was supported by The Wellcome Trust Programme Grant Number: /2/98/A.

11 T.L. Hodgson et al. / Neuropsychologia 000 (2001) References [1] Alexander GE, Delong MR, Strick PL. Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience 1986;9: [2] Asaad WF, Rainer G, Miller EK. Neural activity in the primate prefrontal cortex during associative learning. Neuron 1998;21: [3] Baddeley A, Della Sala S. Working memory and executive control. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B 1996;351: [4] Baker SC, Rogers RD, Owen AM, Frith CD, Dolan RJ, Frackowiak RSJ, Robbins TW. Neural systems engaged in planning: a PET study of the Tower of London Task. Neuropsychologia 1996;34(6): [5] Ballard DH, Hayhoe MM, Pelz JB. Memory representations in natural tasks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1995;7(1): [6] Ballard DH, Hayhoe MM, Pook PK, Rao RPN. Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition. Behavioural and Brain Sciences 1997;20(4): [7] Braver TS, Barch DM, Cohen JD. Cognition and control in schizophrenia: a computational model of dopamine and prefrontal function. Biological Psychiatry 1999;46: [8] Brown RG, Marsden CD. Cognitive function in Parkinson s disease: from description to theory. Trends in Neuroscience 1990;13(1):21 8. [9] Brozoski TJ, Brown RM, Rosvold HE, Goldman PS. Cognitive deficits caused by regional depletion of dopamine in prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkey. Science 1979;205: [10] Buswell GT. How People Look at Pictures. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, [11] Carpenter PA, Just MA. Eye fixations during mental rotation. In: Senders JW, Fisher DF, Monty RA, editors. Eye movements and the higher psychological functions. Hilsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1978: [12] Clark A. Being There: Putting Brain, Body and World Together Again. Massachusetts: MIT press, [13] Cohen JD, Braver TS, O Reilly RC. A computational approach to prefrontal cortex, cognitive control and schizophrenia: recent developments and current challenges. Philoshopical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 1996;351: [14] Cohen JD, Servan-Schreiber D. Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. Psychological Review 1992;99(1): [15] Dagher A, Owen AM, Boecker H, Brooks DJ. Mapping the network for planning: a correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London task. Brain 1999;122(10): [16] Deacon T. The Symbolic Species: The Coevolution of Language and Brain. New York: Norton & Co, [17] Dominey P, Decety J, Broussolle E, Chazot G, Jeannerod M. Motor imagery of a lateralized sequential task is asymmetrically slowed in hemi-parkinsons patients. Neuropsychologia 1995;33(6): [18] Dubois B, Danze F, Pillon B, Cusimano G, Lhermitte F, Agid Y. Cholinergic-dependent cognitive deficits in Parkinson s disease. Annals of Neurology 1987;22(1): [19] Epelboim J, Suppes P. Scanpaths reveal cognitive operations used in geometry problems. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 1997;38(4):1754. [20] Members of the UPDRS Development Committee, Fahn S, Elton RL. Unified Parkinson s disease rating scale. In: Fahn S, Marsden CD, Goldstein M, Calne DB, editors. Recent developments in Parkinson s disease, vol. 2. New York: MacMillan, 1987: [21] Fikes R, Nilsson N. STRIPS: a new approach to the application of theorem proving to problem solving. Artificial Intelligence 1971;2: [22] Gauntlett-Gilbert J, Roberts RC, Brown VJ. Mechanisms underlying attentional set-shifting in Parkinson s disease. Neuropsychologia 1999;37: [23] Gotham AM, Brown RG, Marsden CD. Frontal cognitive function in patients with Parkinson s disease on and off Levodopa. Brain 1988;111(2): [24] Groner R, Walder F, Groner M. Looking at faces: Local and global aspects of scanpaths, In: Gale AG, Johnson F, editors. Theoretical and applied aspects of eye movement research. North Holland, Amsterdam, p [25] Hallet PE, Adams BD. The predictability of saccadic latency in a novel voluntary oculomotor task. Vision Research 1980;20: [26] Hayhoe MH, Bensinger DG, Ballard DH. Task constraints in visual working memory. Vision Research 1997;38(1): [27] Hodgson TL, Bajwa A, Owen AM, Kennard C. Strategic control of gaze direction during the Tower of London task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2000;12(5): [28] Hodgson TL, Chamberlain MM, Kennard C. Gaze Control, Set Switching and the Prefrontal Cerebral Cortex. New Orleans: Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, [29] Hodgson TL, Dittrich W, Henderson L, Kennard C. Eye movements and spatial working memory in Parkinson s disease. Neuropsychologia 1999;37(8): [30] Hoehn MM, Yahr MD. Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology 1967;17: [31] Kirsh D, Maglio P. On distinguishing between epistemic and pragmatic action. Cognitive Science 1994;18: [32] Kish SJ, Shannak K, Hornykiewicz O. Uneven patterns of dopamine loss in the striatum of patients with idiopathic Parkinson s disease. Pathophysiologic and clinical implications. New England Journal of Medicine 1988;318: [33] Land MF, Furneax S. The knowledge base of the oculomotor system. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 1997;352: [34] Land MF, Mennie N, Rusted J. The roles of vision and eye movements in the control of activities of daily living. Perception 1999;28: [35] Lange KW, Robbins TW, Marsden CD, James M, Owen AM, Paul GM. L-dopa withdrawal in Parkinson s disease selectively impairs cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal-lobe dysfunction. Psychopharmacology 1992;107(2-3): [36] Miller EK. The prefrontal cortex: complex neural properties for complex behavior. Neuron 1999;22:15 7. [37] Morris RG, Ahmed S, Syed GM, Toone BK. Neural correlates of planning ability: frontal lobe activation in the Tower of London test. Neuropsychologia 1993;31: [38] Morris RG, Downes JJ, Sahakian BJ, Polkey CE, Robbins TW. Planning and spatial working memory in Parkinson s disease. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 1988;51: [39] Morris RG, Rushe T, Woodruff PWR, Murray RM. Problemsolving in schizophrenia-a specific deficit in planning ability. Schizophrenia Research 1995;14(3): [40] Mortimer JA, Pirrozzolo FJ, Hansch EC, Webster DD. Relationship of motor symptoms to intellectual deficits in Parkinson s disease. Neuroloy 1982;32: [41] Noton D, Stark LW. Scanpaths in saccadic eye movements while viewing and recognizing patterns. Vision Research 1971;11: [42] Owen AM. Unpublished Ph. D. thesis. University of London [43] Owen AM. Cognitive planning in humans: neuropsychological, neuroanatomical and neuropharcological perspectives. Progress in Neurobiology 1997;53:

12 12 T.L. Hodgson et al. / Neuropsychologia 000 (2001) [44] Owen AM, Beksinska M, James M, Leigh PN, Summers BA, Marsden CD, Quinn NP, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Visuospatial memory deficits at different stages of Parkinson s disease. Neuropsychologia 1993;31(7): [45] Owen AM, Downes JD, Sahakian BJ, Polkey CE, Robbins TW. Planning and spatial working memory deficits following frontal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia 1990;28: [46] Owen AM, Doyon J, Petrides M, Evans AC. Planning and spatial working memory examined with positron emission tomography (PET). European Journal of Neuroscience 1996; 8: [47] Owen AM, James M, Leigh PN, Summers BA, Marsden CD, Quinn NP, Lange KW, Robbins TW. Fronto striatal cognitive deficits at different stages of Parkinson s disease. Brain 1992;115(6): [48] Owen AM, Roberts AC, Hodges JR, Summers BA, Polkey CE, Robbins TW. Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal-lobe damage or Parkinson s disease. Brain 1993;116(5): [49] Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, Hodges JR, Summers BA, Polkey CE, Robbins TW. Dopamine-dependent fronto striatal planning deficits in early Parkinson s disease. Neuropsychology 1995;9: [50] Phillips ML, David AS. Visual scan paths are abnormal in deluded schizophrenics. Neuropsychologia 1997;15(1): [51] Phillips WA, Singer W. In search of common foundations for cortical computation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1997; 20(4): [52] Polson P, Jeffries R. Problem solving as search and understanding. In: Sternberg RJ, editor. Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence, vol. 1. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc, [53] Postle BR, Jonides J, Smith EE, Corkin S, Growdon JH. Spatial, but not object delayed response is impaired in early Parkinson s disease. Neuropsychology 1997;11(2): [54] Roberts AC, Robins TW, Everitt BJ. The effects of intradimensional and extradimensional shifts on visual discimination learning in humans and non-human primates. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B 1988;40: [55] Shallice T. Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 1982;298: [56] Stark LW, Ellis SR. Scanpath revisted: cognitive models direct active looking. In: Fisher DF, Monty RA, Senders JW, editors. Eye Movements: Cognition and Visual Perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1981: [57] Taylor AE, Saint-Cyr JS, Lange AE. Frontal lobe dysfunction in Parkinson s disease. Brain 1986;76: [58] Ward G, Allport A. Planning and problem-solving using the five-disc Tower of London task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A 1997;50(1): [59] Weber H. Presaccadic processes in the generation of pro and anti saccades in human subjects a reaction-time study. Perception 1995;25: [60] Webster DD. Critical analysis of the disability in Parkinson s disease. Modern Treatment 1968;5: [61] Yarbus A. Movements of the eyes. New York: Plenum,

Prefrontal cortex. Executive functions. Models of prefrontal cortex function. Overview of Lecture. Executive Functions. Prefrontal cortex (PFC)

Prefrontal cortex. Executive functions. Models of prefrontal cortex function. Overview of Lecture. Executive Functions. Prefrontal cortex (PFC) Neural Computation Overview of Lecture Models of prefrontal cortex function Dr. Sam Gilbert Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience University College London E-mail: sam.gilbert@ucl.ac.uk Prefrontal cortex

More information

Visual Selection and Attention

Visual Selection and Attention Visual Selection and Attention Retrieve Information Select what to observe No time to focus on every object Overt Selections Performed by eye movements Covert Selections Performed by visual attention 2

More information

Original Articles. Calne, resting tremor. Mortimer, Pirozzolo, Hansch, & Webster, postural disturbance III

Original Articles. Calne, resting tremor. Mortimer, Pirozzolo, Hansch, & Webster, postural disturbance III 2004 97-106 Original Articles 1 2 3 1 1 2 3 47 22 III I II muscular rigidity postural disturbance resting tremor bradykinesia Calne, 2001 Mortimer, Pirozzolo, Hansch, & Webster, 1982 Tel: 02-23627076 E-mail:

More information

FRONTO-STRIATAL COGNITIVE DEFICITS AT DIFFERENT STAGES OF PARKINSON'S DISEASE

FRONTO-STRIATAL COGNITIVE DEFICITS AT DIFFERENT STAGES OF PARKINSON'S DISEASE Brain (1992). 115, 1727-1751 FRONTO-STRIATAL COGNITIVE DEFICITS AT DIFFERENT STAGES OF PARKINSON'S DISEASE by A. M. OWEN,' M. JAMES, 2 P. N. LEIGH, 3 B. A. SUMMERS, 3 C. D. MARSDEN, 4 N. P. QUINN, 4 K.

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

Theoretical Neuroscience: The Binding Problem Jan Scholz, , University of Osnabrück

Theoretical Neuroscience: The Binding Problem Jan Scholz, , University of Osnabrück The Binding Problem This lecture is based on following articles: Adina L. Roskies: The Binding Problem; Neuron 1999 24: 7 Charles M. Gray: The Temporal Correlation Hypothesis of Visual Feature Integration:

More information

Working memory in medicated patients with Parkinson's disease: the central executive seems to work

Working memory in medicated patients with Parkinson's disease: the central executive seems to work J7ournal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 1996;60:313-317 Working memory in medicated patients with Parkinson's disease: the central executive seems to work Nathalie Fournet, Olivier Moreaud,

More information

Planning in Parkinson s disease: A matter of problem structure?

Planning in Parkinson s disease: A matter of problem structure? Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) 384 389 Note Planning in Parkinson s disease: A matter of problem structure? A. McKinlay a,,1, C.P. Kaller b,,1, R.C. Grace a, J.C. Dalrymple-Alford a,d, T.J. Anderson c,d, J.

More information

Dopaminergic basis for deficits in working memory but not attentional set-shifting in Parkinson s disease

Dopaminergic basis for deficits in working memory but not attentional set-shifting in Parkinson s disease Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 823 832 Dopaminergic basis for deficits in working memory but not attentional set-shifting in Parkinson s disease Simon J.G. Lewis a,b,, Aleksandra Slabosz c,e, Trevor W. Robbins

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues Edward E. Smith Columbia University Outline Background Maintenance: Modality specificity and buffers Interference resolution: Distraction and

More information

Cognitive deficits in progressive supranuclear

Cognitive deficits in progressive supranuclear J7ournal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 1994;57:79-88 ognitive deficits in progressive supranuclear palsy, Parkinson's disease, and multiple system atrophy in tests sensitive to frontal lobe

More information

L-Dopa withdrawal in Parkinson's disease selectively impairs cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction

L-Dopa withdrawal in Parkinson's disease selectively impairs cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction Psychopharmacology (1992) 17:394M4 Psychopharmacology (~) Springer-Verlag 1992 L-Dopa withdrawal in Parkinson's disease selectively impairs cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction

More information

Dissociable effects of dopaminergic therapy on spatial versus non-spatial working memory in Parkinson s disease

Dissociable effects of dopaminergic therapy on spatial versus non-spatial working memory in Parkinson s disease Neuropsychologia 41 (2003) 1442 1451 Dissociable effects of dopaminergic therapy on spatial versus non-spatial working memory in Parkinson s disease H. Mollion a, J. Ventre-Dominey a,, P.F. Dominey a,

More information

Biological Risk Factors

Biological Risk Factors Biological Risk Factors Ms Angelina Crea Provisional Psychologist Academic Child Psychiatry Unit Royal Children s Hospital Professor Alasdair Vance Head Academic Child Psychiatry Department of Paediatrics

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

A Computational Model of Prefrontal Cortex Function

A Computational Model of Prefrontal Cortex Function A Computational Model of Prefrontal Cortex Function Todd S. Braver Dept. of Psychology Carnegie Mellon Univ. Pittsburgh, PA 15213 Jonathan D. Cohen Dept. of Psychology Carnegie Mellon Univ. Pittsburgh,

More information

11/14/2017 SUPPORT FOR A MULTI STORE MODEL TEMPORARY MEMORY: SHORT-TERM AND WORKING MEMORY INFORMATION PROCESSING MODEL & THE MODAL MODEL OF MEMORY

11/14/2017 SUPPORT FOR A MULTI STORE MODEL TEMPORARY MEMORY: SHORT-TERM AND WORKING MEMORY INFORMATION PROCESSING MODEL & THE MODAL MODEL OF MEMORY SUPPORT FOR A MULTI STORE MODEL Distinctions between STM and LTM Behavior Biological Neurological TEMPORARY MEMORY: SHORT-TERM AND WORKING MEMORY Learning & Memory Arlo Clark-Foos, Ph.D. Ebbinghaus no

More information

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch.

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch. The Frontal Lobes Readings: KW Ch. 16 Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions E.L. Highly organized college professor Became disorganized, showed little emotion, and began to miss deadlines Scores on intelligence

More information

Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction

Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction Marc Pomplun and Sindhura Sunkara Department of Computer Science, University of Massachusetts at Boston 100 Morrissey

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Gaze Strategies During Planning in First-Episode Psychosis

Gaze Strategies During Planning in First-Episode Psychosis This article, manuscript, or document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association (APA). For non-commercial, education and research purposes, users may access, download, copy, display, and

More information

Characterizing Visual Attention during Driving and Non-driving Hazard Perception Tasks in a Simulated Environment

Characterizing Visual Attention during Driving and Non-driving Hazard Perception Tasks in a Simulated Environment Title: Authors: Characterizing Visual Attention during Driving and Non-driving Hazard Perception Tasks in a Simulated Environment Mackenzie, A.K. Harris, J.M. Journal: ACM Digital Library, (ETRA '14 Proceedings

More information

Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making

Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making Jan M. Wiener (jwiener@bournemouth.ac.uk) Department of Psychology, Bournemouth University Poole, BH12 5BB, UK Olivier De

More information

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization 1 7.1 Overview This chapter aims to provide a framework for modeling cognitive phenomena based

More information

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Intro Examine attention response functions Compare an attention-demanding

More information

Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning

Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning GSTF Journal of Psychology (JPsych) Vol. No., August 5 Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning Hironori Oto 79 Received 6 Jul 5 Accepted 9 Aug 5 Abstract This paper

More information

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2001, 61: 175-179 Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions El bieta Szel¹g 1, Krystyna Rymarczyk 1 and Ernst Pöppel 2 1 Department

More information

The Role of Learned Irrelevance in Attentional Set-Shifting Impairments in Parkinson s Disease

The Role of Learned Irrelevance in Attentional Set-Shifting Impairments in Parkinson s Disease Neuropsychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 20, No. 5, 578 588 0894-4105/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.5.578 The Role of Learned Irrelevance in Attentional

More information

3. Title: Within Fluid Cognition: Fluid Processing and Fluid Storage?

3. Title: Within Fluid Cognition: Fluid Processing and Fluid Storage? Cowan commentary on Blair, Page 1 1. Commentary on Clancy Blair target article 2. Word counts: Abstract 62 Main text 1,066 References 487 (435 excluding 2 references already in the target article) Total

More information

The number line effect reflects top-down control

The number line effect reflects top-down control Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),862-868???-??? The number line effect reflects top-down control JELENA RISTIC University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 11: Attention & Decision making Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis

More information

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2009, 16 (1), 57-61 doi:10.3758/pbr.16.1.57 The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words DANIEL SMILEK, GRAYDEN J. F. SOLMAN, PETER MURAWSKI, AND

More information

Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients

Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients Anna SORKIN¹, Avi PELED 2, Daphna WEINSHALL¹ 1 Interdisciplinary Center for Neural Computation, Hebrew University of Jerusalem,

More information

TREATMENT-SPECIFIC ABNORMAL SYNAPTIC PLASTICITY IN EARLY PARKINSON S DISEASE

TREATMENT-SPECIFIC ABNORMAL SYNAPTIC PLASTICITY IN EARLY PARKINSON S DISEASE TREATMENT-SPECIFIC ABNORMAL SYNAPTIC PLASTICITY IN EARLY PARKINSON S DISEASE Angel Lago-Rodriguez 1, Binith Cheeran 2 and Miguel Fernández-Del-Olmo 3 1. Prism Lab, Behavioural Brain Sciences, School of

More information

CANTAB Test descriptions by function

CANTAB Test descriptions by function CANTAB Test descriptions by function The 22 tests in the CANTAB battery may be divided into the following main types of task: screening tests visual memory tests executive function, working memory and

More information

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems Compare Chap 31 of Purves et al., 5e Chap 24 of Bear et al., 3e Larry Wittie Computer Science, StonyBrook University http://www.cs.sunysb.edu/~cse511

More information

Working Memory Functioning in Medicated Parkinson's Disease Patients and the Effect of Withdrawal of Dopaminergic Medication

Working Memory Functioning in Medicated Parkinson's Disease Patients and the Effect of Withdrawal of Dopaminergic Medication Neuropsychology Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2000, Vol. 14, No. 2, 247-253 0894-4105/00/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0894-4105.14.2.247 Working Memory Functioning in Medicated

More information

Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory

Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory Eiichi Hoshino 1,2, Ken Mogi 2, 1 Tokyo Institute of Technology, Department of Computational Intelligence and Systems Science. 4259

More information

Human Learning of Contextual Priors for Object Search: Where does the time go?

Human Learning of Contextual Priors for Object Search: Where does the time go? Human Learning of Contextual Priors for Object Search: Where does the time go? Barbara Hidalgo-Sotelo, Aude Oliva, Antonio Torralba Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and CSAIL, MIT MIT, Cambridge,

More information

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson *, TW Robbins, A Dickinson, BJ Everitt Departments of Experimental

More information

Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information:

Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information: EVALUATION OF THE MODAL MODEL OF MEMORY Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information: bamponsah@ug.edu.gh College of Education School of Continuing and Distance Education

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Kinematic Modeling in Parkinson s Disease

Kinematic Modeling in Parkinson s Disease Kinematic Modeling in Parkinson s Disease Alexander Hui Department of Bioengineering University of California, San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093 alexhui@ucsd.edu Abstract Parkinson s disease is a slowly progressing

More information

Task Specificity and the Influence of Memory on Visual Search: Comment on Võ and Wolfe (2012)

Task Specificity and the Influence of Memory on Visual Search: Comment on Võ and Wolfe (2012) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2012, Vol. 38, No. 6, 1596 1603 2012 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0030237 COMMENTARY Task Specificity

More information

Cue Saliency and Age as Factors Affecting Performance in a Card Sorting Task

Cue Saliency and Age as Factors Affecting Performance in a Card Sorting Task The Huron University College Journal of Learning and Motivation Volume 52 Issue 1 Article 6 2014 Cue Saliency and Age as Factors Affecting Performance in a Card Sorting Task Rebecca Herbert Huron University

More information

Right lateral prefrontal cortex Specificity for inhibition or strategy use?

Right lateral prefrontal cortex Specificity for inhibition or strategy use? Right lateral prefrontal cortex Specificity for inhibition or strategy use? M. Hornberger 1 & M. Bertoux 1 1 Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK The specific functions

More information

Rapid communication Integrating working memory capacity and context-processing views of cognitive control

Rapid communication Integrating working memory capacity and context-processing views of cognitive control THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2011, 64 (6), 1048 1055 Rapid communication Integrating working memory capacity and context-processing views of cognitive control Thomas S. Redick and Randall

More information

Behavioural Correlates of Cognitive Skill Learning in Parkinson s Disease

Behavioural Correlates of Cognitive Skill Learning in Parkinson s Disease The Open Behavioral Science Journal, 2008, 2, 1-12 1 Behavioural Correlates of Cognitive Skill Learning in Parkinson s Disease M.H. Beauchamp 1, A. Dagher 2, M. Panisset 2,3 and J. Doyon *,1,2,4 1 Université

More information

Basal ganglia motor circuit

Basal ganglia motor circuit Parkinson s Disease Basal ganglia motor circuit 1 Direct pathway (gas pedal) 2 Indirect pathway (brake) To release or augment the tonic inhibition of GPi on thalamus Direct pathway There is a tonic inhibition

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

Time Experiencing by Robotic Agents

Time Experiencing by Robotic Agents Time Experiencing by Robotic Agents Michail Maniadakis 1 and Marc Wittmann 2 and Panos Trahanias 1 1- Foundation for Research and Technology - Hellas, ICS, Greece 2- Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology

More information

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544 Frank Tong Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544 Office: Room 3-N-2B Telephone: 609-258-2652 Fax: 609-258-1113 Email: ftong@princeton.edu Graduate School Applicants

More information

Modeling of Hippocampal Behavior

Modeling of Hippocampal Behavior Modeling of Hippocampal Behavior Diana Ponce-Morado, Venmathi Gunasekaran and Varsha Vijayan Abstract The hippocampus is identified as an important structure in the cerebral cortex of mammals for forming

More information

Toward A Cognitive Computer Vision System

Toward A Cognitive Computer Vision System Toward A Cognitive Computer Vision System D. Paul Benjamin Pace University, 1 Pace Plaza, New York, New York 10038, 212-346-1012 benjamin@pace.edu Damian Lyons Fordham University, 340 JMH, 441 E. Fordham

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article Research Article AMNESIA IS A DEFICIT IN RELATIONAL MEMORY Jennifer D. Ryan, Robert R. Althoff, Stephen Whitlow, and Neal J. Cohen University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Abstract Eye movements were

More information

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set In press, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set Timothy J. Wright 1, Walter

More information

Using Diverse Cognitive Mechanisms for Action Modeling

Using Diverse Cognitive Mechanisms for Action Modeling Using Diverse Cognitive Mechanisms for Action Modeling John E. Laird (laird@umich.edu) Joseph Z. Xu (jzxu@umich.edu) Samuel Wintermute (swinterm@umich.edu) University of Michigan, 2260 Hayward Street Ann

More information

Capacity Limits in Mechanical Reasoning

Capacity Limits in Mechanical Reasoning Capacity Limits in Mechanical Reasoning Mary Hegarty Department of Psychology University of California, Santa Barbara Santa Barbara, CA 93106 hegarty@psych.ucsb.edu Abstract This paper examines capacity

More information

Cognitive-Motor Interference in Persons with Parkinson Disease

Cognitive-Motor Interference in Persons with Parkinson Disease Cognitive-Motor Interference in Persons with Parkinson Disease Tara L. McIsaac, PhD, PT Associate Professor of Physical Therapy A.T. Still University Arizona School of Health Sciences October 11, 2014

More information

Grounded Cognition. Lawrence W. Barsalou

Grounded Cognition. Lawrence W. Barsalou Grounded Cognition Lawrence W. Barsalou Department of Psychology Emory University July 2008 Grounded Cognition 1 Definition of grounded cognition the core representations in cognition are not: amodal symbols

More information

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 11 18 The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Abstract Ralph M. Reitan, Deborah Wolfson Reitan Neuropsychology

More information

Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal

Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 04/24/2018: Lecture 05-2 Note: This Powerpoint presentation

More information

Brain Regions Associated with the Cambridge Brain Sciences Tests

Brain Regions Associated with the Cambridge Brain Sciences Tests Brain Regions Associated with the Cambridge Brain Sciences Tests CAMBRIDGE BRAIN SCIENCES A. Document Overview B. Brain Networks Behind the Cambridge Brain Sciences Tests C. Summary Table of the Brain

More information

Human Learning of Contextual Priors for Object Search: Where does the time go?

Human Learning of Contextual Priors for Object Search: Where does the time go? Human Learning of Contextual Priors for Object Search: Where does the time go? Barbara Hidalgo-Sotelo 1 Aude Oliva 1 Antonio Torralba 2 1 Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, 2 Computer Science

More information

Revealing The Brain s Hidden Potential: Cognitive Training & Neurocognitive Plasticity. Introduction

Revealing The Brain s Hidden Potential: Cognitive Training & Neurocognitive Plasticity. Introduction Revealing The Brain s Hidden Potential: Cognitive Training & Neurocognitive Plasticity. Introduction Global aging poses significant burdens as age-related impairments in cognitive function affect quality

More information

Replacing the frontal lobes? Having more time to think improve implicit perceptual categorization. A comment on Filoteo, Lauritzen & Maddox, 2010.

Replacing the frontal lobes? Having more time to think improve implicit perceptual categorization. A comment on Filoteo, Lauritzen & Maddox, 2010. Replacing the frontal lobes? 1 Replacing the frontal lobes? Having more time to think improve implicit perceptual categorization. A comment on Filoteo, Lauritzen & Maddox, 2010. Ben R. Newell 1 Christopher

More information

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective MILITARY PSYCHOLOGY, 21:(Suppl. 1)S108 S112, 2009 Copyright Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 0899-5605 print / 1532-7876 online DOI: 10.1080/08995600802554748 Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future:

More information

The Relation Between Perception and Action: What Should Neuroscience Learn From Psychology?

The Relation Between Perception and Action: What Should Neuroscience Learn From Psychology? ECOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY, 13(2), 117 122 Copyright 2001, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. The Relation Between Perception and Action: What Should Neuroscience Learn From Psychology? Patrick R. Green Department

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

(In)Attention and Visual Awareness IAT814

(In)Attention and Visual Awareness IAT814 (In)Attention and Visual Awareness IAT814 Week 5 Lecture B 8.10.2009 Lyn Bartram lyn@sfu.ca SCHOOL OF INTERACTIVE ARTS + TECHNOLOGY [SIAT] WWW.SIAT.SFU.CA This is a useful topic Understand why you can

More information

Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory

Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory Limits to Iconic Memory 0 Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory Ronald A. Rensink Departments of Psychology and Computer Science University of British Columbia Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4 Canada Running Head: Limits

More information

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making Matthew A J Apps 1,2, Laura Grima 2, Sanjay Manohar 2, Masud Husain 1,2 1 Nuffield Department of Clinical Neuroscience,

More information

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 3, 316-320 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0096-3445/94/S3.00 COMMENT Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention:

More information

Study of Verbal Working Memory in Patients with Parkinson s Disease

Study of Verbal Working Memory in Patients with Parkinson s Disease Study of Verbal Working Memory in Patients with Parkinson s Disease Gilbert, Belleville, Bherer, & Chouinard, 2005 presented by Jonathan Reinharth May 2, 2005 Parkinson s Disease Symptoms: resting tremors,

More information

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Uri Hasson (uhasson@princeton.edu) Psychology Department, Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08540 USA Geoffrey P. Goodwin (ggoodwin@princeton.edu)

More information

Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex. 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations. 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage

Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex. 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations. 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage Sensory to Motor Transformation Sensory information (visual,

More information

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System Christopher J. Rozell, Don. H. Johnson and Raymon M. Glantz Department of Electrical & Computer Engineering Department of

More information

GBME graduate course. Chapter 43. The Basal Ganglia

GBME graduate course. Chapter 43. The Basal Ganglia GBME graduate course Chapter 43. The Basal Ganglia Basal ganglia in history Parkinson s disease Huntington s disease Parkinson s disease 1817 Parkinson's disease (PD) is a degenerative disorder of the

More information

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL for Cardoso et al. 22 The Neuroimaging Signal is a Linear Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. : Appendix: Homogeneous Linear ( Null ) and Modified Linear

More information

Nature, prevalence and clinical significance. Barcelona, Spain

Nature, prevalence and clinical significance. Barcelona, Spain Nature, prevalence and clinical significance Jaime Kulisevsky Barcelona, Spain 1 Non motor (neuropsychiatric) symptoms are an integral part of Parkinson s s disease (PD) Affective disorders And are associated

More information

How to Build a Cognitive Task

How to Build a Cognitive Task Clinically-Relevant How to Build a Cognitive Task Jonathan D. Cohen Princeton Neuroscience Institute Psychology Department Princeton University Department of Psychiatry University of Pittsburgh Example

More information

Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation

Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (1), 154-158 Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation JOO-SEOK HYUN AND STEVEN J. LUCK University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa In mental rotation, a

More information

Problemas conceptuales de la memoria del trabajo

Problemas conceptuales de la memoria del trabajo Resumen 23 24 25 26 Atkinson, R. Y Shiffring, R. (1968) Human Memory. A Proposed System And Its Control Process. En Spence (Ed) The Psychology Of Learning And Motivation: Advances In Research And Theory.

More information

Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk

Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk Exp Brain Res (1998) 123:346±350 Springer-Verlag 1998 RESEARCH NOTE Jinsung Wang George E. Stelmach Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk Received: 1 April

More information

The Neurobiology of Attention

The Neurobiology of Attention The Neurobiology of Attention by Nadia Fike, MD/PhD Pediatric Neurology Center for Neurosciences Disclosures Nadia Fike, MD/PhD No relevant financial or nonfinancial relationships to disclose. Objectives

More information

IAT 814 Knowledge Visualization. Visual Attention. Lyn Bartram

IAT 814 Knowledge Visualization. Visual Attention. Lyn Bartram IAT 814 Knowledge Visualization Visual Attention Lyn Bartram Why we care in an information-rich world, the wealth of information means a dearth of something else: a scarcity of whatever it is that information

More information

Study Guide for Test 3 Motor Control

Study Guide for Test 3 Motor Control Study Guide for Test 3 Motor Control These chapters come from Magill s (2010), Motor Learning and Control: Concepts and Application(9 th ) edition textbook. Additional materials regarding these key concepts

More information

Rules of apparent motion: The shortest-path constraint: objects will take the shortest path between flashed positions.

Rules of apparent motion: The shortest-path constraint: objects will take the shortest path between flashed positions. Rules of apparent motion: The shortest-path constraint: objects will take the shortest path between flashed positions. The box interrupts the apparent motion. The box interrupts the apparent motion.

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience History of Neural Networks in Artificial Intelligence The concept of neural network in artificial intelligence

Cognitive Neuroscience History of Neural Networks in Artificial Intelligence The concept of neural network in artificial intelligence Cognitive Neuroscience History of Neural Networks in Artificial Intelligence The concept of neural network in artificial intelligence To understand the network paradigm also requires examining the history

More information

The Adolescent Developmental Stage

The Adolescent Developmental Stage The Adolescent Developmental Stage o Physical maturation o Drive for independence o Increased salience of social and peer interactions o Brain development o Inflection in risky behaviors including experimentation

More information

Dr. Mark Ashton Smith, Department of Psychology, Bilkent University

Dr. Mark Ashton Smith, Department of Psychology, Bilkent University UMAN CONSCIOUSNESS some leads based on findings in neuropsychology Dr. Mark Ashton Smith, Department of Psychology, Bilkent University nattentional Blindness Simons and Levin, 1998 Not Detected Detected

More information

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),???-??? 794-799 What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? ULRICH MAYR University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon Schneider and

More information

Prism adaptation in Parkinson's disease

Prism adaptation in Parkinson's disease Reprinted from Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 1988;51: 1584-1587 Copyright 1988 Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry All rights of reproduction of this reprint are reserved

More information

A FIELD STUDY ASSESSING DRIVING PERFORMANCE, VISUAL ATTENTION, HEART RATE AND SUBJECTIVE RATINGS IN RESPONSE TO TWO TYPES OF COGNITIVE WORKLOAD

A FIELD STUDY ASSESSING DRIVING PERFORMANCE, VISUAL ATTENTION, HEART RATE AND SUBJECTIVE RATINGS IN RESPONSE TO TWO TYPES OF COGNITIVE WORKLOAD A FIELD STUDY ASSESSING DRIVING PERFORMANCE, VISUAL ATTENTION, HEART RATE AND SUBJECTIVE RATINGS IN RESPONSE TO TWO TYPES OF COGNITIVE WORKLOAD Yan Yang, Bryan Reimer, Bruce Mehler & Jonathan Dobres The

More information

ADAPTING COPYCAT TO CONTEXT-DEPENDENT VISUAL OBJECT RECOGNITION

ADAPTING COPYCAT TO CONTEXT-DEPENDENT VISUAL OBJECT RECOGNITION ADAPTING COPYCAT TO CONTEXT-DEPENDENT VISUAL OBJECT RECOGNITION SCOTT BOLLAND Department of Computer Science and Electrical Engineering The University of Queensland Brisbane, Queensland 4072 Australia

More information

Choosing the Greater of Two Goods: Neural Currencies for Valuation and Decision Making

Choosing the Greater of Two Goods: Neural Currencies for Valuation and Decision Making Choosing the Greater of Two Goods: Neural Currencies for Valuation and Decision Making Leo P. Surgre, Gres S. Corrado and William T. Newsome Presenter: He Crane Huang 04/20/2010 Outline Studies on neural

More information

The Effect of Weighted Compression Sleeves on Tremor Suppression in Individuals with Parkinson s Disease

The Effect of Weighted Compression Sleeves on Tremor Suppression in Individuals with Parkinson s Disease The Effect of Weighted Compression Sleeves on Tremor Suppression in Individuals with Parkinson s Disease Jennifer Iannello, Garry Johnson, Lauren Pilla Parkinson s Disease Second most common chronic, progressive,

More information