Genetics as a tool for the dissociation of mental operations over the course of development

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Genetics as a tool for the dissociation of mental operations over the course of development"

Transcription

1 Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. ISSN ANNALS OF THE NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES Issue: The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience over the course of development John A. Fossella, Kevin Guise, and Jin Fan Department of Psychiatry, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York, New York Address for correspondance: John A. Fossella, Department of Psychiatry, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, One Gustave L. Levy Place, New York, NY In recent years it has become possible to differentiate separable aspects of attention and to characterize the anatomical structure and dynamic states of their underlying networks. When individual differences in the structure and dynamics of these networks are used as dependent measures in associations with individual genetic variation, it becomes possible to assign cellular and molecular changes that occur over the course of normal development to specific aspects of network structure and function. In this way, a more granular understanding of the physiology of neural networks can be obtained. Here we review a translational research strategy focused on how genetic variation contributes to the normal development of attentional function. We seek to use genetic information to help construct a multinode, multinetwork model that can explain, in part, individual differences in the development of attention over the course of development. Keywords: genetics; brain; development; cognition; dopamine; cingulate Introduction A role for genetics in mental function has been noted since the earliest days of the modern synthesis of Mendelian and population genetics. 1 The remarkable patterns of behavior observed in identical twins who may have been separated at birth as well as the alterations of behavioral development associated chromosomal anomalies show that the genetic material, in addition to environmental forces, is important in shaping mental function. In more recent years, the advancement of gene mapping methodologies has led to the identification of specific genes that account for risk for a variety of forms of mental disability that are both contiguous with healthy mental function such as addiction, 2,3 ADHD, 4 and anxiety 5 as well as disabilities that fall outside the normal distribution such as autism, 6 schizophrenia, 7 and Williams syndrome 8 to cite but a few. The now widespread joint application of genetic and neuroimaging methods has provided a deeper understanding of how genetic risk influences the structure and function of certain neural networks. For example, a recent imaging-genetic study on a T-to-C nucleotide base change in the schizophrenia risk-factor neuregulin 1 gene, found that individuals who carry the TT genotype, when asked to perform a working memory task, showed less brain activity in the frontal cortex than individuals with the CC genotype. 9 This study is notable for providing a translational bridge that connects the genetic risk for schizophrenia with activation of an anatomical network that is known to function abnormally in patients. The findings also reinforce the notion that normal executive control and complex psychopathology can have deep developmental roots. This is because neuregulin has many functions in the developing CNS, which may be related to early prodromal markers of schizophrenia, particularly in executive control. 10 Other similar translational research studies for disorders such as autism, 6 addiction 11 that use such a cognitive-neuro-genetic and imaging-genetic approach are now increasingly common in the literature. These efforts also reveal a rather productive co-application of two historically separate research traditions in psychiatry a molecular genetic approach that seeks to ascertain the genetic origins of mental illness and a cognitive doi: /j x 110 Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci (2010) c 2010 New York Academy of Sciences.

2 neuroscience approach that seeks to understand human behavior in terms of specific neural systems and component neural mechanisms. Because both research methods seek to dissociate and dissect mental function along the lines of naturally occurring biological processes (i.e., to cut nature at its joints ) there has been remarkable synergy in the joint application of both cognitive and genetic methods in the area of psychopathology. As such joint findings continue to emerge along the lines of the major psychopathologies, it becomes increasingly possible to ask what role do genes (that carry risk for mental illness) play in healthy cognitive function. Indeed, this question may be of some relevance, especially in research seeking to ameliorate and/or remediate cognitive disability in patient populations. To this end, there may be some value in understanding how genetics can be used to gain insight into the development of healthy mental function. From the perspective of cognitive science, much is already known about neural systems and neural mechanisms that carry out healthy cognitive function in adults. What can the introduction of genetic methods offer to this already robust research approach? In this review, we suggest that the inherently biological nature of genetic information is useful to further the longstanding aims of cognitive neuroscience that is, to explain human behavior in terms of neural systems and component neural mechanisms. In particular, we emphasize the value of imaging-genetic approaches that synthesize anatomical, physiological, and molecular information within the constraints of mechanistic models of neural network dynamics. Our focus has been on a single area of cognition attention and we have sought to apply a genetic approach to an existing and longstanding program of research in the area of attention led by Michael I. Posner, who has been our mentor and collaborator for more than a decade. Genetic dissection of attention networks in healthy adult populations The ability to focus and shift one s attention is an important aspect of healthy psychological function. In recent decades, this ability has begun to be understood in terms of anatomical and physiological changes in specific neural networks that mature over the course of human development. One method that has been critical to this endeavor has been neuroimaging, which has allowed many mental operations to be analyzed in terms of the brain areas they activate. Studies of attention have been among the most commonly examined by neuroimaging and such research has supported the presence of three networks related to different aspects of attention. Thesenetworkscarryoutthefunctionsofalerting, orienting and executive control In brief, alerting is defined as achieving and maintaining a state of high sensitivity to incoming stimuli; orienting is the selection of information from sensory input; and executive control, a widely used term with many definitions, involves mechanisms for resolving conflict among thoughts, feelings, and responses. Some of the underlying circuitry in the case of alerting are networks that contain frontal, parietal and thalamic regions whose function can be probed by the use warning of signals prior to targets in an functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) setting. 15 The influence of warning signals on the level of alertness is also thought to be due to modulation of neural activity by the norepinepherine system. 14 The orienting of attention involves aligning attention with a source of sensory signals. This may be overt as in eye movements or may occur covertly without any movement. The orienting system for visual events has been associated with posterior brain areas including the superior parietal lobe and temporal parietal junction and, in addition, the frontal eye fields. Orienting can be manipulated by presenting a cue indicating where in space a person should attend, thereby directing attention to the cued location. 16 Event related fmri studies have suggested that the superior parietal lobe is associated with orienting following the presentation of a cue. 15,17 The superior parietal lobe in humans is closely related to the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) in monkeys, which is known to produce eye movements. 18 When a target occurs at an uncued location and attention has to be disengaged and moved to a new location, there is activity in the temporal parietal junction. 17 Lesions of the parietal lobe and superior temporal lobe have been consistently related to difficulties in orienting. 19 Executive control of attention is often studied by tasks that involve stimulus response conflict, such as in various versions of the Stroop task. 20 In the Stroop task subjects must respond to the color of Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci (2010) c 2010 New York Academy of Sciences. 111

3 ink (e.g., red) whereas ignoring the color word name (e.g., b-l-u-e). Resolving conflict in the Stroop task activates midline frontal areas such as the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and lateral prefrontal cortex. 21,22 There is evidence for the activation of this network in tasks involving conflict between central targets and surrounding flankers that may be congruent or incongruent with the target. Experimental tasks may provide a means of fractionating the functional contributions of different areas within the executive attention network. 23 Reliability and heritability of measures of attention As mental operations, such as alerting, orienting and executive processes are dissociated using behavioral and neuroimaging measures, there may arise some interest in other methods to facilitate finer-grained explorations into the nature of the structure and physiology associated with these processes. One such tool can be found in the human genome and its now-complete catalog of individual variation spanning some 20,500 genes. The socalled genetic approach has been used in many other areas of medicine, ecology, and natural history to assign specific molecular factors that are encoded by specific genes to individual differences in many types of structure, function and/or behavior. Certainly, there are many types of individual differences in cognitive performance, brain structure and brain activation which makes the so-called cognitive- and imaging-genetic methods timely endeavors. To begin to study such individual differences in attention networks we have developed an attention network test (ANT) that examines the efficiency of the three brain networks we have discussed above. 24 Differences in reaction times (RT) derived from the task provide three numbers that represent the skill of each individual in the alerting, orienting, and executivenetworks.inasampleof40normalvolunteers we found these numbers to be reliable over two successive presentations. The correlations between two test sessions were 0.52, 0.61, and 0.77 for alerting, orienting, and conflict, respectively. In addition, we found no significant correlation among the three network numbers in this sample, indicating these three attentional networks are separable. Although it is most often the case that genetics and the environment play somewhat equal and interacting roles in the expression of individual differences, it can be informative to compare the correlation of performance among monzygotic twins, having identical genomes, with a correlation of performance among dizygotic twins who share only as much genetic similarity as siblings do. A heritability index can be computed from the differences between these two correlations. Such measures of genetic influences on cognitive performance have been explored in this manner several times in ways that relate to attention. For example, studies using the Continuous Performance Task (CPT) have shown that the d signal detection component of CPT performance has a heritability among normal subjects of The Span of Apprehension task (SPAN), a visual search task, has been shown to have an heritability among normal subjects of and the P/N ratio of the Spontaneous Selective Attention Task (SSAT) was shown to have an heritability among normal subjects of Twin studies using normal control twins show that spatial working memory, divided attention, choice RT and selective attention, 28 attentional set-shifting, 29 sensorimotor gating, 30 smooth pursuit eye tracking 31 are heritable. The heritability observed at the behavioral level is supported by evidence that brain structures associated with attention can show genetic bases. Anatomical studies in rodents, nonhuman primates, and humans have established that genes are major determinants of overall brain size 32,33 for structural aspects of the frontal cortex 34 and corpus callosum. 35 Using the ANT in the context of a twin study, we reported some evidence of heritability for the executive attention component. 24 Twenty-six pairs of adult monozygotic twins showed strong correlations for both the alerting (r = 0.46) and executive (r = 0.73) networks. For another group of 26 dizygotic same sex adult twins, we found a similar although somewhat smaller correlation for the alerting network (r = 0.38), but smaller correlation (0.28) for the executive network. The executive control network yielded a high heritability score of Although the sample size is small and the estimates of heritability are not very precise, these data provided preliminary support for a genetic accounting of variation in the efficiency of the executive 112 Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci (2010) c 2010 New York Academy of Sciences.

4 attention network. It should be noted as well that the data on the orienting response did not show strong reliability or heritability. Selection of candidate genes for population-based association studies With an anatomically characterized, 15 reliable, 24 and heritable 36 assay in hand, our strategy progressed to an exploration of individual genetic factors that might account for individual differences in performance. Given the vast size of the human genome, a great many design and implementation issues abound. In practice, two main phenomena have tended to limit the success of so-called candidate gene explorations. First, the effects of a single genetic variant have been found to be vanishingly small, historically accounting for less than 2% of variance. Genetic modeling studies suggest that when many genes underlie a complex trait, such as attention, or a complex disorder (e.g., ADHD), great difficulty is expected in detecting significant associations of single candidate genes. 37 The second limitation relates to wide disparities in the frequency of genetic variants across ethnic groups. In the course of human evolution, errors in the replication of chromosomal DNA are rare, occurring at a rate 38 of but they do occur often enough in our genome of 3 billion nucleotides, such that dozens of base-pair changes can accumulate per generation per genome. 39 Although many of these minor base pair changes have been lost over time perhaps due to a normal loss of isolated mating enclaves some changes have persisted, and even, perhaps due to natural selection, increased in frequency. Since their arrival, such ancestral errors in DNA replication now referred to as polymorphisms have since been distributed across the globe in an uneven and fragmented way by varied migratory patterns of human populations as far back as 10,000 and even 60,000 years ago. In the course of our own genetic studies, we have recognized that the complexities of genetic structure within human populations are especially troublesome for experimental designs in mixed populations of volunteers. This is especially true in North America and Europe where extensive ethnic admixture has occurred over the past 200 years. In an effort to minimize the limitations introduced by populations with a mixture of ethnicities, we have favored the choosing of candidate genetic variants that are common and found at high frequency across many different ethnic groups. With these limitations in mind, we then attempted to select several promising candidate genes by considering existing knowledge of neural networks, pharmacology, gene expression patterns, animal models, and human genetic anomalies. Once candidates were proposed, individuals with specific alleles could be tested with the appropriate measures to determine whether certain alleles are correlated with performance. Critical overviews of this approach are discussed in great detail elsewhere. 40 One of the most widely cited examples of such a candidate gene selection rationale is that of Egan and colleagues in the study of working and episodic memory. 41 Based on a dopaminergic hypothesis of schizophrenia, Egan and colleagues examined genes related to schizophrenia that might involve the dopamine pathway. Because schizophrenia is highly heritable, studies of families of patients had led to genetic linkage to several chromosomal areas. One of these areas is on chromosome 22, in the 22q11 region that contains the catechol-o-methyl transferase (COMT) gene that produces an enzyme important in the metabolism of dopamine. Egan and colleagues reported that a variant of the COMT gene was related to performance in cognitive tasks involving working memory. This gene accounted for about 4% of the variance in perseveration errors during the Wisconsin card-sorting task. Thus, variation in the COMT gene was expected to mediate several aspects of individual differences in executive function. We have pursued this same rationale to evaluate candidate genes for attention. Specifically, we have focused on dopaminergic genes and their role in executive attention. Neural networks that contain the frontal cortex have been shown to be important for executive function and also are known to be particularly rich in dopamine innervation 42 implicating dopamine as a dominant neuromodulator for the executive network. Some data suggest that dopamine concentrations are higher in the dorsolateral frontal cortex (DLPFC) than in other cortical regions. 43 Depleting DLPFC of dopamine produces deficits in performance on executive function tasks such as on the delayed response task. These deficits can be as severe as Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci (2010) c 2010 New York Academy of Sciences. 113

5 DLPFC lesions. 44 Destruction of the dopaminergic neurons in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) that project to PFC also impairs performance on executive function tasks. 45 Local injection of selective DA (D1) antagonists into DLPFC impairs performance on the delayed response task, 46,47 whereas leaving performance on sensory guided control tasks unimpaired. 48 Disruption of the prefrontal striatal dopamine system by injections of 1-methyl- 4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP) also impairs performance on the delayed response task 49 and halting L-dopa treatment to Parkinson s patients produces deficits on frontal cortex dependent cognitive tasks. 50 Finally, Watanabe and colleagues found that the concentration of extracellular DA in DL-PFC increased significantly whereas monkeys were performing the delayed alternation task (another classic measure of PFC function) but not during a sensory-guided control task, nor in other frontal regions (Area 8 [arcuate] or orbitofrontal) during the delayed alternation task. 51 To begin to test these predictions in a human gene-association design, we carried out a pilot study using a population of 200 adult subjects who performed the ANT on a laboratory computer. 52 Although this strategy lacked the needed statistical power to meet the standards set for the reporting of true associations, 37 no associations or statistical trends were observed for global measures of performance such as overall RT. This may suggest some specificity in the role of genetic factors in contributing to separable neural functions. Cheek swabs were obtained from each volunteer and genomic DNA was extracted and used for genotyping. An analysis of genotype vs. performance was conducted for several dopaminergic genes including the dopamine d4 receptor (DRD4), monoamine oxidase a (MAOA), dopamine transporter (DAT), and COMT genes. The DRD4 and MAOA genes revealed modest associations with behavior. For the DRD4 exon III VNTR polymorphism, a comparison of 4-repeat versus non-4-repeat carriers showed some relation to executive attention, but not to orienting, alertingoroverallrt.thepromoterrepeatpolymorphism in MAOA, 53 an enzyme that is responsible for both dopamine and serotonin breakdown, also showed associations with executive attention. This polymorphism has also been identified as a genetic risk factor in studies of aggression in boys where the 3-repeat allele (which is associated with less efficient conflict resolution in our adult studies) was found to confer some additional risk of aggression in boys who experienced some degree of maltreatment during childhood. 54 Other cognitive studies using somewhat different tasks have also reinforced our initial findings on genetics and aspects of attentional function. In two studies involving tasks that require executive attentional control, alleles of the COMT gene were related to the ability to resolve conflict. 55,56 The COMT gene has also been shown to be related to a measure of the ability to solve novel problems (fluid intelligence). 57 In addition, the orienting network, which has been shown to be influenced by cholinergic drugs in monkeys 58 was associated with a cholinergic genetic polymorphism. Different alleles of a cholinergic gene, the alpha 4 subunit of the neural nicotinic cholinergic receptor (CHRNA4), was related to performance differences in the ability to orient attention during tasks involving visual search, 59 confirming a link between orienting and the cholinergic system. Imaging-genetic analyses on selected dopaminergic candidate genes In advancing our strategic plan beyond the genetic and behavioral levels of analysis, others and we have begun to explore whether fmri can be used to confirm and extend the association between genes and particular underlying networks. As was noted earlier, Egan and colleagues showed that subjects with the Valine allele at the Met158Val polymorphism in COMT showed worse performance and higher levels of brain activation in the prefrontal cortex. 41 In our studies of the attentional networks, subjects who performed the ANT in the scanner showed that the DRD4 and MAOA alleles that were associated with more efficient handling of conflict in the earlier behavioral studies also differed in amount of activation in the anterior cingulate while performing the ANT. 60 Because we ran 16 genotypically unselected subjects we did not have sufficient data to equally subdivide groups based on genotype at the several genes we found to be related to conflict in our behavioral study. However, we did find significant differences in activation for alleles that, as argued, have considerable influence on dopamine activity within the network being studied. Moreover, our scanning data showed significant differences with about eight subjects per cell, 114 Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci (2010) c 2010 New York Academy of Sciences.

6 whereas the same alleles only approach significance in a behavioral study with nearly 100 subjects in each cell. These data support the speculation first proffered by Egan and colleagues that a combination of behavioral and fmri work can provide statistical confirmation even though the influence of any individual gene is rather small. Recent work has now replicated the MAOA finding where the low expression (3-repeat) allele was found to be associated with changes in orbitofrontal volume, amygdala and hippocampus hyperreactivity during aversive recall, and impaired cingulate activation during cognitive inhibition. 61 Further structural MRI investigations showed that the DRD4 gene was associated with cortical structure while the DAT1 gene influenced subcortical structure. 4 This result seems in harmony with the known cortical expression of the DRD4 gene and high subcortical expression of the DAT1 gene. 62,63 A recent report shows that when executive tasks are conducted in the presence of emotional distraction COMT is associated with activity in neural structures associated with cognitive control and task-related processing. 64 Finally, imaginggenetic (but not behavioral-genetic) studies on the ANT showed that carriers of the A1 allele of the TaqIA polymorphism (dbsnp rs ) located inside an haplotype block containing the DRD2 and ANKK1 gene have gene-associated functional activation in an anatomically specific, dopamine-rich region of the brain comprising the anterior cingulate gyrus. 65 From nodes to networks: preliminary explorations of genetic variation and its relation to neural network dynamics As the imaging-genetic literature expands, it is increasingly clear that single genetic polymorphisms can be statistically related to individual differences in the activation of specific brain structures. Findings such as the seminal work of Hariri and colleagues 66 on the 5HTT-LPR, and its many replications since, show that such individual differences in brain activation provide a means to understand how genetic variability can underlie complex emotions and behavior. Subsequent work from the same group, however, extended the finding from a single structure (amygdala) to the interaction of the amygdala and structures in the frontal cortex such as the cingulate cortex. 67 This initial finding, and others like it 68 provide glimpses of how genes might serve to influence, not only the physiological properties of individual structures, but also the dynamic interactions among structures or nodes in more widely distributed neural networks. Our approach has been greatly influenced by these findings and so we have also sought to integrate various MRI-based connectivity measures as well as EEG measures into our studies going forward. To this end, we have begun to consider methods that can supplement fmri, which is heavily relied upon because of its noninvasiveness and high spatial resolution, and is used mainly to detect changes in neuronal activity that correlate with metabolic demand, and not wider changes in information representation within the brain. Methods that detect oscillatory signals at the surface of the scalp provide one such supplementary method. For example, changes in local coherence in neuronal firing in absence of net increases in average population firing rate would go undetected by fmri but not by MEG or EEG. 69 Beyond being complementary to fmri, the signals measured using the latter two techniques may also be more tightly coupled to changes in information representation within the neocortex; whereas one can argue that the signal measured by fmri is epiphenomenal, low-frequency oscillations measured using EEG or MEG have been shown to entrain local population firing and to mediate long distance synchronization of activity. 70,71 It is therefore important to employ these techniques in addition to fmri to gain a more complete understanding of the neuronal processes underlying cognition. We have begun to examine local and longdistance dynamics of cortical oscillations in order to develop a more mechanistic view of cortical control of information processing, and have found that this approach lends itself to testing a unique set of predictions regarding the role of genetic factors in development and function of neocortical networks. The following sections will be mainly theoretical in scope. First, we present a brief review of the sources of cortical oscillations and the relationship between these and behavior, then discuss the role of gap junction proteins in mediating these oscillations, and finally propose a conceptual framework for interpreting genetic data in the context of oscillation data. We suggest a mechanism by which the brain might use genes that encode neuromodulatory Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci (2010) c 2010 New York Academy of Sciences. 115

7 systems to dynamically adjust information processing efficiency via modulation of oscillatory coherence. We close the section by suggesting a method by which the imaging genetics approach could be used to study the contribution of oscillations within specific frequency bands to behavior. The relationship between oscillations and behavior The cortical oscillations examined using MEG or EEG arise from the summed postsynaptic potentials neocortical pyramidal cells, and are imposed by synchronized, rhythmic inhibition via networks of electrically coupled interneurons Oscillations of various frequency bands have been associated with various cognitive and behavioral functions. For example, the power of gamma oscillations (typically defined as Hz) correlates with attentional processes, 78,79 working memory, 80 and language skills; 81 beta frequency oscillations (14 30 Hz) have been associated with sensorimotor integration 82 and visual object recognition; 83 alpha (8 14) with rest/arousal 84 and perceptual processes 85 ; and theta (4 8 Hz in humans, and typically 4 12 Hz in rat hippocampus) with attention 78 and locomotion/exploration in rats. 86 Theta also plays an important role in many other cognitive functions via long distance coordination of activity 70 or entrainment of gamma band activity. 71 Given the importance of internuronal networks in imposing these rhythms, it is reasonable to conjecture that variation in genes whose products differentially affect synchronicity of inhibition should have consequences in terms of individual differences in oscillations of various frequencies and, in turn, behavior. One class of candidates includes the gap junction proteins. A candidate genetic pathway involving gap junctions in oscillatory activity Gap junctions are composed of two apposed heximeric connexin protein hemichannels, one inserted in each cell membrane, that together form physical conduits allowing for direct metabolic and electrical coupling of cells. 87,88 Given a half-life of just a few hours, connexins are continually synthesized, trafficked to the membrane, internalized, and degraded, 89 whichallowsfortightregulationofcellcell coupling. One connexin isoform that may play an important role in cortical oscillations and behavior is connexin 36 (CX36). CX36 is expressed throughout the CNS: in the retina, inferior olivary complex, cerebellum, hippocampus, thalamus, striatum, and in the neocortex, particularly in layers IV and VI CX36 within the CNS is primarily neuronal 92,96 and appears to be most prevalent in interneruons, 97 although it has also been identified in mouse and human microglia. 98 Coupling between internurons of individual classes 75,99 allows for the control of neuronal oscillations across a wide range of frequencies, and CX36 appears to be important for this. For example, theta synchrony among multipolar bursting neurons is deficient in CX36 knockout mice 75 as is the gamma synchrony imposed by low threshold spiking cells. 77 There does not appear to be compensation for CX36 knockout by insertion of other connexin proteins, at least in the inferior olive, 100 and lentiviral expression of a CX36 dominant negative in this same structure demonstrates that decreased gap-junction conductance, and not just secondary developmental consequences of the knockout, negatively impacts synchrony. 101 Therefore, it is likely that the finding of behavioral deficits associated with CX36 knockout in mice in terms of motor learning and spatial navigation are due at least in part to a decrease in oscillatory synchrony. 102 Given the high rate of connexin turnover, one would predict that functional polymorphisms negatively impacting translation of CX36 should be associated with decreased local and long-distance coupling of neuronal oscillations in the neocortex, which should translate into deficits in performance. Fortunately, a relatively common SNP of the CX36 gene has been identified that may permit for examination of the role of this protein in higher-order cognition in humans. Belluardo and colleagues 93 first cloned and sequenced the human CX36 gene and mapped it to chromosomal band 15q14, which was previously identified as a susceptibility locus for juvenile myoclonic epilepsy (JME). 103 Two independent genetic association studies subsequently found statistically significant associations between JME and a synonymous coding SNP (rs ) of the gene. 104,105 Although it is not yet known whether this or another gene in linkage disequilibrium is responsible for conferring risk, there is reason to believe that CX36 does play a role. First, the locus of this SNP lies in an evolutionarily conserved site, suggesting that it may be of some functional importance. Second, modeling of the expected 116 Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci (2010) c 2010 New York Academy of Sciences.

8 folding pattern of the mrna encoded by the mutated gene reveals clear deviations from normal patterns, and a database search revealed that disruption of this site may disrupt binding of two exonic splicing enhancers, resulting in decreased levels of expression. 104 Assuming that there is a decrease in expression, compensation by another connexin isoform seems unlikely because this is not observed in knockout animals 100 and CX36 does not form heterotypic channels. 106 InordertodeterminewhetherornotthisSNP has functional consequences in terms of cognition and behavior, we have performed pilot behavioral testing using the ANT on a group of healthy adults. Preliminary data on a small pilot sample of adults show that the T-allele of rs is associated with impaired behavioral performance in terms of overall RT relative to the ancestral C-allele, and that the T- allele likely also has consequences for higher-order cognition in terms of executive control of attention (unpublished data). We are currently following up on this preliminary finding with a controlled study of the effects of this T-to-C polymorphism on behavior, cortical oscillations, and functional brain activation. In terms of oscillations we expect to see a decrease in prefrontal gamma power and longdistance theta coherence associated with the mutated allele. If the predicted findings are borne out, then future efforts may focus on signaling pathways affecting conductance of these channels and potential relationships between these and various psychopathologies. Stochastic resonance, CX36, and neuromodulators Stochastic resonance refers to a property of nonlinear systems such that there is a roughly quadratic relationship between the level of noise in the system and its efficiency of function; that is, optimal efficiency is achieved with a moderate, rather than very low or very high, signal-to-noise ratio. This idea was initially conceived of to explain periodic occurrence of ice ages, 107 but has because found theoretical usage elsewhere, including the neurosciences. 108,109 In many cases, stochastic resonance is discussed in terms of improved efficiency of a system in signal detection upon adding moderate levels of noise to a weak periodic driving input. However, the noise can be supplied by the system itself, and may vary as a function of, for example, coupling of neuronal oscillations. As noted above, the continuous turnover of connexin proteins leaves opportunity for tight regulation of coupling, and factors that modulate cell-cell coupling could allow for online optimization of the efficiency of network function in a context-dependent manner. Another mechanism may be via direct modulation of excitability of the network to increase its sensitivity to input. The CX36 protein contains a number of regulatory sites subject to phosphorylation by CaMKII and PKA, 110,111 and may also be regulated by Ca 2+ /calmoulin. 112 It has been demonstrated in amacrine cells of the retina that both CX36 and Cx 35 (the perch ortholog of CX36), are phosphorylated in response to dopamine (DA) signaling, and that this decreases gap junctional conductance. 111,113 In developing rat prefrontal cortex, dye coupling among pyramidal cells is decreased by stimulation of D1 and D2 receptors, likely via the camp/pka pathway, 114 and stimulation of D1, but not D2 receptors, increases the excitability of pyramidal cells in the primate prefrontal cortex. 115 The excitability of rat prefrontal fast spiking (FS) interneurons is also increased by D1, but not D2, stimulation. 116 The only study to date that examined DAergic modulation of electrical coupling in cortical FS interneurons examined those of the motor cortex and focused on D1-like mediated signaling, and found no effect on electrical coupling. 117 It is possible that a modulation effect may be found in the prefrontal cortex, or that stimulation of D2- like or both D1- and D2-like receptors is required to produce such an effect. For example, stimulation of D2 receptors modulates electrical coupling in the retina 118 and D1/D2 heterodimer receptors have been shown to activate a signaling cascade not initiated by stimulation of either receptor alone, 119 which, in the nucleus accumbens, results in an increase in CaMKII. 120 To date, these possibilities remain unexplored in the literature. The above findings are of interest because they suggest that DAergic signaling may be used to modulate the processing efficiency of neuronal ensembles via at least two mechanisms. First, it appears that D1-like mediated signaling increases the excitability of neuronal ensembles, which would permit for propagation of information carried by relatively weak inputs. Second, if DAergic modulation Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci (2010) c 2010 New York Academy of Sciences. 117

9 of electrical coupling/oscillatory coherence can be demonstrated in the prefrontal cortex, this would suggest a mechanism by which signal to noise can be optimized in a task dependent manner. 51 If this is true then studying polymorphisms that affect DA signaling pathways that modulate gap junction conductance/oscillatory coherence may advance our understanding of certain psychopathologies by helping us to understand the sources of emergent deficits in the efficiency of prefrontal network-level processing. 121,122 Gap junction proteins and factors that modulate gap junction conductance appear to be important for regulating synchrony of oscillations in general. Another application of the imaging genetics approach would be to understand the contribution of oscillations in specific frequency bands to local and long distance coordination of function, and, in turn, their contribution to behavioral performance. As noted earlier, theta oscillations appear to be important for mediating long distance coordination of processing, 70,71 and it has been hypothesized that networks of multipolar bursting neurons generate theta rhythm in the cortex in response to cholinergic input. 75 Application of acetylcholine to rabbit cortex increases the power of theta band activity 123 and there is increased phase-locking of frontal theta oscillations in Alzheimer s patients under treatment with cholinesterase inhibitors relative to untreated patients. 124 A synonymous mutation (rs ) of the gene encoding the 4 subunit in the nicotinic acetylcholine 4 2 receptor(chrna4)hasbeen shown to have consequences in terms of attentional functions, 59, as well as functional activation of the brain during an attention task. 128 Although the consequences of this SNP in terms of expression are currently unknown, this may be a good starting point in imaging genetics work aimed at better understanding the contribution of these oscillations to efficiency of network function and, in turn, behavior. Considering the where and when of candidate genes in studies of individual differences in network structure and dynamics As described at the outset, we seek, ultimately, to understand how individual differences in attentional function, vis-à-vis neural network structure and dyamics, can be explained, in part, by individual differences in genetic background. Certainly, the multiple neuroimaging methods (such as the MRI and EEG-based methods described above) that are increasingly used to measure neural network structure and dynamics present an attractive context for the application of genetic data. To this end, we further consider this possibility. When neural networks are widely distributed and show many dynamic states, is it possible to use genetic data to dissociate specific states or anatomical regions of interest using genetic data? To begin to explore this question, we ask if anatomical nodes can be differentiated by patterns of gene expression. If so, there may be an opportunity to exploit such gene-expression differences to test for separable roles of nodes within larger networks. For example, Vogt and colleagues examined the expression a wide array of neurotransmitter receptors (protein expression rather than of mrna expression) and found that the expression patterns of the receptors was able to differentiate among 4 subregions of the cingulate cortex. 129 This pattern of gene expression was consistent with prior converging evidence from lesions, evoked activity and architectonics that suggest the cingulate cortex contains 4 functionally separable regions. In the case of this study, the ACC region showed highest AMPA receptor expression and lowest GABA- A receptor expression whereas the MCC region had the lowest AMPA receptor expression and the PCC had the highest cholinergic M1 receptor expression. Relationships between where a gene is expressed and where it exerts an effect during an imaginggenetic study may not be easily intuited, however, because many of the genes used in imaging-genetic studies are widely expressed. For example, a query of gene expression data for COMT in UniGene, a public access repository of genomic data (NCBI- UniGene, 2005) shows a broad pattern of expression (bladder, bone, cervix, heart, kidney, liver, lung, ovary, prostate, skin, stomach, and other areas) and cortical and subcortical neurons in the human brain. Imaging genetic studies, however, have shown that despite the high levels of expression of COMT in the striatum, gene-associated brain activation is mainly found in the DLPFC. 41 However, this finding is consistent with data showing that 118 Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci (2010) c 2010 New York Academy of Sciences.

10 COMT activity accounts for less-than 15% of total dopamine turnover in the striatum, but greaterthan 60% in the PFC. 130 Hence, complexities can arise when trying to reconcile gene-associated brain activity with gene expression. Shifting to a developmental perspective In addition to the consideration of where a gene is expressed vs. the location of its gene-associated structure/function, there may be some additional insight into the dynamics of cognitive function gained from a consideration of when a gene is expressed and the period(s) of development when it exerts its gene-associated structure/function. Cognitive and imaging research on many well-studied developmental disabilities that arise from mutations in single genes or multiple genes within a continuous interval, show that the effects of single genes can exert dramatic effects on early and late phases of cognitive development. Research on developmental disorders, such as Williams Syndrome and fragile-x mental retardation that arise from minute chromosomal lesions shows that even when the genetic lesion is well defined, the developmental outcome can affect many neural systems and show variation in severity. 131 Simple genotype-phenotype mappings do not sufficiently explain this variability and emphasis should be placed not on these mappings, but rather, on the role of genes in developmental processes. 132 For example, a VNTR polymorphism in exon III of the dopamine d4 receptor (DRD4), a gene that is reliably associated with risk for the developmental disability ADHD, shows a correlation with cortical thinning in young children 133 and interacts with parenting style to influence temperament 134 suggesting a biological link between cortical development and cognitive development. Other developmental disabilities such as phenylketoneuria, Angelman syndrome and fragile-x mental retardation reveal the profound way in which single genes alter the normal trajectory of brain and cognitive development. 132 Some genes may even function very early in embryogenesis, but only cause noticeable disruptions rather late in behavior. The forkhead box a2 (FOXA2) gene, for example, regulates the earliest stages of the birth of dopamine neurons but contributes to a Parkinsonian form of neural degeneration when the organism reaches late adulthood. 135 Finally, we know that genes act under the influence of the environment during development, as evidenced by the interaction of early childhood stress with the MAOA and 5HTT genes increasing the risk of aggressive behavior and depression later in life. 54,136 Furthermore, early maternal stress can lead to epigenetic modification of the fetal genome, as seen in promoter of the glucocorticoid receptor gene. 137 Hence, it is of interest to specify the role of genetic variation in terms of its influence on structural and cognitive change during development. Among the most widely explored pathways in cognitive development, and thus well-suited for genetic studies, is the dopamine system. In the seminal work of Diamond, infants show marked improvement in tasks that require them to both inhibit a prepotent response, and also hold-in-mind the location of a target 138 and performance on these measures was shown to be dependent on the frontal cortex in monkeys. 139 Animal models of phenylketoneuria show that cognitive disruptions associated with PKU were dependent on reductions of DA in the prefrontal cortex, 140 which is consistent with Goldman-Rakic and colleagues who, using a primate model, found that the reduction of DA was equally as effective in diminishing executive function as were lesions to the frontal cortex. 44 In primate models, age-related improvement on executive control tasks is paralleled by postnatal increase in DA levels 141 and an increase in DA receptor gene expression. 142 Within the developing brain, DA participates in cellular changes such as myelination, synaptogenesis and pruning but may mainly subserve executive function as a modulator of excitability of recurrent synaptic inputs. 146 The time course of these processes may be quite variable and beyond the range of our current work on 5 13 year olds however, because synaptogenesis and myelination in the ACC may peak before age 5, although pruning may continue well after age Thus, as described above, we have made use of this converging evidence in our studies on adults and are presently using this evidence to craft suitable hypotheses for child studies. For one of our primary candidate genes, COMT, the evidence suggests that it performs an ongoing physiological function, and that variation in the structure of the enzyme leads to individual differences that can be measured, and that arise from, the real-time function of the enzyme. Alternately, current differences observed in brain activity for Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci (2010) c 2010 New York Academy of Sciences. 119

11 separate genetic groups may be consequences of a genetic polymorphism that acted early in the development of a specific brain structure or altered the function of an early developmental process upon which other more mature processes depend. For example, COMT, along with several other genes, are deleted in a disorder called 22q11 deletion syndrome. These children have only a single functioning copy of the chromosomal region containing COMT and show a number of signs of mental retardation as well as physical abnormalities and are also at greatly increased risk of developing schizophrenia. 148 Two experiments have examined a version of the ANT task suitable for children and both found greatly reduced ability to resolve conflict. 149,150 One of these efforts related deficits in prepulse inhibition to performance on the ANT and the results suggest that striatal-cingulate pathways may underlie combined deficits in executive attention and prepulse inhibition. 151 To begin to assess the possible developmental roles of other candidate genes associated with executive attention, we have sought to use tasks that can measure the efficiency of the attention system at different age ranges and have employed a child-friendly version of the ANT as just one example of a suitable task. In choosing this strategy, we acknowledge a few of the many complexities in choosing suitable behavioral assessments for child populations. Practically speaking, it is important to consider tasks that can be adapted to a neuroimaging environment and to consider tasks where performance can be compared across a wide range of ages by parametrically manipulating task difficulty. More importantly though, the behavioral assessments should be consistent with an established psychological model or framework (in our case, attention). Substantial evidence points to a unified conceptual framework for the development of executive control where central attentional mechanisms are involved as well as dissociable processes such as inhibition and working memory. 155 Therefore, we expect attention, and moreover, genes that contribute to the development of the attention system to play a central, but limited, role in the development of executive function. A number of studies have already adapted the central construct of attention, upon which the ANT is built, to studies of attentional function in infants, toddlers and young children. Infants show a propensity to gaze longer during incorrect trials and show increased negative activity on measurements of event related potentials 156 while 2 3 year olds demonstrate correlation in their ability to resolve stimulus response conflict and anticipatory eye movements. 157 A few behavioral performance measures have been found to relate to aspects of temperament using scales that are appropriate for infants and children. In infants, the orienting of attention was found to relate to positive affect and function as a means to distract children from distress and reduce negative affect. 158 In young children, effortful control was related to executive attention as measured by the child-friendly ANT. 159 By age 7, executive attention as measured by performance on the ANT appears stable. 160 However, performance on many traditional executive function tasks involving component functions such as working memory and inhibition (e.g., the Wisconsin Card Sorting, Tower of Hanoi and N-back) continues to improve in adolescence and into early adulthood. Our use of the child version of the ANT is based on this earlier work of Rueda and colleagues 161 as well as the work of Konrad and colleagues who have reported functional activations in children who performed the ANT in the MRI scanner. 162 Each trial begins with a cue (or a blank interval, as in the nocue condition) that informs the child either that a target will appear soon, and/or where it will appear on the screen. The target always occurs either above or below fixation, and consists of a central arrow (in the shape of a smiling fish), surrounded by flanking arrows (smiling fish). The flankers point either in the same direction as the target arrow (congruent) or in the opposite direction (incongruent) and a subtraction of RTs of congruent from incongruent target trials provides a measure of conflict resolution that assesses the efficiency of the executive attention network. Our current data were obtained from a collection of 110 healthy children somewhat evenly distributed in gender and ages 5 13 years old who performed the task while under adult supervision. 163 A view of both the distribution of raw and of normalized (conflict RT/overall RT) shows that children experience a RT cost of about 90 ms related to the resolution of stimulus response conflict and that there is little change in the efficiency of conflict resolution as children mature beyond this age range. This 120 Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci (2010) c 2010 New York Academy of Sciences.

Cognitive, affective, & social neuroscience

Cognitive, affective, & social neuroscience Cognitive, affective, & social neuroscience Time: Wed, 10:15 to 11:45 Prof. Dr. Björn Rasch, Division of Cognitive Biopsychology University of Fribourg 1 Content } 5.11. Introduction to imaging genetics

More information

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus)

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus) EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus) Introduction 17 subjects were tested to measure effects of a Brain.fm focus session on cognition. With 4 additional subjects, we recorded EEG data during baseline and while

More information

Acetylcholine (ACh) Action potential. Agonists. Drugs that enhance the actions of neurotransmitters.

Acetylcholine (ACh) Action potential. Agonists. Drugs that enhance the actions of neurotransmitters. Acetylcholine (ACh) The neurotransmitter responsible for motor control at the junction between nerves and muscles; also involved in mental processes such as learning, memory, sleeping, and dreaming. (See

More information

Oscillations: From Neuron to MEG

Oscillations: From Neuron to MEG Oscillations: From Neuron to MEG Educational Symposium, MEG UK 2014, Nottingham, Jan 8th 2014 Krish Singh CUBRIC, School of Psychology Cardiff University What are we trying to achieve? Bridge the gap from

More information

Biomarkers in Schizophrenia

Biomarkers in Schizophrenia Biomarkers in Schizophrenia David A. Lewis, MD Translational Neuroscience Program Department of Psychiatry NIMH Conte Center for the Neuroscience of Mental Disorders University of Pittsburgh Disease Process

More information

The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition

The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition Chair: Jason Samaha, University of Wisconsin-Madison Co-Chair: Ali Mazaheri, University of Birmingham

More information

Attentional Phenotypes for the Analysis of Higher Mental Function

Attentional Phenotypes for the Analysis of Higher Mental Function Short Communication TheScientificWorldJOURNAL (2002) 2, 217 223 ISSN 1537-744X; DOI 10.1100/tsw.2002.93 Attentional Phenotypes for the Analysis of Higher Mental Function John Fossella 1, Michael I. Posner,1,

More information

ESSENTIAL PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY, Neurobiology of Schizophrenia Carl Salzman MD Montreal

ESSENTIAL PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY, Neurobiology of Schizophrenia Carl Salzman MD Montreal ESSENTIAL PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY, 2011 Neurobiology of Schizophrenia Carl Salzman MD Montreal EVOLVING CONCEPTS OF SCHIZOPHRENIA Psychotic illness with delusions, hallucinations, thought disorder and deterioration;

More information

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch.

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch. The Frontal Lobes Readings: KW Ch. 16 Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions E.L. Highly organized college professor Became disorganized, showed little emotion, and began to miss deadlines Scores on intelligence

More information

Human Cognitive Developmental Neuroscience. Jan 27

Human Cognitive Developmental Neuroscience. Jan 27 Human Cognitive Developmental Neuroscience Jan 27 Wiki Definition Developmental cognitive neuroscience is an interdisciplinary scientific field that is situated at the boundaries of Neuroscience Psychology

More information

The Nervous System. Neuron 01/12/2011. The Synapse: The Processor

The Nervous System. Neuron 01/12/2011. The Synapse: The Processor The Nervous System Neuron Nucleus Cell body Dendrites they are part of the cell body of a neuron that collect chemical and electrical signals from other neurons at synapses and convert them into electrical

More information

The Brain on ADHD. Ms. Komas. Introduction to Healthcare Careers

The Brain on ADHD. Ms. Komas. Introduction to Healthcare Careers The Brain on ADHD Ms. Komas Introduction to Healthcare Careers Ms. Komas Period 9/2/2016 Komas 1 HOOK: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) plagues between 5% and 7% of children and less than

More information

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization 1 7.1 Overview This chapter aims to provide a framework for modeling cognitive phenomena based

More information

Title: Chapter 5 Recorded Lecture. Speaker: Amit Dhingra Created by: (remove if same as speaker) online.wsu.edu

Title: Chapter 5 Recorded Lecture. Speaker: Amit Dhingra Created by: (remove if same as speaker) online.wsu.edu Title: Chapter 5 Recorded Lecture Speaker: Title: What Anthony is the title Berger/Angela of this lecture? Williams Speaker: Amit Dhingra Created by: (remove if same as speaker) online.wsu.edu Chapter

More information

The Adolescent Developmental Stage

The Adolescent Developmental Stage The Adolescent Developmental Stage o Physical maturation o Drive for independence o Increased salience of social and peer interactions o Brain development o Inflection in risky behaviors including experimentation

More information

Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003

Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003 Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003 Name: Student #: BEFORE YOU BEGIN!!! 1) Count the number of pages in your exam. The exam is 8 pages long; if you do not

More information

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation.

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation. REVIEW OF FRONTAL LOBE STRUCTURES Main organization of frontal cortex: 1. Motor area (precentral gyrus). 2. Premotor & supplementary motor areas (immediately anterior to motor area). Includes premotor,

More information

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14 CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines Assignment 5: Neural mechanisms of object-based attention Due: Friday, April 14 This Assignment is a guided reading of the 2014 paper, Neural Mechanisms of Object-Based

More information

Emotion and Cognition: An Intricately Bound Developmental Process

Emotion and Cognition: An Intricately Bound Developmental Process Child Development, March/April 2004, Volume 75, Number 2, Pages 366 370 Emotion and Cognition: An Intricately Bound Developmental Process Martha Ann Bell and Christy D. Wolfe Regulatory aspects of development

More information

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories?

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories? CASE 49 A 43-year-old woman is brought to her primary care physician by her family because of concerns about her forgetfulness. The patient has a history of Down syndrome but no other medical problems.

More information

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m.

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m. Normal CNS, Special Senses, Head and Neck TOPIC: CEREBRAL HEMISPHERES FACULTY: LECTURE: READING: P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center Wednesday, 16 March

More information

Mounting evidence implicates cerebellum in autism

Mounting evidence implicates cerebellum in autism NEWS Mounting evidence implicates cerebellum in autism BY SARAH DEWEERDT 6 JANUARY 2014 Young children who don t point at interesting objects or make eye contact may be showing early warning signs of autism.

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may have as many as 200,000

More information

Veronika Borbélyová, MSc., PhD.

Veronika Borbélyová, MSc., PhD. Veronika Borbélyová, MSc., PhD. borbelyova.veronika88@gmail.com History Eugen Bleuler autism (from the Greek words autos = self, ismus = orientation, status) the patient reduces the contact with the outside

More information

Chapter 3. Biological Processes

Chapter 3. Biological Processes Biological Processes Psychology, Fifth Edition, James S. Nairne What s It For? Biological Solutions Communicating internally Initiating and coordinating behavior Regulating growth and other internal functions

More information

Computational & Systems Neuroscience Symposium

Computational & Systems Neuroscience Symposium Keynote Speaker: Mikhail Rabinovich Biocircuits Institute University of California, San Diego Sequential information coding in the brain: binding, chunking and episodic memory dynamics Sequential information

More information

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J.

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J. The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem By Andrew Laguna, S.J. Outline I. Introduction II. The Visual System III. What is the Binding Problem? IV. Possible Theoretical Solutions

More information

SAMPLE EXAMINATION QUESTIONS

SAMPLE EXAMINATION QUESTIONS SAMPLE EXAMINATION QUESTIONS PLEASE NOTE, THE QUESTIONS BELOW SAMPLE THE ENTIRE LECTURE COURSE AND THEREORE INCLUDE QUESTIONS ABOUT TOPICS THAT WE HAVE NOT YET COVERED IN CLASS. 1. Which of the following

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR In Physiology Today What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may

More information

Unit 3: The Biological Bases of Behaviour

Unit 3: The Biological Bases of Behaviour Unit 3: The Biological Bases of Behaviour Section 1: Communication in the Nervous System Section 2: Organization in the Nervous System Section 3: Researching the Brain Section 4: The Brain Section 5: Cerebral

More information

Curricular Requirement 3: Biological Bases of Behavior

Curricular Requirement 3: Biological Bases of Behavior Curricular Requirement 3: Biological Bases of Behavior Name: Period: Due Key Terms for CR 3: Biological Bases of Behavior Key Term Definition Application Acetylcholine (Ach) Action potential Adrenal glands

More information

Motor Systems I Cortex. Reading: BCP Chapter 14

Motor Systems I Cortex. Reading: BCP Chapter 14 Motor Systems I Cortex Reading: BCP Chapter 14 Principles of Sensorimotor Function Hierarchical Organization association cortex at the highest level, muscles at the lowest signals flow between levels over

More information

The role of phase synchronization in memory processes

The role of phase synchronization in memory processes The role of phase synchronization in memory processes Juergen Fell and Nikolai Axmacher Abstract In recent years, studies ranging from single-unit recordings in animals to electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography

More information

A Computational Model of Prefrontal Cortex Function

A Computational Model of Prefrontal Cortex Function A Computational Model of Prefrontal Cortex Function Todd S. Braver Dept. of Psychology Carnegie Mellon Univ. Pittsburgh, PA 15213 Jonathan D. Cohen Dept. of Psychology Carnegie Mellon Univ. Pittsburgh,

More information

Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives)

Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives) Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives) Recognize that behavior is multi-factorial with genetic components Understand how multi-factorial traits are studied. Explain the terms: prevalence, incidence,

More information

Outline. Outline. Background Literature and Slides 10/23/2013. The Schizophrenia Research Forum online: Molecules, Neural Networks and Behavior

Outline. Outline. Background Literature and Slides 10/23/2013. The Schizophrenia Research Forum online: Molecules, Neural Networks and Behavior Background Literature and Slides Molecules, Neural Networks and Behavior Roberto Fernández Galán, PhD Assistant Professor of Neurosciences Mt. Sinai Health Care Foundation Scholar Alfred P. Sloan Research

More information

We are IntechOpen, the world s leading publisher of Open Access books Built by scientists, for scientists. International authors and editors

We are IntechOpen, the world s leading publisher of Open Access books Built by scientists, for scientists. International authors and editors We are IntechOpen, the world s leading publisher of Open Access books Built by scientists, for scientists 4,000 116,000 120M Open access books available International authors and editors Downloads Our

More information

Neural Communication. Central Nervous System Peripheral Nervous System. Communication in the Nervous System. 4 Common Components of a Neuron

Neural Communication. Central Nervous System Peripheral Nervous System. Communication in the Nervous System. 4 Common Components of a Neuron Neural Communication Overview of CNS / PNS Electrical Signaling Chemical Signaling Central Nervous System Peripheral Nervous System Somatic = sensory & motor Autonomic = arousal state Parasympathetic =

More information

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Brain and Cognition 54 (2004) 235 239 www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Kim Cornish, a,b, * Rachel

More information

LEAH KRUBITZER RESEARCH GROUP LAB PUBLICATIONS WHAT WE DO LINKS CONTACTS

LEAH KRUBITZER RESEARCH GROUP LAB PUBLICATIONS WHAT WE DO LINKS CONTACTS LEAH KRUBITZER RESEARCH GROUP LAB PUBLICATIONS WHAT WE DO LINKS CONTACTS WHAT WE DO Present studies and future directions Our laboratory is currently involved in two major areas of research. The first

More information

Emotion Explained. Edmund T. Rolls

Emotion Explained. Edmund T. Rolls Emotion Explained Edmund T. Rolls Professor of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford and Fellow and Tutor in Psychology, Corpus Christi College, Oxford OXPORD UNIVERSITY PRESS Contents 1 Introduction:

More information

The role of theta oscillations in memory and decision making

The role of theta oscillations in memory and decision making The role of theta oscillations in memory and decision making Thesis by Marijke Beulen Neuroscience and Cognition, Utrecht University July 2011 Supervisor: Dr. Marieke van Vugt Department of Artificial

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

Physiological and Physical Basis of Functional Brain Imaging 6. EEG/MEG. Kâmil Uludağ, 20. November 2007

Physiological and Physical Basis of Functional Brain Imaging 6. EEG/MEG. Kâmil Uludağ, 20. November 2007 Physiological and Physical Basis of Functional Brain Imaging 6. EEG/MEG Kâmil Uludağ, 20. November 2007 Course schedule 1. Overview 2. fmri (Spin dynamics, Image formation) 3. fmri (physiology) 4. fmri

More information

biological psychology, p. 40 The study of the nervous system, especially the brain. neuroscience, p. 40

biological psychology, p. 40 The study of the nervous system, especially the brain. neuroscience, p. 40 biological psychology, p. 40 The specialized branch of psychology that studies the relationship between behavior and bodily processes and system; also called biopsychology or psychobiology. neuroscience,

More information

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems Compare Chap 31 of Purves et al., 5e Chap 24 of Bear et al., 3e Larry Wittie Computer Science, StonyBrook University http://www.cs.sunysb.edu/~cse511

More information

Psychology in Your Life

Psychology in Your Life Sarah Grison Todd Heatherton Michael Gazzaniga Psychology in Your Life SECOND EDITION Chapter 2 The Role of Biology in Psychology 1 2016 W. W. Norton & Company, Inc. 2.1 How Do Our Nervous Systems Affect

More information

The Neurobiology of Psychiatric Disorders

The Neurobiology of Psychiatric Disorders The Neurobiology of Psychiatric Disorders Vikaas S. Sohal, MD PhD Department of Psychiatry Center for Integrative Neuroscience Sloan Swartz Center for Theoretical Neurobiology Overview 1. Classification

More information

Connect with amygdala (emotional center) Compares expected with actual Compare expected reward/punishment with actual reward/punishment Intuitive

Connect with amygdala (emotional center) Compares expected with actual Compare expected reward/punishment with actual reward/punishment Intuitive Orbitofronal Notes Frontal lobe prefrontal cortex 1. Dorsolateral Last to myelinate Sleep deprivation 2. Orbitofrontal Like dorsolateral, involved in: Executive functions Working memory Cognitive flexibility

More information

Neuro Q no.2 = Neuro Quotient

Neuro Q no.2 = Neuro Quotient TRANSDISCIPLINARY RESEARCH SEMINAR CLINICAL SCIENCE RESEARCH PLATFORM 27 July 2010 School of Medical Sciences USM Health Campus Neuro Q no.2 = Neuro Quotient Dr.Muzaimi Mustapha Department of Neurosciences

More information

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: A Comparison of Electrophysiological and Other Neuroimaging Approaches Leun J. Otten Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology University

More information

Organization of the nervous system. The withdrawal reflex. The central nervous system. Structure of a neuron. Overview

Organization of the nervous system. The withdrawal reflex. The central nervous system. Structure of a neuron. Overview Overview The nervous system- central and peripheral The brain: The source of mind and self Neurons Neuron Communication Chemical messengers Inside the brain Parts of the brain Split Brain Patients Organization

More information

STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM

STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE BRAIN The central nervous system (CNS), consisting of the brain and spinal cord, receives input from sensory neurons and directs

More information

Cerebral Cortex 1. Sarah Heilbronner

Cerebral Cortex 1. Sarah Heilbronner Cerebral Cortex 1 Sarah Heilbronner heilb028@umn.edu Want to meet? Coffee hour 10-11am Tuesday 11/27 Surdyk s Overview and organization of the cerebral cortex What is the cerebral cortex? Where is each

More information

What do you notice?

What do you notice? What do you notice? https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=5v5ebf2kwf8&t=143s Models for communication between neuron groups Communication through neuronal coherence Neuron Groups 1 and 3 send projections to

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. Authors Year Patients Male gender (%) Mean age (range) Adults/ Children

More information

The Nervous System. Biological School. Neuroanatomy. How does a Neuron fire? Acetylcholine (ACH) TYPES OF NEUROTRANSMITTERS

The Nervous System. Biological School. Neuroanatomy. How does a Neuron fire? Acetylcholine (ACH) TYPES OF NEUROTRANSMITTERS Biological School The Nervous System It is all about the body!!!! It starts with an individual nerve cell called a NEURON. Synapse Neuroanatomy Neurotransmitters (chemicals held in terminal buttons that

More information

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and 77 5 Discussion The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and implicit learning systems. Chapter 2 described the effects of performing a working memory task

More information

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Jessica R. Newton and Mriganka Sur Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences Picower Center for Learning & Memory Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge,

More information

Shift 1, 8 July 2018, 09:30-13:00

Shift 1, 8 July 2018, 09:30-13:00 Shift 1, 8 July 2018, 09:30-13:00 CNS patterning A001-A014 Stem cells: basic biology and postnatal neurogenesis - part I Development of neural systems: Molecular and genetic characterisationa Epigenetic

More information

How does adversity in childhood get under the skin

How does adversity in childhood get under the skin How does adversity in childhood get under the skin What can we learn from neuroscience and epigenetics? Eamon McCrory Professor of Developmental Neuroscience & Psychopathology, UCL Consultant Clinical

More information

Cortical Control of Movement

Cortical Control of Movement Strick Lecture 2 March 24, 2006 Page 1 Cortical Control of Movement Four parts of this lecture: I) Anatomical Framework, II) Physiological Framework, III) Primary Motor Cortex Function and IV) Premotor

More information

Jay M. Baraban MD, PhD January 2007 GENES AND BEHAVIOR

Jay M. Baraban MD, PhD January 2007 GENES AND BEHAVIOR Jay M. Baraban MD, PhD jay.baraban@gmail.com January 2007 GENES AND BEHAVIOR Overview One of the most fascinating topics in neuroscience is the role that inheritance plays in determining one s behavior.

More information

Title: Pinpointing resilience in Bipolar Disorder

Title: Pinpointing resilience in Bipolar Disorder Title: Pinpointing resilience in Bipolar Disorder 1. AIM OF THE RESEARCH AND BRIEF BACKGROUND Bipolar disorder (BD) is a mood disorder characterised by episodes of depression and mania. It ranks as one

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 7: Network models Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis II 6 Single neuron

More information

Choosing the Greater of Two Goods: Neural Currencies for Valuation and Decision Making

Choosing the Greater of Two Goods: Neural Currencies for Valuation and Decision Making Choosing the Greater of Two Goods: Neural Currencies for Valuation and Decision Making Leo P. Surgre, Gres S. Corrado and William T. Newsome Presenter: He Crane Huang 04/20/2010 Outline Studies on neural

More information

COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE HOW TO STUDY MORE EFFECTIVELY (P 187-189) Elaborate Think about the meaning of the information that you are learning Relate to what you already know Associate: link information together Generate and test

More information

What do you notice? Edited from

What do you notice? Edited from What do you notice? Edited from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ffayobzdtc8&t=83s How can a one brain region increase the likelihood of eliciting a spike in another brain region? Communication through

More information

Genetics and Genomics in Medicine Chapter 8 Questions

Genetics and Genomics in Medicine Chapter 8 Questions Genetics and Genomics in Medicine Chapter 8 Questions Linkage Analysis Question Question 8.1 Affected members of the pedigree above have an autosomal dominant disorder, and cytogenetic analyses using conventional

More information

Researchers probe genetic overlap between ADHD, autism

Researchers probe genetic overlap between ADHD, autism NEWS Researchers probe genetic overlap between ADHD, autism BY ANDREA ANDERSON 22 APRIL 2010 1 / 7 Puzzling link: More than half of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder meet the diagnostic

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

Guided Reading Activities

Guided Reading Activities Name Period Chapter 28: Nervous Systems Guided Reading Activities Big idea: Nervous system structure and function Answer the following questions as you read modules 28.1 28.2: 1. Your taste receptors for

More information

Organization of the nervous system. [See Fig. 48.1]

Organization of the nervous system. [See Fig. 48.1] Nervous System [Note: This is the text version of this lecture file. To make the lecture notes downloadable over a slow connection (e.g. modem) the figures have been replaced with figure numbers as found

More information

The Role of Smoking in Cocaine. Addiction

The Role of Smoking in Cocaine. Addiction The Role of Smoking in Cocaine Addiction Luca Colnaghi Eric Kandel Laboratory Columbia University in the City of New York Department of Neuroscience Index 1- The Brain, memory, metaplasticity 2- Cocaine

More information

Modules 4 & 6. The Biology of Mind

Modules 4 & 6. The Biology of Mind Modules 4 & 6 The Biology of Mind 1 Neuron - 100 Billion - Communication System Glial cells Cell body (nucleus) Dendrites Axon Axon Terminals (terminal buttons) Synaptic cleft 3 4 Communication Within

More information

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:.

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:. Cortical Organization Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:. 2. Secondary cortex: located immediately adjacent to primary cortical areas,

More information

Neurobiological Foundations of Reward and Risk

Neurobiological Foundations of Reward and Risk Neurobiological Foundations of Reward and Risk... and corresponding risk prediction errors Peter Bossaerts 1 Contents 1. Reward Encoding And The Dopaminergic System 2. Reward Prediction Errors And TD (Temporal

More information

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex PARIETAL LOBE The Parietal Lobes develop at about the age of 5 years. They function to give the individual perspective and to help them understand space, touch, and volume. The location of the parietal

More information

shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all of the above.

shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all of the above. Section: Chapter 14: Multiple Choice 1. Alex the parrot: pp.529-530 shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all

More information

What can we do to improve the outcomes for all adolescents? Changes to the brain and adolescence-- Structural and functional changes in the brain

What can we do to improve the outcomes for all adolescents? Changes to the brain and adolescence-- Structural and functional changes in the brain The Adolescent Brain-- Implications for the SLP Melissa McGrath, M.A., CCC-SLP Ball State University Indiana Speech Language and Hearing Association- Spring Convention April 15, 2016 State of adolescents

More information

Sleep-Wake Cycle I Brain Rhythms. Reading: BCP Chapter 19

Sleep-Wake Cycle I Brain Rhythms. Reading: BCP Chapter 19 Sleep-Wake Cycle I Brain Rhythms Reading: BCP Chapter 19 Brain Rhythms and Sleep Earth has a rhythmic environment. For example, day and night cycle back and forth, tides ebb and flow and temperature varies

More information

Exam 2 PSYC Fall (2 points) Match a brain structure that is located closest to the following portions of the ventricular system

Exam 2 PSYC Fall (2 points) Match a brain structure that is located closest to the following portions of the ventricular system Exam 2 PSYC 2022 Fall 1998 (2 points) What 2 nuclei are collectively called the striatum? (2 points) Match a brain structure that is located closest to the following portions of the ventricular system

More information

APNA 25th Annual Conference October 19, Session 1022

APNA 25th Annual Conference October 19, Session 1022 When Words Are Not Enough The Use of Sensory Modulation Techniques to Replace Self- Injurious Behaviors in Patients with Borderline Personality Disorder General Organization of the Brain Lita Sabonis,

More information

Reversing the Effects of Fragile X Syndrome

Reversing the Effects of Fragile X Syndrome CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS OF BASIC RESEARCH Paul J. Lombroso, M.D., Marilee P. Ogren, Ph.D. Assistant Editors Reversing the Effects of Fragile X Syndrome MARILEE P. OGREN, PH.D., AND PAUL J. LOMBROSO, M.D.

More information

Basic Nervous System anatomy. Neurobiology of Happiness

Basic Nervous System anatomy. Neurobiology of Happiness Basic Nervous System anatomy Neurobiology of Happiness The components Central Nervous System (CNS) Brain Spinal Cord Peripheral" Nervous System (PNS) Somatic Nervous System Autonomic Nervous System (ANS)

More information

Integrating Mental Processes: Thinking and Problem Solving

Integrating Mental Processes: Thinking and Problem Solving Integrating Mental Processes: Thinking and Problem Solving Recruitment of executive attention is normally associated with a subjective feeling of mental effort. Lionel Naccache, Stanislas Dehaene, Laurent

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience of Autism

Cognitive Neuroscience of Autism Townsend, J., Westerfield, M. (in press). Autism and Asperger s Syndrome: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective. In, Carol Armstrong, Ed., Handbook of Medical Neuropsychology. New York: Springer Science.

More information

Searching for the Cause of Autism:

Searching for the Cause of Autism: Searching for the Cause of Autism: How genetics and social experience may intersect Dr Lane Strathearn, MBBS FRACP PhD Professor, Department of Pediatrics, University of Iowa Physician Director, Center

More information

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience Levels of organization Central Nervous System 1m 10 11 neurons Neural systems and neuroanatomy Systems 10cm Networks 1mm Neurons 100μm 10 8 neurons Professor Daniel Leeds

More information

590,000 deaths can be attributed to an addictive substance in some way

590,000 deaths can be attributed to an addictive substance in some way Mortality and morbidity attributable to use of addictive substances in the United States. The Association of American Physicians from 1999 60 million tobacco smokers in the U.S. 14 million dependent on

More information

marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD

marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD in this talk what is marijuana? the brain on marijuana is the teen brain special? current research what is marijuana?

More information

Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives)

Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives) Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives) Recognize that behavior is multi-factorial with genetic components Understand how multi-factorial traits are studied. Explain the terms: incidence, prevalence,

More information

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Brain electrical activity methods description and explanation of method state effects trait effects Positron emission tomography

More information

Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition. Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT

Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition. Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT 1 Courtesy of Elsevier, Inc., http://www.sciencedirect.com. Used with permission.

More information

Making Things Happen 2: Motor Disorders

Making Things Happen 2: Motor Disorders Making Things Happen 2: Motor Disorders How Your Brain Works Prof. Jan Schnupp wschnupp@cityu.edu.hk HowYourBrainWorks.net On the Menu in This Lecture In the previous lecture we saw how motor cortex and

More information

Biological Risk Factors

Biological Risk Factors Biological Risk Factors Ms Angelina Crea Provisional Psychologist Academic Child Psychiatry Unit Royal Children s Hospital Professor Alasdair Vance Head Academic Child Psychiatry Department of Paediatrics

More information

Prefrontal dysfunction in drug addiction: Cause or consequence? Christa Nijnens

Prefrontal dysfunction in drug addiction: Cause or consequence? Christa Nijnens Prefrontal dysfunction in drug addiction: Cause or consequence? Master Thesis Christa Nijnens September 16, 2009 University of Utrecht Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience Department of Neuroscience

More information

TNS Journal Club: Interneurons of the Hippocampus, Freund and Buzsaki

TNS Journal Club: Interneurons of the Hippocampus, Freund and Buzsaki TNS Journal Club: Interneurons of the Hippocampus, Freund and Buzsaki Rich Turner (turner@gatsby.ucl.ac.uk) Gatsby Unit, 22/04/2005 Rich T. Introduction Interneuron def = GABAergic non-principal cell Usually

More information

The Neuroscience of Addiction: A mini-review

The Neuroscience of Addiction: A mini-review The Neuroscience of Addiction: A mini-review Jim Morrill, MD, PhD MGH Charlestown HealthCare Center Massachusetts General Hospital Disclosures Neither I nor my spouse/partner has a relevant financial relationship

More information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information Supplementary Information The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice Joseph W. Kable and Paul W. Glimcher a 10 0 b 10 0 10 1 10 1 Discount rate k 10 2 Discount rate k 10 2 10

More information