FUNCTIONAL ORGANIZATION OF THE RAT HEAD-DIRECTION CIRCUIT

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "FUNCTIONAL ORGANIZATION OF THE RAT HEAD-DIRECTION CIRCUIT"

Transcription

1 Chapter 9 FUNCTIONAL ORGANIZATION OF THE RAT HEAD-DIRECTION CIRCUIT Hugh T. Blair 1 and Patricia E. Sharp 2 1 Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA Department of Psychology, Bowling Green State University, Bowling Green, OH Rats, like humans and many other animals, possess a sense of direction that helps them to plan navigational trajectories and calculate bearings to familiar objects and locations (Barlow, 1964; Mittelstaedt and Mittelstaedt, 1980; 1982; Gallistel, 1990; McNaughton, Chen, and Markus, 1991). The rat brain contains a population of neurons called head-direction (HD) cells, which are believed to provide the neural basis for this sense of direction (Ranck, 1984; Taube, 1990a,b; but see Golob, Stackman, Wong, and Taube, 2001). In this chapter, we describe the functional organization of circuitry in the rat brain that allows HD cells to compute the animal s directional heading. HEAD-DIRECTION CELLS A single HD cell fires action potentials only when the rat s head is facing in a particular direction, referred to as the cell s preferred firing direction. Each individual HD cell is tuned to have its own preferred firing direction, so that together, the entire population of HD cells provides a distributed representation of any direction that the rat faces. Figure 1 illustrates the spike activity of a typical HD cell, which was recorded from the anterior thalamus of a freely behaving rat. This cell fired whenever the rat s head faced southeast (135º), and was silent when the rat faced in other directions. Throughout this chapter, we shall denote north as 0º, east as 90º, and so on, as in Figure 1. According to this convention, degree measurements always increase in the clockwise (CW) direction, and decrease in the counterclockwise (CCW) direction. The behavior of a HD cell can be characterized by its directional tuning function, which plots the firing rate of the cell as a function of the rat s 163

2 164 The Neural Basis of Navigation 0 o N 270 o W E 90 o S 180 o 135 o = Preferred Firing Direction Time Figure 1. Spike activity of a typical HD cell, recorded from the anterior thalamus of a freely behaving rat. Overhead view shows the path of the rat s movements during a time period of about 3.5 seconds. A raster plot (bottom) shows spikes fired by the HD cell during this period. Notice that the HD cell fires spikes whenever the rat faces in the cell s preferred firing direction of southeast, or 135 degrees (gray boxes), and the cell remains silent at other times. Preferred Firing Direction = 135 o Peak Firing Rate = 54 Hz Tuning Width= 80 o Baseline Firing Rate = 0 Hz Head Direction (Degrees) Figure 2. Directional tuning function of the HD cell that was shown in Figure 1. Note that there is a prominent peak in the tuning function, centered over the cell s preferred firing direction of southeast (that is, 135 degrees). Several descriptive parameters can be derived from the tuning function.

3 H.T. Blair & P.E. Sharp 165 directional heading (Figure 2). The directional tuning function shown here is typical, in that it is roughly Gaussian, or triangular in shape. Several descriptive parameters can be derived from the HD tuning function: the preferred firing direction is computed by taking the mean value of the tuning curve on the abscissa, the tuning width is twice the standard deviation of the tuning peak, the peak firing rate is the maximum value of the tuning peak on the ordinate, and the baseline firing rate is the cell s firing rate outside of the tuning peak. It is important to note that HD cells are not influenced by the position of the rat s head with respect to its body; they are only influenced by the direction of the head relative to the surrounding environment. Furthermore, HD cell activity is modulated only by the direction of the rat s head in the horizontal plane (yaw), and not by the angle of the head in other planes, such as pitch or roll (Stackman and Taube, 1997). Establishing the Directional Reference Frame How do HD cells achieve their remarkable direction-specific firing properties? One explanation could be that, like a compass, they are sensitive to the earth s magnetic field. However, experimental evidence does not support this explanation. Unlike a compass needle, which always points toward geomagnetic north, HD cells can change their directional preference with respect to the earth s magnetic field. Indeed, a given HD cell typically has different preferred firing directions in different environments. For example, a HD cell that prefers west in one environment (such as the rat s home cage) might prefer north in another environment (such as an experiment room). Under appropriate circumstances, HD cells can even change their directional preference within a single environment. Figure 3 shows an example of how HD cells can re-establish their directional preference in a single spatial environment. An HD cell was recorded while a rat foraged for food pellets in a cylindrical chamber. The wall of the chamber was painted with an alternating series of eight black and white stripes, so that each 90º segment of the wall was visually identical (see overhead view, Figure 3). The rat was placed into the cylinder beside a white stripe, and a HD cell was then recorded for five minutes while the animal foraged. The rat was then picked up, held by the experimenter for a moment, and immediately placed back into the cylinder beside a different white stripe, so that the animal s starting position was visually identical to the previous placement, but rotated by 90º. This procedure was repeated four consecutive times, placing the rat beside a different white stripe each time, always with the right side of its body adjacent to the wall. Figure 3 shows that the HD cell s

4 166 The Neural Basis of Navigation Firing Rate (Hz) Head Direction (degrees) 4 Figure 3. The preferred firing direction of a single HD cell depends upon the rat s starting position in the environment. This cell s firing direction shifted by 90 degrees during each of four consecutive sessions in the cylinder (tuning functions, right), in exact correspondence with the difference in the rat s starting position at the beginning of the session (overhead view, left). preferred firing direction was shifted by 90º for each placement, in exact correspondence with the shift in the rat s starting position. If HD cells were able to detect the earth s magnetic field, it is unlikely that they would shift their preferred firing direction along with the rat s starting position in this way. Presumably, the HD cell in Figure 3 shifts its preferred firing direction because the four starting positions are visually identical, and the rat is unaware that the starting position has changed at each placement. In the absence of other orienting cues, HD cells use the rat s starting position in the cylinder as a reference point to establish their directional preference. However, if other sensory orienting cues are available, the HD system can use them to help establish the directional reference frame. Sensory Orienting Cues Experiments have shown that the directional reference frame of the HD system is influenced by several kinds of sensory cues. These influences provide important clues about the functional organization of the HD circuitry. Visual Cues and Landmark Learning Perhaps the easiest way to obtain directional bearings in a familiar spatial environment is by looking around for recognizable landmarks. For example, a person who is familiar with New York City knows that the Empire State Building lies to the south of Central Park. Thus, an observer in Central Park can find south by using the Empire State building as a visual landmark.

5 H.T. Blair & P.E. Sharp 167 Rodents, like people, seem to use visual landmarks for navigational orientation (Collett, Cartwright, and Smith, 1986). Supporting this, the preferred firing direction of HD cells can be influenced in predictable ways by altering the positions of familiar visual landmarks in the surrounding environment (Taube et al., 1990b; Chen, Lin, Green, Barnes, and McNaughton, 1994; Mizumori and Williams, 1993; Taube and Burton, 1995; Goodridge and Taube, 1995; Knierim, Kudrimoti, and McNaughton, 1998). For example, if a visual landmark that normally lies to the north of an experimental chamber is moved so that it lies to the east, HD cells may collectively rotate their preferred firing directions clockwise by 90º, in correspondence with the 90º shift in the position of the landmark (see the accompanying chapter by Taube et al. in this volume). However, it appears that rats must learn about the spatial position of a visual landmark before it can be used for directional orienting, because unfamiliar landmarks exert less influence over the preferred firing directions of HD cells than familiar landmarks do (McNaughton, Markus, Wilson, and Knierim, 1993; Taube and Burton, 1995; Goodridge et al., 1998). Although landmark orientation exerts a strong influence on HD cells, visual landmarks cannot be the cells only source of directional information, because once HD cells have established their preferred firing direction in an environment, they can maintain this directional firing preference even when the rat is blindfolded, or placed in complete darkness (Mizumori and Williams, 1993; Goodridge et al., 1998; see also Chapter 8). How can HD cells continue to compute the rat s directional heading in the absence of visual cues? Vestibular Signals and Angular Path Integration Once the directional reference frame has been established in a given environment, HD cells may rely on angular path integration to keep track of the rat s directional heading as it moves through that environment (McNaughton et al., 1991). To perform angular path integration, HD cells are thought to combine information about the rat s current head direction with information about the angular velocity at which the head is turning, and this combined information is then used to predict the rat s future head direction. For example, if the rat is presently facing north, and turning its head clockwise at a velocity of 360º/s, then the circuit can calculate that the rat will face northeast after 1/8 s has elapsed, and update the HD signal accordingly. To perform angular path integration, the HD circuit must receive precise information about the angular velocity of the rat s head each time the animal turns to face a new direction. Evidence indicates that the HD system relies upon vestibular signals to provide this angular velocity information (Blair and Sharp, 1996; Stackman and Taube, 1997; Knierim et al., 1998). Figure 4 shows an experiment that demonstrates how vestibular signals can exert an

6 168 The Neural Basis of Navigation influence on the preferred firing direction of HD cells (Blair and Sharp, 1996). HD cells were recorded from the anterior thalamus while a rat foraged for food pellets, in the same cylindrical chamber that was shown previously in Figure 3. After 15 minutes of recording, the entire chamber was rotated by 90 (and the rat along with it), so that the chamber remained visually unchanged before and after the rotation (since each 90º segment of the wall was visually identical). The rotation of the chamber was not signaled to the rat by any visual cues or optic flow, because the wall and floor of the cylinder appeared to remain stationary as they rotated along with the rat, and the rat could not see external cues beyond the high walls of the cylinder. 90 FAST Firing Rate (% max) PRE POST 90 SLOW Firing Rate (% max) Head Direction (degrees) Figure 4. Vestibular cues influence the preferred firing direction of HD cells. When the recording chamber is rotated quickly (top), HD cells maintain a stable directional preference, but when the chamber is rotated slowly (bottom), the directional preference rotates along with the chamber. This indicates that HD cells are sensitive to vestibular signals. The rotation of the cylinder was performed at one of two speeds: either quickly at ~60 /s, or slowly at ~1 /s. Figure 5A shows that in response to fast rotation of the cylinder, the preferred firing direction of a typical HD cell does not rotate along with the cylinder. That is, the HD cell s directional preference remains locked to the fixed environment outside the cylinder, even though the rat cannot directly perceive this external environment. By contrast, Figure 5B shows that in response to slow rotation of the chamber, an HD cell rotates its preferred firing direction along with the cylinder, and therefore fails to maintain a stable preferred firing direction.

7 H.T. Blair & P.E. Sharp 169 The main difference between the fast and slow rotations of the cylinder is that the fast rotation can be sensed by the rat s vestibular system, whereas the slow rotation cannot. Hence, the results shown in Figure 5 suggest that vestibular signals are important for updating the HD signal. Further supporting this conclusion, it has been shown that lesions of the vestibular nuclei completely abolish the HD signal (Stackman and Taube, 1997). Hence, vestibular signals appear to be essential to the functioning of the HD system. Correction of Integration Errors The results shown in Figure 5B demonstrate an important limitation of the angular path integration process: if the angular velocity of the rat s head is not measured correctly (for example, when the head rotates at a speed which is below vestibular threshold), the HD signal will become inaccurate. Even small errors in estimating the rat s head-turning velocity could cause significant errors in the accuracy of the HD signal, which would accumulate over time to make the directional signal unstable. How can such errors be prevented? It has been proposed that visual landmarks may help to solve this problem, by periodically resetting the HD signal to erase any integration errors that may have accumulated over time (Skaggs et al., 1995; Knierim et al., 1998). This proposal is supported by evidence showing that when rats are deprived of visual information, either by blindfolding them or placing them in complete darkness, the preferred direction of HD cells becomes less stable and begins to drift (Mizumori and Williams, 1993; Goodridge et al., 1998). This is exactly what would be expected if the angular path integration system relies on a visual landmark orienting system to prevent the accumulation of integration errors over time. As discussed below, this cooperation between angular path integration and landmark orienting systems may involve interactions among distinct brain circuits that perform different functions to compute the HD signal. FUNCTIONAL ANATOMY OF THE HD CIRCUIT HD cells have been found in several regions of the rat brain, including the postsubiculum (PoS; Ranck, 1984; Taube et al., 1990a), anterodorsal thalamus (AD; Blair and Sharp, 1995; Taube, 1995), laterodorsal thalamus (LD; Mizumori and Williams, 1993), striatum (Wiener, 1993), lateral mammillary nucleus (LMN; Blair et al., 1998; Stackman and Taube, 1998), retrosplenial cortex (RsC; Chen et al., 1994; Cho and Sharp, 2000), and dorsal tegmental nucleus (DTN; Sharp and Cho, 2001). Some of these regions are likely to

8 170 The Neural Basis of Navigation play an active role in generating the HD signal, while other regions may passively receive the signal from areas where it is generated. Figure 5 shows connections among several brain structures that are thought to participate in computing the HD signal. As discussed in the previous section, HD cells are believed to compute the rat s directional heading using two interacting processes: landmark orientation and angular path integration. Here, we shall argue that cortical structures containing HD cells, such as PoS and RsC, function mainly to support landmark orientation, whereas subcortical structures, such as LMN and DTN, are mostly involved in angular path integration. LANDMARK ORIENTING CORTEX Visual Cells Place Cells VCX HIP PoS/ RsC AD DTN LMN SUBCORTEX AV Cells NPH ANGULAR PATH INTEGRATION Excitation Inhibition Figure 5. Functional anatomy of the HD system. Shaded boxes denote regions that are known to contain HD cells. Abbreviations: AD, anterodorsal thalamus; DTN, dorsal tegmental nucleus; HIP, hippocampus; LMN, lateral mammillary nucleus; NPH, nucleus propositus hypoglossi; PoS, postsubiculum; RsC, retrosplenial cortex; VCX, visual cortex. Cortical HD Cells and Landmark Orientation HD cells are found abundantly within certain regions of the limbic cortex, including PoS (Ranck, 1984; Taube et al., 1990a) and RsC (Chen et al., 1994). These regions receive significant input from early visual cortex (Vogt and Miller, 1983), which could mediate the influence of visual landmarks upon

9 H.T. Blair & P.E. Sharp 171 the HD signal. Supporting this idea, RsC contains neurons that are strongly responsive to visual orienting cues (Chen, Lin, Barnes, and McNaughton, 1994). Furthermore, lesions of PoS interfere with the ability of visual landmarks to influence the preferred firing direction of HD cells recorded in AD (Goodridge and Taube, 1997). These findings suggest that cortical regions containing HD cells, such as PoS and RsC, may be involved in updating the HD signal based on visual landmarks. However, it is important to note that directional orientation using visual landmarks requires more than just the ability to visually recognize landmarks. It also requires knowledge of the spatial layout of the surrounding environment. To see why this is so, consider the example of the New Yorker who uses the Empire State Building to identify which direction is south from Central Park. What happens when this observer travels to some other location in New York City, such as Washington Square, or Penn Station? In that case, it would be necessary to know that the Empire State Building lies to the north of Washington Square, and to the east of Penn Station. That is, the same landmark indicates different directions when it is observed from different locations. Therefore, when using a visual landmark to obtain directional bearings, it is not enough to simply identify the landmark; it is also necessary to know the observer s current location within the spatial environment, and to know the directional bearing of the landmark relative to that location. For this reason, a brain system that performs landmark orientation must process more than just visual information. It must also process information about the layout of the surrounding spatial environment, and about the observer s location within that environment. Role of the Hippocampus in the HD System PoS and RsC, in addition to their visual inputs, also receive input from the hippocampus via the subiculum (van Groen and Wyss, 1990a,b). The hippocampus and subiculum contain a population of neurons called place cells that encode the rat s location in space (O'Keefe and Dostrovsky, 1971; Ranck, 1973; Sharp and Green, 1994). Place cells are thought to store a cognitive map of the spatial environment, and are probably critical for selflocalization (O Keefe and Nadel, 1978). PoS and RsC receive input from both visual cortex and the hippocampus. Hence, PoS and RsC seem anatomically well-situated to combine information about visual landmarks with information about the rat s current spatial location, and use this combined information to compute the rat s directional heading. Thus, we propose that the hippocampal system may interact with PoS and RsC to perform landmark orientation to compute the HD signal. In contradiction to this idea, Golob and Taube (1999) have proposed that the hippocampus participates primarily in angular path integration, rather than landmark orientation. These authors argue that this proposal is supported by

10 172 The Neural Basis of Navigation their finding that, in rats with hippocampal lesions (unlike normal rats), HD cells exhibit drifting of their preferred firing direction when the animal is first introduced into a novel environment. The authors claim that this is what would be expected if angular path integration were impaired. However, as explained above, the landmark orientation system is thought to periodically reset the angular path integration circuit, to prevent drifting of the HD signal that would otherwise result from the accumulation of integration errors over time (Skaggs et al., 1995). Thus, the drifting of the HD signal that was observed by Golob and Taube (1999) in hippocampal-lesioned rats is exactly what would be expected following damage to the landmark orientation system, because a primary function of the landmark system should be to prevent the HD signal from drifting. Golob and Taube (1999) found that after continued exposure to an environment, HD cells in hippocampal-lesioned rats eventually stopped drifting, and achieved stable directional firing. Once this stable directional firing was achieved, HD cells seemed to be influenced by visual landmarks, as in normal rats. Golob and Taube (1999) interpreted this result as evidence that landmark orientation is intact in rats with hippocampal lesions, since it is difficult to understand how landmarks could influence HD cells if the landmark orientation system were damaged. However, it is equally difficult to see how the preferred firing direction of HD cells could stop drifting and become stable if the angular path integration system were damaged. Hence, the effects of hippocampal lesions upon HD cells are rather confusing and difficult to interpret. Contrary to Golob and Taube s (1999) proposal, we do not think these results provide compelling evidence that the hippocampus plays a role in angular path integration. Instead, we shall argue that angular path integration occurs mainly in subcortical structures that contain HD cells. Subcortical HD Cells and Angular Path Integration HD cells are found within two subcortical regions LMN and DTN that are closely associated with the vestibular system. DTN receives direct inputs from the vestibular sensory nuclei (Lui, Chang, and Wickern, 1984), especially the nucleus prepositus hypoglossi (NPH). As shown in Figure 5, the primary ascending input to LMN comes from the DTN, and LMN sends reciprocal projections back to DTN (Liu et al., 1984; Allen and Hopkins, 1989; Gonzalo-Ruiz, Alonso, Sanz, and Llinas, 1992). The ultrastructural anatomy of the projection from DTN to LMN suggests that it is inhibitory (Hayakawa and Zyo, 1992), whereas the projection from LMN to DTN is mainly excitatory (Allen and Hopkins, 1990). For reasons to be explained below, these connections suggest that LMN and DTN are anatomically well

11 H.T. Blair & P.E. Sharp 173 positioned to participate in angular path integration (Sharp, Blair, and Cho, 2001). LMN Several lines of evidence suggest that LMN may be involved in angular path integration. First, when the rat turns its head to face a new direction, the HD signal is updated first in LMN, before it is updated in other brain areas such as AD or PoS (Blair et al., 1998; Stackman and Taube, 1998). The fact that LMN HD cells are among the first to be updated during head turns suggests that LMN may be a site where angular path integration of head-turning movement occurs. Second, bilateral lesions of LMN completely abolish the HD signal in AD (Blair et al., 1998), demonstrating that input from LMN is essential for HD cell activity in AD. Although this finding is consistent with the notion that LMN is necessary for angular path integration, the result could also be explained if LMN merely conveys the HD signal to AD from other structures where path integration takes place. Third, the tuning functions of HD cells in LMN are modulated by the angular velocity of the rat s head (Figure 6), a property that has been predicted for HD cells that participate in angular path integration (McNaughton et al., 1991; Skaggs et al., 1995; Zhang, 1996b). Firing Rate (Hz) A LEFT HEMI Head Direction (degrees from preferred) B RIGHT HEMI Figure 6. CW (solid lines) and CCW (dashed lines) tuning functions for two HD cells recorded in LMN. A: HD cell recorded from the left hemisphere of LMN has broader tuning width during CW turns, due to leftward shift of the left edge of the peak. B: HD cell in the right LMN shows the opposite pattern, with broader tuning during CCW turns, due to rightward shift of the right edge of the peak. Figure 6A shows tuning functions for a typical HD cell recorded from the left hemisphere of LMN. Two separate tuning peaks are shown for this HD cell: one that includes only spikes that occurred when the rat s head was turning CW (solid lines), and another that includes only spikes that occurred when the head was turning CCW (dashed lines). By comparing the shape of the CW and CCW tuning peaks, it is possible to see how the tuning properties of the cell differ when the head is turning in opposite directions. The left

12 174 The Neural Basis of Navigation LMN cell in Figure 6A has a broader tuning function during CW head turns than during CCW head turns, because the left edge of the CW tuning curve is shifted to the left relative to the CCW tuning curve. This is typical for HD cells recorded in the left hemisphere of LMN (Blair et al., 1998). Conversely, HD cells recorded in the right LMN have broader tuning during CCW than CW turns, due to shifting of the right edge of the curve (Figure 6B). Such hemispheric asymmetries have not been observed for HD cells recorded in AD, RsC, or PoS. DTN HD cells in DTN are also modulated by the angular velocity of the rat s head, but in a seemingly different way than LMN HD cells (Sharp, Tinkelman and Cho, 2001). Figure 7 shows tuning functions for two HD cells recorded in DTN: one from the left and the other from the right hemisphere. As in Figure 6, two separate tuning functions (CW and CCW) are shown for each DTN HD cell. DTN HD cells exhibit very broad tuning widths in comparison with HD cells in other brain regions. Notice that the left DTN cell in Figure 7A has a higher firing rate when the rat s head is turning CW than CCW, whereas the reverse is true for the right DTN cell in Figure 7B. But unlike the LMN HD cells in Figure 6, the tuning widths of DTN HD cells in Figure 7 are similar during head turns in either direction. Firing Rate (Hz) A B Head Direction (degrees from preferred) Figure 7. CW (solid lines) and CCW (dashed lines) tuning functions for two HD cells recorded in DTN. A: HD cell recorded from the left hemisphere of DTN has higher firing rate during CW turns. B: HD in the right DTN has higher firing rate during CCW turns. In addition to HD cells, DTN also contains a large number of angular velocity (AV) cells, which fire at a rate that is proportional to the angular velocity of the rat s head (Sharp, Tinkelman and Cho, 2001). AV cells fire at a constant rate when the rat s head is not turning, but increase their rate when the head turns in the cell s preferred turning direction (either CW or CCW), and decrease their rate when the head turns in the opposite, or antipreferred,

13 H.T. Blair & P.E. Sharp 175 turning direction. AV cells are found abundantly in the medial vestibular nuclei, including NPH, which projects to DTN. Hence, vestibular inputs to DTN are likely to provide the source of the AV signal. In summary, it appears that the rat s head-turning behavior exerts different modulatory effects upon HD cells in LMN and DTN. Head turns modulate the firing rate of DTN HD cells, with minimal effect on their tuning widths. Conversely, head turns seem to modulate the tuning widths of LMN HD cells, with minimal effect on their peak firing rates. This modulation of subcortical HD cells by the rat s head turning behavior may provide important clues about the computational organization of the HD circuit. A RECURRENT NETWORK MODEL The firing properties of HD cells and AV cells in LMN and DTN strongly suggest that these structures are somehow involved in angular path integration. In this section, we present a computational hypothesis to propose how these neurons might be connected together to form a neural circuit that performs angular path integration. Population Coding and Angular Path Integration Each individual HD cell is tuned to have its own preferred firing direction, so that together, the entire population of HD cells provides a distributed representation of any direction that the rat faces. This kind of distributed representation scheme is common to many brain systems, and is sometimes referred to as a neural population vector (Georgopoulos, Kalaska, Caminiti, and Massey, 1984). To understand how HD cells implement a distributed representation of the rat s directional heading, it is helpful to visualize the HD population vector as a layer of HD cells (Figure 8A), arranged so that adjacent cells have adjacent preferred firing directions (note that the layer has circular topography, so that the rightmost neuron in the layer is adjacent to the leftmost neuron). This arrangement is for illustration purposes only; it is not known whether HD cells are topographically organized within any region of the rat brain. The pattern of activity in the layer forms a peak that looks very much like the tuning function of an individual HD cell, because the HD cell that represents the rat s current head direction is firing at a high rate, while cells representing nearby directions are less active, and cells representing directions that are far from the current direction do not fire at all. The activity peak can be centered over any HD cell within the layer, to represent whatever direction the rat s

14 176 The Neural Basis of Navigation head is currently facing. But the activity of HD cells should always form a single peak within the layer, and never assume other patterns that cannot be interpreted as a coherent representation of a single directional heading. When the rat turns its head, the activity of HD cells must be updated to reflect changes in the rat s head direction. For example, if the rat is facing north, and then turns CW by 90º to face east, the peak of HD cell activity must move to the right by 90º, so that HD cells representing east will be maximally active when the head turn is completed (Figure 8B). During the head turn, the peak of activity must shift through the layer of HD cells, and the dynamics of this shifting must be controlled precisely, so that the rate of shifting is exactly proportional to the velocity of head turning. Consequently, the activity peak always remains accurately centered over those HD cells that represent rat s current directional heading. A Recurrent Attractor Network Several theoretical models have proposed that the HD population vector is implemented in the brain by a neural architecture called a recurrent attractor network (Redish et al., 1996; Skaggs et al., 1995; Zhang, 1996a). A recurrent attractor network is a population of neurons that are interconnected in such a way that, over time, the network s activity will reliably converge to a stable pattern, called an attractor state (Hopfield, 1982). HD cell tuning properties indicate that the pattern of activity in the HD cell population vector constantly retains a peak-shaped profile, as in Figure 8. A likely explanation for this observation is that the HD cell activity peak is an attractor state of a recurrent network (Redish et al., 1996; Skaggs et al., 1995; Zhang, 1996a). If so, then this peak-shaped attractor state must be multi-stable, meaning that the peak is equally stable when it is centered over any HD cell within the layer. This allows the activity peak to represent whatever direction the rat s head is facing, without any bias towards representing certain directions over others. Theoretical studies have shown that a multi-stable, peak-shaped attractor state can emerge in a recurrent network with center-surround connectivity, in which neurons excite their nearby neighbors, and inhibit their more distant neighbors (Ben-Yishai, Lev Bar-Or, and Sompolinsky, 1995; Redish et al., 1996; Zhang, 1996a). Figure 8A illustrates how center-surround connectivity might be implemented in the HD network. For clarity, only the connections from a single HD cell are shown, but all cells in the network are assumed to be similarly connected to their neighbors. Excitatory connections are assumed to be direct connections among HD cells, whereas inhibitory connections are made through interneurons. For reasons to be explained below, we suggest that these inhibitory interneurons may reside in DTN. By modulating these interneurons with input from AV cells, the recurrent network can perform angular path integration.

15 H.T. Blair & P.E. Sharp 177 A B North North East LMN HD Cell CW AV Cell Excitation DTN Inhibitory Cell CCW AV Cell Inhibition Figure 8. A recurrent attractor model of the HD circuit. A: When the rat faces north, a stable peak of activity (gray bars) forms over the HD cell representing north, due to a center/surround pattern of excitation and inhibiton among HD cells. B: When the rat turns clockwise to face east, leftward inhibition among HD cells is enhanced by increased input from AV cells, causing the peak of activity to shift rightward until it is centered over the HD cell representing east. Angular Path Integration in a Recurrent Network As shown in Figure 8B, the peak of activity must shift through the layer of HD cells during head turns, to track changes in the rat s directional heading. This shifting behavior can occur in a recurrent attractor network if the pattern of lateral connections between HD cells is modified in an appropriate way (Skaggs et al., 1995; Redish et al., 1996; Zhang, 1996a). For example, during a CW head turn, the activity peak should shift rightward through the layer of HD cells, as in Figure 8B. Such a rightward shift will occur if HD cells excite their rightward neighbors more than leftward neighbors, or inhibit their leftward neighbors more than their rightward neighbors, or both. However, adjusting the strengths of lateral connections among HD cells is risky, because as discussed above, the stability of the peak-shaped attractor state depends upon the center-surround pattern of connections between HD cells. Changing this connection pattern can easily destroy the attractor state, causing the activity peak to become unstable and dissolve into a meaningless pattern. To preserve the shape of the activity peak as it shifts through the layer, the lateral connections among HD cells must be modified in a very precise way. Zhang (1996a) has shown that, in order for the activity peak to retain its shape when it shifts through the layer, the connection strengths between HD cells must be modified according to a derivative rule. Briefly, this rule states that the change in connection strength between any two HD cells must be proportional to the spatial derivative of the connection pattern within the entire layer. Details of this proof are beyond the scope of the present chapter,

16 178 The Neural Basis of Navigation but Zhang (1996b) has shown that the derivative rule can be implemented in a straightforward manner by a relatively simple recurrent network architecture. Anatomy of the Angular Path Integration Circuit In this section, we combine theoretical predictions of recurrent network models with physiological evidence reviewed in the previous section to suggest a specific anatomical organization of the HD attractor network within subcortical regions of the rat brain. LMN sends excitatory projections to DTN, and DTN sends inhibitory projections to LMN (see Figure 5). Hence, DTN is well positioned to provide lateral inhibition among HD cells in LMN. This suggests that a recurrent network for computing the HD signal might be implemented by connections between LMN and DTN. If so, then the primary layer of HD cells might reside in LMN, and lateral inhibition among these LMN HD cells could be routed through inhibitory cells in DTN. Furthermore, since DTN contains AV cells and receives vestibular information from NPH, DTN could be a site where angular velocity signals modulate the strength of lateral inhibition among HD cells, to implement angular path integration during head turns. When the rat s head is not turning, LMN HD cells should provide equal excitation and inhibition to their neighbors on either side, so that the peak of activity remains stationary within the HD cell layer (Figure 8A). However, when the rat turns its head in the clockwise direction, CW AV cells increase their firing rate, and CCW AV cells decrease their firing rate. These AV cells could modulate DTN cells that mediate lateral inhibition among LMN HD cells, so that during CW head turns, LMN HD cells inhibit their rightward neighbors less than their leftward neighbors (Figure 8B). This would cause the peak of activity to shift rightward (CW) through the layer of HD cells, in correspondence with the CW movement of the rat s head, thereby implementing a process of angular path integration. A similar, but reversed, process would occur during CCW turns, causing the activity peak to shift to the left. Physiological Evidence Our anatomical hypothesis might help to explain the firing properties of cells recorded in DTN and LMN. Recall that DTN contains both AV cells and HD cells (Sharp, Tinkelman and Cho, 2001), and the firing rate of many DTN HD cells is modulated by the angular velocity of the rat s head (see Figure 7). These are precisely the properties that would be expected of interneurons that provide angular-velocity modulated lateral connections among HD cells in a recurrent attractor-integrator network (Skaggs et al., 1995; Redish et al., 1996;

17 H.T. Blair & P.E. Sharp 179 Zhang, 1996b; Sharp, Tinkelman and Cho, 2001). However, the tuning width, but not the peak firing rate, of LMN HD cells is modulated by the angular velocity of the rat s head (see Figure 6). In theory, this modulation of LMN tuning widths should not occur in a recurrent network that implements Zhang s (1996a) derivative rule, because strict adherence to this rule would prevent the HD tuning peak from changing its shape as the peak shifts through the layer. The derivative rule predicts that the HD tuning peak should have the same tuning width at all times, regardless of how the rat s head is turning. This is true for HD cells in some brain structures, such as PoS and AD (Blair and Sharp, 1995; Taube and Muller, 1998), but it is not true for LMN HD cells. Are these findings consistent with a role for LMN in angular path integration? Zhang (1996b) has shown that, in order to obtain stable tuning widths in a recurrent attractor-integrator model of the HD circuit, lateral connections among HD cells must be modulated multiplicatively by the AV signal. If modulation of lateral connections by the AV signal is not perfectly multiplicative, then the tuning curve can become slightly skewed during head turns, causing them to change their tuning width in much the same way that LMN HD cells do during head turns. This suggests that the modulation of LMN HD cell tuning widths during head turns might be explained if DTN cells are modulated by angular head velocity in a manner that is not perfectly multiplicative. Further empirical and theoretical studies are needed to examine whether LMN cell firing properties can be fully explained by an attractor-integrator network model. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS As a rat navigates through space, HD cells provide an ongoing signal of the animal s momentary directional heading, and they are thought to provide the neural basis for the rat s sense of direction. Considerable progress has been made in identifying how specific brain structures contribute to generating this directional signal. Available evidence suggests that HD cells rely on two interacting processes to compute the rat s directional heading: landmark orientation and angular path integration (McNaughton et al., 1991). In this chapter, we have argued that cortical regions containing HD cells, such as PoS and RsC, are primarily responsible for landmark orientation. By contrast, subcortical regions, such as DTN and LMN, may be mostly involved in angular path integration. One of the most exciting conclusions to emerge from HD cell research is that the rat HD circuit seems to provide a striking biological example of a recurrent multi-stable attractor network. Networks of this kind seem to be of fundamental importance in neurobiology, since they can perform many basic

18 180 The Neural Basis of Navigation functions such as noise suppression, input selection, short-term memory, integration, and arithmetic multiplication (Ben-Yishai et al., 1995; Salinas and Abbott, 1996). Here we have proposed that a recurrent attractor network for computing the rat s directional heading may reside in the connections between LMN and DTN. Further study of this circuit may yield important insights into the structure and function of biological attractor networks. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Support for this work was provided by NIH (MH11102 and NS35191), the Whitehall Foundation (A94-06) and NSF ( ). REFERENCES Allen G.V., Hopkins D.A. (1989) Mammillary body in the rat: topography and synaptology of projections from the subicular complex, prefrontal cortex, and midbrain tegmentum. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 286: Allen G.V., Hopkins D.A. (1990) Topography and synaptology of mammillary body projections to the mesencephalon and pons in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 301: Barlow J.S. (1964) Inertial navigation as a basis for animal navigation. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 6: 76. Ben-Yishai R., Lev Bar-Or R.L, Sompolinsky H. (1995) Theory of orientation tuning in visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 92: Blair H.T., Sharp P.E. (1995) Anticipatory head-direction signals in anterior thalamus: Evidence for a thalamocortical circuit that integrates angular head motion to compute head direction. Journal of Neuroscience, 15: Blair H.T., Sharp P.E. (1996) Visual and vestibular influences on head-direction cells in the anterior thalamus of the rat. Behavioral. Neuroscience 110: 643. Blair H.T., Cho J., Sharp P.E. (1998) Role of the lateral mammillary nucleus in the rat headdirection circuit: A combined single-unit recording and lesion study. Neuron, 21: Chen L.L., Lin L.H., Barnes C.A., McNaughton B.L. (1994b) Head-direction cells in the rat posterior cortex. II. Contributions of visual and ideothetic information to the directional firing. Experimental Brain Research, 101: Chen L.L., Lin L.H., Green E.J., Barnes C.A., McNaughton B.L. (1994a) Head-direction cells in the rat posterior cortex. I. Anatomical distribution and behavioral modulation. Experimental Brain Research 101: 8. Cho J., Sharp P.E. (2001) Head direction, place, and movement correlates for cells in the rat retrosplenial cortex. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115: Collette T.S., Cartwright B.A., Smith B.A. (1986) Landmark learning and visuo-spatial memory in gerbils. Journal of Comparative Physiology (A), 158: Gallistel C.R. (1990) The Organization of Learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Georgopoulos A.P., Kalaska J.F., Caminiti R., Massey J.T. (1984) The representation of movement direction in the motor cortex: single cell and population studies. In Dynamic

19 H.T. Blair & P.E. Sharp 181 Aspects of Neocortical Function, G.M. Edelman, W.E. Gall, and W.M. Cowan, Eds., (pp ). New York: John Wiley and Sons. Golob E.J., Taube J.S. (1999) Head direction cells in rats with hippocampal or overlying neocortical lesions: evidence for impaired angular path integration. Journal of Neuroscience, 19: Golob E.J., Stackman R.W., Wong A.C., Taube J.S. (2001) On the behavioral significance of head direction cells: neural and behavioral dynamics during spatial memory tasks. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115: Gonzalo-Ruiz A., Alonso A., Sanz J.M., Llinas R.R. (1992) Afferent projections to the mammillary complex of the rat, with special reference to those from surrounding hypothalamic regions. Jounral of Compuational Neurology, 321: Goodridge J.P., Taube J.S. (1995) Preferential use of the landmark navigational system by head-direction cells in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109: Goodridge J.P., Taube J.S. (1997) Interaction between the postsubiculum and anterior thalamus in the generation of head-direction cell activity. Journal of Neuroscience 17: Goodridge J.P., Dudchenko P.A., Worboys K.A., Golob E.J., Taube J.S. (1998) Cue control and head direction cells. Behavioral Neuroscience. 112: Hayakawa T., Zyo K. (1992) Ultrastructural study of ascending projections to the lateral mammillary nucleus of the rat. Anatomy and Embryology, 185: Hopfield, J.J. (1982) Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 79: Knierim J.J., Kudrimoti H.S., McNaughton B.L. (1995) Place cells, head-direction cells, and the learning of landmark stability. The Journal of Neuroscience, 15: Liu R., Chang L., Wickern G. (1984) The dorsal tegmental nucleus: an axoplasmic transport study. Brain Research, 310: McNaughton B.L., Chen L.L., Markus E.J. (1991) "Dead reckoning", landmark learning, and the sense of direction: a neurophysiological and computational hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3: McNaughton B.L., Markus E.J., Wilson M.A., Knierim J.J. (1993) Familiar landmarks can correct for cumulative integration error in the inertially-based dead-reckoning system. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 19: 795. Mittelstaedt M.L., Mittelstaedt M.L. (1980) Homing by path integration in a mammal. Naturwissenschaften, 67: 566. Mittelstaedt M.L., Mittelstaedt M.L. (1982) Homing by path integration. In Avian Navigation, H.Papi, G.Wallraff, Eds., (pp ). Heidelberg-Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Mizumori S.J.Y., Williams J.D. (1993) Directionally selective mnemonic properties of neurons in the lateral dorsal nucleus of the thalamus of rats. Journal of Neuroscience, 13: O Keefe J., Dostrovsky J. (1971) The hippocampus as a spatial map: Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Research, 34: O Keefe J. Nadel L. (1978) The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Ranck J.B. Jr. (1973) Studies on single neurons in dorsal hippocampal formation and septum in unrestrained rats. Experimental Neurology, 41: Ranck J.B. Jr. (1984) Head-direction cells in the deep layers of dorsal presubiculum in freely moving rats. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 10: 599. Redish A.D., Elga A.N., Touretzky D.S. (1996) A coupled attractor model of the rodent head direction system. Network, 7: 671. Salinas E., Abbott L.F. (1996) A model of multiplicative neural responses in parietal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 93: Sharp P.E., Blair H.T., Cho J. (2001) The anatomical and computational basis of the rat headdirection cell signal. Trends in Neurosciences, 24:

20 182 The Neural Basis of Navigation Sharp P.E., Tinkelman A., Cho J. (2001) Angular velocity and head direction cells recorded from the dorsal tegmental nucleus of Gudden in the rat: Implications for path integration in the head direction cell circuit. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115: Sharp P.E., Green C. (1994) Spatial correlates of firing patterns of single cells in the subiculum of the freely moving rat. Journal of Neuroscience, 14: Skaggs W.E., Knierim J.J., Kudrimoti H.S., McNaughton B.L. (1995) A model of the neural basis of the rat s sense of direction, in: Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 7, G. Tesauro, D. Touretzky, T. Leen, eds., Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Stackman R.W., Taube J.S. (1998) Firing properties of rat lateral mammillary single units: head direction, head pitch, and angular head velocity. Journal of Neuroscience, 18: Stackman R.W., Taube J.S. (1997) Firing properties of head-direction cells in the rat anterior thalamic nucleus: Dependence on vestibular input. Journal of Neuroscience, 17: Taube J.S. (1995) Head-direction cells recorded in the anterior thalamic nuclei of freely moving rats. Journal of Neuroscience, 15: 70. Taube J.S., Burton H.L. (1995) Head-direction cell activity monitored in a novel environment and during a cue-conflict situtation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74: Taube J.S., Muller R.U. (1998) Comparisons of head direction cell activity in the postsubiculum and anterior thalamus of freely moving rats. Hippocampus, 8: Taube J.S., Muller R.U., Ranck J.B. Jr. (1990a) Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis. Journal of Neuroscience, 10: 420. Taube J.S., Muller R.U., Ranck J.B. Jr. (1990b) Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. II. Effects of environmental manipulations. Journal of Neuroscience, 10: 420. Van Groen T., Wyss J..M. (1990a) The postsubicular cortex in the rat: characterization of the fourth region of the subicular cortex and its connections. Brain Research, 529: Van Groen T., Wyss J..M. (1990b) Connections of the retrosplenial granular cortex in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 300: Vogt B.A., Miller M.W. (1983) Cortical connections between rat cingulate cortex and visual, motor, and postsubicular cortices. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 216: Wiener S.I. (1993) Spatial and behavioral correlates of striatal neurons in rats performing a self-initiated navigation task. Journal of Neuroscience, 13: Zhang K. (1996b) Representation of spatial orientation by the intrinsic dynamics of the head direction cell ensemble: a theory. Journal of Neuroscience, 16: Zhang K. (1996b) Representing head direction by attractor dynamics and the dynamic shift mechanisms. In Computational Neuroscience: Trends in Research, J. M. Bower, Ed. (pp ) San Diego: Academic Press.

Space and brain: a tabula rasa for the sense of direction. Paul A. Dudchenko

Space and brain: a tabula rasa for the sense of direction. Paul A. Dudchenko Space and brain: a tabula rasa for the sense of direction Paul A. Dudchenko University of Stirling Psychology, School of Natural Sciences Stirling, FK9 4LA United Kingdom Phone: +44 131 650 3531 p.a.dudchenko@stir.ac.uk

More information

Cue Control and Head Direction Cells

Cue Control and Head Direction Cells Behavioral Neuroscience 1998, Vol. 112, No. 4,749-761 Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 735-744/98/$3. Cue Control and Head Direction Cells Jeremy P. Goodridge, Paul A. Dudchenko,

More information

ASSOCIATIVE MEMORY AND HIPPOCAMPAL PLACE CELLS

ASSOCIATIVE MEMORY AND HIPPOCAMPAL PLACE CELLS International Journal of Neural Systems, Vol. 6 (Supp. 1995) 81-86 Proceedings of the Neural Networks: From Biology to High Energy Physics @ World Scientific Publishing Company ASSOCIATIVE MEMORY AND HIPPOCAMPAL

More information

The Head Direction Signal: Origins and Sensory-Motor Integration

The Head Direction Signal: Origins and Sensory-Motor Integration Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2007. 30:181 207 First published online as a Review in Advance on March 6, 2007 The Annual Review of Neuroscience is online at neuro.annualreviews.org This article s doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112854

More information

Navigation: Inside the Hippocampus

Navigation: Inside the Hippocampus 9.912 Computational Visual Cognition Navigation: Inside the Hippocampus Jakob Voigts 3 Nov 2008 Functions of the Hippocampus: Memory and Space Short term memory Orientation Memory consolidation(h.m)

More information

Self-organizing continuous attractor networks and path integration: one-dimensional models of head direction cells

Self-organizing continuous attractor networks and path integration: one-dimensional models of head direction cells INSTITUTE OF PHYSICS PUBLISHING Network: Comput. Neural Syst. 13 (2002) 217 242 NETWORK: COMPUTATION IN NEURAL SYSTEMS PII: S0954-898X(02)36091-3 Self-organizing continuous attractor networks and path

More information

Interaction between the Postsubiculum and Anterior Thalamus in the Generation of Head Direction Cell Activity

Interaction between the Postsubiculum and Anterior Thalamus in the Generation of Head Direction Cell Activity The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1, 1997, 17(23):9315 9330 Interaction between the Postsubiculum and Anterior Thalamus in the Generation of Head Direction Cell Activity Jeremy P. Goodridge and Jeffrey

More information

Y a-t il un pilote dans le cerveau? Données récentes Sur les bases neuronales de l orientation spatiale

Y a-t il un pilote dans le cerveau? Données récentes Sur les bases neuronales de l orientation spatiale Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Marseille NeuroSTIC Grenoble, June 23 24, 2016 Y a-t il un pilote dans le cerveau? Données récentes Sur les bases neuronales de l orientation spatiale Why is it important

More information

A Neural Systems Analysis of Adaptive Navigation

A Neural Systems Analysis of Adaptive Navigation Molecular Neurobiology Copyright 2000 Humana Press Inc. All rights of any nature whatsoever reserved. ISSN0893-7648/98/21(1): 000 000/$0.00 A Neural Systems Analysis of Adaptive Navigation Sheri J. Y.

More information

A Computational Model of Rodent Spatial Learning and Some Behavioral. experiments.

A Computational Model of Rodent Spatial Learning and Some Behavioral. experiments. A Computational Model of Rodent Spatial Learning and Some Behavioral Experiments Karthik Balakrishnan (balakris@cs.iastate.edu) Rushi Bhatt (rushi@cs.iastate.edu) Vasant Honavar (honavar@cs.iastate.edu)

More information

Encoding Spatial Context: A Hypothesis on the Function of the Dentate Gyrus-Hilus System

Encoding Spatial Context: A Hypothesis on the Function of the Dentate Gyrus-Hilus System Encoding Spatial Context: A Hypothesis on the Function of the Dentate Gyrus-Hilus System A. Minai, ECECS Department, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH J. Best, Department of Psychology, Miami University,

More information

Some Thoughts on Place Cells and the Hippocampus

Some Thoughts on Place Cells and the Hippocampus Some Thoughts on Place Cells and the Hippocampus Jeffrey S. Taube* Department of Psychology, Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire HIPPOCAMPUS 9:452 457 (1999) ABSTRACT: This commentary addresses five

More information

Backward Shift of Head Direction Tuning Curves of the Anterior Thalamus: Comparison with CA1 Place Fields

Backward Shift of Head Direction Tuning Curves of the Anterior Thalamus: Comparison with CA1 Place Fields Neuron 52, 717 729, November 22, 2006 ª2006 Elsevier Inc. DOI 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.003 Backward Shift of Head Direction Tuning Curves of the Anterior Thalamus: Comparison with CA1 Place Fields Xintian

More information

EDVARD MOSER AND THE GRID CELLS MICROSTRUCTURE OF A SPATIAL MAP IN THE ENTORHINAL CORTEX

EDVARD MOSER AND THE GRID CELLS MICROSTRUCTURE OF A SPATIAL MAP IN THE ENTORHINAL CORTEX FAMOUS RESEARCHERS EDVARD MOSER AND THE GRID CELLS MICROSTRUCTURE OF A SPATIAL MAP IN THE ENTORHINAL CORTEX Y. TSENKINA EDVARD I. MOSER is a professor of neuroscience and a director of the Norwegian Centre

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation Baran Çürüklü Department of Computer Science and Engineering Mälardalen University Västerås S-721 23, Sweden Abstract. In this paper

More information

Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive 622 The Journal of Neuroscience, January 11, 2006 26(2):622 631 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Head Direction Cell Representations Maintain Internal Coherence during Conflicting Proximal and Distal Cue Rotations:

More information

Distinct Ensemble Codes in Hippocampal Areas CA3 and CA1

Distinct Ensemble Codes in Hippocampal Areas CA3 and CA1 Distinct Ensemble Codes in Hippocampal Areas CA3 and CA1 Stefan Leutgeb, 1 Jill K. Leutgeb, 1 Alessandro Treves, 1,2 May-Britt Moser, 1 Edvard I. Moser 1* 1 Centre for the Biology of Memory, MTFS, Norwegian

More information

A. David Redish and David S. Touretzky y. Computer Science Department and. Carnegie Mellon University. Abstract

A. David Redish and David S. Touretzky y. Computer Science Department and. Carnegie Mellon University. Abstract Separating hippocampal maps A. David Redish and David S. Touretzky y Computer Science Department and Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition Carnegie Mellon University Pittsburgh, PA 15213-3891 October

More information

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields.

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Yingxue Wang, Sandro Romani, Brian Lustig, Anthony Leonardo, Eva Pastalkova Supplementary Materials Supplementary Modeling

More information

How do spatial learning and memory occur in the brain? Coordinated learning of entorhinal grid cells and hippocampal place cells

How do spatial learning and memory occur in the brain? Coordinated learning of entorhinal grid cells and hippocampal place cells 1 How do spatial learning and memory occur in the brain? Coordinated learning of entorhinal grid cells and hippocampal place cells Praveen K. Pilly and Stephen Grossberg * Center for Adaptive Systems Boston

More information

Brain anatomy and artificial intelligence. L. Andrew Coward Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia

Brain anatomy and artificial intelligence. L. Andrew Coward Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia Brain anatomy and artificial intelligence L. Andrew Coward Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia The Fourth Conference on Artificial General Intelligence August 2011 Architectures

More information

Spatial View Cells and the Representation of Place in the Primate Hippocampus

Spatial View Cells and the Representation of Place in the Primate Hippocampus Spatial View Cells and the Representation of Place in the Primate Hippocampus Edmund T. Rolls* Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, England HIPPOCAMPUS 9:467 480 (1999)

More information

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization 1 7.1 Overview This chapter aims to provide a framework for modeling cognitive phenomena based

More information

Cognition 119 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Cognition. journal homepage:

Cognition 119 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Cognition. journal homepage: Cognition 119 (2011) 419 429 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Spatial updating according to a fixed reference direction of a briefly

More information

Evaluating the Effect of Spiking Network Parameters on Polychronization

Evaluating the Effect of Spiking Network Parameters on Polychronization Evaluating the Effect of Spiking Network Parameters on Polychronization Panagiotis Ioannou, Matthew Casey and André Grüning Department of Computing, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey, GU2 7XH, UK

More information

Neural Representation of Space in Rats and Robots

Neural Representation of Space in Rats and Robots Neural Representation of Space in Rats and Robots David S. Touretzky, Hank S. Wan, and A. David Redish Computer Science Department Carnegie Mellon University Pittsburgh, PA 15213-3891 Abstract We describe

More information

Appeared in Neural Computation 5(6): This is the Tech Report version, released as Tech Report CMU-CS

Appeared in Neural Computation 5(6): This is the Tech Report version, released as Tech Report CMU-CS Appeared in Neural Computation 5(6):869-884. This is the Tech Report version, released as Tech Report CMU-CS-93-117. Neural Representation of Space Using Sinusoidal Arrays David S. Touretzky 1 A. David

More information

SPATIAL PROCESSING IN THE BRAIN: The Activity of Hippocampal Place Cells

SPATIAL PROCESSING IN THE BRAIN: The Activity of Hippocampal Place Cells Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2001. 24:459 86 Copyright c 2001 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved SPATIAL PROCESSING IN THE BRAIN: The Activity of Hippocampal Place Cells Phillip J. Best, 1 Aaron M. White,

More information

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Jessica R. Newton and Mriganka Sur Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences Picower Center for Learning & Memory Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge,

More information

Basal Ganglia. Introduction. Basal Ganglia at a Glance. Role of the BG

Basal Ganglia. Introduction. Basal Ganglia at a Glance. Role of the BG Basal Ganglia Shepherd (2004) Chapter 9 Charles J. Wilson Instructor: Yoonsuck Choe; CPSC 644 Cortical Networks Introduction A set of nuclei in the forebrain and midbrain area in mammals, birds, and reptiles.

More information

Why is our capacity of working memory so large?

Why is our capacity of working memory so large? LETTER Why is our capacity of working memory so large? Thomas P. Trappenberg Faculty of Computer Science, Dalhousie University 6050 University Avenue, Halifax, Nova Scotia B3H 1W5, Canada E-mail: tt@cs.dal.ca

More information

Exploring the Functional Significance of Dendritic Inhibition In Cortical Pyramidal Cells

Exploring the Functional Significance of Dendritic Inhibition In Cortical Pyramidal Cells Neurocomputing, 5-5:389 95, 003. Exploring the Functional Significance of Dendritic Inhibition In Cortical Pyramidal Cells M. W. Spratling and M. H. Johnson Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development,

More information

The Physiology of the Senses Chapter 8 - Muscle Sense

The Physiology of the Senses Chapter 8 - Muscle Sense The Physiology of the Senses Chapter 8 - Muscle Sense www.tutis.ca/senses/ Contents Objectives... 1 Introduction... 2 Muscle Spindles and Golgi Tendon Organs... 3 Gamma Drive... 5 Three Spinal Reflexes...

More information

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1 Announcements exam 1 this Thursday! review session: Wednesday, 5:00-6:30pm, Meliora 203 Bryce s office hours: Wednesday, 3:30-5:30pm, Gleason https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=zdw7pvgz0um M Cells M cells

More information

Self-Organization and Segmentation with Laterally Connected Spiking Neurons

Self-Organization and Segmentation with Laterally Connected Spiking Neurons Self-Organization and Segmentation with Laterally Connected Spiking Neurons Yoonsuck Choe Department of Computer Sciences The University of Texas at Austin Austin, TX 78712 USA Risto Miikkulainen Department

More information

Systems Neuroscience Dan Kiper. Today: Wolfger von der Behrens

Systems Neuroscience Dan Kiper. Today: Wolfger von der Behrens Systems Neuroscience Dan Kiper Today: Wolfger von der Behrens wolfger@ini.ethz.ch 18.9.2018 Neurons Pyramidal neuron by Santiago Ramón y Cajal (1852-1934, Nobel prize with Camillo Golgi in 1906) Neurons

More information

Computing Goal Locations from. A. David Redish and David S. Touretzky. To appear in Symbolic Visual Learning, K. Ikeuchi and M. Veloso, eds.

Computing Goal Locations from. A. David Redish and David S. Touretzky. To appear in Symbolic Visual Learning, K. Ikeuchi and M. Veloso, eds. 1 Navigating with Landmarks: Computing Goal Locations from Place Codes A. David Redish and David S. Touretzky Carnegie Mellon University To appear in Symbolic Visual Learning, K. Ikeuchi and M. Veloso,

More information

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Lecture 5:. Reading Assignments: None 1 Projection 2 Projection 3 Convention: Visual Angle Rather than reporting two numbers (size of object and distance to observer),

More information

Place fields and the cognitive map

Place fields and the cognitive map This is the pre-peer reviewed version of the following article: Place fields and the cognitive map Paul A. Dudchenko 1,2 and Emma R. Wood 2 which has been published at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/hipo.22450/full

More information

Persistent Firing Supported by an Intrinsic Cellular Mechanism in a Component of the Head Direction System

Persistent Firing Supported by an Intrinsic Cellular Mechanism in a Component of the Head Direction System The Journal of Neuroscience, April 15, 2009 29(15):4945 4952 4945 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Persistent Firing Supported by an Intrinsic Cellular Mechanism in a Component of the Head Direction System

More information

Space and Context in the Rodent Hippocampal Region

Space and Context in the Rodent Hippocampal Region Space and Context in the Rodent Hippocampal Region Mark C. Fuhs CMU-CS-06-170 November 2006 School of Computer Science Computer Science Department Carnegie Mellon University Pittsburgh, PA 15213 thesis

More information

Reactive agents and perceptual ambiguity

Reactive agents and perceptual ambiguity Major theme: Robotic and computational models of interaction and cognition Reactive agents and perceptual ambiguity Michel van Dartel and Eric Postma IKAT, Universiteit Maastricht Abstract Situated and

More information

Disruption of the Head Direction Cell Signal after Occlusion of the Semicircular Canals in the Freely Moving Chinchilla

Disruption of the Head Direction Cell Signal after Occlusion of the Semicircular Canals in the Freely Moving Chinchilla The Journal of Neuroscience, November 18, 2009 29(46):14521 14533 14521 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Disruption of the Head Direction Cell Signal after Occlusion of the Semicircular Canals in the Freely

More information

Changes in reward contingency modulate the trial-to-trial variability of hippocampal place cells

Changes in reward contingency modulate the trial-to-trial variability of hippocampal place cells Changes in reward contingency modulate the trial-to-trial variability of hippocampal place cells Andrew M. Wikenheiser and A. David Redish J Neurophysiol 106:589-598, 2011. First published 18 May 2011;

More information

Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings. Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014

Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings. Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014 Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014 Goals for the lecture Be able to: Conceptually understand some of the analysis and jargon encountered in a typical (sensory)

More information

Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010

Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010 Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010 Asymmetry in learning in the reverse direction Full recovery from UP using DOWN: initial return to naïve values within 10 minutes,

More information

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:.

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:. Cortical Organization Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:. 2. Secondary cortex: located immediately adjacent to primary cortical areas,

More information

Henry Molaison. Biography. From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Henry Molaison. Biography. From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Henry Molaison From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Henry Gustav Molaison (February 26, 1926 December 2, 2008), known widely as H.M., was an American memory disorder patient who had a bilateral medial

More information

Cerebellum: little brain. Cerebellum. gross divisions

Cerebellum: little brain. Cerebellum. gross divisions Cerebellum The anatomy of the cerebellum and its gross divisions Its principal input and output pathways The organization of the cerebellar cortex Role of climbing vs. mossy fibre input The parallel-fibre/

More information

Spatial Learning and Localization in Animals: A Computational Model and Behavioral Experiments

Spatial Learning and Localization in Animals: A Computational Model and Behavioral Experiments Spatial Learning and Localization in Animals: A Computational Model and Behavioral Experiments Karthik Balakrishnan, Rushi Bhatt, and Vasant Honavar Artificial Intelligence Research Laboratory Department

More information

Informationsverarbeitung im zerebralen Cortex

Informationsverarbeitung im zerebralen Cortex Informationsverarbeitung im zerebralen Cortex Thomas Klausberger Dept. Cognitive Neurobiology, Center for Brain Research, Med. Uni. Vienna The hippocampus is a key brain circuit for certain forms of memory

More information

A sketch of the central nervous system and its origins. MIT 9.14 Classes 31

A sketch of the central nervous system and its origins. MIT 9.14 Classes 31 A sketch of the central nervous system and its origins G. E. Schneider 2014 Part 9: Hypothalamus & Limbic System MIT 9.14 Classes 31 The vertebrate medial pallium; in mammals: the hippocampal formation

More information

Cerebellum: little brain. Cerebellum. gross divisions

Cerebellum: little brain. Cerebellum. gross divisions Cerebellum The anatomy of the cerebellum and its gross divisions Its principal input and output pathways The organization of the cerebellar cortex Role of climbing vs. mossy fibre input The parallel-fibre/

More information

Hector Penagos. Licenciatura en Ffsica, Universidad de las Americas-Puebla, 2000

Hector Penagos. Licenciatura en Ffsica, Universidad de las Americas-Puebla, 2000 Interactions between Anterior Thalamus and Hippocampus during Different Behavioral States in the Rat by Hector Penagos Licenciatura en Ffsica, Universidad de las Americas-Puebla, 2000 SUBMITTED TO THE

More information

Anatomy of the Hippocampus

Anatomy of the Hippocampus Anatomy of the Hippocampus Lecture 3.2 David S. Touretzky September, 2015 Human Hippocampus 2 Human Hippocampus 3 Hippocampus Means Seahorse Dissected human hippocampus next to a specimen of hippocampus

More information

Motor systems III: Cerebellum April 16, 2007 Mu-ming Poo

Motor systems III: Cerebellum April 16, 2007 Mu-ming Poo Motor systems III: Cerebellum April 16, 2007 Mu-ming Poo Population coding in the motor cortex Overview and structure of cerebellum Microcircuitry of cerebellum Function of cerebellum -- vestibulo-ocular

More information

Basics of Computational Neuroscience

Basics of Computational Neuroscience Basics of Computational Neuroscience 1 1) Introduction Lecture: Computational Neuroscience, The Basics A reminder: Contents 1) Brain, Maps,, Networks,, and The tough stuff: 2,3) Membrane Models 3,4) Spiking

More information

Thalamocortical Feedback and Coupled Oscillators

Thalamocortical Feedback and Coupled Oscillators Thalamocortical Feedback and Coupled Oscillators Balaji Sriram March 23, 2009 Abstract Feedback systems are ubiquitous in neural systems and are a subject of intense theoretical and experimental analysis.

More information

Nature Neuroscience doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Characterization of viral injections.

Nature Neuroscience doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Characterization of viral injections. Supplementary Figure 1 Characterization of viral injections. (a) Dorsal view of a mouse brain (dashed white outline) after receiving a large, unilateral thalamic injection (~100 nl); demonstrating that

More information

A Neuromorphic VLSI Head Direction Cell System

A Neuromorphic VLSI Head Direction Cell System > TCAS-I 8378 < 1 A Neuromorphic VLSI Head Direction Cell System Tarek M. Massoud, Timothy K. Horiuchi, Member, IEEE Abstract The head direction (HD) cell system in the brain of mammals is thought to be

More information

Thalamus and Sensory Functions of Cerebral Cortex

Thalamus and Sensory Functions of Cerebral Cortex Thalamus and Sensory Functions of Cerebral Cortex I: To describe the functional divisions of thalamus. II: To state the functions of thalamus and the thalamic syndrome. III: To define the somatic sensory

More information

Mechanosensation. Central Representation of Touch. Wilder Penfield. Somatotopic Organization

Mechanosensation. Central Representation of Touch. Wilder Penfield. Somatotopic Organization Mechanosensation Central Representation of Touch Touch and tactile exploration Vibration and pressure sensations; important for clinical testing Limb position sense John H. Martin, Ph.D. Center for Neurobiology

More information

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m.

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m. Normal CNS, Special Senses, Head and Neck TOPIC: CEREBRAL HEMISPHERES FACULTY: LECTURE: READING: P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center Wednesday, 16 March

More information

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Summarized by Y.-J. Park, M.-H. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang

More information

Relative contributions of cortical and thalamic feedforward inputs to V2

Relative contributions of cortical and thalamic feedforward inputs to V2 Relative contributions of cortical and thalamic feedforward inputs to V2 1 2 3 4 5 Rachel M. Cassidy Neuroscience Graduate Program University of California, San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093 rcassidy@ucsd.edu

More information

9.14 Classes #21-23: Visual systems

9.14 Classes #21-23: Visual systems 9.14 Classes #21-23: Visual systems Questions based on Schneider chapter 20 and classes: 1) What was in all likelihood the first functional role of the visual sense? Describe the nature of the most primitive

More information

Sensory coding and somatosensory system

Sensory coding and somatosensory system Sensory coding and somatosensory system Sensation and perception Perception is the internal construction of sensation. Perception depends on the individual experience. Three common steps in all senses

More information

SPATIAL UPDATING OF SELF-POSITION AND ORIENTATION DURING REAL, IMAGINED, AND VIRTUAL LOCOMOTION

SPATIAL UPDATING OF SELF-POSITION AND ORIENTATION DURING REAL, IMAGINED, AND VIRTUAL LOCOMOTION Research Report SPATIAL UPDATING OF SELF-POSITION AND ORIENTATION DURING REAL, IMAGINED, AND VIRTUAL LOCOMOTION Roberta L. Klatzky, 1 Jack M. Loomis, 2 Andrew C. Beall, 3 Sarah S. Chance, 2 and Reginald

More information

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga,, R. B. Ivry,, and G. R. Mangun,, Norton, 2002. Summarized by Y.-J. Park, M.-H. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang

More information

A model of the interaction between mood and memory

A model of the interaction between mood and memory INSTITUTE OF PHYSICS PUBLISHING NETWORK: COMPUTATION IN NEURAL SYSTEMS Network: Comput. Neural Syst. 12 (2001) 89 109 www.iop.org/journals/ne PII: S0954-898X(01)22487-7 A model of the interaction between

More information

Observational Learning Based on Models of Overlapping Pathways

Observational Learning Based on Models of Overlapping Pathways Observational Learning Based on Models of Overlapping Pathways Emmanouil Hourdakis and Panos Trahanias Institute of Computer Science, Foundation for Research and Technology Hellas (FORTH) Science and Technology

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

Supplementary Figure 1 Information on transgenic mouse models and their recording and optogenetic equipment. (a) 108 (b-c) (d) (e) (f) (g)

Supplementary Figure 1 Information on transgenic mouse models and their recording and optogenetic equipment. (a) 108 (b-c) (d) (e) (f) (g) Supplementary Figure 1 Information on transgenic mouse models and their recording and optogenetic equipment. (a) In four mice, cre-dependent expression of the hyperpolarizing opsin Arch in pyramidal cells

More information

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping Retinotopy & Phase Mapping Fani Deligianni B. A. Wandell, et al. Visual Field Maps in Human Cortex, Neuron, 56(2):366-383, 2007 Retinotopy Visual Cortex organised in visual field maps: Nearby neurons have

More information

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object Supplemental materials for: The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object David Whitney*, David A. Westwood, & Melvyn A. Goodale* *Group on Action and Perception, The

More information

Medical Neuroscience Tutorial

Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Pain Pathways Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Pain Pathways MAP TO NEUROSCIENCE CORE CONCEPTS 1 NCC1. The brain is the body's most complex organ. NCC3. Genetically determined circuits are the foundation

More information

LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization

LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization THE VISUAL SYSTEM Higher Visual Processing Martin Paré Assistant Professor Physiology & Psychology Most of the cortex that covers the cerebral hemispheres is

More information

of specic visual patterns. The activity of these neurons implicitly represents properties like agent-landmark distance and egocentric orientation to v

of specic visual patterns. The activity of these neurons implicitly represents properties like agent-landmark distance and egocentric orientation to v Spatial Cognition and Neuro-Mimetic Navigation: A Model of Hippocampal Place Cell Activity Angelo Arleo and Wulfram Gerstner Centre for Neuro-Mimetic Systems, MANTRA, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology

More information

SOMATIC SENSATION PART I: ALS ANTEROLATERAL SYSTEM (or SPINOTHALAMIC SYSTEM) FOR PAIN AND TEMPERATURE

SOMATIC SENSATION PART I: ALS ANTEROLATERAL SYSTEM (or SPINOTHALAMIC SYSTEM) FOR PAIN AND TEMPERATURE Dental Neuroanatomy Thursday, February 3, 2011 Suzanne S. Stensaas, PhD SOMATIC SENSATION PART I: ALS ANTEROLATERAL SYSTEM (or SPINOTHALAMIC SYSTEM) FOR PAIN AND TEMPERATURE Reading: Waxman 26 th ed, :

More information

Motor Functions of Cerebral Cortex

Motor Functions of Cerebral Cortex Motor Functions of Cerebral Cortex I: To list the functions of different cortical laminae II: To describe the four motor areas of the cerebral cortex. III: To discuss the functions and dysfunctions of

More information

Input-speci"c adaptation in complex cells through synaptic depression

Input-specic adaptation in complex cells through synaptic depression 0 0 0 0 Neurocomputing }0 (00) } Input-speci"c adaptation in complex cells through synaptic depression Frances S. Chance*, L.F. Abbott Volen Center for Complex Systems and Department of Biology, Brandeis

More information

Asymmetrical Fos Expression in the Unilateral 6-OHDA Model in Rats

Asymmetrical Fos Expression in the Unilateral 6-OHDA Model in Rats Asymmetrical Fos Expression in the Unilateral 6-OHDA Model in Rats BY MARY CLARE KANE B.A., University of Illinois at Chicago, 2000 M.A., University of Illinois at Chicago, 2005 THESIS Submitted as partial

More information

Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition. Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT

Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition. Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT 1 Courtesy of Elsevier, Inc., http://www.sciencedirect.com. Used with permission.

More information

Associative Decorrelation Dynamics: A Theory of Self-Organization and Optimization in Feedback Networks

Associative Decorrelation Dynamics: A Theory of Self-Organization and Optimization in Feedback Networks Associative Decorrelation Dynamics: A Theory of Self-Organization and Optimization in Feedback Networks Dawei W. Dong* Lawrence Berkeley Laboratory University of California Berkeley, CA 94720 Abstract

More information

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J.

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J. The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem By Andrew Laguna, S.J. Outline I. Introduction II. The Visual System III. What is the Binding Problem? IV. Possible Theoretical Solutions

More information

VISUAL CORTICAL PLASTICITY

VISUAL CORTICAL PLASTICITY VISUAL CORTICAL PLASTICITY OCULAR DOMINANCE AND OTHER VARIETIES REVIEW OF HIPPOCAMPAL LTP 1 when an axon of cell A is near enough to excite a cell B and repeatedly and consistently takes part in firing

More information

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) What happens beyond the retina? What happens in Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)- 90% flow Visual cortex Information Flow Superior colliculus 10% flow Slide 2 Information

More information

A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences

A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences Marc Pomplun 1, Yueju Liu 2, Julio Martinez-Trujillo 2, Evgueni Simine 2, and John K. Tsotsos 2 1 Department

More information

Chapter 3. Structure and Function of the Nervous System. Copyright (c) Allyn and Bacon 2004

Chapter 3. Structure and Function of the Nervous System. Copyright (c) Allyn and Bacon 2004 Chapter 3 Structure and Function of the Nervous System 1 Basic Features of the Nervous System Neuraxis: An imaginary line drawn through the center of the length of the central nervous system, from the

More information

Cerebellum and spatial cognition: A connectionist approach

Cerebellum and spatial cognition: A connectionist approach Cerebellum and spatial cognition: A connectionist approach Jean-Baptiste Passot 1,2, Laure Rondi-Reig 1,2, and Angelo Arleo 1,2 1- UPMC Univ Paris 6, UMR 7102, F-75005 Paris, France 2- CNRS, UMR 7102,

More information

Ch.20 Dynamic Cue Combination in Distributional Population Code Networks. Ka Yeon Kim Biopsychology

Ch.20 Dynamic Cue Combination in Distributional Population Code Networks. Ka Yeon Kim Biopsychology Ch.20 Dynamic Cue Combination in Distributional Population Code Networks Ka Yeon Kim Biopsychology Applying the coding scheme to dynamic cue combination (Experiment, Kording&Wolpert,2004) Dynamic sensorymotor

More information

A Neural Model of Context Dependent Decision Making in the Prefrontal Cortex

A Neural Model of Context Dependent Decision Making in the Prefrontal Cortex A Neural Model of Context Dependent Decision Making in the Prefrontal Cortex Sugandha Sharma (s72sharm@uwaterloo.ca) Brent J. Komer (bjkomer@uwaterloo.ca) Terrence C. Stewart (tcstewar@uwaterloo.ca) Chris

More information

Neural response time integration subserves. perceptual decisions - K-F Wong and X-J Wang s. reduced model

Neural response time integration subserves. perceptual decisions - K-F Wong and X-J Wang s. reduced model Neural response time integration subserves perceptual decisions - K-F Wong and X-J Wang s reduced model Chris Ayers and Narayanan Krishnamurthy December 15, 2008 Abstract A neural network describing the

More information

An Investigation of the. Postsubiculum s role in Spatial. Cognition

An Investigation of the. Postsubiculum s role in Spatial. Cognition An Investigation of the Postsubiculum s role in Spatial Cognition David Bett Doctor of Philosophy The University of Stirling 2011-1 - Abstract The hippocampal formation has been implicated in spatial formation

More information

Multi-joint limbs permit a flexible response to unpredictable events

Multi-joint limbs permit a flexible response to unpredictable events Exp Brain Res (1997) 117:148±152 Springer-Verlag 1997 RESEARCH NOTE E.M. Robertson R.C. Miall Multi-joint limbs permit a flexible response to unpredictable events Received: 24 March 1997 / Accepted: 7

More information

A Hybrid Model for Rodent Spatial Learning and Localization1

A Hybrid Model for Rodent Spatial Learning and Localization1 A Hybrid Model for Rodent Spatial Learning and Localization1 Rushi Bhatt Karthik Balakrishnan Vasant Honavar Dept. of Computer Science Allstate Research Dept. of Computer Science Iowa State University

More information

Cognitive Modelling Themes in Neural Computation. Tom Hartley

Cognitive Modelling Themes in Neural Computation. Tom Hartley Cognitive Modelling Themes in Neural Computation Tom Hartley t.hartley@psychology.york.ac.uk Typical Model Neuron x i w ij x j =f(σw ij x j ) w jk x k McCulloch & Pitts (1943), Rosenblatt (1957) Net input:

More information

Head direction maps remain stable despite grid map fragmentation

Head direction maps remain stable despite grid map fragmentation NEURAL CIRCUITS ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 29 March 2012 doi: 10.3389/fncir.2012.00009 despite grid map fragmentation Jonathan R. Whitlock 1 and Dori Derdikman 2 * 1 Kavli Institute for Systems

More information

Grid Cell Firing May Arise From Interference of Theta Frequency Membrane Potential Oscillations in Single Neurons

Grid Cell Firing May Arise From Interference of Theta Frequency Membrane Potential Oscillations in Single Neurons Grid Cell Firing May Arise From Interference of Theta Frequency Membrane Potential Oscillations in Single Neurons Michael E. Hasselmo,* Lisa M. Giocomo, and Eric A. Zilli HIPPOCAMPUS 17:1252 1271 (2007)

More information