Information/Orientation Subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised as an Indicator of Suspicion of Insufficient Effort*

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Information/Orientation Subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised as an Indicator of Suspicion of Insufficient Effort*"

Transcription

1 The Clinical Neuropsychologist /00/ $ , Vol. 14, No. 1, pp Swets & Zeitlinger Information/Orientation Subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised as an Indicator of Suspicion of Insufficient Effort* Yana Suchy 1 and Jerry J. Sweet 1, 2 1 Evanston Hospital and 2 Northwestern University Medical School ABSTRACT The utility of the Information/Orientation (IO) subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised in identifying insufficient effort was examined. Performances of 50 benefit-seeking patients were compared to previously generated rms based on a clinical sample. Of the 50 benefit-seeking patients, 6 (12%) had IO scores that were outside the performance ranges of the entire clinical sample. An additional 6 of 50 (24%) had performances that were comparable to less than 2% of the rmative sample. These results are consistent with previously published estimates of insufficient effort base rates in benefit-seeking populations and demonstrate the utility of IO as an indicator of suspicion of insufficient effort. Assessment of effort and motivation to perform poorly or well (hereafter referred to as effort ) has become a standard component of neuropsychological evaluations, especially in situations in which poor performance will result in benefit or gain. When assessing effort, clinicians face a number of challenges. First, measures used for effort assessment often have low sensitivity. This is related to several factors, including the fact that most malingerers are t likely to perform below their true ability on all administered measures (Goebel, 1983), and that some may opt to generate long response latencies or exhibit psychological distress, rather than make errors (Beetar & Williams, 1995; Iverson, 1995; Tenhula & Sweet, 1996). Additionally, sensitivity is directly related to the stringency of utilized cutoffs, which in turn reflect whether loss in specificity is acceptable to a given clinician. Given that low specificity (i.e., misidentification of n-malingerers as malingerers) amounts to accusing patients with bona fide deficits of criminal acts (e.g., fraud, perjury), many clinicians prefer to decrease sensitivity in favor of increasing specificity (Sweet, 1999, p. 258). However, true sensitivity in the real world is virtually impossible to determine, as at least some true malingerers likely go undetected (Faust, Hart, & Guilmette, 1988; Heaton, Smith, Lehman, & Vogt, 1978). The second challenge in effort assessment is that, as neuropsychologists become more sophisticated at identifying insufficient effort or malingering, their clients, too, become increasingly sophisticated at feigning deficits in a more realistic fashion. In fact, in some cases clients may even be coached by their attorneys (Youngjohn, 1995). Finally, because clinicians need to consider multiple measures of effort and ability in their assessment, there is the additional challenge of performing the assessment within reasonable time limits. The challenges in effort assessment may best be addressed by identifying complex performance patterns within commonly used measures of ability, especially if such patterns defy (mis) * Address correspondence to: Yana Suchy, Neuropsychology Service, Department of Psychiatry, Evanston Northwestern Healthcare, 2650 Ridge Avenue, Evanston, IL USA. yana.suchy@prodigy.net. Accepted for publication: November 25, 1999.

2 INFORMATION/ORIENTATION AS AN INDICATOR OF SUSPICION 57 conceptions held by the general public about the effects of brain dysfunction on a particular cognitive domain (e.g., for common misconceptions about memory, see Wiggins & Brandt, 1988). Characterization of such patterns potentially affords development of multiple indices of effort within a given battery of ability measures, thereby reducing the need to administer additional time-consuming tasks. This approach may also provide a further safeguard against coaching, as evaluation of effort within actual ability measures can be based on complex rmative tables and contain multiple moderator variables, such as age and education. Examples of the use of performance patterns include characterization of differential performance on easy and difficult items of the Booklet Category Test (Bolter, Pica, & Zych, 1985; Tenhula & Sweet, 1996), or comparison of performances on Vocabulary and Digit Span subtests of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale-Revised (Millis & Ross, 1996; Mittenberg, Theroux-Richera, Zielinski, & Heilbronner, 1995). Within widely used memory measures, examples include characterization of profiles of four memory indices from the California Verbal Learning Test (e.g., Millis & Putnam, 1997; Millis, Putnam, Adams, & Ricker, 1995), or comparison of General Memory and Attention/Concentration indices from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R: Mittenberg, Azrin, Millsaps, & Heilbronner, 1993). The success of these approaches in separating neurologic profiles from insufficient effort served as a springboard for the present study. The purpose of the present study was to determine whether recently generated rmative tables for Information/Orientation (IO) scores from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R) (Sweet, Suchy, Leahy, Abramowitz, & Nowinski, in press) could be used for identification of infrequent performance patterns that may serve to raise suspicion regarding insufficient effort. The attractiveness of these tables for assessment of effort lies in the fact that they consider three moderator variables (age, and performances on the Visual Reproduction II and Logical Memory II subtests of the WMS-R). The inclusion of these moderator variables would likely make feigning of a realistically impaired performance difficult without access to the actual testing materials and rmative data. These rmative tables were based on a sample that was free of individuals seeking a benefit or compensation. The IO subtest of the WMS-R contains orientation questions, as well as questions about autobiographical information (e.g., age, date of birth). The possibility that some benefit-seeking individuals may choose to feign impairments on these items is suggested by prior research that has shown that individuals simulating brain dysfunction may fail autobiographical questions (Rosenfeld, Ellwanger, & Sweet, 1995; Wiggins & Brandt, 1988). Although many clinicians are w making the transition to the more recent third edition of the Wechsler Memory Scale, the WMS-III, the results of the present study can be generalized to this newer version, as the IO subtest has t changed at all and the memory variables are expressed as T scores, rather than raw scores, and remain virtually unchanged conceptually. Additionally, WMS-R may continue to be used by some clinicians for some time yet, in particular in legal settings when considerable expertise with any given instrument is required. In order to determine the effectiveness of IO performance in detecting insufficient effort, IO scores in a sample of individuals whose evaluations occurred within the context of possible benefit-seeking were compared to those in a rmative clinical sample. IO scores that were significantly below the scores observed in the rmative clinical sample were deemed to indicate a red flag for insufficient effort. As prior research has estimated that base rates of insufficient effort on neuropsychological testing among benefit-seeking populations range from 7.5% to 33% (Binder, 1993; Frederick, Sarfaty, Johnston, & Powel, 1994; Trueblood & Schmidt, 1993), it was hypothesized that unusually low performance on IO would be found in a comparable proportion of the present benefit-seeking sample.

3 58 YANA SUCHY AND JERRY J. SWEET METHOD Participants Participants consisted of 50 benefit-seeking patients (34 male and 16 female) referred to Evanston Hospital Neuropsychology Service for neuropsychological evaluations. Benefit-seeking was defined as any circumstance in which poor performance on neurocognitive measures could potentially lead to obtaining, or maintaining, a direct benefit, whether as a lump-sum, or in the form of a disability or social-security income. In the present sample, 44 patients were referred for neuropsychological evaluations for the purpose of providing an independent opinion pertaining to litigated claims of damages or disability claims, and 3 patients were referred for assessment of whether continuation of disability benefits was warranted. For the 3 remaining patients, the presence of benefit-seeking issues was indicated in the available records and in the database, although the exact circumstances of the referral could t be determined in retrospect. The patients ranged in age from 20 to 77 years (M = 43.28, SD = 13.62) and had between 9 and 20 years of education (M = 13.70, SD = 2.62). Forty patients had presumably suffered a traumatic brain injury (), 3 had suffered solvent exposures, 3 had suffered an axic event, 2 had had cerebrovascular accidents, 1 had suffered an electrical injury, and one presented with depression. As a validation of the presence of varying degrees of effort among the present sample, the frequency of participants performances that were below previously established cut-offs on available indices of insufficient effort are presented in Table 1. Specifically, the following performances were considered to represent positive evidence for insufficient effort if they were below empirically established cut-offs based on prior research: Multi Digit Memory Test (MDMT; Martin, Bolter, Todd, Gouvier, & Niccolls, 1993); California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) recognition score, and trials 1 through 5 total (Millis et al., 1995; Sweet et al., in press); Booklet Category total errors, total errors from subtests I and II, number of errors from subtest VII, and number of errors on easy items (Tenhula & Sweet, 1996); and Rey Memory Fifteen Item Test (MFIT) number of correct rows (Arnett, Hammeke, & Schwarz, 1995). Note that the above indices were examined only for the 40 patients with traumatic brain injury because these measures have t been validated for other populations. As previously recommended (Ben-Porath, 1994), the specific cut-offs are t presented here in order to protect the findings of malingering research against coaching by n-psychologists. Procedure Raw IO scores in the present sample were compared to those obtained by a rmative clinical sample of 312 individuals (Sweet et al., in press). Demographic and diagstic characteristics of the rmative sample were comparable to those of the present sample. Specifically, the rmative sample consisted of 312 individuals (162 males and 150 females) referred to the Evanston Hospital Neuropsychology Service for clinical assessment. They Table 1. Frequency of Failed Insufficient Effort Indices among 40 Participants with Traumatic Brain Injury in the Present Benefit-Seeking Sample. Insufficient Effort Index Available protocols Percent failed MDMT total correct Rey MFIT rows CVLT five trial total recognition Booklet Category subtests I&IItotal subtest VII total errors easy item errors Bolter items errors Note. MDMT = Multi Digit Memory Test; CLVT = California Verbal Learning Test; MFIT = Memory for Fifteen Items Test.

4 INFORMATION/ORIENTATION AS AN INDICATOR OF SUSPICION 59 ranged in age from 17 to 92 years (M = 55.5, SD = 17.7) and had 8 to 23 years of education (M = 14.7, SD = 2.7). Diagses included cerebral vascular accident (n = 64), traumatic brain injury (50), probable Alzheimer s Disease (35), Parkinson s Disease (25), intracranial neoplasm (21), seizure disorder (12), multiple sclerosis (3), and other degenerative dementias (3). Additionally, 52 patients had multiple neurological conditions or had cognitive impairment as a result of less frequent conditions in our referral base, such as encephalitis, lupus erythematosus, and HIV. Ather 28 individuals had neurological disorders for which a specific etiological condition could t be identified. Of the remaining 19 patients, 14 met DSM criteria for major depression and 5 neurologically rmal individuals had n-psychotic psychological conditions for which more specific diagses could t be identified. The rmative tables that are based on this sample are organized by age and performances on the Visual Reproduction II (VRII) and Logical Memory II (LMII) subtests of the WMS-R. The actual ranges of scores obtained by participants in the rmative sample (hereafter referred to as Liberal Ranges ) are summarized in the flowchart in Figure 1, and are presented as values in dark gray rectangles on the right-hand side of the chart under the heading IO Raw Scores (values presented in white diamonds will be discussed later in this section). When following the flowchart, readers should start in the upper left-hand corner. The first row of the flowchart indicates that, for those rmative sample participants who had VRII or LMII T scores above 60, IO raw scores ranged from 13 to 14 (T scores were generated by a standard conversion from percentile values available in the WMS- R manual; Wechsler, 1987). It can also be gleaned from the first row of the chart that VRII or LMII T scores above 60 were present in 47 of the 312 rmative sample participants (this value is presented in parentheses in the dark gray rectangle). Following the flowchart further, it can be seen that age became an important moderator variable for those rmative sample participants whose VRII or LMII T scores were t above 60. For example, the second row of the flowchart shows that, for participants whose VRII or LMII T scores were t above 60 and whose ages were below 46, raw IO scores ranged from 10 to 14. Subsequent branches of the flowchart indicate IO performance ranges for the remainder of the rmative sample. Note that the Liberal Ranges characterize the entire clinical rmative sample. In other words, participants in the rmative sample had IO scores outside the Liberal Ranges, implying 100% specificity. The numbers of patients in each branch of the flowchart are presented in parentheses under each IO range. The correspondence of the flowchart with the rmative tables was evaluated by five graduate students who were otherwise t involved in this study. An examination of frequency distributions of scores in the rmative sample further revealed that certain rmative participants exhibited somewhat outlying IO scores. Specifically, among rmative sample participants whose memory T scores were t above 60 and whose ages were below 46 (see the second row of the flowchart), only 5 had an IO score below 12. Additionally, only 1 patient in the 49-to-69 age range with at least one average memory performance (i.e., T score between 40 and 60) had an IO score below (see the third row of the flowchart). These 6 participants represent only 1.9% (i.e., 6 of 312) of the rmative sample, and from that perspective their IO scores appear to be unusually low. For this reason, where appropriate, IO scores of individuals in the present benefit-seeking sample were also compared to Constricted Ranges (presented in Figure 1 as values in white diamonds, where appropriate). These ranges simply indicate that, among individuals below age 45, IO scores below 12 are highly unusual, and, among individuals between the ages 49 and 69 who have at least one average memory performance, IO scores below are also highly unusual. For both the Liberal Ranges and the Constricted Ranges, the incidence of unusual profiles (i.e., IO scores outside of the ranges indicated in Figure 1) among the benefit-seeking patients was calculated. The number of profiles outside of the Liberal Ranges and the Constricted Ranges were compared to each other to determine which set of ranges would have better diagstic utility. In order to investigate potential causes of unusual IO scores other than insufficient effort, available records of all participants with unusual IO scores were carefully examined. Particular attention was paid to the presence of factors that would affect clinical decision-making with respect to effort, such as time since injury, results of neuroimaging studies, and environmental factors during testing. Additionally, when available, other indices of effort were examined. Formal statistical evaluation of these results was t conducted because of variability among test batteries used in individual cases.

5 60 YANA SUCHY AND JERRY J. SWEET MEMORY T-SCORES AGE (in years) IO RAW SCORES START HERE VRII or LMII > to 14 (n=47) Age below to 14 (n=72) 12 to 14 (n=67) VRII or LMII to 60 Age 46 to 69 9 to 14 (n=79) to 14 (n=78) Age > 69 7 to 14 (n=43) VRII or LMII 30 to 39 Age 46 to 69 8 to 13 (n=25) Age > 69 4 to 14 (n=29) VRII and LMII < 30 3 to 14 (n=17) Fig. 1. Flowchart summarizing the expected ranges of IO scores associated with patients ages and Logical Memory II or Visual Reproduction II T scores, derived from a rmative sample of 312 clinical referrals.

6 INFORMATION/ORIENTATION AS AN INDICATOR OF SUSPICION 61 RESULTS Liberal Ranges In the present benefit-seeking sample, 6 (i.e., 12 %) out of 50 individuals had IO scores that were outside of the Liberal Ranges (i.e., outside of the entire rmative sample). Of these 6, 5 were in litigation and 1 was seeking benefit, the exact nature of which could t be determined in retrospect. Constricted Ranges An additional 6 of 50 (i.e., 12%) benefit-seeking participants had IO scores outside of the Constricted Ranges, which, combined with the frequency outside the Liberal Ranges, is a total of 24% of the benefit-seeking sample demonstrating unusually poor profiles. Of these additional 6 patients, 5 were in litigation and 1 was assessed to determine readiness to return to work following a period of disability. In contrast, only 6 of 312 (i.e., 1.9%) of the rmative sample had IO scores outside of the Constricted Ranges. Although true sensitivity of this approach cant be determined because the actual incidence of insufficient effort in this sample is t kwn, the present result is consistent with previously reported insufficient effort base rates among benefit-seeking populations (Binder, 1993; Frederick et al., 1994; Trueblood & Schmidt, 1993). Review of Normative Sample Records Available records for the 6 rmative sample cases who had unusual IO scores were also examined. This review revealed that 5 of the 6 patients had identifiable, legitimate, neurological explanations for their poor IO performances. Specifically, 2 of these cases were inpatients recovering from acute neurologic conditions (i.e., 2 weeks status post status epilepticus superimposed on multiple sclerosis exacerbation; and 3 weeks status post cerebrovascular accident superimposed on radiation necrosis and residual neoplasm); 1 was 6 weeks status post mild traumatic brain injury associated with seizure activity; 1 was a case of an early onset Alzheimer s Disease (meeting criteria established by the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders [NINCD] and the Alzheimer s Disease and Related Disorders Association; McKhann et al., 1984); and 1 was assessed following recovery from encephalitis with significant neurologic sequelae confirmed by abrmal CT, MRI, and EEG results. The sixth patient had recovered from viral encephalitis and, in addition to continued neurocognitive deficits, exhibited poor cooperation during testing, apparently due to personality characteristics, which may have resulted in less than optimal effort. Review of Benefit-Seeking Sample Records The characteristics of the 12 benefit-seeking patients whose IO scores were unusually low are summarized in Table 2. Of these 12 patients, 2 had histories suggesting that their performances may have been unusually low for reasons other than insufficient effort. In one case, an individual (#4 in Table 2) with frank memory limitations was brought by her attorney from ather state. This patient erroneously referred to the town in which testing took place as Chicago, when in fact she was in Evanston, a town in the Chicago area. Additionally, this patient did t kw the name of the hospital (Evanston Hospital). It is conceivable that these two errors would t have been committed had this patient been familiar with the geographical area in which testing took place, in which case her performance would have been within rmal limits. Additionally, a review of this patient s file did t indicate any other suspicious or unusually low performances. For these reasons, the suspicion regarding effort raised by low IO scores can be disregarded in this case. The second individual (#5 in Table 2) whose poor IO performance was deemed possibly legitimate had suffered a documented mild traumatic brain injury in a motor vehicle accident 5 weeks previously. Although some patients recover fully within such a time period, a more commonly used upper limit for resolution of symptoms of a mild head injury is 3 months (Levin et al., 1987). Because once again there were other indicators of insufficient effort within this patient s file, it was determined that the benefit of the doubt was in order in this case.

7 62 YANA SUCHY AND JERRY J. SWEET Table 2. Demographic and Psychometric Characteristics of Patients with Secondary Gain who Exhibited Unusually Poor Information-Orientation Profiles. Patient # Age Educ. VRII T LMII T IO Other FAILED Effort Indices Other PASSED Effort Indices Etiology * 6* 9* 10* * 12* CVLT total = 26 CVLT recognition = 12 MDMT = 39/72 MFIT rows = 0 CVLT total = 41 CVLT recognition = 12 ne ne t applicable ne ne CVLT total = 41 MDMT = 35/72 CVLT total = 31 Category total = 96 Category VII = 6 Category easy errors = 4 CVLT total = 41 CVLT recognition = 10 Category VII = 6 CVLT total = 34 None administered MFIT items, CLVT, Category Category MDMT, MFIT, CVLT, Category MDMT, MFIT, CVLT, Category t applicable CVLT, Category CVLT, Category MDMT, MFIT, CVLT recognition, Category CVLT recognition, MFIT Category, CVLT, MDMT, MFIT Category, CVLT, MDMT /Out of town /Five weeks post MVA axia Note. Educ. = years of education; VRII T= Visual Reproduction II T score; LMII T = Logical Memory II T score; MDMT= Multi Digit Memory Test; CLVT = California Verbal Learning Test; MFIT = Memory for Fifteen Items Test; = traumatic brain injury. * IO performances that were outside of the Liberal Ranges.

8 INFORMATION/ORIENTATION AS AN INDICATOR OF SUSPICION 63 Review of available records of the remaining 10 benefit-seeking patients whose IO performances were identified as unusually poor did t reveal any neurologic conditions or environmental variables that could explain their poor performances. Overall, of the 10 patients, 2 exhibited one other positive clinical indicator of insufficient effort, 3 exhibited two other positive clinical indicators of insufficient effort, 1 had three other indicators, and 1 patient had five other indicators. For 2 of these 10 patients, indicators of insufficient effort, other than unusual IO scores, were found in their profile. Finally, 1 patient (#6 in Table 2) had suffered axia, rather than, and for this reason effort indices were t applicable in this case. Taken together, these results demonstrate that the sensitivity of IO as a red flag for insufficient effort improves when Constricted Ranges are used, with practical loss of specificity. In other words, less than 2% of clinical referrals performed outside of these ranges, and these consisted primarily of patients with independently confirmed serious neurologic findings for whom assessment of poor effort due to benefitseeking is typically t an issue. DISCUSSION The present study demonstrates the utility of the IO subtest from the WMS-R as a potential indicator of insufficient effort or malingering among patients for whom benefit-seeking issues could be identified. Specifically, when IO performances among benefit-seeking patients were compared to those among the clinical rmative sample, 10 of 50 benefit-seeking patients (i.e., 20%) were identified as likely exhibiting some degree of insufficient effort. Suspicion was raised, but insufficient effort was ruled out regarding 2 of the 50 (i.e., 4%) patients. In contrast, suspicion was raised in only 6 of 312 (i.e., 1.9%) of the clinical patients, with insufficient effort able to be ruled out easily in 5 of the 6 due to neurologic conditions being present. The sixth patient exhibited poor cooperation during testing, and his effort was, in fact, questionable due to psychological/personality issues. These results support the use of an unusual IO performance as a red-flag regarding insufficient effort, which should then be further investigated. In order to evaluate the appropriateness of more versus less stringent cut-offs, the present study identified insufficient effort by comparing IO performances of the present sample to two sets of rmative ranges: (a) the Liberal Ranges, which reflect the range of performance of the entire rmative sample, and (b) the Constricted Ranges, which reflect the range of performance of 98.1% of the rmative sample. The results demonstrate that the use of Constricted Ranges yields better sensitivity, with real practical decrease in specificity. In other words, Constricted Ranges yielded better sensitivity by identifying twice as many suspicious profiles among benefit-seeking patients as did the Liberal Ranges. In addition, Constricted Ranges also identified one rmative sample patient who was t fully cooperative during testing due to personality issues. Although the use of Constricted Ranges also led to incorrect suspicion regarding 5 of the 312 clinical patients, specificity did t suffer because, as ted earlier, review of patient records revealed that these 5 patients had acute or severe neurological dysfunctions that were independently verifiable and capable of producing their performances. In other words, given their clinical circumstances, these patients would t be misclassified as exhibiting insufficient effort despite poor IO performances. Although the numbers of patients red-flagged by the present method is consistent with insufficient effort base-rate estimates reported in the literature (Binder, 1993; Frederick et al., 1994; Trueblood & Schmidt, 1993), the exact sensitivity is t possible to determine as true incidence of insufficient effort in this sample (or in any clinical sample) is t kwn. Additionally, as each individual patient who malingers or exhibits insufficient effort may use different strategies or rely on a different set of misconceptions about brain dysfunction, it is highly unlikely that all persons with insufficient effort would perform outside of rmal ranges on IO. In fact, an examination of red-flagged patients performances on other available indicators of insuffi-

9 64 YANA SUCHY AND JERRY J. SWEET cient effort confirms this interpretation. Of the 7 individuals who exhibited suspicious performances on other measures (see Table 2, column Other Failed Effort Indices for patients number 1, 2, 3, 9, 10,, & 12), 6 performed within rmal limits on a number of other insufficient effort indices (patient #1 did t have other indices available; see Table 2, column Other Passed Effort Indices ). Specifically, considering the performances of the 7 patients on MDMT, MFIT, two CLVT indices (total and recognition), and four Booklet Category indices (total errors, total errors on subtests I and II, total errors on subtest VII, and total easy item errors), these patients performed within previously established rmal limits on a total of 25 other indices (3.6 per patient on average). This observation suggests that one index of insufficient effort may be highly sensitive, and that a multitude of indices may need to be examined with any given patient. This high variability among patients performances can also be seen in Table 1, which shows frequencies with which individual effort indices were failed in the present sample. The need to use multiple strategies to identify insufficient effort and possible malingering has been stressed repeatedly in the relevant literature (Nies & Sweet, 1994; Sweet, 1999). A question remains as to whether the present method may have flagged some of the benefitseeking sample patients incorrectly as exhibiting insufficient effort. Because hard evidence of feigning, such as a less-than-chance performance on a forced choice test, is rarely available in most clinical situations (and did t occur in the present sample), answering this question can only be accomplished by inferential means. Specifically, in the present study, patients were inferred to exhibit insufficient effort or malingering if they met three criteria: (a) the absence of clinical circumstance that could account for unusually poor performance, (b) seeking of a reward that is contingent on a poor performance, and (c) performing more poorly on IO than patients t seeking rewards. It could, of course, be argued that causes other than insufficient effort may be responsible for poor performances under these circumstances. However, in the very least, confluence of the three criteria suggests that a relationship exists between poor performance and the reward contingency. In the present benefit-seeking sample, 10 of 50 participants (i.e., 20%) met these criteria. Of te is that 3 of the benefit-seeking patients who met all three of the above criteria otherwise performed within rmal limits on other available indicators of insufficient effort. Although this result may simply represent yet ather instantiation of the inconsistency and variability in performances among persons with insufficient effort, it also suggests that single unusual performances may be more likely to occur in the context of seeking compensation/benefit than in other clinical settings. The simple fact that these unusual performances occur within otherwise apparently valid profiles suggests that they may have occurred due to purposeless effort fluctuations, which may represent a symptom of as yet t fully understood psychological or psychiatric sequelae of undergoing litigation. The results of the present study have both clinical and theoretical implications. First on a clinical level, the results support the utility of the rmative tables generated by Sweet et al. (in press) for identification of unusual performances that may occur due to insufficient effort. The fact that these tables include three moderator variables (i.e., LMII, VRII, and age) represents an important advance and may lend new direction to future rmative studies. The present results further demonstrate that the presence of one, or even several, rmal performances on indices of insufficient effort does t adequately rule out an unusual, n-neurologic profile in a benefit-seeking patient. Rather, it may simply indicate that the test taker, if in fact feigning deficits, is sophisticated. In contrast, deficient performances on all indices may indicate an unsophisticated, naive malingerer. Future research may attempt to investigate whether a ratio of failed to n-failed measures of effort may help predict the proportion of common clinical measures of ability on which feigning of deficits occurred. Additionally, future research may investigate whether a relationship exists between the types of effort measures that are failed by a given patient and the types of measures of abil-

10 INFORMATION/ORIENTATION AS AN INDICATOR OF SUSPICION 65 ity on which feigning of deficits occurred. Finally, as caution needs to be exercised when generalizing these results to populations in other geographical regions or in different clinical settings, perhaps more extensive rms need to be developed. As mentioned above, one of the limitations of the present study is that the generalizability of the rmative sample to other populations is t clear at this time. Similarly, the generalizability of the results obtained from the present benefitseeking sample to other benefit-seeking populations is t clear, as the levels of sophistication among individuals who feign deficits is likely to vary. In addition, it needs to be ted that the utility of the present method seems to be limited to young and middle-aged patients, as clinical IO ranges among patients above the age of 70 are too broad for use in identifying poor effort. Finally, individuals who feign deficits in a consistent manner (i.e., feign very poor performance both on visual and auditory memory and on the IO subtest) would t be detected by the present method. REFERENCES Arnett, P., Hammeke, T., & Schwartz, L. (1995). Quantitative and qualitative performance on Rey s 15-items test in neurological patients and dissimulators. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9, Beetar,J., & Williams, J. (1995). Malingering response styles on the Memory Assessment Scales and symptom validity tests. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 10, Ben-Porath, Y. (1994). The ethical dilemma of coached malingering research. Psychological Assessment, 6, Binder, L. (1993). Assessment of malingering after mild head trauma with the Portland Digit Recognition Test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, Bolter, J.F., Pica, J.J., & Zych, K. (1985). Item error frequencies on the Halstead Category Test: An index of performance validity. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Academy of Neuropsychology, Philadelphia, PA. Faust, D., Hart, K., & Guilmette, T.J. (1988). Pediatric malingering: The capacity of children to fake believable deficits on neuropsychological testing. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, Frederick, R., Sarfaty, S., Johnston, J.D., & Powel, J. (1994). Validation of a detector of response bias on a forced-choice test of nverbal ability. Neuropsychology, 8, Goebel, R. (1983). Detection of faking on the Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39, Heaton, R.K., Smith, H.H. Jr., Lehman, R.A.W., & Vogt, A.J. (1978). Prospects for faking believable deficits on neuropsychological testing. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, Iverson, G. (1995). Qualitative aspects of malingered memory deficits. Brain Injury, 9, Levin, H.S., Mattis, S., Ruff, R., Eisenberg, H.M., Marshall, L.F., Tabaddor, K., High, W.M., & Frankowski, R.F. (1987). Neurobehavioral outcome following mir head injury: A three-center study. Journal of Neurosurgery, 66, Martin, R., Bolter, J., Todd, M., Gouvier, W.D., & Niccolls, R. (1993). Effects of sophistication and motivation on the detection of malingered memory performance using a computerized forced-choice task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., & Stadlan, E.M. (1984). Clinical diagsis of Alzheimer s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer s disease. Neurology, 34, Millis, S., Putnam, S., Adams, K., & Ricker, J. (1995). The California Verbal Learning Test in the detection of incomplete effort in neuropsychological evaluation. Psychological Assessment, 7, Millis, S., & Putnam, S. (1997). The California Verbal Learning Test in the assessment of financially compensable mild head injury: Further developments. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, Millis, S., & Ross, S. (1996). Dissimulation indices of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised: A replication and extension. Presented at the European Meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society, Veldhoven, Netherlands. Mittenberg, W., Azrin, R., Millsaps, & Heilbronner, R. (1993). Identification of malingered head injury on the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. Psychological Assessment, 5, Mittenberg, W., Theroux-Fichera, S., Zielinski, R., & Heilbronner, R. (1995). Identification of malingered head injury on the Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Battery. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,, Nies, K., & Sweet, J.J. (1994). Neuropsychological assessment and malingering: A critical review of past and present strategies. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 9,

11 66 YANA SUCHY AND JERRY J. SWEET Rosenfeld, J.P., Ellwanger, J., & Sweet, J.J. (1995). Detecting simulated amnesia with event-related brain potentials. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 19, 1. Sweet, J.J. (1999). Malingering: Differential diagsis. In J. Sweet (Ed.), Forensic neuropsychology: Fundamentals and practice (pp ). Royersford, PA: Swets & Zeitlinger Publishers. Sweet, J.J., Suchy, Y., Leahy, B., Abramowitz, C., & Nowinski, C. (in press). Normative clinical relationships between orientation and memory: Age as an important moderator variable. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. Sweet, J.J., Wolfe, P., Sattlberger, L., Numan, B., Rosenfeld, P., Clingerman, S., & Nies, K. (in press). Further investigation of traumatic brain injury versus insufficient effort on the California Verbal learning Test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. Tenhula, W., & Sweet, J. (1996). Double cross-validation of the Booklet Category Test in detecting malingered traumatic brain injury. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10, Trueblood, W., & Schmidt, M. (1993). Malingering and other validity considerations in the neuropsychological evaluations of mild head injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Memory Scale (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Wiggins, E., & Brandt, J. (1988). The detection of simulated amnesia. Law and Human Behavior, 12, Youngjohn, J.R. (1995). Confirmed attorney coaching prior to neuropsychological evaluation. Psychological Assessment, 2,

Effects of Coaching on Detecting Feigned Cognitive Impairment with the Category Test

Effects of Coaching on Detecting Feigned Cognitive Impairment with the Category Test Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 15, No. 5, pp. 399 413, 2000 Copyright 2000 National Academy of Neuropsychology Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0887-6177/00 $ see front matter PII S0887-6177(99)00031-1

More information

Performance profiles and cut-off scores on the Memory Assessment Scales

Performance profiles and cut-off scores on the Memory Assessment Scales Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) 489 496 Performance profiles and cut-off scores on the Memory Assessment Scales Sid E. O Bryant a, Kevin Duff b, Jerid Fisher c, Robert J. McCaffrey a,d,

More information

Ecological Validity of the WMS-III Rarely Missed Index in Personal Injury Litigation. Rael T. Lange. Riverview Hospital.

Ecological Validity of the WMS-III Rarely Missed Index in Personal Injury Litigation. Rael T. Lange. Riverview Hospital. This is the authors version of a paper that will be published as: Lange, Rael T. Lange and Sullivan, Karen A. and Anderson, Debbie (2005) Ecological validity of the WMS-III Rarely Missed Index in personal

More information

The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status Effort Scale

The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status Effort Scale Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27 (2012) 190 195 The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status Effort Scale Julia Novitski 1,2, Shelly Steele 2, Stella Karantzoulis 3, Christopher

More information

Comparison of Predicted-difference, Simple-difference, and Premorbid-estimation methodologies for evaluating IQ and memory score discrepancies

Comparison of Predicted-difference, Simple-difference, and Premorbid-estimation methodologies for evaluating IQ and memory score discrepancies Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) 363 374 Comparison of Predicted-difference, Simple-difference, and Premorbid-estimation methodologies for evaluating IQ and memory score discrepancies Reid

More information

THE VALIDITY OF THE LETTER MEMORY TEST AS A MEASURE OF MEMORY MALINGERING: ROBUSTNESS TO COACHING. A dissertation presented to.

THE VALIDITY OF THE LETTER MEMORY TEST AS A MEASURE OF MEMORY MALINGERING: ROBUSTNESS TO COACHING. A dissertation presented to. THE VALIDITY OF THE LETTER MEMORY TEST AS A MEASURE OF MEMORY MALINGERING: ROBUSTNESS TO COACHING A dissertation presented to the faculty of the College of Arts and Sciences of Ohio University In partial

More information

Discriminant Function Analysis of Malingerers and Neurological Headache Patients Self- Reports of Neuropsychological Symptoms

Discriminant Function Analysis of Malingerers and Neurological Headache Patients Self- Reports of Neuropsychological Symptoms Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 13, No. 6, pp. 561 567, 1998 Copyright 1998 National Academy of Neuropsychology Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0887-6177/98 $19.00.00 PII S0887-6177(97)00072-3

More information

Effects of severe depression on TOMM performance among disability-seeking outpatients

Effects of severe depression on TOMM performance among disability-seeking outpatients Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 21 (2006) 161 165 Effects of severe depression on TOMM performance among disability-seeking outpatients Y. Tami Yanez, William Fremouw, Jennifer Tennant, Julia Strunk,

More information

The Albany Consistency Index for the Test of Memory Malingering

The Albany Consistency Index for the Test of Memory Malingering Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27 (2012) 1 9 The Albany Consistency Index for the Test of Memory Malingering Jessica H. Gunner 1, *, Andrea S. Miele 1, Julie K. Lynch 2, Robert J. McCaffrey 1,2 1

More information

Chapter 23. Detection of Children s Malingering on Raven s Standard Progressive Matrices*

Chapter 23. Detection of Children s Malingering on Raven s Standard Progressive Matrices* Chapter 23 Detection of Children s Malingering on Raven s Standard Progressive Matrices* R. Kim McKinzey, Jörg Prieler, and John Raven** Abstract A formula for detecting faked Raven s Standard Progressive

More information

Head injury and the ability to feign neuropsychological deficits

Head injury and the ability to feign neuropsychological deficits Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) 37 48 Head injury and the ability to feign neuropsychological deficits Chad D. Vickery, David T.R. Berry, Chantel S. Dearth, Victoria L. Vagnini, Raymond

More information

Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan

Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan Grant L. Iverson, Ph.D, Professor Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation Harvard Medical School & Red Sox

More information

The Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM): normative data from cognitively intact, cognitively impaired, and elderly patients with dementia

The Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM): normative data from cognitively intact, cognitively impaired, and elderly patients with dementia Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) 455 464 The Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM): normative data from cognitively intact, cognitively impaired, and elderly patients with dementia Gordon Teichner,

More information

SYMPTOM VALIDITY TESTING OF FEIGNED DISSOCIATIVE AMNESIA: A SIMULATION STUDY

SYMPTOM VALIDITY TESTING OF FEIGNED DISSOCIATIVE AMNESIA: A SIMULATION STUDY Psychology, Crime & Law, 2002, Vol. 8, pp. 311 318 SYMPTOM VALIDITY TESTING OF FEIGNED DISSOCIATIVE AMNESIA: A SIMULATION STUDY HARALD MERCKELBACH*, BEATRIJS HAUER and ERIC RASSIN Department of Experimental

More information

Your choice of SVTs is fundamental to the Slick et al criteria Paul Green Ph.D. paulgreen@shaw.ca www.wordmemorytest.com Central to the criteria is the presence of cognitive symptom exaggeration or feigning

More information

Minimizing Misdiagnosis: Psychometric Criteria for Possible or Probable Memory Impairment

Minimizing Misdiagnosis: Psychometric Criteria for Possible or Probable Memory Impairment Original Research Article DOI: 10.1159/000215390 Accepted: January 30, 2009 Published online: April 28, 2009 Minimizing Misdiagnosis: Psychometric Criteria for Possible or Probable Memory Impairment Brian

More information

KEVIN J. BIANCHINI, PH.D., ABPN

KEVIN J. BIANCHINI, PH.D., ABPN KEVIN J. BIANCHINI, PH.D., ABPN Slick et al., 1999 Bianchini et al., 2005 4 4 Criterion A: Evidence of significant external incentive Criterion B: Evidence from physical evaluation 1. Probable effort

More information

The unexamined lie is a lie worth fibbing Neuropsychological malingering and the Word Memory Test

The unexamined lie is a lie worth fibbing Neuropsychological malingering and the Word Memory Test Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 17 (2002) 709 714 The unexamined lie is a lie worth fibbing Neuropsychological malingering and the Word Memory Test David E. Hartman Private Practice/Chicago Medical

More information

Criterion validity of the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II) after traumatic brain injury

Criterion validity of the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II) after traumatic brain injury Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 143 149 Criterion validity of the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II) after traumatic brain injury Monica L. Jacobs, Jacobus Donders

More information

Qualitative scoring of the Rey 15-Item Memory Test in a forensic population

Qualitative scoring of the Rey 15-Item Memory Test in a forensic population Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School 2002 Qualitative scoring of the Rey 15-Item Memory Test in a forensic population James A. Martin Louisiana State

More information

Interpretive Report. Client Information

Interpretive Report. Client Information Interpretive Report Developed by Michelle R. Widows, PhD, Glenn P. Smith, PhD, and PAR Staff Client Information Client name: Sample Client Client ID: SIMS Test date: 08/12/2013 Date of birth: 02/03/1975

More information

Exaggerated MMPI-2 symptom report in personal injury litigants with malingered neurocognitive deficit

Exaggerated MMPI-2 symptom report in personal injury litigants with malingered neurocognitive deficit Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 673 686 Exaggerated MMPI-2 symptom report in personal injury litigants with malingered neurocognitive deficit Glenn J. Larrabee Suite 202, 630 South Orange

More information

Factors Influencing the Face Validity of Effort Tests: Timing of Warning and Feedback

Factors Influencing the Face Validity of Effort Tests: Timing of Warning and Feedback University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers Graduate School 2 Factors Influencing the Face Validity of Effort Tests: Timing

More information

Characterization of the Medical Symptom Validity Test in evaluation of clinically referred memory disorders clinic patients

Characterization of the Medical Symptom Validity Test in evaluation of clinically referred memory disorders clinic patients Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 753 761 Characterization of the Medical Symptom Validity Test in evaluation of clinically referred memory disorders clinic patients Abstract Laura L.S. Howe

More information

Using Neuropsychological Experts. Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD

Using Neuropsychological Experts. Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD Using Neuropsychological Experts Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD Prepared for Advocate. Arizona Association for Justice/Arizona Trial Lawyers Association. September, 2011 Neurocognitive Associates 9813 North

More information

Use of the California Verbal Learning Test to Detect Proactive Interference in the Traumatically Brain Injured

Use of the California Verbal Learning Test to Detect Proactive Interference in the Traumatically Brain Injured Use of the California Verbal Learning Test to Detect Proactive Interference in the Traumatically Brain Injured Bobbi Numan, Jerry J. Sweet, and Charan Ranganath Northwestern University, Evanston Hospital

More information

Detection and diagnosis of malingering in electrical injury

Detection and diagnosis of malingering in electrical injury Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 20 (2005) 365 373 Detection and diagnosis of malingering in electrical injury Kevin Bianchini a,b, Jeffrey M. Love a,1, Kevin W. Greve a,b,, Donald Adams c Abstract

More information

Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers

Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers Graduate School 2002 Portland Digit Recognition Test the Test of Memory Malingering and standard neuropsychological

More information

Commentary on Delis and Wetter, Cogniform disorder and cogniform condition: Proposed diagnoses for excessive cognitive symptoms

Commentary on Delis and Wetter, Cogniform disorder and cogniform condition: Proposed diagnoses for excessive cognitive symptoms Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 683 687 Abstract Commentary Commentary on Delis and Wetter, Cogniform disorder and cogniform condition: Proposed diagnoses for excessive cognitive symptoms

More information

Demystifying the Neuropsychological Evaluation Report. Clinical Neuropsychologist 17 March 2017 Program Director, Neurobehavioral Program

Demystifying the Neuropsychological Evaluation Report. Clinical Neuropsychologist 17 March 2017 Program Director, Neurobehavioral Program Demystifying the Neuropsychological Evaluation Report Jennifer R. Cromer, PhD BIAC Annual Conference Clinical Neuropsychologist 17 March 2017 Program Director, Neurobehavioral Program 84% of neuropsychologists

More information

Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised*

Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised* The Clinical Neuropsychologist -//-$., Vol., No., pp. - Swets & Zeitlinger Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised* Rodney D. Vanderploeg, John A. Schinka, Tatyana Jones, Brent J. Small,

More information

Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples

Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 16 (2001) 183±191 Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples Grant L. Iverson* Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, 2255 Wesbrook

More information

Clinical Utility of Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised and Predicted IQ Discrepancies in Closed Head Injury

Clinical Utility of Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised and Predicted IQ Discrepancies in Closed Head Injury @ Pergamon Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 12, No. 8, pp. 757 762, 1997 Copyright 1997 Nationaf Academy ofneuropsychology Printed inthe USA, All rights reserved 0887-6177/97$17.00+.00 PIIS0887-6177(97)OO049-8

More information

Simulated subaverage performance on the Block Span task of the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales- Fifth Edition

Simulated subaverage performance on the Block Span task of the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales- Fifth Edition Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Master's Theses Graduate School 2011 Simulated subaverage performance on the Block Span task of the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales- Fifth Edition

More information

Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores

Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 20 (2005) 1043 1051 Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores Brian L.

More information

Donald A. Davidoff, Ph.D., ABPDC Chief, Neuropsychology Department, McLean Hospital Assistant Professor of Psychology, Harvard Medical School

Donald A. Davidoff, Ph.D., ABPDC Chief, Neuropsychology Department, McLean Hospital Assistant Professor of Psychology, Harvard Medical School Donald A. Davidoff, Ph.D., ABPDC Chief, Neuropsychology Department, McLean Hospital Assistant Professor of Psychology, Harvard Medical School Interests: Adult/Geriatric/Forensic Neuropsychology ddavidoff@mclean.harvard.edu

More information

What s Wrong With My Client: Understanding Psychological Testing in Order to Work Effectively With Your Expert

What s Wrong With My Client: Understanding Psychological Testing in Order to Work Effectively With Your Expert What s Wrong With My Client: Understanding Psychological Testing in Order to Work Effectively With Your Expert Common Standardized, Norm Referenced Psychological Tests: Diagnostic ( Personality ) Tests:

More information

Responses to DSM-5. DSM-5 and Malingering. DSM-5: Development and Implementation. Oxford Medicine Online

Responses to DSM-5. DSM-5 and Malingering. DSM-5: Development and Implementation. Oxford Medicine Online Oxford Medicine Online You are looking at 1-10 of 2272 items for: DSM Responses to DSM-5 Joel Paris Print Publication Year: 2015 Published Online: Apr 2015 ISBN: 9780199395095 eisbn: 9780190243982 DOI:

More information

Relationships among postconcussional-type symptoms, depression, and anxiety in neurologically normal young adults and victims of mild brain injury $

Relationships among postconcussional-type symptoms, depression, and anxiety in neurologically normal young adults and victims of mild brain injury $ Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 16 2001) 435±445 Relationships among postconcussional-type symptoms, depression, and anxiety in neurologically normal young adults and victims of mild brain injury

More information

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 11 18 The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Abstract Ralph M. Reitan, Deborah Wolfson Reitan Neuropsychology

More information

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist M------- P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL EVALUATION Name: Date of Birth: Date of Evaluation: 05-28-2015 Tests Administered: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth

More information

Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition

Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 917 924 Abstract Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition Alison J. Donnell a, Neil Pliskin a, James Holdnack

More information

Improving Accuracy in the. through the use of Technology

Improving Accuracy in the. through the use of Technology Improving Accuracy in the Assessment of Malingering through the use of Technology Lisa Drago Piechowski, PhD, ABPP American School of Professional Psychology, Washington DC Key Points Assessment of malingering

More information

Clinical Study Depressive Symptom Clusters and Neuropsychological Performance in Mild Alzheimer s and Cognitively Normal Elderly

Clinical Study Depressive Symptom Clusters and Neuropsychological Performance in Mild Alzheimer s and Cognitively Normal Elderly Hindawi Publishing Corporation Depression Research and Treatment Volume 2011, Article ID 396958, 6 pages doi:10.1155/2011/396958 Clinical Study Depressive Symptom Clusters and Neuropsychological Performance

More information

CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Course Syllabus, Spring 2018 Columbia University

CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Course Syllabus, Spring 2018 Columbia University CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Course Syllabus, Spring 2018 Columbia University Instructor: E mett McCaskill, PhD Office: 356 SchExt, Columbia University; 415-O Milbank Hall, Barnard College Email: e.mccaskill@columbia.edu

More information

Medical Symptom Validity Test Performance Following Moderate-Severe Traumatic Brain Injury: Expectations Based on Orientation Log Classification

Medical Symptom Validity Test Performance Following Moderate-Severe Traumatic Brain Injury: Expectations Based on Orientation Log Classification Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 32 (2017) 339 348 Medical Symptom Validity Test Performance Following Moderate-Severe Traumatic Brain Injury: Expectations Based on Orientation Log Classification Abstract

More information

Malingering (AADEP Position Paper) The gross volitional exaggeration or fabrication of symptoms/dysfunction for the purpose of obtaining substantial m

Malingering (AADEP Position Paper) The gross volitional exaggeration or fabrication of symptoms/dysfunction for the purpose of obtaining substantial m Malingering in Pain Management Gerald M. Aronoff, M.D. Medical Director, Carolina Pain Associates, Charlotte, NC, USA General Principles Treating physician vs IME physician Subjective history is not fact

More information

The vulnerability to coaching across measures of malingering

The vulnerability to coaching across measures of malingering Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School 2007 The vulnerability to coaching across measures of malingering Adrianne M. Brennan Louisiana State University

More information

WPE. WebPsychEmpiricist

WPE. WebPsychEmpiricist McKinzey, R. K., Podd, M., & Kreibehl, M. A. (6/25/04). Concurrent validity of the TOMM and LNNB. WebPsychEmpiricist. Retrieved (date), from http://wpe.info/papers_table.html WPE WebPsychEmpiricist Concurrent

More information

Chapter 1 Applications and Consequences of Psychological Testing

Chapter 1 Applications and Consequences of Psychological Testing Chapter 1 Applications and Consequences of Psychological Testing Topic 1A The Nature and Uses of Psychological Testing The Consequences of Testing From birth to old age, people encounter tests at all most

More information

Neuropsychological Testing (NPT)

Neuropsychological Testing (NPT) Neuropsychological Testing (NPT) POLICY Psychological testing (96101-03) refers to a series of tests used to evaluate and treat an individual with emotional, psychiatric, neuropsychiatric, personality

More information

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) 703 708 Test review Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., 2002 1. Test description The Trail Making Test

More information

Forensic Neuropsychology: are we there yet?

Forensic Neuropsychology: are we there yet? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 827 845 Forensic Neuropsychology: are we there yet? Jim Hom The Neuropsychology Center, 9400 N. Central Expressway, Suite 904, Dallas, TX 75231, USA Accepted

More information

(Received 30 March 1990)

(Received 30 March 1990) Person, individ. Diff. Vol. II, No. 11, pp. 1153-1157, 1990 0191-8869/90 $3.00 + 0.00 Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved Copyright 1990 Pergamon Press pic ESTIMATING PREMORBID INTELLIGENCE BY

More information

The Use of Significant Others to Enhance the Detection of Malingerers From Traumatically Brain-Injured Patients

The Use of Significant Others to Enhance the Detection of Malingerers From Traumatically Brain-Injured Patients Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 15, No. 6, pp. 465 477, 2000 Copyright 2000 National Academy of Neuropsychology Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0887-6177/00 $ see front matter PII S0887-6177(00)00058-5

More information

Neuropsychological Performance in Cannabis Users and Non-Users Following Motivation Manipulation

Neuropsychological Performance in Cannabis Users and Non-Users Following Motivation Manipulation University at Albany, State University of New York Scholars Archive Psychology Honors College 5-2010 Neuropsychological Performance in Cannabis Users and Non-Users Following Motivation Manipulation Michelle

More information

Memory for Complex Pictures: Development and Validation of Digit Test of Effort

Memory for Complex Pictures: Development and Validation of Digit Test of Effort University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers Graduate School 2007 Memory for Complex Pictures: Development and Validation of

More information

25 HIGHLIGHTS ON THE USE OF THE MMPI/MMPI- 2 IN A NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TEST BATTERY 1

25 HIGHLIGHTS ON THE USE OF THE MMPI/MMPI- 2 IN A NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TEST BATTERY 1 25 HIGHLIGHTS ON THE USE OF THE MMPI/MMPI- 2 IN A NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TEST BATTERY 1 DATE 9/10/13 James N. Butcher Professor Emeritus University of Minnesota The MMPI scales have been shown to make a valid

More information

Base Rates of Impaired Neuropsychological Test Performance Among Healthy Older Adults

Base Rates of Impaired Neuropsychological Test Performance Among Healthy Older Adults Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 13, No. 6, pp. 503 511, 1998 Copyright 1998 National Academy of Neuropsychology Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0887-6177/98 $19.00.00 PII S0887-6177(97)00037-1

More information

10/5/2015. Advances in Pediatric Neuropsychology Test Interpretation Part I: Importance of Considering Normal Variability. Financial Disclosures

10/5/2015. Advances in Pediatric Neuropsychology Test Interpretation Part I: Importance of Considering Normal Variability. Financial Disclosures Advances in Pediatric Neuropsychology Test Interpretation: Importance of Considering Normal Variability and Performance Variability Brian L. Brooks, PhD Alberta Children s Hospital University of Calgary

More information

A confirmatory factor analysis of the WMS-III in a clinical sample with crossvalidation in the standardization sample

A confirmatory factor analysis of the WMS-III in a clinical sample with crossvalidation in the standardization sample Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 629 641 A confirmatory factor analysis of the WMS-III in a clinical sample with crossvalidation in the standardization sample D. Bradley Burton a,, Joseph

More information

21/05/2018. Today s webinar will answer. Presented by: Valorie O Keefe Consultant Psychologist

21/05/2018. Today s webinar will answer. Presented by: Valorie O Keefe Consultant Psychologist Today s webinar will answer. 1. What is the RBANS, and how is the updated version different than the original version? 2. What are the neurocognitive areas assessed by the RBANS and what scores are available?

More information

Trends in Symptom Validity, Memory and Psychological Test Performance as. Functions of Time and Malingering Rating. A Thesis. Submitted to the Faculty

Trends in Symptom Validity, Memory and Psychological Test Performance as. Functions of Time and Malingering Rating. A Thesis. Submitted to the Faculty Trends in Symptom Validity, Memory and Psychological Test Performance as Functions of Time and Malingering Rating A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of Drexel University by Maryellen McClain in partial

More information

A Dissertation. Submitted to the Faculty. Xavier University. in Partial Fulfillment of the. Requirements for the Degree of. Doctor of Psychology

A Dissertation. Submitted to the Faculty. Xavier University. in Partial Fulfillment of the. Requirements for the Degree of. Doctor of Psychology A Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of Xavier University in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Psychology by Julianne Gast, M.A. January, 2009 Approved: Christine M.

More information

CHAPTER 5. The intracarotid amobarbital or Wada test: unilateral or bilateral?

CHAPTER 5. The intracarotid amobarbital or Wada test: unilateral or bilateral? CHAPTER 5 Chapter 5 CHAPTER 5 The intracarotid amobarbital or Wada test: unilateral or bilateral? SG Uijl FSS Leijten JBAM Arends J Parra AC van Huffelen PC van Rijen KGM Moons Submitted 2007. 74 Abstract

More information

NEUROCOGNITIVE VARIABLES UNDERLYING GROUP PERFORMANCE ON A MEASURE OF EFFORT: THE MEDICAL SYMPTOM VALIDITY TEST (MSVT) Julie Hart Covert, M.S.

NEUROCOGNITIVE VARIABLES UNDERLYING GROUP PERFORMANCE ON A MEASURE OF EFFORT: THE MEDICAL SYMPTOM VALIDITY TEST (MSVT) Julie Hart Covert, M.S. NEUROCOGNITIVE VARIABLES UNDERLYING GROUP PERFORMANCE ON A MEASURE OF EFFORT: THE MEDICAL SYMPTOM VALIDITY TEST (MSVT) Julie Hart Covert, M.S. Dissertation Prepared for the Degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

More information

International Journal of Forensic Psychology Copyright Volume 1, No. 2 SEPTEMBER 2004 pp

International Journal of Forensic Psychology Copyright Volume 1, No. 2 SEPTEMBER 2004 pp International Journal of Forensic Psychology Copyright 2004 Volume 1, No. 2 SEPTEMBER 2004 pp. 94-98 Commentary on The Detection of Feigned Uncoached and Coached Posttraumatic Stress Disorder with the

More information

CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Course Syllabus, Spring 2015 Columbia University

CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Course Syllabus, Spring 2015 Columbia University CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Course Syllabus, Spring 2015 Columbia University Instructor: E mett McCaskill, PhD Office: 356 SchExt, Columbia University; 415-O Milbank Hall, Barnard College Email: ew87@columbia.edu

More information

The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System Tower Test Resilience to Response Bias

The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System Tower Test Resilience to Response Bias Ursidae: The Undergraduate Research Journal at the University of Northern Colorado Volume 1 Number 2 Article 2 January 2012 The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System Tower Test Resilience to Response

More information

Memory Retraining with Adult Male Alcoholics

Memory Retraining with Adult Male Alcoholics Archives of C/inica/ Neuropsychology, Vol. 4, pp. 227-232. 1989 0887.6177189 $3.00 +.oo Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. Copyright 0 1989 National Academy of Neuropsychologists Memory Retraining

More information

Neuropsychological test performance of Hawaii high school athletes: Hawaii ImPACT normative data

Neuropsychological test performance of Hawaii high school athletes: Hawaii ImPACT normative data 1 Neuropsychological test performance of Hawaii high school athletes: Hawaii ImPACT normative data William T. Tsushima PhD, Ross Oshiro MS, and Daniel Zimbra BA Abstract Objective: Establishing normative

More information

CHAPTER 17: HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY CHAPTER 18: NEUROPSYCHOLOGY

CHAPTER 17: HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY CHAPTER 18: NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OUTLINE CHAPTER 17: HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY Role of Health Psychologists Stress & Illness Pain Management CHAPTER 18: NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Brain trauma Disorders of brain atrophy Neuro-assessment Professor Fazakas-DeHoog

More information

NEUROBEHAVIORAL EVALUATION OF HOUSEHOLD EXPOSURE TO DURSBAN 1

NEUROBEHAVIORAL EVALUATION OF HOUSEHOLD EXPOSURE TO DURSBAN 1 RAYMOND SINGER, PH.D. A Professional Association 444444444444444444444444444444444444444444444444444 36 Alondra Road / Santa Fe, New Mexico / 87508 Alternate office: 180 E. 79th Street / Suite 1-C / New

More information

Diagnostic accuracy of the Structured Inventory of Malingered Symptomatology (SIMS) in detecting instructed malingering

Diagnostic accuracy of the Structured Inventory of Malingered Symptomatology (SIMS) in detecting instructed malingering Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 145 152 Diagnostic accuracy of the Structured Inventory of Malingered Symptomatology (SIMS) in detecting instructed malingering Harald Merckelbach a,, Glenn

More information

An Initial Validation of Virtual Human Administered Neuropsychological Assessments

An Initial Validation of Virtual Human Administered Neuropsychological Assessments Annual Review of Cybertherapy and Telemedicine 2017 123 An Initial Validation of Virtual Human Administered Neuropsychological Assessments Thomas D. PARSONS a,*, Paul SCHERMERHORN b, Timothy MCMAHAN a,

More information

Practical Neuropsychology for the NZ setting; from Assessment Planning to Formulation of Practical Recommendations. Dr Susan Shaw

Practical Neuropsychology for the NZ setting; from Assessment Planning to Formulation of Practical Recommendations. Dr Susan Shaw Practical Neuropsychology for the NZ setting; from Assessment Planning to Formulation of Practical Recommendations. Dr Susan Shaw Outline This workshop is based upon practical experience Includes what

More information

Healthy Children Get Low Scores Too: Prevalence of Low Scores on the NEPSY-II in Preschoolers, Children, and Adolescents

Healthy Children Get Low Scores Too: Prevalence of Low Scores on the NEPSY-II in Preschoolers, Children, and Adolescents Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 25 (2010) 182 190 Healthy Children Get Low Scores Too: Prevalence of Low Scores on the NEPSY-II in Preschoolers, Children, and Adolescents Brian L. Brooks 1, *, Elisabeth

More information

Determining causation of traumatic versus preexisting. conditions. David Fisher, Ph.D., ABPP, LP Chairman of the Board PsyBar, LLC

Determining causation of traumatic versus preexisting. conditions. David Fisher, Ph.D., ABPP, LP Chairman of the Board PsyBar, LLC Determining causation of traumatic versus preexisting psychological conditions David Fisher, Ph.D., ABPP, LP Chairman of the Board PsyBar, LLC 952 285 9000 Part 1: First steps to determine causation Information

More information

Psychiatric Mis-Diagnosis in Pediatric Pain Problems

Psychiatric Mis-Diagnosis in Pediatric Pain Problems Psychiatric Mis-Diagnosis in Pediatric Pain Problems Richard Barnum, M.D. Somatization Disorders Conversion Disorder DSM IV Somatization Disorder A. A history of many physical complaints beginning before

More information

Psychiatric Mis-Diagnosis in Pediatric Pain Problems. DSM IV Somatization Disorder. DSM IV Somatization Disorder. Somatization Disorders

Psychiatric Mis-Diagnosis in Pediatric Pain Problems. DSM IV Somatization Disorder. DSM IV Somatization Disorder. Somatization Disorders Psychiatric Mis-Diagnosis in Pediatric Pain Problems Richard Barnum, M.D. Somatization s Conversion DSM IV Somatization A. A history of many physical complaints beginning before age 30 years, occurring

More information

Measurement and Classification of Neurocognitive Disability in HIV/AIDS Robert K. Heaton Ph.D University of California San Diego Ancient History

Measurement and Classification of Neurocognitive Disability in HIV/AIDS Robert K. Heaton Ph.D University of California San Diego Ancient History Measurement and Classification of Neurocognitive Disability in HIV/AIDS Robert K. Heaton Ph.D University of California San Diego Ancient History Group Means for NP and MMPI Variables N=381 Consecutive

More information

TOPF (Test of Pre-Morbid Function)

TOPF (Test of Pre-Morbid Function) TEST OF PREMORBID FUNCTIONING TOPF (Test of Pre-Morbid Function) Case Studies TOPF (Test of Pre-Morbid Function) Case Studies Case Study 1 Client C is a 62-year-old White male with 18 years of education,

More information

MEGAN CIOTA, PH.D. Curriculum Vitae Address: 3939 Houma Boulevard, #223 Metairie, LA Phone Number: (504) Fax: (504)

MEGAN CIOTA, PH.D. Curriculum Vitae Address: 3939 Houma Boulevard, #223 Metairie, LA Phone Number: (504) Fax: (504) MEGAN CIOTA, PH.D. Curriculum Vitae Address: 3939 Houma Boulevard, #223 Metairie, LA 70006 Phone Number: (504) 780-1702 Fax: (504) 780-1705 EDUCATION Ph.D. Major: Clinical Psychology, Behavioral Medicine

More information

Process of a neuropsychological assessment

Process of a neuropsychological assessment Test selection Process of a neuropsychological assessment Gather information Review of information provided by referrer and if possible review of medical records Interview with client and his/her relative

More information

A Guide to Clinical Interpretation of the Test of Variables of Attention (T.O.V.A. TM )

A Guide to Clinical Interpretation of the Test of Variables of Attention (T.O.V.A. TM ) A Guide to Clinical Interpretation of the Test of Variables of Attention (T.O.V.A. TM ) Steven J. Hughes, PhD, LP, ABPdN Director of Education and Research The TOVA Company 2008 The TOVA Company Purpose:

More information

The DSM-5 Draft: Critique and Recommendations

The DSM-5 Draft: Critique and Recommendations The DSM-5 Draft: Critique and Recommendations Psychological Injury and Law ISSN 1938-971X Volume 3 Number 4 Psychol. Inj. and Law (2010) 3:320-322 DOI 10.1007/s12207-010-9091- y 1 23 Your article is protected

More information

Accessibility and Disability Service. A Guide to Services for Students with

Accessibility and Disability Service. A Guide to Services for Students with Accessibility and Disability Service 4281 Chapel Lane ~ 0106 Shoemaker 301.314.7682 Fax: 301.405.0813 adsfrontdesk@umd.edu www.counseling.umd.edu/ads A Guide to Services for Students with Attention-Deficit

More information

Use a diagnostic neuropsychology HOW TO DO IT PRACTICAL NEUROLOGY

Use a diagnostic neuropsychology HOW TO DO IT PRACTICAL NEUROLOGY 170 PRACTICAL NEUROLOGY HOW TO DO IT Pract Neurol: first published as 10.1046/j.1474-7766.2003.08148.x on 1 June 2003. Downloaded from http://pn.bmj.com/ Use a diagnostic neuropsychology on 16 October

More information

A Multi-Method Assessment Approach to the Detection of Malingered Pain: Association with the MMPI-2 Restructured Form

A Multi-Method Assessment Approach to the Detection of Malingered Pain: Association with the MMPI-2 Restructured Form Eastern Kentucky University Encompass Online Theses and Dissertations Student Scholarship 2011 A Multi-Method Assessment Approach to the Detection of Malingered Pain: Association with the MMPI-2 Restructured

More information

Neuropsychological Assessment: Applications to the Work Setting

Neuropsychological Assessment: Applications to the Work Setting : Applications to the Work Setting! Arlin Pachet, Ph.D, R.Psych., ABPP Board Certified Clinical Neuropsychologist! September 30, 2014 1 Presentation Objectives! (NPA) overview What is an NPA? What is a

More information

DOES IMPAIRED EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING DIFFERENTIALLY IMPACT VERBAL MEMORY MEASURES IN OLDER ADULTS WITH SUSPECTED DEMENTIA?

DOES IMPAIRED EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING DIFFERENTIALLY IMPACT VERBAL MEMORY MEASURES IN OLDER ADULTS WITH SUSPECTED DEMENTIA? The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20: 230 242, 2006 Copyright # Taylor and Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 1385-4046 print=1744-4144 online DOI: 10.1080/13854040590947461 DOES IMPAIRED EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING DIFFERENTIALLY

More information

RATING MENTAL WHOLE PERSON IMPAIRMENT UNDER THE NEW SABS: New Methods, New Challenges. CSME/CAPDA Conference, April 1, 2017

RATING MENTAL WHOLE PERSON IMPAIRMENT UNDER THE NEW SABS: New Methods, New Challenges. CSME/CAPDA Conference, April 1, 2017 RATING MENTAL WHOLE PERSON IMPAIRMENT UNDER THE NEW SABS: New Methods, New Challenges CSME/CAPDA Conference, April 1, 2017 William H. Gnam, PhD, MD, FRCPC (william.gnam@gmail.com) Consultant Psychiatrist

More information

MEDICAL POLICY No R4 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL TESTING

MEDICAL POLICY No R4 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL TESTING NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL TESTING Effective Date: October 1, 2015 Review Dates: 7/07, 6/08, 6/09, 8/09, 8/10, 8/11, 8/12, 8/13, 8/14, 8/15, 8/16, 8/17 Date Of Origin: July 2007 Status: Current

More information

Rating Mental Impairment with AMA Guides 6 th edition:

Rating Mental Impairment with AMA Guides 6 th edition: Rating Mental Impairment with AMA Guides 6 th edition: Practical Considerations and Strategies CSME/CAPDA C-CAT Course, March 24, 2018 William H. Gnam, PhD, MD, FRCPC (william.gnam@gmail.com) Consultant

More information

Comparison of Performance of the Test of Memory Malingering and Word Memory Test in a Criminal Forensic Sample

Comparison of Performance of the Test of Memory Malingering and Word Memory Test in a Criminal Forensic Sample Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology Advance Access published May 5, 2015 Comparison of Performance of the Test of Memory Malingering and Word Memory Test in a Criminal Forensic Sample Rachel L. Fazio

More information

Optimizing Concussion Recovery: The Role of Education and Expectancy Effects

Optimizing Concussion Recovery: The Role of Education and Expectancy Effects Rehabilitation Institute of Michigan Optimizing Concussion Recovery: The Role of Education and Expectancy Effects Robin Hanks, Ph.D., ABPP Chief of Rehabilitation Psychology and Neuropsychology Professor

More information

Utility of a clinically derived abbreviated form of the WAIS-III

Utility of a clinically derived abbreviated form of the WAIS-III Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 917 927 Utility of a clinically derived abbreviated form of the WAIS-III Joy H. Wymer, Katrina Rayls 1, Mark T. Wagner 2 Psychological Assessment Center,

More information

Empire BlueCross BlueShield Professional Commercial Reimbursement Policy

Empire BlueCross BlueShield Professional Commercial Reimbursement Policy Subject: Documentation Guidelines for Central Nervous System Assessments and Tests NY Policy: 0046 Effective: 12/01/2014 11/30/2015 Coverage is subject to the terms, conditions, and limitations of an individual

More information

Hydro One Networks Inc. 483 Bay Street, Toronto, Ontario M5G 2P5. July 7, Attention: Consultation Secretariat

Hydro One Networks Inc. 483 Bay Street, Toronto, Ontario M5G 2P5. July 7, Attention: Consultation Secretariat Hydro One Networks Inc. 483 Bay Street, Toronto, Ontario M5G 2P5 July 7, 2017 Attention: Consultation Secretariat Workplace Safety & Insurance Board Consultation Secretariat 200 Front Street West, 17 th

More information