Head injury and the ability to feign neuropsychological deficits

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Head injury and the ability to feign neuropsychological deficits"

Transcription

1 Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) Head injury and the ability to feign neuropsychological deficits Chad D. Vickery, David T.R. Berry, Chantel S. Dearth, Victoria L. Vagnini, Raymond E. Baser, Dona E. Cragar, Stephen A. Orey Abstract Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, 115 Kastle Hall, Lexington, KY , USA Accepted 25 July 2002 This study investigated the possibility that head-injured patients, by virtue of their exposure to medical and legal evaluations, are better able to feign deficits than controls. Both internal and external validity issues were addressed in a malingering simulation using 46 moderately to severely head injured and 46 matched control subjects who were administered a battery of neuropsychological and motivational tests under standard or malingering instructions. Results showed no significant interaction between malingering instructions and head injury status on commonly used motivational tests or neuropsychological tests, nor were the head injured malingerers better able to avoid detection using established cutting scores on motivational tests. These results suggest that head injured individuals are no more able to feign neuropsychological deficits successfully than non-head injured individuals National Academy of Neuropsychology. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Head-injured patients; Neuropsychological deficits; Malingering 1. Introduction Since the 1980s, clinical and research interest in detecting feigned neuropsychological deficits has intensified (Larrabee, 2000; Reynolds, 1998; Sweet, 1999). One important factor here has been the demonstration that relatively unsophisticated individuals may successfully This paper is based on a dissertation submitted by Chad Vickery in partial fulfillment of the requirements for a doctorate in clinical psychology. Corresponding author. Tel.: ; fax: address: dtrb@uky.edu (D.T.R. Berry) /$ see front matter 2002 National Academy of Neuropsychology. PII: S (02)

2 38 C.D. Vickery et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) portray deficits on neuropsychological testing (Faust, Hart, & Guilmette, 1988). A second major factor has been the increasing acceptance of neuropsychological results in legal and administrative settings determining substantial benefits (Taylor, 1999). Patient honesty may not be taken for granted in compensation-seeking circumstances (Berry, Baer, Rinaldo, & Wetter, 2002). As neuropsychologists have developed and validated objective tests for the detection of malingered cognitive deficits in response to concern about feigning of deficits, increasing attention has been devoted to the quality of the underlying research in the area. Rogers (1997) systematically reviews methodological issues in research on the detection of feigned psychological deficits and identifies a number of important concerns involving issues of internal and external validity. Internal validity, or confidence that the experimental manipulation is the sole explanation for changes in the dependent variable, is usually higher in simulation (analog) designs, which typically involve instructing normal individuals to feign deficits with results compared to those from genuine patients. In contrast, external validity, or generalizability of findings, tends to be higher in known-groups designs which involve independently identified groups of honest and feigning patients from clinically relevant settings. Rogers (1997) recommends converging support from both types of designs before accepting a malingering detection procedure as valid. An alternative strategy involves minimizing the weaknesses of each type of design. For example, Rogers suggests a four group design crossing the factors of patient status (patient or normal) and instruction set (feigning or honest). Rogers proposes other refinements such as post-test checks on compliance of participants with their instructions, use of specific scenarios for believable feigning that participants may easily identify with, provision of coaching information likely to be obtained by real-world feigners, and offering incentives for successful feigning to make the experimental situation more comparable to real-world circumstances. The present investigation attempted to fulfill Rogers (1997) suggestions for increasing experimental rigor in an analog study of the detection of malingered neuropsychological deficits. Head-injured (HI) patients and matched controls were instructed to feign or answer honestly while completing a battery including malingering and standard neuropsychological tests. Perhaps the most clinically relevant feature of the study was a test of the possibility that HI individuals, by virtue of their direct experience with head injury and related medical and legal procedures, might be better at feigning deficits than normals (Haines & Norris, 1995; Hayes, Hilsabeck, & Gouvier, 1999; Nies & Sweet, 1994). 2. Method 2.1. Participants Prospective HI participants were recruited by fliers in waiting rooms of appropriate clinical specialists, announcements at head injury support group meetings, and advertisements in newspapers. HI participants were required to have experienced a head injury that resulted in loss of consciousness for 1 h, undergone evaluation in a regional medical center, be willing to sign releases for medical records, have no current involvement in any form of compensation-seeking,

3 C.D. Vickery et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) and have no significant substance abuse history. Most of the initial 49 HI subjects had been injured in moving vehicle accidents with the remainder experiencing falls (18%) or assaults (11%). All HI participants denied current compensation-seeking, although 60% indicated that they had done so in the past. The 53 initial community volunteers (CVs) were recruited through advertisements and fliers in business and entertainment establishments. CV participants were matched to HI groups on demographic characteristics and required to have no history of significant neurological, psychiatric, or substance abuse or history of head injury with positive loss of consciousness Materials All participants completed an IRB approved informed consent agreement, demographic & medical questionnaires, a battery including both motivational and standard neuropsychological tests and a post-testing debriefing form requesting ratings using a 10-point scale (from low (0) to high (10)) on understanding, memory, compliance, and effort to follow instructions. HI participants also received the Mini Mental Status Examination (MMSE: Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975), to evaluate capacity to provide informed consent, and completed a release form providing permission to obtain medical records regarding their head injury. A Coaching Sheet was prepared for malingering participants. This material included commonly experienced head-injury symptoms as described on websites of eight state or national brain injury foundations and eight attorneys who provided such information. Symptoms appearing in at least half of each web site type were included. Additionally, tips for avoiding detection as a malingerer from previous studies (Frederick & Foster, 1991; Inman et al., 1998; Martin, Bolter, Todd, Gouvier, & Nicholls, 1993; Rose, Hall, & Szaldi-Petree, 1998) were included. Because of ethical concerns regarding publishing information that might compromise the validity of psychological tests (Ben-Porath, 1994; Berry, Lamb, Wetter, Baer, & Widiger, 1994), this information is available on request from the corresponding author. Standard neuropsychological tests administered to all participants included the Controlled Oral Word Association Test, Digit Span and Digit Symbol subtests of the WAIS-III, the Finger Oscillation Test, Word List Immediate and Delayed subtests from the WMS-III, the Grooved Pegboard Test, the New Adult Reading Test Revised, and the Stroop Color-Word Reading Test. Extensive information on these well-established neuropsychological tests is available in Lezak (1995) and Wechsler (1997). Raw scores from Wechsler series tests were transformed to scaled scores, whereas remaining tests were transformed to T-scores using the Heaton, Grant, and Matthews (1991) norms. Malingering tests included the Digit Memory Test (DMT; Hiscock & Hiscock, 1989) modified to Guilmette, Hart, Giuliano, and Leininger s 36-item short form (1994), Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM; Tombaugh, 1997), and Letter Memory Test (LMT; Inman et al., 1998). The DMT is a forced-choice digit recognition test with increasing delays that has been extensively validated and found to have a large effect size (Cohen s d) in a recent meta-analysis of malingering procedures (Vickery, Berry, Inman, Harris, & Orey, 2001). Internal consistency reliability for the DMT has been adequate at.87 (Inman et al., 1998). A cutting score of <90% correct has been validated to identify inadequate motivation on the DMT (Vickery et al., 2001).

4 40 C.D. Vickery et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) The TOMM (Tombaugh, 1997) involves forced-choice recognition of simple line drawings in immediate and delayed trials and is insensitive to depression (Rees, Tombaugh, & Boulay, 2001). A cutting score of <90% correct on the second of two learning trials or on a delayed recognition trial has been demonstrated to have high sensitivity and specificity by Rees, Tombaugh, Gansler, and Moczynski (1998). The LMT (Inman et al., 1998) is a forced-choice test of motivation that uses consonant letters as stimuli in 45 trials crossing the factors of number of stimuli to be remembered (3, 4, or 5) and number of foils (1, 2, or 3) presented with the target stimulus. Inman et al. reported large effect sizes for the LMT when comparing HI controls to analog feigners (2.0) or to forensic patients independently identified as probable feigners (3.5). Internal consistency reliability was approximately.94. The LMT was insensitive to depression, and using a cutting score of <93% correct, Inman et al. found sensitivities ranging from 83 to 95% and a specificity rate of 100%. Orey, Cragar, and Berry (2000) and Inman and Berry (2002) found comparable specificities but lower sensitivities Procedure HI and CV participants who met screening requirements were evaluated by two research assistants. RA1 explained the study, obtained consent, supervised completion of the history forms, and administered the NART-R under standard instructions. For HI participants, RA1 also obtained the medical release and administered the MMSE under standard instructions. Using a criterion of <24 on the MMSE, no participants were excluded for potential inability to provide informed consent. All participants were randomly assigned by RA1 to control or malingering instructions, creating four groups: HI Controls (HIC), HI Malingerers (HIM), CV Controls (CVC), and CV Malingerers (CVM). HIC and CVC participants were instructed by RA1 to perform to the best of their ability on subsequent tests, but not to reveal their instructions to RA2 who would test them. They were also told that they would receive $75.00 at the end of the study. HIM and CVM participants were told by RA1 that they would receive a minimum of $75.00 for participation and a bonus of $20.00 if they successfully fulfilled instructions without being detected. They then read a detailed scenario describing an evaluation to determine readiness to return to work following a head injury. They were asked to imagine that they were not able to return to work and were to complete the tests to document disability. They were cautioned to avoid obvious feigning and detection. They were then given the Coaching Sheet and time to consider how they would approach the testing. When they indicated their readiness to begin, RA1 checked for any questions, cautioned them not to reveal their instructions to RA2 and reminded them of the potential bonus for successful feigning. RA2, blind to the instruction set, administered the tests in a counterbalanced order, following standard procedures with brief rest breaks as needed. After completion of testing, RA1 replaced RA2 and requested completion of post-test debriefing forms. HIC and CVC participants were then paid the honorarium of $75.00 and allowed to leave. HIM and CVM participants were asked to complete the post-test forms while their tests were scored. RA1 left the room during this time and returned to tell each malingering participant that he or she had won the bonus and provided the $95.00 honorarium.

5 C.D. Vickery et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) Obtained data were screened to increase internal validity. Two HI participants were dropped for inadequate injury severity indicators and one was dropped for a history of severe psychopathology, leaving 23 in both the HIM and HIC groups. Five CV participants were eliminated for a self-reported history of head injury with positive loss of consciousness. Only a single participant provided an outlying low rating on any of the feedback scales (<6) and this CV subject was dropped, leaving 23 in both the CVM and CVC groups. 3. Results Medical records for HI participants indicated that these patients had generally experienced moderate to severe head injuries. Mean admitting GCS rating was 8.6 (S.D. = 4.1), 85% had positive neuroimaging results (CT or MRI), and 34% had neurosurgical intervention. HI participants reported that they were a mean of 61.7 months post-injury (S.D. = 40.5), had a mean duration of loss of consciousness of 13.8 days (S.D. = 20.1), and a mean duration of post-traumatic amnesia of 41.3 days (S.D. = 47.9). Comparison of the HIM and HIC groups on these parameters using t-tests or chi-square procedures indicated no statistically significant differences (all Ps >.05). Table 1 presents demographic and other relevant variables for the experimental groups. Application of 2 2 ANOVA or log-linear chi-square procedures revealed no significant interactions of the instructional set and head injury factors on any variable or any significant main effects for instruction set; thus, only univariate main effects for the HI factor are presented in Table 1. Evaluation of the main effects indicated that the groups were comparable on all variables except NART-R estimated Full Scale IQ, where a main effect for head injury status reflected significantly lower IQ estimates for HI participants (F(1, 88) = 8.86, P =.004) and history of psychiatric treatment (χ 2 (1,N = 92) = 26.36, P<.001) where a similar effect indicated that HI participants were more likely to report psychiatric treatment. Although not shown in the table, no significant differences were found on current or premorbid occupational status using the WAIS-R occupational categories (Wechsler, 1981). Results from standard neuropsychological tests appear in Table 2 and were analyzed using Pillai s Trace, which is robust to violations of variance assumptions provided there are an equal number of subjects in each cell (Bray & Maxwell, 1985). A 2 2 MANOVA revealed significant main effects for instruction set (Pillai s Trace =.379, approximate F(9, 72) = 3.99, P<.001) and head injury (Pillai s Trace =.346, approximate F(8, 72) = 3.46, P =.001) but no significant interaction (Pillai s Trace =.150, approximate F(9, 72) = 1.16, P =.334). In light of the multiple statistical tests, evaluation of univariate main effects used a conservative P<.005 criterion for statistical significance. Univariate ANOVAs indicated reliably lower scores (P <.005) for malingering groups on seven of nine variables with a median d of 1.1. Reliably lower scores were also found for HI groups on five of nine variables, with a median effect size of.89 (Cohen s d; Cohen, 1977). Thus, on standard neuropsychological tests, effect sizes of head injury and malingering instructions were large and comparable. The non-significant interaction term suggests no differential ability of HI participants to feign successfully and this conclusion was supported by the low d scores for the interaction terms for the ANOVAs (median d =.11).

6 42 C.D. Vickery et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) Table 1 Characteristics of four experimental groups Variable HIM HIC CVM CVC P (HI) Age M S.D Education M S.D NART-R M S.D No. of drinks M S.D Male (%) White (%) Right (%) Psych (%) <.001 Note. P = probability for ANOVA term; HIM = Head-Injured Malingerers; HIC = Head-Injured Controls; CVM = Community Volunteer Malingerers; CVC = Community Volunteer Controls; HI = Head Injury Status main factor. Age and education given in years. NART-R = WAIS-R estimated Full Scale IQ; No. of drinks = reported number of alcoholic drinks per week; Male = percentage of group of male gender; White = percentage of group of Caucasian ethnic background; Right = percentage of group reporting right hand dominance; Psych = percentage of group reporting history of psychiatric treatment. =significant at the.05 level. Table 3 presents results from the motivational tests. A 2 2 MANOVA indicated a significant main effect for instruction set (Pillai s Trace =.466, approximate F(4, 81) = 17.64, P<.001). However, no significant effect of head injury status (Pillai s Trace =.054, approximate F(4, 81) = 1.15, P =.34) or interaction effect was observed (Pillai s Trace =.054, approximate F(4, 81) = 1.15, P =.34). In light of the non-significant interaction, as well as low ds for the interaction terms (median d =.11) only the main effect of instructions was explored using univariate ANOVAs and presented in Table 2 with a P<.005 threshold for significance (main effects for head injury status are included for comparison). Significantly lower scores were noted for malingerers on every motivational test. The d scores indicate minimal effect sizes for head injury status, but very large effects for instruction set. This indicates that the motivational tests were quite sensitive to instruction set, but insensitive to the presence of head injury. The lack of a significant interaction effect suggests no differential ability of HI participants to avoid detection on motivational tests. Classification parameters for each test using the recommended cutting score are presented in Table 4. Chi-square tests failed to reveal significant differences between HI and CV groups on any classification parameter, providing no evidence for differential ability to feign successfully by head injury status.

7 C.D. Vickery et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) Table 2 Standard neuropsychological test results from four experimental groups IS HIS Variable HIM HIC CVM CVC P d P d Digit span M S.D Word lists (imm recall) M S.D Word lists (del recall) M S.D Word lists (recog) M S.D Digit symbol M S.D Finger tapping (dom) M S.D Grooved pegboard (dom) M S.D COWA M S.D Stroop PR M S.D Note. P = probability for main effects of IS and HIS factors; IS HIS not shown; d = Cohen s d-value effect size; Digit span through Digit symbol given as scaled scores. Finger tapping through COWA given as T scores. To evaluate further the question of differential feigning ability by head injury status, a malingering success variable was created. A successful malingerer was defined as one who obtained one or more impaired scores ( 5th percentile) on standard neuropsychological tests and passed (above the cutting score) all motivational tests. All other combinations were deemed failed malingerers. Using these definitions, one (4.3%) individual in the HIM group was a successful malingerer as were 3 (13%) individuals in the CVM group. These proportions were not significantly different (χ 2 (1,N = 92) = 1.09, P =.30), failing to suggest any superiority in malingering for HI participants.

8 44 C.D. Vickery et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) Table 3 Motivational test results from four experimental groups Variable HIM HIC CVM CVC P d P d LMT M S.D DMT M S.D TOMM (Trial 2) M S.D TOMM (retention) M S.D Note. P = probability for ANOVA term; HIM = Head-Injured Malingerers; HIC = Head-Injured Controls; CVM = Community Volunteer Malingerers; CVC = Community Volunteer Controls; P = P value; IS = Instructional Set main factor; HIS = Head Injury Status main factor; IS HIS not shown; d = Cohen s d-value effect size; LMT = Letter Memory Test; DMT = Digit Memory Test; TOMM = Test of Memory Malingering; All values given as percent correct. Table 4 Classification rates for motivational tests by procedure and head injury status Participants Test HI CV HI + CV P LMT Sens Spec HR DMT Sens Spec HR TOMM a Sens Spec HR Note. P = probability for χ 2 test comparing results from HI and CV participants; LMT = Letter Memory Test; DMT = Digit Memory Test; TOMM = Test of Memory Malingering; Sens. = sensitivity, percentage of HIM or CVM groups correctly classified at cutting score; Spec. = specificity, percentage of HIC or CVC groups correctly classified at cutting score; HR = overall hit rate, percentage of all groups correctly classified. a Failure on the TOMM is determined by <90% correct performance on either Trial 2 of the learning trials or on the delayed retention trial, as per normative guidelines; therefore, only one value is reported for the TOMM on this table. IS HIS

9 C.D. Vickery et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) Table 5 Classification rate data requiring failure of increasing numbers of motivational tests for identifying malingering BR = 15% BR = 40% BR = 50% No. of tests failed Sens. Spec. HR PPP NPP PPP NPP PPP NPP Note. BR = base rate, percentage of individuals in sample who were malingering; No. of tests failed = number of motivational tests in which individual s performance falls below cutting score (see text); Sens. = sensitivity, percentage of malingering groups (HIM + CVM) falling below cutting score; Spec. = specificity, percentage of honest groups (HIC + CVC) falling above cutting score; HR = hit rate, percentage of both groups correctly classified by cutting score; PPP = positive predictive power, percentage of those with positive test sign who were malingering; NPP = negative predictive power, percentage of those with negative test sign who were not malingering. In clinical practice, it is common for multiple motivational tests to be administered, although empirical evaluation of decision strategies in this situation is rare (Inman & Berry, 2002; Iverson & Franzen, 1996). Table 5 explores the effect of increasingly stringent criteria for identifying motivational impairment using DMT, TOMM, and LMT results from the combined HI and CV groups. Considering only basic classification parameters, the highest hit rate is achieved using a criterion of failing one or more motivational tests. However, the usual tradeoff between sensitivity and specificity is seen with increasingly stringent criteria. Table 5 also provides clinically relevant data on positive predictive power (PPP) and negative predictive power (NPP) at three base rates of malingering. PPP is the probability that an individual with a positive test sign in fact has the condition in question. NPP is the probability that an individual with a negative test sign does not have the condition in question (Gouvier, Hayes, & Smiroldo, 1998). Inspection of PPP and NPP data at various base rates indicates that in low base rate environments (15%), NPP is usually high whereas PPP is lower. Thus, in low base rate environments, a more conservative threshold for predicting malingering may be appropriate, such as requiring that three or more malingering tests be failed (PPP = 100%; NPP = 89.4%). In contrast, in settings where the base rate of malingering is high (50%), using conservative criteria to predict malingering, such as requiring that three or more motivational tests be failed, results in high PPP (100%) but unacceptably low NPP (59.4%). More liberal criteria for predicting malingering may be appropriate in high base rate settings. These issues must be carefully weighed before adopting any decision strategy. 4. Discussion This study compared neuropsychological and motivational test results from HI and CV participants responding honestly or malingering deficits. Steps taken to increase internal validity including matching groups on demographic variables, providing detailed specific scenarios, allowing malingering participants time to formulate a feigning strategy, and screening participants for compliance with instructions. Measures to increase external validity included

10 46 C.D. Vickery et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) blinding test administrators to participant instructions, administering multiple motivational tests in the context of a neuropsychological battery, providing coaching information from easily available sources, warnings to feign believably, offering financial incentives for successful malingering, and using clinically relevant samples. Results from both neuropsychological and motivational tests showed no interaction of instruction set and head injury status. Similarly, there were no significant differences between HI and CV groups on a malingering success variable. Overall, HI and CV participants were quite comparable as they followed their instructional sets with no evidence for malingering superiority in those with the experience of a significant head injury. These results are consistent with other reports that have failed to find a significant effect of head injury on ability to malinger successfully (Hayes, Martin, & Gouvier, 1995; Inman & Berry, 2002; Ju & Varney, 2000; Rees et al., 1998). Based on the present study and other literature, whatever factors may contribute to ability to feign successfully, simply experiencing a genuine head injury does not appear to be one of them. However, while the present methodology provided general strategies to facilitate malingering, presentation of very specific information on motivational tests by interested individuals would likely compromise their effectiveness (Youngjohn, 1995). Both head injury and malingering instructions resulted in depressed scores on standard neuropsychological tests. The effect sizes for these two variables were large and quite comparable, emphasizing the importance of ruling out malingering as an explanation for impaired scores on testing (Larrabee, 2000). Results from motivational tests showed a main effect for instruction set but not for head injury status and no interaction. These results suggest that the motivational tests studied here, as intended by their developers, are overwhelmingly more sensitive to feigning than to presence of a head injury. Classification rates for each motivational test indicated moderately high to perfect specificity at recommended cutting scores, low to moderately high sensitivity, and moderate to moderately high overall hit rates. Overall, results from the classification data were supportive of all of the motivational tests studied here. However, in clinical practice, it is common to administer multiple motivational tests. Consistent with the earlier findings of Iverson and Franzen (1996) and Inman & Berry (2002), the highest hit rates for test combinations in the present study were obtained using a criterion of one or more positive findings on these tests to identify malingering. However, in low base rate environments, it would be necessary to use a conservative criterion to achieve acceptable PPP. Results also emphasized that when base rates for malingering are high, NPP will be unacceptably low if the criterion for predicting malingering is too stringent. Of course, these results would change if other motivational tests with different operating characteristics were used. These issues must be carefully considered when interpreting results from motivational tests. Limitations of the present study include the modest number of participants in each group, the significant length of time that had passed between experiencing the injury and current testing in the HI participants, the modest financial incentive offered for successful malingering, use of a short form of the DMT, the arbitrary threshold used to screen for compliance on feedback variables, and the differences on estimated Full Scale IQ and psychiatric history between HI and CV groups. These last results are consistent with previous reports of lower IQs following head injury (Kane, Parsons, & Goldstein, 1985) and the treatment for psychiatric disorders that

11 C.D. Vickery et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) is frequently required following significant head injuries (Lezak, 1995, pp ). Future research in this area might address remaining issues. Like any clinical error, falsely identifying an evaluee as malingering is a potentially devastating mistake. As is true for any diagnostic decision, conclusions regarding malingering should be arrived at only after careful review of all information and ruling out alternative explanations. However, false negative results are not without costs to society and social safety net programs. Thus, the possibility of motivational impairment should be objectively addressed in any neuropsychological evaluation for which potential incentives for poor performance are present. References Ben-Porath, Y. (1994). The ethical dilemma of coached malingering research. Psychological Assessment, 6, Berry, D. T. R., Baer, R. A., Rinaldo, J. C., & Wetter, M. W. (2002). Assessment of malingering. In J. Butcher (Ed.), Clinical personality assessment: Practical approaches (2nd ed., pp ). New York: Oxford Press. Berry, D. T. R., Lamb, D. G., Wetter, M. W., Baer, R. A., & Widiger, T. A. (1994). Ethical considerations in research on coached malingering. Psychological Assessment, 6, Bray, J. H., & Maxwell, S. E. (1985). Multivariate analysis of variance. London: Sage Publications. Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic Press. Faust, D., Hart, K., & Guilmette, T. J. (1988). Pediatric malingering: The capacity of children to fake believable deficits on neuropsychological testing. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state: A practical method of grading cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, Frederick, R. I., & Foster, H. G. (1991). Multiple measures of malingering on a forced-choice test of cognitive ability. Psychological Assessment, 3, Gouvier, W. D., Hayes, J. S., & Smiroldo, B. B. (1998). The significance of base rates, test sensitivity, test specificity, and subjects knowledge of symptoms in assessing TBI sequelae and malingering. In C. R. Reynolds (Ed.), Detection of malingering during head injury litigation (pp ). New York: Plenum Press. Guilmette, T. J., Hart, K. J., Giuliano, A. J., & Leininger, B. E. (1994). Detecting simulated memory impairment: Comparison of the rey 15-item test and the hiscock forced choice procedure. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 8, Haines, M. E., & Norris, M. P. (1995). Detecting the malingering of cognitive deficits: An update. Neuropsychology Review, 5, Hayes, J. S., Hilsabeck, R. C., & Gouvier, W. D. (1999). Malingering traumatic brain injury: Current issues and caveats in assessment and classification. In N. R. Varney & J. R. Roberts (Eds.), The evaluation and treatment of mild traumatic brain injury (pp ). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Hayes, J. S., Martin, R., & Gouvier, W. D. (1995). Influence of prior knowledge and experience on the ability to feign mild head injury symptoms in head injured and non-head injured college students. Applied Neuropsychology, 2, Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., & Matthews, C. G. (1991). Comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead Reitan battery: Demographic corrections, research findings, and clinical applications. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Hiscock, M., & Hiscock, C. K. (1989). Refining the forced-choice method for the detection of malingering. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, Inman, T. H., & Berry, D. T. R. (2002). Cross-validation of indicators of malingering: A comparison of nine neuropsychological tests, four tests of malingering, and behavioral observations. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, 1 23.

12 48 C.D. Vickery et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) Inman, T. H., Vickery, C. D., Berry, D. T. R., Lamb, D. G., Edwards, C. L., & Smith, G. T. (1998). Development and initial validation of a new procedure for evaluating adequacy of effort given during neuropsychological testing: The Letter Memory Test. Psychological Assessment, 10, Iverson, G. L., & Franzen, M. D. (1996). Using multiple objective memory procedures to detect simulated malingering. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18, Ju, D., & Varney, N. R. (2000). Can head injury patients simulate malingering? Applied Neuropsychology, 7, Kane, R. L., Parsons, O. A., & Goldstein, G. (1985). Statistical relationships and discriminative accuracy of the Halstead Reitan, Luria Nebraska and Wechsler IQ scores in the identification of brain damage. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 7, Larrabee, G. J. (2000). Forensic neuropsychological assessment. In R. D. Vanderploeg (Ed.), Clinician s guide to neuropsychological assessment (pp ). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. Martin, R. C., Bolter, J. F., Todd, M. E., Gouvier, W. D., & Niccolls, R. (1993). Effects of sophistication and motivation on the detection of malingered memory performance using a computerized forced-choice task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, Nies, K. J., & Sweet, J. J. (1994). Neuropsychological assessment and malingering: A critical review of past and present strategies. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 9, Orey, S. A., Cragar, D. E., & Berry, D. T. R. (2000). The effects of two motivational manipulations on the neuropsychological performances of mildly head injured college students. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, Rees, L. M., Tombaugh, T. N., & Boulay, L. (2001). Depression and the Test of Memory Malingering. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16, Rees, L. M., Tombaugh, T. N., Gansler, D. A., & Moczynski, N. P. (1998). Five validation experiments of the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM). Psychological Assessment, 10, Reynolds, C. R. (Ed.). (1998). Detection of malingering during head injury litigation. New York: Plenum Press. Rogers, R. (1997). Researching dissimulation. In R. Rogers (Ed.), Clinical assessment of malingering and deception (2nd ed., pp ). New York: Guilford Press. Rose, F. E., Hall, S., & Szaldi-Petree, D. (1998). A comparison of four tests of malingering and the effects of coaching. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13, Sweet, J. J. (Ed.). (1999). Forensic neuropsychology: Fundamentals and practice. Exton, PA: Swets & Zeitlinger. Taylor, J. S. (1999). The legal environment pertaining to clinical neuropsychology. In J. Sweet (Ed.), Forensic neuropsychology: Fundamentals and practice (pp ). Exton, PA: Swets & Zeitlinger. Tombaugh, T. N. (1997). The test of memory malingering (TOMM): Normative data from cognitively intact and cognitively impaired individuals. Psychological Assessment, 9, Vickery, C. D., Berry, D. T. R., Inman, T. H., Harris, M. J., & Orey, S. A. (2001). Detection of inadequate effort on neuropsychological testing: A meta-analytic review of selected procedures. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16, Wechsler, D. (1981). The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Test Revised: Manual. New York: Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. (1997). The WAIS-III/WMS-III: Technical manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Youngjohn, J. R. (1995). Confirmed attorney coaching prior to neuropsychological evaluation assessment. Assessment, 2,

Effects of Coaching on Detecting Feigned Cognitive Impairment with the Category Test

Effects of Coaching on Detecting Feigned Cognitive Impairment with the Category Test Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 15, No. 5, pp. 399 413, 2000 Copyright 2000 National Academy of Neuropsychology Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0887-6177/00 $ see front matter PII S0887-6177(99)00031-1

More information

Performance profiles and cut-off scores on the Memory Assessment Scales

Performance profiles and cut-off scores on the Memory Assessment Scales Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) 489 496 Performance profiles and cut-off scores on the Memory Assessment Scales Sid E. O Bryant a, Kevin Duff b, Jerid Fisher c, Robert J. McCaffrey a,d,

More information

Chapter 23. Detection of Children s Malingering on Raven s Standard Progressive Matrices*

Chapter 23. Detection of Children s Malingering on Raven s Standard Progressive Matrices* Chapter 23 Detection of Children s Malingering on Raven s Standard Progressive Matrices* R. Kim McKinzey, Jörg Prieler, and John Raven** Abstract A formula for detecting faked Raven s Standard Progressive

More information

Effects of severe depression on TOMM performance among disability-seeking outpatients

Effects of severe depression on TOMM performance among disability-seeking outpatients Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 21 (2006) 161 165 Effects of severe depression on TOMM performance among disability-seeking outpatients Y. Tami Yanez, William Fremouw, Jennifer Tennant, Julia Strunk,

More information

DETECTION OF MALINGERED MENTAL RETARDATION

DETECTION OF MALINGERED MENTAL RETARDATION University of Kentucky UKnowledge University of Kentucky Master's Theses Graduate School 2006 DETECTION OF MALINGERED MENTAL RETARDATION Lili Odom Graue University of Kentucky, lili.graue@uky.edu Click

More information

Factors Influencing the Face Validity of Effort Tests: Timing of Warning and Feedback

Factors Influencing the Face Validity of Effort Tests: Timing of Warning and Feedback University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers Graduate School 2 Factors Influencing the Face Validity of Effort Tests: Timing

More information

Comparison of Predicted-difference, Simple-difference, and Premorbid-estimation methodologies for evaluating IQ and memory score discrepancies

Comparison of Predicted-difference, Simple-difference, and Premorbid-estimation methodologies for evaluating IQ and memory score discrepancies Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) 363 374 Comparison of Predicted-difference, Simple-difference, and Premorbid-estimation methodologies for evaluating IQ and memory score discrepancies Reid

More information

THE VALIDITY OF THE LETTER MEMORY TEST AS A MEASURE OF MEMORY MALINGERING: ROBUSTNESS TO COACHING. A dissertation presented to.

THE VALIDITY OF THE LETTER MEMORY TEST AS A MEASURE OF MEMORY MALINGERING: ROBUSTNESS TO COACHING. A dissertation presented to. THE VALIDITY OF THE LETTER MEMORY TEST AS A MEASURE OF MEMORY MALINGERING: ROBUSTNESS TO COACHING A dissertation presented to the faculty of the College of Arts and Sciences of Ohio University In partial

More information

The Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM): normative data from cognitively intact, cognitively impaired, and elderly patients with dementia

The Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM): normative data from cognitively intact, cognitively impaired, and elderly patients with dementia Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) 455 464 The Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM): normative data from cognitively intact, cognitively impaired, and elderly patients with dementia Gordon Teichner,

More information

APPLYING REACTION TIME (RT) AND EVENT-RELATED POTENTIAL (ERPS) MEASURES TO DETECT MALINGERED NEUROCOGNITIVE DEFICIT

APPLYING REACTION TIME (RT) AND EVENT-RELATED POTENTIAL (ERPS) MEASURES TO DETECT MALINGERED NEUROCOGNITIVE DEFICIT University of Kentucky UKnowledge University of Kentucky Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School 2007 APPLYING REACTION TIME (RT) AND EVENT-RELATED POTENTIAL (ERPS) MEASURES TO DETECT MALINGERED NEUROCOGNITIVE

More information

VALIDATION OF THE MILLER FORENSIC ASSESSMENT OF SYMPTOMS TEST (M- FAST) IN A CIVIL FORENSIC POPULATION

VALIDATION OF THE MILLER FORENSIC ASSESSMENT OF SYMPTOMS TEST (M- FAST) IN A CIVIL FORENSIC POPULATION University of Kentucky UKnowledge University of Kentucky Master's Theses Graduate School 2006 VALIDATION OF THE MILLER FORENSIC ASSESSMENT OF SYMPTOMS TEST (M- FAST) IN A CIVIL FORENSIC POPULATION Jessica

More information

THE EFFICIACY OF THE MMPI-2 LEES- HALEY FAKE BAD SCALE (FBS) FOR DIFFERENTIATING NEUROCOGNITIVE AND PSYCHIATRIC FEIGNERS

THE EFFICIACY OF THE MMPI-2 LEES- HALEY FAKE BAD SCALE (FBS) FOR DIFFERENTIATING NEUROCOGNITIVE AND PSYCHIATRIC FEIGNERS University of Kentucky UKnowledge University of Kentucky Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School 2003 THE EFFICIACY OF THE MMPI-2 LEES- HALEY FAKE BAD SCALE (FBS) FOR DIFFERENTIATING NEUROCOGNITIVE AND

More information

The Albany Consistency Index for the Test of Memory Malingering

The Albany Consistency Index for the Test of Memory Malingering Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27 (2012) 1 9 The Albany Consistency Index for the Test of Memory Malingering Jessica H. Gunner 1, *, Andrea S. Miele 1, Julie K. Lynch 2, Robert J. McCaffrey 1,2 1

More information

The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status Effort Scale

The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status Effort Scale Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27 (2012) 190 195 The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status Effort Scale Julia Novitski 1,2, Shelly Steele 2, Stella Karantzoulis 3, Christopher

More information

Information/Orientation Subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised as an Indicator of Suspicion of Insufficient Effort*

Information/Orientation Subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised as an Indicator of Suspicion of Insufficient Effort* The Clinical Neuropsychologist 1385-4046/00/1401-056$15.00 2000, Vol. 14, No. 1, pp. 56-66 Swets & Zeitlinger Information/Orientation Subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised as an Indicator of Suspicion

More information

Base Rates of Impaired Neuropsychological Test Performance Among Healthy Older Adults

Base Rates of Impaired Neuropsychological Test Performance Among Healthy Older Adults Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 13, No. 6, pp. 503 511, 1998 Copyright 1998 National Academy of Neuropsychology Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0887-6177/98 $19.00.00 PII S0887-6177(97)00037-1

More information

Exaggerated MMPI-2 symptom report in personal injury litigants with malingered neurocognitive deficit

Exaggerated MMPI-2 symptom report in personal injury litigants with malingered neurocognitive deficit Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 673 686 Exaggerated MMPI-2 symptom report in personal injury litigants with malingered neurocognitive deficit Glenn J. Larrabee Suite 202, 630 South Orange

More information

Potential for interpretation disparities of Halstead Reitan neuropsychological battery performances in a litigating sample,

Potential for interpretation disparities of Halstead Reitan neuropsychological battery performances in a litigating sample, Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 21 (2006) 809 817 Potential for interpretation disparities of Halstead Reitan neuropsychological battery performances in a litigating sample, Abstract Christine L.

More information

Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples

Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 16 (2001) 183±191 Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples Grant L. Iverson* Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, 2255 Wesbrook

More information

University of Kentucky Master's Theses

University of Kentucky Master's Theses University of Kentucky UKnowledge University of Kentucky Master's Theses Graduate School 2006 THE UTILITY OF THE STRUCTURED INVENTORY OF MALINGERED SYMPTOMATOLOGY AS A SCREEN FOR THE FEIGNING OF NEUROCOGNITIVE

More information

Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised*

Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised* The Clinical Neuropsychologist -//-$., Vol., No., pp. - Swets & Zeitlinger Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised* Rodney D. Vanderploeg, John A. Schinka, Tatyana Jones, Brent J. Small,

More information

WPE. WebPsychEmpiricist

WPE. WebPsychEmpiricist McKinzey, R. K., Podd, M., & Kreibehl, M. A. (6/25/04). Concurrent validity of the TOMM and LNNB. WebPsychEmpiricist. Retrieved (date), from http://wpe.info/papers_table.html WPE WebPsychEmpiricist Concurrent

More information

Discriminant Function Analysis of Malingerers and Neurological Headache Patients Self- Reports of Neuropsychological Symptoms

Discriminant Function Analysis of Malingerers and Neurological Headache Patients Self- Reports of Neuropsychological Symptoms Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 13, No. 6, pp. 561 567, 1998 Copyright 1998 National Academy of Neuropsychology Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0887-6177/98 $19.00.00 PII S0887-6177(97)00072-3

More information

The unexamined lie is a lie worth fibbing Neuropsychological malingering and the Word Memory Test

The unexamined lie is a lie worth fibbing Neuropsychological malingering and the Word Memory Test Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 17 (2002) 709 714 The unexamined lie is a lie worth fibbing Neuropsychological malingering and the Word Memory Test David E. Hartman Private Practice/Chicago Medical

More information

Professional Practice Guidelines for the Use and Interpretation of SVTs: The Time Has Come. Jerry Sweet May 20, 2011 London, England

Professional Practice Guidelines for the Use and Interpretation of SVTs: The Time Has Come. Jerry Sweet May 20, 2011 London, England Professional Practice Guidelines for the Use and Interpretation of SVTs: The Time Has Come Jerry Sweet May 20, 2011 London, England Preview Statements of the obvious Symptom validity testing is a mature

More information

Qualitative scoring of the Rey 15-Item Memory Test in a forensic population

Qualitative scoring of the Rey 15-Item Memory Test in a forensic population Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School 2002 Qualitative scoring of the Rey 15-Item Memory Test in a forensic population James A. Martin Louisiana State

More information

Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan

Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan Grant L. Iverson, Ph.D, Professor Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation Harvard Medical School & Red Sox

More information

25 HIGHLIGHTS ON THE USE OF THE MMPI/MMPI- 2 IN A NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TEST BATTERY 1

25 HIGHLIGHTS ON THE USE OF THE MMPI/MMPI- 2 IN A NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TEST BATTERY 1 25 HIGHLIGHTS ON THE USE OF THE MMPI/MMPI- 2 IN A NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TEST BATTERY 1 DATE 9/10/13 James N. Butcher Professor Emeritus University of Minnesota The MMPI scales have been shown to make a valid

More information

Characterization of the Medical Symptom Validity Test in evaluation of clinically referred memory disorders clinic patients

Characterization of the Medical Symptom Validity Test in evaluation of clinically referred memory disorders clinic patients Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 753 761 Characterization of the Medical Symptom Validity Test in evaluation of clinically referred memory disorders clinic patients Abstract Laura L.S. Howe

More information

Diagnostic accuracy of the Structured Inventory of Malingered Symptomatology (SIMS) in detecting instructed malingering

Diagnostic accuracy of the Structured Inventory of Malingered Symptomatology (SIMS) in detecting instructed malingering Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 145 152 Diagnostic accuracy of the Structured Inventory of Malingered Symptomatology (SIMS) in detecting instructed malingering Harald Merckelbach a,, Glenn

More information

The vulnerability to coaching across measures of malingering

The vulnerability to coaching across measures of malingering Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School 2007 The vulnerability to coaching across measures of malingering Adrianne M. Brennan Louisiana State University

More information

Criterion validity of the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II) after traumatic brain injury

Criterion validity of the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II) after traumatic brain injury Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 143 149 Criterion validity of the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II) after traumatic brain injury Monica L. Jacobs, Jacobus Donders

More information

An empirical analysis of the BASC Frontal Lobe/Executive Control scale with a clinical sample

An empirical analysis of the BASC Frontal Lobe/Executive Control scale with a clinical sample Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 21 (2006) 495 501 Abstract An empirical analysis of the BASC Frontal Lobe/Executive Control scale with a clinical sample Jeremy R. Sullivan a,, Cynthia A. Riccio b

More information

Interpretive Report. Client Information

Interpretive Report. Client Information Interpretive Report Developed by Michelle R. Widows, PhD, Glenn P. Smith, PhD, and PAR Staff Client Information Client name: Sample Client Client ID: SIMS Test date: 08/12/2013 Date of birth: 02/03/1975

More information

Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers

Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers Graduate School 2002 Portland Digit Recognition Test the Test of Memory Malingering and standard neuropsychological

More information

THE WORD READING TEST OF EFFORT IN ADULT LEARNING DISABILITY: A SIMULATION STUDY

THE WORD READING TEST OF EFFORT IN ADULT LEARNING DISABILITY: A SIMULATION STUDY The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20: 315 324, 2006 Copyright # Taylor and Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 1385-4046 print=1744-4144 online DOI: 10.1080/13854040590947434 THE WORD READING TEST OF EFFORT IN ADULT

More information

The Use of Significant Others to Enhance the Detection of Malingerers From Traumatically Brain-Injured Patients

The Use of Significant Others to Enhance the Detection of Malingerers From Traumatically Brain-Injured Patients Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 15, No. 6, pp. 465 477, 2000 Copyright 2000 National Academy of Neuropsychology Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0887-6177/00 $ see front matter PII S0887-6177(00)00058-5

More information

Ecological Validity of the WMS-III Rarely Missed Index in Personal Injury Litigation. Rael T. Lange. Riverview Hospital.

Ecological Validity of the WMS-III Rarely Missed Index in Personal Injury Litigation. Rael T. Lange. Riverview Hospital. This is the authors version of a paper that will be published as: Lange, Rael T. Lange and Sullivan, Karen A. and Anderson, Debbie (2005) Ecological validity of the WMS-III Rarely Missed Index in personal

More information

Memory Retraining with Adult Male Alcoholics

Memory Retraining with Adult Male Alcoholics Archives of C/inica/ Neuropsychology, Vol. 4, pp. 227-232. 1989 0887.6177189 $3.00 +.oo Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. Copyright 0 1989 National Academy of Neuropsychologists Memory Retraining

More information

Your choice of SVTs is fundamental to the Slick et al criteria Paul Green Ph.D. paulgreen@shaw.ca www.wordmemorytest.com Central to the criteria is the presence of cognitive symptom exaggeration or feigning

More information

A Dissertation. Submitted to the Faculty. Xavier University. in Partial Fulfillment of the. Requirements for the Degree of. Doctor of Psychology

A Dissertation. Submitted to the Faculty. Xavier University. in Partial Fulfillment of the. Requirements for the Degree of. Doctor of Psychology A Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of Xavier University in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Psychology by Julianne Gast, M.A. January, 2009 Approved: Christine M.

More information

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER FOCUSED QUESTION For individuals with memory and learning impairments due to traumatic brain injury, does use of the self-generation effect (items self-generated by the subject)

More information

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Performance in Above Average and Superior School Children: Relationship to Intelligence and Age

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Performance in Above Average and Superior School Children: Relationship to Intelligence and Age Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 13, No. 8, pp. 713 720, 1998 Copyright 1998 National Academy of Neuropsychology Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0887-6177/98 $19.00.00 PII S0887-6177(98)00007-9

More information

Using contextual analysis to investigate the nature of spatial memory

Using contextual analysis to investigate the nature of spatial memory Psychon Bull Rev (2014) 21:721 727 DOI 10.3758/s13423-013-0523-z BRIEF REPORT Using contextual analysis to investigate the nature of spatial memory Karen L. Siedlecki & Timothy A. Salthouse Published online:

More information

Using Neuropsychological Experts. Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD

Using Neuropsychological Experts. Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD Using Neuropsychological Experts Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD Prepared for Advocate. Arizona Association for Justice/Arizona Trial Lawyers Association. September, 2011 Neurocognitive Associates 9813 North

More information

Healthy Children Get Low Scores Too: Prevalence of Low Scores on the NEPSY-II in Preschoolers, Children, and Adolescents

Healthy Children Get Low Scores Too: Prevalence of Low Scores on the NEPSY-II in Preschoolers, Children, and Adolescents Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 25 (2010) 182 190 Healthy Children Get Low Scores Too: Prevalence of Low Scores on the NEPSY-II in Preschoolers, Children, and Adolescents Brian L. Brooks 1, *, Elisabeth

More information

Neuropsychological Performance in Cannabis Users and Non-Users Following Motivation Manipulation

Neuropsychological Performance in Cannabis Users and Non-Users Following Motivation Manipulation University at Albany, State University of New York Scholars Archive Psychology Honors College 5-2010 Neuropsychological Performance in Cannabis Users and Non-Users Following Motivation Manipulation Michelle

More information

ADHD Rating Scales Susceptibility to Faking in a College Student Sample

ADHD Rating Scales Susceptibility to Faking in a College Student Sample ADHD Rating Scales Susceptibility to Faking in a College Student Sample April Bryington Fisher Pennsylvania State University Marley W. Watkins Arizona State University Abstract Clinicians must often rely

More information

Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition

Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 917 924 Abstract Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition Alison J. Donnell a, Neil Pliskin a, James Holdnack

More information

Improving Accuracy in the. through the use of Technology

Improving Accuracy in the. through the use of Technology Improving Accuracy in the Assessment of Malingering through the use of Technology Lisa Drago Piechowski, PhD, ABPP American School of Professional Psychology, Washington DC Key Points Assessment of malingering

More information

Comparison of Male and Female Response Behaviour on Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2

Comparison of Male and Female Response Behaviour on Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 Comparison of Male and Female Response Behaviour on Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 Dr. Hayfa Tayseer Elbokai & Dr. Aziz Ahmad Alrhamneh Special Education Dep., Al- Balqa' Applied University,

More information

Personality and Individual Differences

Personality and Individual Differences Personality and Individual Differences 98 (2016) 85 90 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Relations of naturally

More information

Published online: 12 Aug 2014.

Published online: 12 Aug 2014. This article was downloaded by: [E. Arthur Shores] On: 16 August 2014, At: 02:05 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Clinical Utility of Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised and Predicted IQ Discrepancies in Closed Head Injury

Clinical Utility of Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised and Predicted IQ Discrepancies in Closed Head Injury @ Pergamon Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 12, No. 8, pp. 757 762, 1997 Copyright 1997 Nationaf Academy ofneuropsychology Printed inthe USA, All rights reserved 0887-6177/97$17.00+.00 PIIS0887-6177(97)OO049-8

More information

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) 703 708 Test review Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., 2002 1. Test description The Trail Making Test

More information

International Journal of Forensic Psychology Copyright Volume 1, No. 3 SEPTEMBER 2006 pp. 1-21

International Journal of Forensic Psychology Copyright Volume 1, No. 3 SEPTEMBER 2006 pp. 1-21 International Journal of Forensic Psychology Copyright 2006 Volume 1, o. 3 SEPTEMBER 2006 pp. 1-21 The Pervasive Influence of Effort on europsychological Tests Paul Green + eurobehavioural Associates,

More information

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 11 18 The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Abstract Ralph M. Reitan, Deborah Wolfson Reitan Neuropsychology

More information

Demystifying the Neuropsychological Evaluation Report. Clinical Neuropsychologist 17 March 2017 Program Director, Neurobehavioral Program

Demystifying the Neuropsychological Evaluation Report. Clinical Neuropsychologist 17 March 2017 Program Director, Neurobehavioral Program Demystifying the Neuropsychological Evaluation Report Jennifer R. Cromer, PhD BIAC Annual Conference Clinical Neuropsychologist 17 March 2017 Program Director, Neurobehavioral Program 84% of neuropsychologists

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores

Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 20 (2005) 1043 1051 Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores Brian L.

More information

Background 6/24/2014. Validity Testing in Pediatric Populations. Michael Kirkwood, PhD, ABPP/CN. Conflict of Interest Statement

Background 6/24/2014. Validity Testing in Pediatric Populations. Michael Kirkwood, PhD, ABPP/CN. Conflict of Interest Statement Validity Testing in Pediatric Populations Michael Kirkwood, PhD, ABPP/CN Background Board Certified Clinical Neuropsychologist at Children s Hospital Colorado Exclusively pediatric-focused Patient work

More information

NEUROBEHAVIORAL EVALUATION OF HOUSEHOLD EXPOSURE TO DURSBAN 1

NEUROBEHAVIORAL EVALUATION OF HOUSEHOLD EXPOSURE TO DURSBAN 1 RAYMOND SINGER, PH.D. A Professional Association 444444444444444444444444444444444444444444444444444 36 Alondra Road / Santa Fe, New Mexico / 87508 Alternate office: 180 E. 79th Street / Suite 1-C / New

More information

MMPI-2 short form proposal: CAUTION

MMPI-2 short form proposal: CAUTION Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 521 527 Abstract MMPI-2 short form proposal: CAUTION Carlton S. Gass, Camille Gonzalez Neuropsychology Division, Psychology Service (116-B), Veterans Affairs

More information

What s Wrong With My Client: Understanding Psychological Testing in Order to Work Effectively With Your Expert

What s Wrong With My Client: Understanding Psychological Testing in Order to Work Effectively With Your Expert What s Wrong With My Client: Understanding Psychological Testing in Order to Work Effectively With Your Expert Common Standardized, Norm Referenced Psychological Tests: Diagnostic ( Personality ) Tests:

More information

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP)

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) Couillet, J., Soury, S., Lebornec, G., Asloun, S., Joseph, P., Mazaux, J., & Azouvi, P. (2010). Rehabilitation of divided attention after severe traumatic brain injury:

More information

Construct validity of the Continuous Recognition Memory test

Construct validity of the Continuous Recognition Memory test University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory - Faculty and Staff Publications Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory

More information

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER Kesler, S., Hadi Hosseini, S. M., Heckler, C., Janelsins, M., Palesh, O., Mustian, K., & Morrow, G. (2013). Cognitive training for improving executive function in chemotherapy-treated

More information

Malingering Detection among Accommodation- Seeking University Students

Malingering Detection among Accommodation- Seeking University Students Brigham Young University BYU ScholarsArchive All Theses and Dissertations 2010-06-25 Malingering Detection among Accommodation- Seeking University Students Spencer Paul Clayton Brigham Young University

More information

***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully replicate the published version*** Auditory Temporal Processes in the Elderly

***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully replicate the published version*** Auditory Temporal Processes in the Elderly Auditory Temporal Processes 1 Ben-Artzi, E., Babkoff, H., Fostick, L. (2011). Auditory temporal processes in the elderly. Audiology Research, 1, 21-23 ***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully

More information

PRELIMINARY NORMS FOR YEAR OLDS ON THE MEMORY TEST FOR OLDER ADULTS (MTOA:S) ABSTRACT

PRELIMINARY NORMS FOR YEAR OLDS ON THE MEMORY TEST FOR OLDER ADULTS (MTOA:S) ABSTRACT The University of British Columbia PRELIMINARY NORMS FOR 85-99 YEAR OLDS ON THE MEMORY TEST FOR OLDER ADULTS (MTOA:S) Anita M. Hubley University of British Columbia Vancouver, BC, Canada Poster presented

More information

Comparison of Performance of the Test of Memory Malingering and Word Memory Test in a Criminal Forensic Sample

Comparison of Performance of the Test of Memory Malingering and Word Memory Test in a Criminal Forensic Sample Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology Advance Access published May 5, 2015 Comparison of Performance of the Test of Memory Malingering and Word Memory Test in a Criminal Forensic Sample Rachel L. Fazio

More information

Simulated subaverage performance on the Block Span task of the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales- Fifth Edition

Simulated subaverage performance on the Block Span task of the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales- Fifth Edition Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Master's Theses Graduate School 2011 Simulated subaverage performance on the Block Span task of the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales- Fifth Edition

More information

Serial 7s and Alphabet Backwards as Brief Measures of Information Processing Speed

Serial 7s and Alphabet Backwards as Brief Measures of Information Processing Speed Pergamon Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 11, No. 8, pp. 651-659, 1996 Copyright 9 1996 National Academy of Neuropsychology Printed in the USA. All fights reserved 0887-6177/96 $15.00 +.00 PH

More information

SYMPTOM VALIDITY TESTING OF FEIGNED DISSOCIATIVE AMNESIA: A SIMULATION STUDY

SYMPTOM VALIDITY TESTING OF FEIGNED DISSOCIATIVE AMNESIA: A SIMULATION STUDY Psychology, Crime & Law, 2002, Vol. 8, pp. 311 318 SYMPTOM VALIDITY TESTING OF FEIGNED DISSOCIATIVE AMNESIA: A SIMULATION STUDY HARALD MERCKELBACH*, BEATRIJS HAUER and ERIC RASSIN Department of Experimental

More information

International Journal of Forensic Psychology Copyright Volume 1, No. 3 SEPTEMBER 2006 pp

International Journal of Forensic Psychology Copyright Volume 1, No. 3 SEPTEMBER 2006 pp International Journal of Forensic Psychology Copyright 2006 Volume 1, No. 3 SEPTEMBER 2006 pp. 29-37 The Word Memory Test and the One-in-Five-Test in an Analogue Study with Russian Speaking Participants

More information

Malingering detection in a Spanish population with a known-groups design

Malingering detection in a Spanish population with a known-groups design Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 23 (2008) 365 377 Malingering detection in a Spanish population with a known-groups design Raquel Vilar-López a,, Manuel Gómez-Río b, Sandra Santiago-Ramajo a, Antonio

More information

The effect of distraction on the Word Memory Test and Test of Memory Malingering performance in patients with a severe brain injury

The effect of distraction on the Word Memory Test and Test of Memory Malingering performance in patients with a severe brain injury Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society (2008), 14, 1074 1080. Copyright 2008 INS. Published by Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. doi:10.10170s135561770808137x BRIEF COMMUNICATION

More information

An Initial Validation of Virtual Human Administered Neuropsychological Assessments

An Initial Validation of Virtual Human Administered Neuropsychological Assessments Annual Review of Cybertherapy and Telemedicine 2017 123 An Initial Validation of Virtual Human Administered Neuropsychological Assessments Thomas D. PARSONS a,*, Paul SCHERMERHORN b, Timothy MCMAHAN a,

More information

Minimizing Misdiagnosis: Psychometric Criteria for Possible or Probable Memory Impairment

Minimizing Misdiagnosis: Psychometric Criteria for Possible or Probable Memory Impairment Original Research Article DOI: 10.1159/000215390 Accepted: January 30, 2009 Published online: April 28, 2009 Minimizing Misdiagnosis: Psychometric Criteria for Possible or Probable Memory Impairment Brian

More information

Are people with Intellectual disabilities getting more or less intelligent II: US data. Simon Whitaker

Are people with Intellectual disabilities getting more or less intelligent II: US data. Simon Whitaker Are people with Intellectual disabilities getting more or less intelligent II: US data By Simon Whitaker Consultant Clinical Psychologist/Senior Visiting Research Fellow The Learning Disability Research

More information

A Multi-Method Assessment Approach to the Detection of Malingered Pain: Association with the MMPI-2 Restructured Form

A Multi-Method Assessment Approach to the Detection of Malingered Pain: Association with the MMPI-2 Restructured Form Eastern Kentucky University Encompass Online Theses and Dissertations Student Scholarship 2011 A Multi-Method Assessment Approach to the Detection of Malingered Pain: Association with the MMPI-2 Restructured

More information

The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System Tower Test Resilience to Response Bias

The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System Tower Test Resilience to Response Bias Ursidae: The Undergraduate Research Journal at the University of Northern Colorado Volume 1 Number 2 Article 2 January 2012 The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System Tower Test Resilience to Response

More information

The Use of Brief Assessment Batteries in Multiple Sclerosis. History of Cognitive Studies in MS

The Use of Brief Assessment Batteries in Multiple Sclerosis. History of Cognitive Studies in MS This is the html version of the file http://wwwvagov/ms/library/managing/robert_kane_brief_assessment_batteries_in_msppt Google automatically generates html versions of documents as we crawl the web 1

More information

Trends in Symptom Validity, Memory and Psychological Test Performance as. Functions of Time and Malingering Rating. A Thesis. Submitted to the Faculty

Trends in Symptom Validity, Memory and Psychological Test Performance as. Functions of Time and Malingering Rating. A Thesis. Submitted to the Faculty Trends in Symptom Validity, Memory and Psychological Test Performance as Functions of Time and Malingering Rating A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of Drexel University by Maryellen McClain in partial

More information

WAIS-R Subtest Pattern Clusters in Closed-Head-Injured and Healthy Samples*

WAIS-R Subtest Pattern Clusters in Closed-Head-Injured and Healthy Samples* The Clinical Neuropsychologist 1997, Vol. 11, No. 3, pp. 249-257 1385-4046/97/1103-249$12.00 Swets & Zeitlinger WAIS-R Subtest Pattern Clusters in Closed-Head-Injured and Healthy Samples* J.R. Crawford

More information

KEVIN J. BIANCHINI, PH.D., ABPN

KEVIN J. BIANCHINI, PH.D., ABPN KEVIN J. BIANCHINI, PH.D., ABPN Slick et al., 1999 Bianchini et al., 2005 4 4 Criterion A: Evidence of significant external incentive Criterion B: Evidence from physical evaluation 1. Probable effort

More information

DISCRIMINATING BETWEEN ADHD, ADHD WITH A COMORBID PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDER AND MALINGERED ADHD IN A COLLEGE SAMPLE

DISCRIMINATING BETWEEN ADHD, ADHD WITH A COMORBID PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDER AND MALINGERED ADHD IN A COLLEGE SAMPLE University of Kentucky UKnowledge Theses and Dissertations--Psychology Psychology 2013 DISCRIMINATING BETWEEN ADHD, ADHD WITH A COMORBID PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDER AND MALINGERED ADHD IN A COLLEGE SAMPLE Kimberly

More information

Client/Testing Information

Client/Testing Information Revised Comprehensive Norms for an Expanded Halstead-Reitan Battery: Demographically Adjusted Neuropsychological Norms for African American and Caucasian Adults Developed By Robert K. Heaton, PhD, S. Walden

More information

Medical Symptom Validity Test Performance Following Moderate-Severe Traumatic Brain Injury: Expectations Based on Orientation Log Classification

Medical Symptom Validity Test Performance Following Moderate-Severe Traumatic Brain Injury: Expectations Based on Orientation Log Classification Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 32 (2017) 339 348 Medical Symptom Validity Test Performance Following Moderate-Severe Traumatic Brain Injury: Expectations Based on Orientation Log Classification Abstract

More information

E. Miriam Schmitt-Monreal

E. Miriam Schmitt-Monreal RUNNING HEAD: Feigned TBI TOMM, DCT and Coaching 1 Feigning Cognitive Symptoms after TBI: Validation of the TOMM and DCT and the Influence of Coaching E. Miriam Schmitt-Monreal Master Thesis - Master programme

More information

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT S A R A H R A S K I N, P H D, A B P P S A R A H B U L L A R D, P H D, A B P P

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT S A R A H R A S K I N, P H D, A B P P S A R A H B U L L A R D, P H D, A B P P NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT S A R A H R A S K I N, P H D, A B P P S A R A H B U L L A R D, P H D, A B P P NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL EXAMINATION A method of examining the brain; abnormal behavior is linked to

More information

10/5/2015. Advances in Pediatric Neuropsychology Test Interpretation Part I: Importance of Considering Normal Variability. Financial Disclosures

10/5/2015. Advances in Pediatric Neuropsychology Test Interpretation Part I: Importance of Considering Normal Variability. Financial Disclosures Advances in Pediatric Neuropsychology Test Interpretation: Importance of Considering Normal Variability and Performance Variability Brian L. Brooks, PhD Alberta Children s Hospital University of Calgary

More information

Donald A. Davidoff, Ph.D., ABPDC Chief, Neuropsychology Department, McLean Hospital Assistant Professor of Psychology, Harvard Medical School

Donald A. Davidoff, Ph.D., ABPDC Chief, Neuropsychology Department, McLean Hospital Assistant Professor of Psychology, Harvard Medical School Donald A. Davidoff, Ph.D., ABPDC Chief, Neuropsychology Department, McLean Hospital Assistant Professor of Psychology, Harvard Medical School Interests: Adult/Geriatric/Forensic Neuropsychology ddavidoff@mclean.harvard.edu

More information

Commentary on Delis and Wetter, Cogniform disorder and cogniform condition: Proposed diagnoses for excessive cognitive symptoms

Commentary on Delis and Wetter, Cogniform disorder and cogniform condition: Proposed diagnoses for excessive cognitive symptoms Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 683 687 Abstract Commentary Commentary on Delis and Wetter, Cogniform disorder and cogniform condition: Proposed diagnoses for excessive cognitive symptoms

More information

Relationships among postconcussional-type symptoms, depression, and anxiety in neurologically normal young adults and victims of mild brain injury $

Relationships among postconcussional-type symptoms, depression, and anxiety in neurologically normal young adults and victims of mild brain injury $ Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 16 2001) 435±445 Relationships among postconcussional-type symptoms, depression, and anxiety in neurologically normal young adults and victims of mild brain injury

More information

Correlation Between Intelligence Test Scores and Executive Function Measures

Correlation Between Intelligence Test Scores and Executive Function Measures Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 15, No. 1, pp. 31 36, 2000 Copyright 1999 National Academy of Neuropsychology Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0887-6177/00 $ see front matter PII S0887-6177(98)00159-0

More information

CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY PSYC32

CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY PSYC32 University of Toronto at Scarborough Department of Psychology CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY PSYC32 Ψ Course Instructor: Zakzanis Lab Instructor: Konstantine Eliyas Jeffay Course Code: PSYC32H3 Lecture: Tuesdays,

More information

The NART as an index of prior intellectual functioning: a retrospective validity study covering a 66-year interval

The NART as an index of prior intellectual functioning: a retrospective validity study covering a 66-year interval Psychological Medicine, 2001, 31, 451 458. 2001 Cambridge University Press Printed in the United Kingdom The NART as an index of prior intellectual functioning: a retrospective validity study covering

More information

THE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF POST-POLIO FATIGUE. Richard L. Bruno, Thomas Galski, John DeLuca.

THE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF POST-POLIO FATIGUE. Richard L. Bruno, Thomas Galski, John DeLuca. FROM The Post-Polio Institute and The International Centre for Post-Polio Education and Research postpolioinfo@aol.com Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 1993; 74: 1061-1065. THE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY

More information