Prevention of coordinated eye movements and steering impairs driving performance

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Prevention of coordinated eye movements and steering impairs driving performance"

Transcription

1 Exp Brain Res (2005) 163: DOI /s RESEARCH ARTICLE D. E. Marple-Horvat Æ M. Chattington Æ M. Anglesea D. G. Ashford Æ M. Wilson Æ D. Keil Prevention of coordinated eye movements and steering impairs driving performance Received: 1 July 2004 / Accepted: 9 November 2004 / Published online: 20 April 2005 Ó Springer-Verlag 2005 Abstract When approaching a bend in the road, a driver looks across to the inside kerb before turning the steering wheel. Eye movements and steering are tightly linked, with the eyes leading, which means that the oculomotor controller can assist the neural centres controlling steering. This optimum coordination is observed for all drivers; but despite being the preferred solution to the motor-control problem of successfully steering along a winding road, the question remains as to how crucial such coordinated eye and steering movements are for driving performance. Twenty subjects repeatedly drove a simulated stage of the World Rally Championship, aiming to complete the course in the fastest possible time. For the first six repetitions they used the usual coordination of eye movements and steering; for drives 7 12 they were instructed to fixate on a small spot in the centre of the screen (centre gaze). Prevention of coordination in this way impaired their performance (drives 6 and 7 compared), dramatically increasing their time taken to complete the course, equivalent to slipping 19 places down the leader board in the actual rally stage. This indicates that the usual pattern of eye movements correlated with steering is crucial for driving performance. Further experiments are suggested to reveal whether any attentional demand associated with keeping the eyes still contributes to the loss in performance. Keywords Eye movements Æ Steering Æ Driving Æ Performance Æ Impaired D. E. Marple-Horvat (&) Æ M. Chattington M. Anglesea Æ D. G. Ashford Æ M. Wilson Æ D. Keil Institute for Biophysical and Clinical Research into Human Movement (IRM), Manchester Metropolitan University, Hassall Road, Alsager, Cheshire, ST7 2HL, UK d.e.marple-horvat@mmu.ac.uk Tel.: Fax: Introduction Driving is a classic example of visually guided behaviour in which the eyes move in relation to, and just before, another action (see Herman et al. 1981; Hollands and Marple-Horvat 2001; Land and Hayhoe 2001 for other examples). In diverse situations the degree of coordination between eye movements and other actions, and their relative timing, largely determines performance (Miall and Reckess 2002). Individuals who are unable to move their eyes appropriately in relation to their actions, making eye movements of the wrong size or at the wrong time, perform badly (Van Donkelaar and Lee 1994; Crowdy et al. 2000; Marple-Horvat and Crowdy 2004). Persuasively, such individuals improve their performance if they first improve their eye movements (Crowdy et al. 2002). Driving on the open road involves the continuous action of steering, and it is therefore possible to define the relationship between a driver s eye movements and the movement of the steering wheel in terms of the degree of coordination or linkage between the two, and their relative timing. When approaching a bend in the road, a driver looks across to the inside of the curve before turning the steering wheel (Land and Lee 1994). Eye movements and steering are tightly linked, with the eyes leading by half a second or more. Because all drivers do this without specific instruction, it seems reasonable to conclude that this behaviour represents optimum coordination, and the preferred solution by the CNS of the motor-control problem of successfully steering along a winding road. Such tangent point steering has identifiable benefits in terms of acquiring visual information to guide steering (Land and Lee 1994; Land and Horwood 1995; Land 1998). Equally, with eye movements leading (as in so many other situations) it probably reflects a general principle of brain function and motor control. Because the eyes lead, the oculomotor controller can assist the neural centres controlling steering. Such a suggestion fits

2 412 well with current frameworks for understanding coordinated movement (involving different parts of the body) and the role of the cerebellum, where such coordination is probably established (Miall et al. 2000; Miall and Reckess 2002). Despite being the preferred solution to the motor control problem of steering along a winding road, the question remains as to how crucial such coordinated eye and steering movements are for driving performance. Deliberate disturbance of a universally adopted driving strategy in a real vehicle on the road, though informative in this respect, clearly involves risks. We, and others (e.g. Wilkie and Wann 2003), have therefore used a simulated driving paradigm to assess the effect on performance. We asked subjects to repeatedly drive a simulated stage of the World Rally Championship, aiming to complete the course in the fastest possible time. Initially they adopted (without instruction or suggestion) the usual coordination of eye movements and steering. Subsequently, they were instructed to drive whilst looking straight ahead at a spot placed in the centre of the screen. Prevention of any eye movements, and so of coordination, in this way dramatically increased the time taken to complete the course. This indicates that the usual pattern of eye movements correlated with steering is crucial for driving performance; disruption of coordination impairs driving ability. Methods Subjects Twenty subjects took part, 10 males and 10 females (age 20.9±1.7 years, mean±sd). All were qualified drivers with a clean UK driving licence and had no corrected vision. Local ethical committee approval and written informed consent were obtained before testing. Driving task The driving task was taken from the Colin McRae Rally 2 simulation, a driving environment simplified by the absence of other vehicles and pedestrians. Subjects were seated in a driving simulator incorporating a 42 inch (diagonal) plasma screen, force feedback steering wheel, accelerator and brake pedals and a rally car seat. Each subject was asked to repeatedly drive the same set course (France, Stage 1), aiming to complete the course in the fastest possible time. The route involved a large number of turns (47) and each subject drove under two different conditions. Under the first condition (control trials 1 6) they negotiated the course six times successively and were given no advice on their driving; they were allowed to adopt their own driving style and racing strategy. In the second condition (test trials 7 12) their eye movements were restricted. A small (4 4 mm) white spot was placed in the centre of the screen and subjects were instructed to keep their gaze fixed on the spot while driving. Under the test condition, therefore, subjects were denied eye movements and looked straight ahead (centre gaze). They negotiated six successive test drives. Again, no other advice was given regarding driving style or racing strategy. Data acquisition During driving, the subject s eye movements were monitored at 60 Hz using an ASL 5000 pan and tilt eye tracker (Applied Science Laboratories, Bedford, Massachusetts, USA) mounted at dashboard height. Analogue signals representing horizontal and vertical components of any eye movements (calibrated to 1 accuracy using nine equally spaced calibration points covering the full area of the plasma screen) were digitised on-line at 200 Hz using a CED 1401 A/D converter (Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK) for subsequent computer-based analysis using Spike 2 software. The two signals were also fed to the ASL eye tracker control unit which generated on a small monitor visible only to the experimenters crosshairs representing the horizontal and vertical coordinates of gaze superimposed on the subject s view of the road through the windscreen; this scene plus gaze output was recorded on videotape. Care was taken to make sure that none of the monitor screens showing the tracked eye, analogue data, or scene view with crosshairs were visible to the subject. A potentiometer placed inside the steering column to measure steering wheel rotation yielded an analogue signal that was digitised at 200 Hz alongside the eye movement signals for subsequent analysis. Analysis Signals representing the horizontal component of eye movements and steering wheel rotation were analysed within the CED Spike 2 environment. Cross-correlation of the two signals over the time taken to complete a drive (typically 4 5 min) yielded a cross-correlogram, the peak of which identified the correlation coefficient (r) which is a measure of the covariation of the two signals. Calculation of (r 2 100) gave the percentage variance in steering wheel movement attributable to covariation with horizontal eye movement. These statistics were used first to define the usual degree of coordination between eye movements and steering during control trials, and second to ensure that in test trials subjects followed the instruction to keep their gaze fixed straight ahead as best as they were able. True centre gaze maintained accurately without interruption throughout a complete drive, which necessitated many turns of the steering wheel to negotiate the course, would result in a correlation coefficient of zero, i.e. no coordination (or covariation). Because all subjects experienced difficulty

3 413 maintaining centre gaze in test runs, we also computed frequency distributions of horizontal gaze component for each drive, and calculated mean and SD values as an additional measure of how successfully subjects followed the instruction to look straight ahead. The rally car seat and headrest were a snug fit, and subjects (who were observed directly from diagonally behind at all times while driving) made very little in the way of head and torso movements. With the head still, eye movements equate to gaze movements. Together with the controlled lighting within the simulator, this provided ideal conditions for eye tracking, and the pantilt eye tracker coped very well with any small head movements that did occur. On rare occasions when the eye was briefly lost (mainly due to force feedback steering wheel vibration if the car strayed off the road) drop-out occurred in the horizontal eye movement trace. This was removed by manual interpolation before cross correlation. Typically, a 240-s record of a complete drive required interpolation of only a few seconds in total (5.8±3.4 s, mean±sd). The horizontal and vertical eye movement signals provided by the ASL 5000 were therefore very reliable, and the gaze cross-hairs superimposed on the scene of the road ahead very accurate and stable. This was particularly evident at the start of each control drive, when the subject s gaze travelled sequentially down through the traffic lights waiting for the go signal. Because subjects were trying to complete the course as quickly as possible, racing against the clock, completion time was taken as the primary measure of performance. The number of crashes that halted the car was also recorded as a general indicator of performance (catastrophic or complete loss of control) and racing strategy; for further details, see Results, below. Further analysis of variance was conducted using Statistica (Statsoft, USA). A 2 group (male, female) 4 trial (1, 6, 7, 12) mixed design ANOVA with repeated measures on the last factor was performed. Of particular interest was any increase in completion time between trials and any differential effects of gender s (control 1) to s (control 6), a reduction of 14%. Under test conditions the same trend was seen, but there was less improvement overall, 22.1 s (8%). Mean completion time for the last drive under restricted eye movements (test 12) was longer at s than for the last drive under control conditions (control 6), s. Variation in completion times around the mean value for a particular trial was a measure of consistency of performance across all 20 subjects. The largest standard error of the mean was obtained for the first trial in which subjects were forbidden to move their eyes (test 7); the smallest was obtained for the last trial under control conditions (control 6), when subjects exhibited their usual, preferred coordination and had enjoyed the most practice. Figure 1 (bottom) presents male and female data separately. A repeated measures mixed factorial ANO- VA (2 4) analysis of completion times confirmed there was a main effect of group (F (1,18) =26.962, P<0.001) with men having a lower mean finishing time over the four trials analysed (trials 1, 6, 7 and 12). Mean finishing Results Eighteen of the 20 participants (90%) took longer to complete the course when eye movements were first restricted; on average, completion time increased by 30.3 s from s to 280 s, which is a 12% increase (test 7 v control 6; Fig. 1, top). One male subject s time was hardly affected, and one went 10 s faster in the first test drive than the last control. The biggest increase in completion time was 76.9 s (for a female subject). Within the block of trials under a particular condition, practice by repetition improved performance; there was a clear trend for time taken to reduce progressively from the first to the last trial. Under control conditions, mean time taken reduced by 38.2 s from Fig. 1 The effect of eye movements and practice on completion time. Top: mean completion times are shown ±s.e.m. bars for repeated driving of the same course with eye movements permitted (control, drives 1 6) then forbidden (test, drives 7 12). N=20 subjects. Bottom: male and female completion times (N=10 in each case) shown separately

4 414 time was s for men and s for women, a difference of 33.2 s (P<0.05). There was also a main effect of trial (F (3,54) =24.336, P<0.001). Tukey s HSD post-hoc analysis of the trial effect indicated a significant difference (P<0.001) between drive trials 1 and 6, indicating there was a learning effect. A significant difference (P<0.001) was also visible between drives 6 and 7, brought about by the change in the task requirement. There was also a significant difference (P<0.001) between drives 7 and 12 showing that after the drivers were instructed to look straight ahead they did subsequently improve their performance. Finally, there was also a significant interaction effect between trial and group (F (3,53) =2.805, P<0.05). Subsequent Tukey s HSD post-hoc analysis indicated that this interaction effect occurred at trial 7 (P<0.01), where the female drivers were significantly more negatively affected by the change in task requirements. Analysis of subjects eye movements and steering revealed that these were highly coordinated in the first six drives (controls 1 6). This was immediately evident in the live video feed showing the view of the road with gaze crosshairs superimposed. When approaching a bend in the road, all subjects looked across to the inside kerb (or grass verge) before turning the steering wheel, and gaze remained fixed on this tangent point until they were emerging from the bend. Figure 2, top left and right, are typical video still frames showing gaze fixed on the left and right road margins whilst approaching left and right hand bends, respectively. Figure 3 (top) illustrates, under control conditions, eye movements and steering for a 30 s epoch of driving, which included negotiating several bends in the road. The horizontal component of gaze and steering wheel rotation show a high degree of covariation; eye movements and steering are tightly coordinated, with the eyes leading (see, for example, at turning points highlighted by cursors). Vertical eye movements (not shown) were minimal in size and frequency, and were not correlated with steering. By contrast, Fig. 2, bottom left and right, are video frames obtained at the same two bends in the road, for the same driver, but in a test drive when the subject was following instructions and maintaining centre gaze (no eye movements). Gaze is clearly displaced away from the inner road margin. Figure 3 (bottom) confirms that for a 30 s epoch of driving under test conditions, and for a similar stretch of road, this subject kept gaze centred with a high degree of success, and only few and small departures left and right (compare with usual eye movements (top) on the same scale). Figure 4 shows the frequency distributions of horizontal gaze for the same individual under control and test conditions (control 6 and test 7, respectively). Gaze ranged over about ±15 degrees (99% of distribution within this range) around a near-zero mean under control conditions (Fig. 4, left). When instructed to fixate centrally, gaze was within ±2 degrees of centre 96% of the time (Fig. 4, right). Fig. 2 Video frames showing gaze superimposed on the view of the road. Top: gaze crosshairs (in red for visibility) fixed on the left and right (inside) road margins on approach to left and right-hand bends, when driving with eye movements permitted. Bottom: gaze crosshairs fixed on the centre of the scene on approach to the same bends, but with eye movements forbidden (centre gaze)

5 415 Fig. 3 Eye movements and steering during control and test drives. Top: horizontal component of eye movements (above) and steering wheel rotation (below) exhibiting the typical pattern of coordination when eye movements were permitted. Bottom: eye movements (above) and steering (below) in a test drive, when eye movements were forbidden Figure 5 shows that in the absence of any instruction about where to look, for all subjects there was a high degree of correlation between their eye movements and steering (control drives 1 6). With practice, there was a slight increase in correlation to a value of 0.75, identical for male and female drivers. This means that 56% (r 2 100) of the variation in steering wheel position (left right rotations) is attributable to covariation with horizontal (left right) eye movements. Only 44% of the variation in steering wheel position is independent of eye movements. In the absence of any instruction, and with the benefit of practice in the previous five drives, the coordination between eye movements and steering in drive 6 was the subject s preferred and optimum coordination. Eye movements led steering, but there was no relationship between the precise time lead adopted by an individual (defined by the peak of the cross correlogram) and their completion time; subjects who were faster overall did not look to the inside kerb earlier (r 2 =0.050, gradient of regression line not significantly different from zero). In the first test drive (drive 7), subjects obeyed the instruction to fixate gaze on the centre spot on the screen, which dramatically reduced the correlation between any remaining eye movements and steering. The correlation coefficient of 0.34 represents 12% covariation, or 88% independent variation. In subsequent drives, all subjects showed some occasional (or episodic) tendency to revert to their usual pattern of coordination, but correlation remained low, with r circa 0.4, representing 16% covariation. Beyond the manipulation of eye movements, either allowed or not, subjects were free to pursue their own driving strategy. The number of crashes in each drive was noted because visual inspection suggested, first, that the number of crashes in a drive might well affect completion time, and, second, that there might be a different approach to crashing, which represents brief complete loss of control, between genders. Crashes were always a failure to steer around a bend in the road (there were no other cars or obstacles); i.e. drivers went off the road to the right hand side on left hand bends and went off to the left hand side on right hand bends.

6 416 Fig. 4 Frequency distributions of horizontal gaze component. Left: under control conditions, gaze distributed circa 20 left and right of centre. Right: under test conditions, gaze centred with few, small deviations Fig. 5 Correlation between eye movements and steering for drives under control and test conditions. Cross correlation of the horizontal gaze and steering signals across a complete drive yielded a correlation coefficient, r. Values of r were higher in drives 1 6, when eye movements were permitted (no instructions given), and lower in drives 7 12 when eye movements were forbidden (subjects instructed to fix their gaze on the centre of the screen) Under control conditions there was a relationship between number of crashes and completion time; regression analysis yielded r 2 =0.189 (P<0.05), or 19% of the variation in completion time could be explained by the number of crashes (Fig. 6, top). Under test conditions, there was no relationship, r 2 =0.007 (P>0.05). Male and female drivers did not crash with significantly different frequency under control conditions (Fig. 6, bottom). On average (across all six control drives) drivers crashed three times in each drive (males 3.40, females 3.17). Males did not significantly change their frequency of crashing under test conditions, but females did, reducing to twice on average (2.25) per drive. When eye movements were denied, females crashed less frequently than males (unpaired t-test, P<0.05), and less frequently than they had under control conditions (paired t-test, P<0.05). Having identified that drivers took longer to complete the course when constrained to look straight ahead, analysis of their steering was undertaken to identify any differences between steering under normal (drive 6) and constrained (drive 7) conditions which might underlie the drop in performance. A frequency distribution was generated for each driver showing how much of the time in drive 6 the wheel was turned by different amounts. The twenty individual distributions were then averaged to produce a single average distribution which reflects the way drivers steered in their most practised, last attempt under normal conditions, with eye movements (Fig. 7, top). A corresponding plot was generated for their first drive without eye movements (Fig. 7, centre). The difference between the two (test control) shows any difference in steering that accompanied the difference in eye movements (Fig. 7, bottom). The difference distribution shows that when drivers were constrained to look straight ahead they spent more time steering straight ahead or nearly so. The central peak in the distribution is higher under test conditions than control, producing a central peak in the difference distribution. Similarly, the difference plot shows that the steering wheel spent more time turned by small amounts away from centre when the driver looked straight ahead, 7.8% more of the total drive time, about 20 s, within the range 45 degrees to +25 degrees (area under the difference plot within these boundaries). Correspondingly, the wheel was turned further away from centre less of the time, shown by the deficits in the difference plot flanking the central surplus. Statistical comparison of corresponding pairs of bins in the control and test drive

7 417 Fig. 6 Number of crashes, and effect on drive completion times. Top: regression analysis of completion time against number of crashes under control (blue) and test (red) conditions (N=20 subjects). Bottom: number of crashes under control and test conditions shown separately for male and female drivers distributions identified all significantly different bins in the difference plot, and these are colour coded red (paired t-test, P<0.05). Discussion Prevention of eye movements impairs driving performance Since Land and Lee (1994) first demonstrated that eye movements and steering are highly coordinated when driving, the question has remained as to how crucial this preferred coordination is for performance, and how much driving would be impaired if the usual coordination was disrupted. There are obvious safety issues raised by any such intervention while a person is actually driving, but this study provides clear evidence, in a driving simulation, that the effect is dramatic. Subjects racing against the clock in a well-practised simulated stage of the World Rally Championship (France, stage 1) took, on average, 30 s longer to complete the stage, an Fig. 7 Differences in steering when eye movements were or were not made. Frequency distribution shows the percentage of drive time for which the wheel was turned by different amounts away from centre (driving straight ahead). Top: steering when horizontal eye movements coordinated with steering were made. Centre: steering when driver looked straight ahead. Bottom: difference between steering under the two conditions. Bins that are significantly different are in red. Note: there was an offset error of between 5 and 10 in steering wheel calibration so that the peak in the distributions appears in bin 10 to 5 increase of 12%, when eye movements were denied, abolishing the usual pattern of coordination. This impairment of driving performance is dramatic; comparison with actual race times from the stage (in the World Rally Championship of 2003) reveals that such an increase in time taken to complete the course is

8 418 equivalent to slipping 19 places down the leader board in the actual rally stage. This therefore shows that the usual pattern of eye movements correlated with steering is not just a preference, but is crucial for driving performance. There are good reasons why this should be so. Land and co-workers (Land and Lee 1994; Land and Horwood 1995; Land 1998) have provided a framework for understanding why tangent point steering might be the best way for the visuomotor control system to acquire the visual information it requires when driving along a winding open road. But two other possible explanations (i.e. other than the absence of coordinated eye movements) merit discussion at this point. Subjects did report that effort was required to keep gaze fixed on the centre spot. This could represent a motor control effort incurred by the necessity to override the usual oculomotor control signal driving the eyes left and right to tangent points during normal driving; or, it could represent attentional effort in keeping looking straight ahead. Whilst prevention of coordinated eye movements was the direct intervention in test drives, it might have the consequence of placing an additional attentional demand on the driver, which itself might in part explain the impaired driving performance. The presence or absence of eye movements under the two driving conditions is the demonstrable primary difference, but it is important to note in terms of underlying mechanisms that any difference in attentional load as a secondary consequence of what the eyes are doing might contribute to the observed impairment and should be assessed in future experiments by deliberately manipulating attentional load, but without manipulating eye movements. Unfortunately, previous studies which manipulated attention (Lamble et al. 1999; Summala 1999) by requiring subjects to look at and process in-car displays of course also manipulated drivers eye movements which were not measured and are therefore inconclusive. Experiments manipulating attention by using auditory distractants, and in which eye movements are measured, are therefore planned for the future. The task we have used was deliberately stripped of attentional demands (other vehicles, pedestrians, junctions, road signs, lane discipline) other than those related to steering successfully at speed. As a consequence, we do not think that subjects were at or close to their attentional limits which itself makes it unlikely that attention is a main explanation of the impaired performance in the current study when the eyes did not coordinate with steering. Our conclusion remains, therefore, that it is the appropriateness of what the eyes are doing that determines performance (i.e. are they coordinated with steering or not in this case). A second possibility that might explain the drop in performance is that when fixating artificially, gaze is displaced by up to 20 degrees from the point on the road that would normally be fixated. Summala (1999) has shown that an aspect of driving performance deteriorates at eccentricities of 23 to 38 degrees, so displacement of the eye from the tangent point could itself be the reason for the reduced performance. Figure 4 does indeed show that when fixating centrally, gaze could be displaced by almost 20 from the point on the road that would normally be fixated. However, such large departures could only arise very rarely and briefly. In the example shown, departures greater than 15 would only arise for less than 1% of the total drive time (because 99% of the distribution in Fig. 4 (left) lies within ±15 degrees of centre) or about 2.8 s (from Fig. 1, drive 7). We are not convinced that a circumstance arising only for 2.8 s in total can explain an increase in drive time of seconds (Fig. 1). Our experiment is, therefore, very different from Summala s, which involved 23 or 38-degree vertical eccentricities (forced looking downwards) for much of the time to look at an in-car display, so also requiring repeated vergence, accommodation and pupil size changes, and deliberate cognitive loading to respond to a numeric display. Our experiment is in a real sense the opposite of Summala s, because in our study drivers were forced to look straight ahead down the road, whereas in Summala s they were forced to look away. Those studies do not therefore account for the impairment seen in our experiments as arising from gaze displacement rather than lack of coordinated eye movement. We therefore consider that neither of the alternatives presented above can provide the main explanation for the observed impairment, so remain convinced that it is the absence of eye movements coordinated with steering that is the crucial factor. Wilkie and Wann (2003) made a parallel finding when they compared simulated driving with eye movements denied or permitted. Even during short (10-s) drives at constant low to moderate speed (29 km h 1 ), steering performance as measured by deviation of trajectory from the centre of the road was best when subjects were allowed to move their eyes to sample naturally the road ahead, and worst when they were made to look straight ahead (no eye movements). A lack of eye movements implies absence both of the usual outgoing oculomotor control signal (and any efference copy of the same), and of sensory feedback on eye eccentricity from muscle spindles, either of which might provide information useful for the control of steering. Perhaps the most compelling evidence that the oculomotor controller invariably influences the CNS controller for steering comes from the twin observations that in normal circumstances as the eyes look left and right (at the tangent point ahead) steering follows left and right; and when the eyes are restricted to looking straight ahead, steering tends more to straight ahead (Fig. 7), in drives exhibiting loss of performance (completion time). Such a link between good or bad eye movements and good or bad other body movements in relation to visual targets has previously been demonstrated in walking subjects (see below). In a general sense, it seems that where the eyes lead, other movements tend to follow.

9 419 In different driving environments, other competing requirements (than simply steering) might impinge upon eye movements and gaze distribution. Thus, at very high speed (formula 1 racing), head movements are related to steering (Land and Tatler 2001). This might reflect the necessity to keep the resultant force vector, due to gravity acting vertically on the head mass and centrifugal force acting horizontally, aligned through the axis of the neck vertebrae, perhaps to avoid neck muscle fatigue or horizontal shear forces on the brain in the cranium. At very low speed, in a cluttered urban environment, driving and steering become intermittent, with the necessity to sample many other features than the road ahead, such as other vehicles, traffic signals and pedestrians. On a winding open road, however, steering and driving involve a simple, well established, continuous pattern of eye steering coordination. The analysis provided by Land of the observed coordination of eye movements and steering in terms of acquiring visual information most appropriate or useful for steering emphasises sensory or perceptual aspects: what visual information is required. We stress here that there are additional reasons underlying the observed coordination that relate more to the way in which the brain solves problems involving coordinated movements under visual guidance in general. Thus, a strategy in which the eyes move with a time or phase lead over other body movements provides for the oculomotor controller to assist the neural centres controlling, for example, voluntary limb movements (van Donkelaar and Lee 1994; Crowdy et al. 2000; Miall and Reckess 2002). Such an influence is demonstrable in patients who have been able to improve their limb movements by improving their eye movements (Crowdy et al. 2002), is probably established within the cerebellum, which is known to be crucial for motor coordination (Miall et al. 2000), and is evident in tight, relatively invariant, timing relationships (Hollands and Marple-Horvat 2001). Current and future work aims to establish whether disruption of eye movements and steering by pharmacological agents (such as alcohol) or fatigue in a driving simulation and driving on the road leads to impairment of performance (Cooper 2004), and, conversely, whether improving eye movements can improve driving performance. Gender differences in a simulated driving task Male drivers were faster at each stage than their female counterparts. The immediate effect on completion time when eye movements were denied was qualitatively similar; both groups suffered an increase in time taken, performance was impaired. But the effect on females was significantly larger, an increase of 41.7 s (16%) which was more than double the effect on males, who suffered an increase of 18.8 s (7%) on average. However, although females initially suffered a greater loss in performance (in test 7), they subsequently improved by four times more than males. By the last drive (test 12) their times had reduced by 36 s on average; males, by comparison, improved much less, by a modest 8.1 s. These differences seem, at least in part, to arise from a different approach or attitude to the simulated racing task. Males seemed to adopt a more aggressive, attacking style of driving, which they were less prepared to modify or abandon when they found themselves in the unusual and difficult conditions of being forbidden to move their eyes. Thus, under test conditions, males crashed as frequently as under control conditions (and as females under control conditions); but females moderated their driving, reducing the number of crashes by 29% at the expense of a greater slowing overall (greater increase in completion time). These differences in performance and attitude may well reflect a male/female difference in gaming strategy. We think this a more likely explanation than difference in driving experience, because male and female subjects covered a similar narrow age range, and most had held a full driving licence for a similar length of time, 2 3 years. It is by no means clear that the same difference would exist, or to the same extent, in a real driving situation, although a greater willingness to modify driving style and drive more slowly in difficult situations might underlie gender differences in crash statistics and insurance costs that favour female drivers. Another factor might explain the smaller immediate effect on male completion times when eye movements were forbidden. If males were less successful in following the instruction to look straight ahead and not move their eyes, making small eye movements, but in the right direction and appropriately timed to assist steering, more often than females, then this might explain why their performance was less impaired. Comparison of frequency distributions and ranking of SD values of horizontal gaze in the first test drive did reveal that the worst (biggest) three SD values were from males. Apart from this, however, male and female SD values covered an overlapping range; among the best ten values, six were from males. Furthermore, regression analysis of completion time on gaze SD value for male drivers in the first test run revealed that only 12% of the variation in completion time could be explained by variation in gaze SD (completion time reducing with increasing eye movements). We conclude, therefore, that only a small part of the observed gender difference could arise from different male/female success rates in not moving the eyes. Acknowledgements Mark Chattington was supported by an IRM PhD studentship, and Damian Keil by an IRM research fellowship. Steve Gilbey and Darryl Knights provided expert technical assistance, including building the driving simulator and providing data acquisition and analysis programs.

10 420 References Cooper HL (2004) Effect of acute alcohol loading on visuomotor performance and utility as a model for cerebellar dysfunction. MSc thesis, University of Bristol Crowdy KA, Hollands MA, Ferguson IT, Marple-Horvat DE (2000) Evidence for interactive locomotor and oculomotor deficits in cerebellar patients during visually guided stepping. Exp Brain Res 135(4): Crowdy KA, Kaur-Mann D, Cooper HL, Mansfield AG, Offord JL, Marple-Horvat DE (2002) Rehearsal by eye movement improves visuomotor performance in cerebellar patients. Exp Brain Res 146(2): van Donkelaar P, Lee RG (1994) Interactions between the eye and hand motor systems: disruptions due to cerebellar dysfunction. J Neurophysiol 72: Herman R, Herman R, Maulucci R (1981) Visually triggered eyearm movements in man. Exp Brain Res 42(3/4): Hollands MA, Marple-Horvat DE (2001) Co-ordination of eye and leg movements during visually guided stepping. J Motor Behav 33(2): Lamble D, Kauranen T, Laakso M, Summala H. (1999) Cognitive load and detection thresholds in car following situations: safety implications for using mobile (cellular) telephones while driving. Accident Anal Prev 31: Land MF (1998) The visual control of steering. In: Harris LR (ed) Vision and action. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp Land MF, Hayhoe M (2001) In what ways do eye movements contribute to everyday activities? Vision Res 41(25/26): Land MF, Horwood J (1995) Which parts of the road guide steering? Nature 377: Land MF, Lee DN (1994) Where we look when we steer. Nature 369: Land MF, Tatler BW (2001) Steering with the head: the visual strategy of a racing driver. Curr Biol 11: Marple-Horvat DE, Crowdy KA (2004) Direct visualisation of gaze and hypometric saccades in cerebellar patients during visually guided stepping. Gait Posture, in press (e-pub Feb 2004) Miall RC, Imamizu H, Miyauchi S (2000) Activation of the cerebellum in co-ordinated eye and hand tracking movements: an fmri study. Exp Brain Res 135:22 33 Miall RC, Reckess GZ (2002) The cerebellum and the timing of coordinated eye and hand tracking. Brain Cogn 48: Summala H (1999) Forced peripheral vision driving paradigm: evidence for the hypothesis that car drivers learn to keep in lane with peripheral vision. In: Gale AG, Brown ID, Haslegrave CM, Taylor SP (eds) Vision in Vehicles VI. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp Wilkie RM, Wann JP (2003) Eye-movements aid the control of locomotion. J Vision 3:

Journal of Motor Behavior, 2008, Vol. 40, No. 3, Copyright 2008 Heldref Publications

Journal of Motor Behavior, 2008, Vol. 40, No. 3, Copyright 2008 Heldref Publications Journal of Motor Behavior, 8, Vol. 4, No. 3, 9 Copyright 8 Heldref Publications Eye Movements Drive Steering: Reduced Eye Movement Distribution Impairs Steering and Driving Performance Mark Wilson Institute

More information

Characterizing Visual Attention during Driving and Non-driving Hazard Perception Tasks in a Simulated Environment

Characterizing Visual Attention during Driving and Non-driving Hazard Perception Tasks in a Simulated Environment Title: Authors: Characterizing Visual Attention during Driving and Non-driving Hazard Perception Tasks in a Simulated Environment Mackenzie, A.K. Harris, J.M. Journal: ACM Digital Library, (ETRA '14 Proceedings

More information

Changing Driver Behavior Through Unconscious Stereotype Activation

Changing Driver Behavior Through Unconscious Stereotype Activation University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2009 Driving Assessment Conference Jun 23rd, 12:00 AM Changing Driver Behavior Through Unconscious Stereotype Activation Rob Gray Arizona

More information

Chapter 5 Car driving

Chapter 5 Car driving 5 Car driving The present thesis addresses the topic of the failure to apprehend. In the previous chapters we discussed potential underlying mechanisms for the failure to apprehend, such as a failure to

More information

OPTIC FLOW IN DRIVING SIMULATORS

OPTIC FLOW IN DRIVING SIMULATORS OPTIC FLOW IN DRIVING SIMULATORS Ronald R. Mourant, Beverly K. Jaeger, and Yingzi Lin Virtual Environments Laboratory 334 Snell Engineering Center Northeastern University Boston, MA 02115-5000 In the case

More information

Traffic Sign Detection and Identification

Traffic Sign Detection and Identification University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2013 Driving Assessment Conference Jun 19th, 12:00 AM Traffic Sign Detection and Identification Vaughan W. Inman SAIC, McLean, VA Brian

More information

TEACHING DEAF PEOPLE TO DRIVE

TEACHING DEAF PEOPLE TO DRIVE TEACHING DEAF PEOPLE TO DRIVE HANDBOOK FOR DRIVING INSTRUCTORS Published by the IMTD Published by the IMTD Page 1 V0.2-04/09/2012 This booklet has been produced by the Institute of Master Tutors of Driving

More information

The Effects of Age and Distraction on Reaction Time in a Driving Simulator

The Effects of Age and Distraction on Reaction Time in a Driving Simulator University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2001 Driving Assessment Conference Aug 15th, 12:00 AM The Effects of Age and Distraction on Reaction Time in a Driving Simulator Justin

More information

Proper Bike Position (Fitting)

Proper Bike Position (Fitting) Proper Bike Position (Fitting) COMFORT + EFFICIENCY = PERFORMANCE (The Golden Rule) Three reasons why bike set up is very important for everyone: 1/ Optimum Performance. 2/ Injury Prevention 3/ Your body

More information

DRIVE CLEAR OF PAIN A COMFORTABLE RIDE WITH ERGONOMIC ADVICE THE KEY TO YOU WILL BE SITTING PRETTY SIMPLE TIPS FOR

DRIVE CLEAR OF PAIN A COMFORTABLE RIDE WITH ERGONOMIC ADVICE THE KEY TO YOU WILL BE SITTING PRETTY SIMPLE TIPS FOR DRIVE CLEAR OF PAIN THE KEY TO SIMPLE TIPS FOR DRIVING WELL STAYING FIT YOU WILL BE SITTING PRETTY A COMFORTABLE RIDE WITH ERGONOMIC ADVICE BROUGHT TO YOU BY THE CHARTERED SOCIETY OF PHYSIOTHERAPY induc

More information

Vision Science III Handout 15

Vision Science III Handout 15 Vision Science III Handout 15 NYSTAGMUS Nystagmus describes a pattern of eye movements in which the eyes move to and fro, usually with alternating Slow and Fast phases. Nystagmus occurs normally in some

More information

Rules of apparent motion: The shortest-path constraint: objects will take the shortest path between flashed positions.

Rules of apparent motion: The shortest-path constraint: objects will take the shortest path between flashed positions. Rules of apparent motion: The shortest-path constraint: objects will take the shortest path between flashed positions. The box interrupts the apparent motion. The box interrupts the apparent motion.

More information

Driving (for work or fun) Can Contribute to the Development of Repetitive Strain Injuries:

Driving (for work or fun) Can Contribute to the Development of Repetitive Strain Injuries: Driving (for work or fun) Can Contribute to the Development of Repetitive Strain Injuries: Driving in a vehicle for long distances can contribute to lower and upper back and neck pain, poor circulation

More information

Evaluating the Safety of Verbal Interface Use while Driving

Evaluating the Safety of Verbal Interface Use while Driving Special Issue Speech-Based Interfaces in Vehicles 23 Research Report Evaluating the Safety of Verbal Interface Use while Driving Shinichi Kojima, Yuji Uchiyama, Hiroyuki Hoshino, Takero Hongo Abstract

More information

Analysis of Glance Movements in Critical Intersection Scenarios

Analysis of Glance Movements in Critical Intersection Scenarios Analysis of Glance Movements in Critical Intersection Scenarios Marina Plavši, Klaus Bengler, Heiner Bubb Lehrstuhl für Ergonomie Technische Universität München 85747 Garching b. München, Germany ABSTRACT

More information

Stimulus-Response Compatibilitiy Effects for Warning Signals and Steering Responses

Stimulus-Response Compatibilitiy Effects for Warning Signals and Steering Responses University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2003 Driving Assessment Conference Jul 24th, 12:00 AM Stimulus-Response Compatibilitiy Effects for Warning Signals and Steering Responses

More information

Adaptation to visual feedback delays in manual tracking: evidence against the Smith Predictor model of human visually guided action

Adaptation to visual feedback delays in manual tracking: evidence against the Smith Predictor model of human visually guided action Exp Brain Res (2006) DOI 10.1007/s00221-005-0306-5 RESEARCH ARTICLE R. C. Miall Æ J. K. Jackson Adaptation to visual feedback delays in manual tracking: evidence against the Smith Predictor model of human

More information

DRIVE CLEAR OF PAIN ERGONOMIC ADVICE THE KEY TO DRIVING WELL YOU WILL BE SITTING PRETTY. simple TIps for STAYING FIT

DRIVE CLEAR OF PAIN ERGONOMIC ADVICE THE KEY TO DRIVING WELL YOU WILL BE SITTING PRETTY. simple TIps for STAYING FIT DRIVE CLEAR OF PAIN THE KEY TO DRIVING WELL simple TIps for STAYING FIT YOU WILL BE SITTING PRETTY a comfortable ride WITH ERGONOMIC ADVICE BrOUgHT TO YOU BY THE CHARTERED SOCIETY OF PHYSIOTHERAPY induc

More information

Iran. T. Allahyari, J. Environ. et Health. al., USEFUL Sci. Eng., FIELD 2007, OF Vol. VIEW 4, No. AND 2, RISK pp OF... processing system, i.e

Iran. T. Allahyari, J. Environ. et Health. al., USEFUL Sci. Eng., FIELD 2007, OF Vol. VIEW 4, No. AND 2, RISK pp OF... processing system, i.e Iran. J. Environ. Health. Sci. Eng., 2007, Vol. 4, No. 2, pp. 133-138 USEFUL FIELD OF VIEW AND RISK OF ACCIDENT IN SIMULATED CAR DRIVING 1 T. Allahyari, *1 G. Nasl Saraji, 1 J. Adl, 2 M. Hosseini, 3 M.

More information

Psychological Research

Psychological Research Psychol Res (1984) 46:121-127 Psychological Research Springer-Verlag 1984 Research note: Peak velocity timing invariance Alan M. Wing I and Ed Miller 2 1 Medical Research Council Applied Psychology Unit,

More information

VIDEONYSTAGMOGRAPHY (VNG)

VIDEONYSTAGMOGRAPHY (VNG) VIDEONYSTAGMOGRAPHY (VNG) Expected outcomes Site of lesion localization: Determine which sensory input, motor output, and/ or neural pathways may be responsible for reported symptoms. Functional ability:

More information

PCT 101. A Perceptual Control Theory Primer. Fred Nickols 8/27/2012

PCT 101. A Perceptual Control Theory Primer. Fred Nickols 8/27/2012 PCT 101 A Perceptual Control Theory Primer Fred Nickols 8/27/2012 This paper presents a simplified, plain language explanation of Perceptual Control Theory (PCT). PCT is a powerful and practical theory

More information

Naturalistic Driving Performance During Secondary Tasks

Naturalistic Driving Performance During Secondary Tasks University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2007 Driving Assessment Conference Jul 11th, 12:00 AM Naturalistic Driving Performance During Secondary Tasks James Sayer University

More information

FACTORS AFFECTING THE SMOOTH PURSUIT OF A MOVING TARGET

FACTORS AFFECTING THE SMOOTH PURSUIT OF A MOVING TARGET FACTORS AFFECTING THE SMOOTH PURSUIT OF A MOVING TARGET Ben Ames, Kivi Dennis, Tracey Hoang, and Paul Jobson Clemson University Abstract In the past few years there has been little study on the role of

More information

Detecting and Reading Text on HUDs: Effects of Driving Workload and Message Location

Detecting and Reading Text on HUDs: Effects of Driving Workload and Message Location Detecting and Reading Text on HUDs: Effects of Driving Workload and Message Location Omer Tsimhoni*, Paul Green*, and Hiroshi Watanabe** *University of Michigan Transportation Research Institute Human

More information

The Technical Model: an Overview Explanation of the Technical Model

The Technical Model: an Overview Explanation of the Technical Model A Technical Model for Pole Vault Success Michael A. Young Louisiana State University ****************This is a modified version of an article published in Track Coach Pole vaulting is perhaps the most

More information

DISTRACTION AN ACADEMIC PERSPECTIVE. Steve Reed Loughborough Design School

DISTRACTION AN ACADEMIC PERSPECTIVE. Steve Reed Loughborough Design School DISTRACTION AN ACADEMIC PERSPECTIVE Steve Reed Loughborough Design School WHAT IS DISTRACTION? Anything that takes a driver s attention away from his/her primary task driving! Multi-tasking is rarely safe

More information

This is a repository copy of Leading to Distraction: Driver distraction, lead car, and road environment.

This is a repository copy of Leading to Distraction: Driver distraction, lead car, and road environment. This is a repository copy of Leading to Distraction: Driver distraction, lead car, and road environment. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/93186/ Version: Accepted

More information

THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING

THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING George J. Andersen, Rui Ni Department of Psychology University of California Riverside Riverside, California, USA E-mail: Andersen@ucr.edu E-mail: ruini@ucr.edu

More information

MARK WILSON, NICKOLAS C. SMITH, MARK CHATTINGTON, MIKE FORD, & DILWYN E. MARPLE-HORVAT

MARK WILSON, NICKOLAS C. SMITH, MARK CHATTINGTON, MIKE FORD, & DILWYN E. MARPLE-HORVAT Journal of Sports Sciences, November 2006; 24(11): 1223 1233 The role of effort in moderating the anxiety performance relationship: Testing the prediction of processing efficiency theory in simulated rally

More information

Koji Sakai. Kyoto Koka Women s University, Ukyo-ku Kyoto, Japan

Koji Sakai. Kyoto Koka Women s University, Ukyo-ku Kyoto, Japan Psychology Research, September 2018, Vol. 8, No. 9, 435-442 doi:10.17265/2159-5542/2018.09.002 D DAVID PUBLISHING Effect of Pursuit Eye Movement and Attentional Allocation on Perceptual-Motor Prediction

More information

Exercise Program WARNING: DO NOT USE THIS OR ANY OTHER EXERCISE PROGRAM WITHOUT FIRST BEING CLEARED BY YOUR DOCTOR.

Exercise Program WARNING: DO NOT USE THIS OR ANY OTHER EXERCISE PROGRAM WITHOUT FIRST BEING CLEARED BY YOUR DOCTOR. Exercise Program WARNING: DO NOT USE THIS OR ANY OTHER EXERCISE PROGRAM WITHOUT FIRST BEING CLEARED BY YOUR DOCTOR. Things you MUST know before you begin Posture It is absolutely critical to maintain proper

More information

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object Supplemental materials for: The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object David Whitney*, David A. Westwood, & Melvyn A. Goodale* *Group on Action and Perception, The

More information

ANTICIPATING DYNAMIC LOADS IN HANDLING OBJECTS.

ANTICIPATING DYNAMIC LOADS IN HANDLING OBJECTS. ANTICIPATING DYNAMIC LOADS IN HANDLING OBJECTS. Alan M. Wing Sensory Motor Neuroscience Centre, The University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham, B15 2TT, UK. a.m.wing@bham.ac.uk J. Randall Flanagan

More information

Biologically-Inspired Human Motion Detection

Biologically-Inspired Human Motion Detection Biologically-Inspired Human Motion Detection Vijay Laxmi, J. N. Carter and R. I. Damper Image, Speech and Intelligent Systems (ISIS) Research Group Department of Electronics and Computer Science University

More information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time David E. Huber (dhuber@psyc.umd.edu) Department of Psychology, 1147 Biology/Psychology Building College Park, MD 2742 USA Denis Cousineau (Denis.Cousineau@UMontreal.CA)

More information

Exploration and Exploitation in Reinforcement Learning

Exploration and Exploitation in Reinforcement Learning Exploration and Exploitation in Reinforcement Learning Melanie Coggan Research supervised by Prof. Doina Precup CRA-W DMP Project at McGill University (2004) 1/18 Introduction A common problem in reinforcement

More information

A PCT Primer. Fred Nickols 6/30/2011

A PCT Primer. Fred Nickols 6/30/2011 Fred Nickols 6/30/2011 This paper presents a simplified, plain language explanation of Perceptual Control Theory (PCT). PCT is a powerful theory of human behavior and one that I find far more satisfying

More information

Perception of Heading and Driving Distance From Optic Flow

Perception of Heading and Driving Distance From Optic Flow Perception of Heading and Driving Distance From Optic Flow Markus Lappe, Antje Grigo, Frank Bremmer, Harald Frenz Zoology & Neurobiology, Ruhr University, 44780 Bochum, Germany Tel. +49/234/32-24350, Fax.

More information

Things you need to know about the Normal Distribution. How to use your statistical calculator to calculate The mean The SD of a set of data points.

Things you need to know about the Normal Distribution. How to use your statistical calculator to calculate The mean The SD of a set of data points. Things you need to know about the Normal Distribution How to use your statistical calculator to calculate The mean The SD of a set of data points. The formula for the Variance (SD 2 ) The formula for the

More information

The Future of Exercise

The Future of Exercise The Future of Exercise (1997 and Beyond) ArthurJonesExercise.com 9 Requirements for Proper Exercise (con t) The relatively poor strength increases that were produced in the unworked range of movement during

More information

The Design of Driving Simulator Performance Evaluations for Driving With Vision Impairments and Visual Aids. Aaron J. Mandel

The Design of Driving Simulator Performance Evaluations for Driving With Vision Impairments and Visual Aids. Aaron J. Mandel The Design of Driving Simulator Performance Evaluations for Driving With Vision Impairments and Visual Aids Aaron J. Mandel Schepens Eye Research Institute Harvard Medical School, Boston MA Loss of Vision

More information

Hampstead Hill School Health and Safety Policy: Display Screen Equipment

Hampstead Hill School Health and Safety Policy: Display Screen Equipment All Hampstead Hill School Policies are always to be read and considered in conjunction with Equal Opportunities, Race Equality and Inclusion Policies. This Policy of Hampstead Hill School applies to all

More information

PREVENTING DISTRACTED DRIVING. Maintaining Focus Behind the Wheel of a School Bus

PREVENTING DISTRACTED DRIVING. Maintaining Focus Behind the Wheel of a School Bus PREVENTING DISTRACTED DRIVING Maintaining Focus Behind the Wheel of a School Bus OUR THANKS! This educational program was funded by the National Highway Traffic Safety Administration with a grant from

More information

Chapter 3 CORRELATION AND REGRESSION

Chapter 3 CORRELATION AND REGRESSION CORRELATION AND REGRESSION TOPIC SLIDE Linear Regression Defined 2 Regression Equation 3 The Slope or b 4 The Y-Intercept or a 5 What Value of the Y-Variable Should be Predicted When r = 0? 7 The Regression

More information

CONTENTS OF ACTIVATE 11

CONTENTS OF ACTIVATE 11 CONTENTS OF ACTIVATE Preface...7 Physical Education Curriculum Objectives...10 Curriculum Outcomes For Activate... Acknowledgements & music...12 Introduction to ACTIVATE...14 Background to the Activate

More information

Chapter 7: Descriptive Statistics

Chapter 7: Descriptive Statistics Chapter Overview Chapter 7 provides an introduction to basic strategies for describing groups statistically. Statistical concepts around normal distributions are discussed. The statistical procedures of

More information

Highways Agency. Managed Motorways 2 Concept Development. Tasks 2, 3, 4 and 5. Key Findings Report CPR1062

Highways Agency. Managed Motorways 2 Concept Development. Tasks 2, 3, 4 and 5. Key Findings Report CPR1062 Highways Agency Managed Motorways 2 Concept Development Tasks 2, 3, 4 and 5 Key Findings Report CPR1062 March 2011 Prepared by: TRL Crowthorne House Nine Mile Ride Wokingham Berkshire RG40 3GA Prepared

More information

LAB 1: MOTOR LEARNING & DEVELOPMENT REACTION TIME AND MEASUREMENT OF SKILLED PERFORMANCE. Name: Score:

LAB 1: MOTOR LEARNING & DEVELOPMENT REACTION TIME AND MEASUREMENT OF SKILLED PERFORMANCE. Name: Score: LAB 1: MOTOR LEARNING & DEVELOPMENT REACTION TIME AND MEASUREMENT OF SKILLED PERFORMANCE Name: Score: Part I: Reaction Time Environments Introduction: Reaction time is a measure of how long it takes a

More information

CONTENTS 1. TEACHING GUIDE

CONTENTS 1. TEACHING GUIDE CONTENTS 1. TEACHING GUIDE PART 1 TEACHING GUIDE... 1 A How to use the manual...3 A1 The teaching guide... 3 A2 The teaching material... 3 A3 The Appendix... 5 A4 The lesson framework... 7 B Safety...8

More information

APPENDIX FOR "DRIVING SIMULATOR FOR PERSONS WITH IMPAIRED COGNITION", PI WILLIAM K DURFEE

APPENDIX FOR DRIVING SIMULATOR FOR PERSONS WITH IMPAIRED COGNITION, PI WILLIAM K DURFEE APPENDIX FOR "DRIVING SIMULATOR FOR PERSONS WITH IMPAIRED COGNITION", PI WILLIAM K DURFEE Contents 1. Drive Log (simulation run): Used by researcher to record events that took place during a simulator

More information

7 Grip aperture and target shape

7 Grip aperture and target shape 7 Grip aperture and target shape Based on: Verheij R, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ. The influence of target object shape on maximum grip aperture in human grasping movements. Exp Brain Res, In revision 103 Introduction

More information

Active Gaze, Visual Look-Ahead, and Locomotor Control

Active Gaze, Visual Look-Ahead, and Locomotor Control Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 28, Vol. 34, No. 5, 115 1164 Copyright 28 by the American Psychological Association 96-1523/8/$12. DOI: 1.137/96-1523.34.5.115 Active

More information

TABLE OF CONTENTS SECTION 1 - TEACHING GUIDE. Preface... i International Education Levels and Ages...iv-vi

TABLE OF CONTENTS SECTION 1 - TEACHING GUIDE. Preface... i International Education Levels and Ages...iv-vi TABLE OF CONTENTS Preface... i International Education Levels and Ages...iv-vi SECTION 1 - TEACHING GUIDE PART 1 TEACHING GUIDE... 1 A How to use the manual...3 A1 The teaching guide... 3 A2 The teaching

More information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL 1 SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL Response time and signal detection time distributions SM Fig. 1. Correct response time (thick solid green curve) and error response time densities (dashed red curve), averaged across

More information

Information about Postural Management

Information about Postural Management Acute Services Division Information about Postural Management Supporting Information Leaflet (4): Postural Management Clinical Genetics Department Laboratory Building Southern General Hospital Telephone

More information

http://www.diva-portal.org This is the published version of a paper presented at Future Active Safety Technology - Towards zero traffic accidents, FastZero2017, September 18-22, 2017, Nara, Japan. Citation

More information

Benjamin Blankertz Guido Dornhege Matthias Krauledat Klaus-Robert Müller Gabriel Curio

Benjamin Blankertz Guido Dornhege Matthias Krauledat Klaus-Robert Müller Gabriel Curio Benjamin Blankertz Guido Dornhege Matthias Krauledat Klaus-Robert Müller Gabriel Curio During resting wakefulness distinct idle rhythms located over various brain areas: around 10 Hz (alpha range) over

More information

CONTENTS. Acknowledgments... 5 Preface... 6

CONTENTS. Acknowledgments... 5 Preface... 6 CONTENTS Acknowledgments..................................................... 5 Preface.............................................................. 6 Chapter 1: Perspective for Physical Education in

More information

VIDEONYSTAGMOGRAPHY (VNG) TUTORIAL

VIDEONYSTAGMOGRAPHY (VNG) TUTORIAL VIDEONYSTAGMOGRAPHY (VNG) TUTORIAL Expected Outcomes Site of lesion localization: Determine which sensory input, motor output, and/or neural pathways may be responsible for the patient s reported symptoms

More information

Effect of Positive and Negative Instances on Rule Discovery: Investigation Using Eye Tracking

Effect of Positive and Negative Instances on Rule Discovery: Investigation Using Eye Tracking Effect of Positive and Negative Instances on Rule Discovery: Investigation Using Eye Tracking Miki Matsumuro (muro@cog.human.nagoya-u.ac.jp) Kazuhisa Miwa (miwa@is.nagoya-u.ac.jp) Graduate School of Information

More information

Future path and tangent point models in the visual control of locomotion in curve driving

Future path and tangent point models in the visual control of locomotion in curve driving https://helda.helsinki.fi Future path and tangent point models in the visual control of locomotion in curve driving Lappi, Otto 2014-12-21 Lappi, O 2014, ' Future path and tangent point models in the visual

More information

CHASING WITH A MODEL EYE

CHASING WITH A MODEL EYE J. exp. Biol. 137, 399-409 (1988) 399 Printed in Great Britain The Company of Biologists Limited 1988 CHASING WITH A MODEL EYE BY STEPHEN YOUNG Department of Pure and Applied Biology, Imperial College,

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

In-Vehicle Communication and Driving: An Attempt to Overcome their Interference

In-Vehicle Communication and Driving: An Attempt to Overcome their Interference Vollrath&Totzke_NHTSA_Artikel.doc Seite 1 In-Vehicle and Driving: An Attempt to Overcome their Interference Mark Vollrath and Ingo Totzke, Center for Traffic Sciences (IZVW), University of Wuerzburg, Germany

More information

CERTIFIED WEIGHTLIFTING PERFORMANCE COACH. Notebook THE SQUAT

CERTIFIED WEIGHTLIFTING PERFORMANCE COACH. Notebook THE SQUAT CERTIFIED WEIGHTLIFTING PERFORMANCE COACH Notebook THE SQUAT THE SQUAT Squatting as a part of Olympic weightlifting cannot be overlooked. It will be a large part of any good training program, but also

More information

Estimation of Driver Inattention to Forward Objects Using Facial Direction with Application to Forward Collision Avoidance Systems

Estimation of Driver Inattention to Forward Objects Using Facial Direction with Application to Forward Collision Avoidance Systems University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2007 Driving Assessment Conference Jul 12th, 12:00 AM Estimation of Driver Inattention to Forward Objects Using Facial Direction with

More information

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler Natural Scene Statistics and Perception W.S. Geisler Some Important Visual Tasks Identification of objects and materials Navigation through the environment Estimation of motion trajectories and speeds

More information

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields.

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Yingxue Wang, Sandro Romani, Brian Lustig, Anthony Leonardo, Eva Pastalkova Supplementary Materials Supplementary Modeling

More information

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design 11-1 Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design Advantages and Limitations Comparing Two Groups Comparing t Test to ANOVA Independent Samples t Test Independent Samples ANOVA Comparing

More information

Building Better Balance

Building Better Balance Building Better Balance The Effects of MS on Balance Individuals with MS experience a decline in their balance due to various MS related impairments. Some of these impairments can be improved with exercise

More information

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES Carryl L. Baldwin and Joseph T. Coyne Department of Psychology Old Dominion University

More information

Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk

Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk Exp Brain Res (1998) 123:346±350 Springer-Verlag 1998 RESEARCH NOTE Jinsung Wang George E. Stelmach Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk Received: 1 April

More information

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation The Role of in Categorisation Mark Suret (m.suret@psychol.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology; Downing Street Cambridge, CB2 3EB UK I.P.L. McLaren (iplm2@cus.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental

More information

Distraction, Cognition, Behaviour and Driving Analysis of a large data set

Distraction, Cognition, Behaviour and Driving Analysis of a large data set 21 st MEETING OF THE INTERNATIONAL TRAFFIC SAFETY DATA AND ANALYSIS GROUP (IRTAD) Ljubljana, Slovenia October, 12-14, 2015 Distraction, Cognition, Behaviour and Driving Analysis of a large data set George

More information

MOORE CHIROPRACTIC CENTER 707 Sunset Street Denton, TX (940)

MOORE CHIROPRACTIC CENTER 707 Sunset Street Denton, TX (940) MOORE CHIROPRACTIC CENTER 707 Sunset Street Denton, TX 76201 (940) 383-9399 Date Date of Accident File # ACCIDENT HISTORY REPORT Name Address City State Zip Children Date of Birth / / Sex M F Marital Status

More information

21-Day Belly Blast Challenge!

21-Day Belly Blast Challenge! 21-Day Belly Blast Challenge! "BONUS" Workouts! Copyright 2013 by LifeFuel Fitness and BrightLion Fitness LLC DISCLAIMER: This e-book is for information purposes only. The information presented is in no

More information

c. Construct a boxplot for the data. Write a one sentence interpretation of your graph.

c. Construct a boxplot for the data. Write a one sentence interpretation of your graph. STAT 280 Sample Test Problems Page 1 of 1 1. An English survey of 3000 medical records showed that smokers are more inclined to get depressed than non-smokers. Does this imply that smoking causes depression?

More information

Artificial Intelligence Lecture 7

Artificial Intelligence Lecture 7 Artificial Intelligence Lecture 7 Lecture plan AI in general (ch. 1) Search based AI (ch. 4) search, games, planning, optimization Agents (ch. 8) applied AI techniques in robots, software agents,... Knowledge

More information

System for Evaluation of the Static Posturography Based on Wii Balance Board

System for Evaluation of the Static Posturography Based on Wii Balance Board System for Evaluation of the Static Posturography Based on Wii Balance Board PIVNICKOVA L., DOLINAY V., VASEK V. Faculty of Applied Informatics Tomas Bata University in Zlin nam. T.G.Masaryka 5555, 760

More information

Balance (Vestibular) Rehabilitation

Balance (Vestibular) Rehabilitation Balance (Vestibular) Rehabilitation When there is a problem in the balance (or vestibular) system either in the ears or in the brain, the symptoms can range from mild to very severe. The symptoms can range

More information

Do the same as above, but turn your head TOWARDS the side that you re holding on to the chair.

Do the same as above, but turn your head TOWARDS the side that you re holding on to the chair. Stretch 4-6 times per day and hold each stretch for a minimum of 30 seconds. Perform the stretch gently without bouncing. Discuss any problems with your Chiropractor. Sit upright with your head and shoulder

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Perceptual learning in a non-human primate model of artificial vision

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Perceptual learning in a non-human primate model of artificial vision SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Perceptual learning in a non-human primate model of artificial vision Nathaniel J. Killian 1,2, Milena Vurro 1,2, Sarah B. Keith 1, Margee J. Kyada 1, John S. Pezaris 1,2 1 Department

More information

Exercise Report For: Augusta James

Exercise Report For: Augusta James Exercise Report For: Optimizing Sport Performance Provided By: Greg Redman BScPT, BScKin, Wave Physiotherapy Phone: 250-763-9283 Fax:, www.wavephysio.ca Page: 1 Stretch hip flexor kneel w/ball Stretch

More information

EXERCISES, TIPS AND STRATEGIES for Self-Improvement

EXERCISES, TIPS AND STRATEGIES for Self-Improvement EXERCISES, TIPS AND STRATEGIES for Self-Improvement 03 :: INTRODUCTION 07 :: 1. MOVING AWAY FROM THE DIAGNOSIS OF I M CRAZY : THE STRENGTHS-BASED APPROACH 12 :: 2. CONNECTING TO OTHERS AND MANAGING YOUR

More information

Ergonomics and the Farm. Keri A. Gill-Smith, Physical Therapist

Ergonomics and the Farm. Keri A. Gill-Smith, Physical Therapist Ergonomics and the Farm Keri A. Gill-Smith, Physical Therapist Farm Injury Rates Farming involves hard physical work Every day about 167 agricultural workers suffer a lost-work-time injury From 2008-2010,

More information

Focusing on the Fundamental Movement Skill of Landing in a Gymnastics lesson

Focusing on the Fundamental Movement Skill of Landing in a Gymnastics lesson Sample Lesson Plan Focusing on the Fundamental Movement Skill of Landing in a Gymnastics lesson Class Level Strand Strand Unit Curriculum Objectives 1 st 6 th Class Gymnastics Movement The child should

More information

Vision and Action. 10/3/12 Percep,on Ac,on 1

Vision and Action. 10/3/12 Percep,on Ac,on 1 Vision and Action Our ability to move thru our environment is closely tied to visual perception. Simple examples include standing one one foot. It is easier to maintain balance with the eyes open than

More information

Year 7 Key Performance Indicators Physical Education (Invasion Games)

Year 7 Key Performance Indicators Physical Education (Invasion Games) Key Performance Indicators Physical Education (Invasion Games) PE 7.1: To develop an understanding of the basic rules of sport. I have a full understanding of the I have a good knowledge of the rules.

More information

DRIVING AT NIGHT. It s More Dangerous

DRIVING AT NIGHT. It s More Dangerous DRIVING AT NIGHT You are at greater risk when you drive at night. Drivers can t see hazards as soon as in daylight, so they have less time to respond. Drivers caught by surprise are less able to avoid

More information

Eye Movement Patterns and Driving Performance

Eye Movement Patterns and Driving Performance University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2011 Driving Assessment Conference Jun 30th, 12:00 AM Eye Movement Patterns and Driving Performance Zheng Bian University of California

More information

Effects of Visual and Cognitive Distraction on Lane Change Test Performance

Effects of Visual and Cognitive Distraction on Lane Change Test Performance University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2007 Driving Assessment Conference Jul 10th, 12:00 AM Effects of Visual and Cognitive Distraction on Lane Change Test Performance Johan

More information

Keeping Autism in Mind

Keeping Autism in Mind Page 1 of 7 With a clear understanding of the nature of Autism there is much that can be done to help people with Autism through their everyday life. In highlighting some of the features of Autism this

More information

Physical Education National Curriculum Key stage 1 Key stage 2 Swimming and water safety

Physical Education National Curriculum Key stage 1 Key stage 2 Swimming and water safety Physical Education National Curriculum Key stage 1 Pupils should develop fundamental movement skills, become increasingly competent and confident and access a broad range of opportunities to extend their

More information

Task difficulty, risk, effort and comfort in a simulated driving task - Implications for Risk Allostasis theory

Task difficulty, risk, effort and comfort in a simulated driving task - Implications for Risk Allostasis theory Task difficulty, risk, effort and comfort in a simulated driving task - Implications for Risk Allostasis theory Ben Lewis-Evans* & Talib Rothengatter 1 Traffic and Environmental Psychology Group, Experimental

More information

2002 Physioball Supplement

2002 Physioball Supplement 2002 Physioball Supplement These exercises are not detailed on the 2002 Off-Ice Training video but will be taught in detail during the 2002 Reach for the Stars Seminar. CORE STRENGTH Physioball/ Sport

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorder: In the Workplace and On the Road

Autism Spectrum Disorder: In the Workplace and On the Road Autism Spectrum Disorder: In the Workplace and On the Road Ronna Fried, Ed.D. The Alan and Lorraine Bressler Clinical and Research Programs for Autism Spectrum Disorder at Massachusetts General Hospital

More information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time David E. Huber (dhuber@psych.colorado.edu) Department of Psychology, 1147 Biology/Psychology Building College Park, MD 2742 USA Denis Cousineau (Denis.Cousineau@UMontreal.CA)

More information

EYE MOVEMENTS DURING VISUAL AND AUDITORY TASK PERFORMANCE

EYE MOVEMENTS DURING VISUAL AND AUDITORY TASK PERFORMANCE NAVAL HEALTH RESEARCH CENTER EYE MOVEMENTS DURING VISUAL AND AUDITORY TASK PERFORMANCE E. Viirre K. Van Orden S. Wing B. Chase C. Pribe V. Taliwal J. Kwak Report No. 04-04 Approved for public release;

More information