Published online: 12 Aug 2014.

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Published online: 12 Aug 2014."

Transcription

1 This article was downloaded by: [E. Arthur Shores] On: 16 August 2014, At: 02:05 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: Registered office: Mortimer House, Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Applied Neuropsychology: Adult Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: A Comparison of the Degree of Effort Involved in the TOMM and the ACS Word Choice Test Using a Dual-Task Paradigm Lucienne Isabel Barhon a, Jennifer Batchelor a, Susanne Meares a, Eugene Chekaluk a & E. Arthur Shores a a Psychology, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia Published online: 12 Aug To cite this article: Lucienne Isabel Barhon, Jennifer Batchelor, Susanne Meares, Eugene Chekaluk & E. Arthur Shores (2014): A Comparison of the Degree of Effort Involved in the TOMM and the ACS Word Choice Test Using a Dual-Task Paradigm, Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, DOI: / To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the Content ) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at

2 APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: ADULT, 0: 1 10, 2014 Copyright # Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: print= online DOI: / A Comparison of the Degree of Effort Involved in the TOMM and the ACS Word Choice Test Using a Dual-Task Paradigm Lucienne Isabel Barhon, Jennifer Batchelor, Susanne Meares, Eugene Chekaluk, and E. Arthur Shores Psychology, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia Downloaded by [E. Arthur Shores] at 02:05 16 August 2014 Key words: The aims of the current study were to: (a) examine the predictive validity and efficacy of the Advanced Clinical Solutions Word Choice Test (WCT) as a measure of effort relative to the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM); (b) investigate whether performing a dual (distraction) task would undermine performance on either test; (c) assess the effect of coaching on the diagnostic accuracy of both the WCT and the TOMM; and (d) establish an optimal cut score for the WCT. The current study used a simulation design based on an analogue design in which normal participants were instructed to either apply full effort or simulate a brain injury on the tasks without being detected. Participants included 93 undergraduate university students who were randomly assigned to 1 of 4 conditions: (a) distraction, (b) uncoached traumatic brain injury (TBI) simulators, (c) coached TBI simulators, or (d) full effort. The results demonstrated that the WCT and the TOMM were effective in detecting simulated cognitive impairment. Both tests were resistant to the effects of distraction and were equally effective in detecting coached and uncoached simulators. A cut score of 42 on the WCT was found to provide optimal specificity and sensitivity on the test. neuropsychology, symptom validity testing, tests Although the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM; Tombaugh, 1996) is a well-validated and widely used measure of effort (Axelrod & Schutte, 2011; Merten, Boosink, & Schmand, 2007; O Bryant & Lucas, 2006; Powell, Gfeller, Hendricks, & Sharland, 2004; Rees, Tombaugh, Gansler, & Moczynski, 1998; Tombaugh, 1997), the fact that information regarding the content, procedure, and cut scores of the test is readily available on multiple Web sites (Bauer & McCaffrey, 2006) poses serious threats to the test s validity. Although there are a number of existing measures of effort, it has been argued that there is a need to develop new alternative measures for which the availability of information is regulated (Bauer & McCaffrey, 2006; Suhr & Gunstad, 2007). Address correspondence to Lucienne Isabel Barhon, Psychology, Macquarie University, Balaclava Rd., North Ryde, Sydney, 2109 Australia. lucienne.barhon@students.mq.edu.au The Word Choice Test (WCT; Pearson, 2009) forms part of the Advanced Clinical Solutions (ACS) package and represents another free-standing symptom validity test (SVT). To date, there has been only one study published on the WCT, in which it was reported that in a traumatic brain injury (TBI) sample, the WCT had a sensitivity of 72% and specificity of 89% (Miller et al., 2011). In this study, the good effort group included those patients with moderate-to-severe TBI who failed no more than one of the five administered SVTs, and the suspect effort group included those who failed two or more SVTs. This study demonstrated that the WCT added to the diagnostic accuracy of the ACS. Miller et al. (2011) offered an equation which reveals that a score of 41 or less is suggestive of poor effort on the WCT ([WCT 0.216] 9.022). The WCT needs to be independently validated in severely impaired individuals. In contrast, there has been a large amount of literature validating the TOMM as a measure of effort. Although

3 2 BARHON ET AL. the TOMM has been found to have a high level of specificity, there has been variability in the literature regarding the sensitivity of this measure (Gervais, Rohling, Green, & Ford, 2004; Iverson, Lange, Brooks, & Rennison, 2010; Rees et al., 1998). Specifically, in very impaired populations such as patients with severe dementia, the TOMM has been found to have a high false-positive rate, whereas in TBI samples, the falsepositive rate is low (Iverson et al., 2010; Merten et al., 2007; Pivovarova, Rosenfeld, Dole, Green, & Zapf, 2009; Rees et al., 1998; Teicher & Wagner, 2004; Tombaugh, 1996, 1997). That the TOMM may have low sensitivity has been demonstrated in both clinical and undergraduate samples (An, Zakzanis, & Joordens, 2012; Armistead-Jehle & Gervais, 2011). The TOMM has been demonstrated to be insensitive to age, education, mood disturbance, and all but the most severe dementias (Merten et al., 2007; Pivovarova et al., 2009; Tombaugh, 1996). Rees et al. (1998) reported the measure to have 100% specificity and 84% to 100% sensitivity across a series of five validation experiments. Using a base rate of 30%, O Bryant and Lucas (2006) reported the TOMM to have a positive predictive value (PPV) of 98% and a negative predictive value (NPV) of 78%. Demonstrating that a measure of effort is specific to poor effort or malingering and therefore insensitive to true cognitive impairment is an important aspect of test validity. That is, the test should primarily measure effort alone and not require any significant cognitive ability (Batt, Shores, & Chekaluk, 2008; Heilbronner et al., 2009). Dual-task paradigms provide one means of demonstrating that a test does not require cognitive ability as dual tasking reduces the individual s capacity to perform at their best without affecting effort. The distraction caused by dual tasking precludes encoding and accordingly reduces the ability to learn information. Dual tasking therefore reduces the ability to perform at one s best, without reducing effort or motivation. In keeping with that contention, Batt et al. (2008) demonstrated that on the TOMM, there was no difference between the performance of those participants fully attending to the task and those who were simultaneously completing a distraction task, suggesting that performance on the TOMM is largely independent of cognitive ability. To date, the effects of distraction on the WCT have not been investigated. One factor that has been recognized to potentially undermine psychological test validity is coaching (Brennan et al., 2009). It has been reported that a large number of attorneys routinely coach their clients on how to approach neuropsychological tests (Brennan et al., 2009; Jelicic, Ceunen, Peters, & Merckelbach, 2011; Powell et al., 2004; Russler, Brett, Klaue, Sailer, & Munte, 2008; Suhr & Gunstad, 2007). Studies investigating the effects of coaching on the TOMM found that although coaching reduced the incidence of detection, the TOMM still detected 80% to 87% (Jelicic et al., 2011) and 93% to 96% (Powell et al., 2004) of coached TBI simulators. Therefore, it is possible that although coaching may cause more sophisticated simulating, it does not necessarily result in a failure of detection on tests of effort. The effects of coaching on the WCT have yet to be examined. One commonly used study design to investigate malingering is a simulation design, where participants are instructed to simulate a condition (such as to malinger the cognitive symptoms of a TBI) while completing tests designed to detect exaggeration in order to assess test efficacy (Erdal, 2004; Heilbronner et al., 2009). Although simulation designs have a number of potential limitations, such as low external validity, inflation of observed specificity, and the potential for a lack of adequate motivation in participants to truly replicate the behavior of a malingerer, there are also a number of benefits (Erdal, 2004; Larrabee, 2007; Powell et al., 2004) for example, confidence of group membership of participants, high internal validity, and confidence that the changes observed in the dependent variables are a result of the experimental manipulations (Larrabee, 2007; Vickery et al., 2004). The aims of the current study were to: (a) examine the predictive validity and efficacy of the WCT as a measure of effort relative to the TOMM; (b) investigate whether performing a dual (distraction) task would undermine performance on either test; (c) assess the effect of coaching on the diagnostic accuracy of both the WCT and the TOMM; and (d) establish an optimal cut score for the WCT. It was hypothesized that there would be no significant difference in the number of simulators detected by the WCT and the TOMM. Further, it was anticipated that performance of the groups applying full effort (full-effort and distraction groups) on both the WCT and the TOMM would be significantly better than that of the two groups instructed to simulate a TBI (coached and uncoached). Therefore, within an undergraduate population, it was expected that the TOMM and the WCT would be of comparable sensitivity and that a comparable degree of effort would be required to complete both measures. It was also hypothesized that performance of the distraction group on the TOMM and on the WCT would not differ from that of the full-effort group. Lastly, it was expected there would be no significant difference between the number of those in the coached and uncoached groups detected by either the WCT or the TOMM. A specific cut score was unable to be hypothesized as it could only be determined by the results returned by the current sample.

4 METHODS Participants Participants were 93 undergraduate psychology students from Macquarie University, most of whom (n ¼ 83) were completing the 1st year of their degree. Each participant completed a two-item manipulation check at the end of the testing session to ensure compliance and effort to follow the given instructions. Participants were asked to rate how much effort they applied toward following their given instructions and how easy it was to follow the instructions. The results of 1 participant were excluded from the analysis due to a reported lack of compliance with test instructions, leaving a final sample size of 92 participants included in the analysis. Those in their 1st year were recruited via an online research participant pool and received course credit for their participation. The remainder participated voluntarily by responding to posters advertising the study. The names of these participants went into a draw for the chance to receive a $50 gift voucher. Three participants reported a history of neurological illness; however, they were not excluded from the analysis, as their scores did not differ from the means obtained by their assigned groups. Procedures and Measures Participants were randomly assigned to the two groups applying full effort (full-effort and distraction groups) and to the two groups instructed to simulate a TBI (coached and uncoached). Power calculations revealed that a sample size of 20 in each group was required to obtain a power of.8 and an effect size of.8 at 95% confidence. All participants gave written consent to participate in the study. A short demographic questionnaire, which contained questions pertaining to age, sex, years of education, and any history of neurological illness, was given to each participant to complete. Word Choice Test (Pearson, 2009). The WCT is a single-trial measure of effort that is described as a test of memory for common words (Pearson, 2009). The WCT is very similar in format to the Word subtest of the Warrington Recognition Memory Test (RMT; Warrington, 1984), although different stimuli (words) are employed. The stimulus booklet for the WCT contains 50 common words, which are individually presented both visually and verbally. Participants are then required to recognize the word previously shown, in a dichotomous forced-choice response format, from pairs of words containing one target and one foil. The WCT, like the TOMM, is scored out of 50, with a score COMPARISON OF THE TOMM AND THE WCT 3 of 1 for each correct response and a score of 0 for each incorrect response. Due to the high number of false positives on the Warrington RMT in patients with severely impaired dementia, the WCT may similarly be sensitive to the cognitive deficits present in severely impaired individuals. The Test of Memory Malingering (Tombaugh, 1997). The TOMM is a three-trial (Trial 1, Trial 2, and Retention Trial) pictorial measure of effort, which was originally designed for use as a test of memory in patients with dementia but was later recognized to be insensitive to memory impairment in many other conditions (Rees et al., 1998; Tombaugh, 1996, 1997). The Retention Trial was not administered in the current research as the two learning trials have been reported to be sufficient in discriminating between simulators and those applying full effort, with minimal increase in sensitivity being gained from administering the last trial (Tombaugh, 1996). The cut score of 45 out of 50 (44 or less indicating a fail) on Trial 2 has been consistently found to correctly discriminate simulators from those applying full effort with very few genuinely cognitively impaired participants scoring below this point (Powell et al., 2004; Rees et al., 1998; Tombaugh, 1996). Therefore, only Trial 2 was used in the analyses to identify detection of those simulating a TBI. In the present study, the TOMM was used as the gold standard with which to compare results on the WCT. The TOMM is commonly employed and widely accepted as a measure of effort in current clinical practice. The TOMM has been found to have high specificity, although the sensitivity of this measure has varied across studies (Gervais et al., 2004; Green, 2011; Iverson et al., 2010; Merten et al., 2007; Pivovarova et al., 2009; Rees et al., 1998; Teicher & Wagner, 2004; Tombaugh, 1996, 1997). Sharland and Gfeller (2007), in their review of National Academy of Neuropsychologists (NAN) professional members and fellows, found that the TOMM was the most frequently used test of effort. This finding was replicated in a UK study as reported by McCarter, Walton, Brooks, and Powell (2009). Full-Effort Group The full-effort group participants were given instructions to complete the tests to the best of their ability. Distraction Group Participants were instructed to complete all the tasks to the best of their ability and to complete the dual (distraction) task during the learning phases of both the TOMM and the WCT. The distraction group is

5 4 BARHON ET AL. therefore identical to the full-effort group apart the simultaneous completion of a distraction task. The distraction task was based on the paradigm suggested by Craik (1982). This task involved orally presenting a number between 1 and 9 every 3 s via a recording. Participants were requested to add 3 to each number and to say their response aloud. Accuracy was not recorded, as the task was simple and was used only as a distraction from the test being administered. The experimenter did, however, follow the responses of all participants to ensure that the majority of numbers were answered correctly and that few numbers were skipped. A practice trial was given before testing began, where the participant was presented just the distraction task for 15 s to ensure they understood the task. The instructions given to participants and the recording used in the present research were the same as that used by Batt et al. (2008), where randomly generated numbers were read at a flat tone onto a recorder. Batt et al. found that administration of a distraction task was a useful way of demonstrating that even when cognitive load was increased and attentional capacity was reduced, participants should be able to complete a measure of effort if it is primarily dependent on effort rather than ability. Although the use of a distraction task changes the nature of the test, the point of the distraction task in the current study was to test which measure was more resilient to interference and thus required less cognitive effort. Uncoached Group Participants in the uncoached group were instructed to simulate the cognitive effects of a TBI during the experiment to try and gain compensation for the injury while remaining undetected. This group was given the scenario taken from the study by Brennan et al. (2009), which asks the participant to simulate having significant brain damage during a cognitive assessment to gain a large settlement in a lawsuit. They are also warned that if they are detected by the examiner, they will lose the lawsuit so they are to simulate the damage in a believable way. Coached Group In addition to the instructions given to the uncoached group, participants in the coached group were prepared using the instructions taken from the study by DenBoer and Hall (2007). These instructions inform the participants of some of the typical symptoms of someone with a brain injury and remind them that major exaggerations such as remembering absolutely nothing will make the examiner easily identify that they are simulating. No further instructions about the level of performance typifying someone with a brain injury nor the methods or instruments used to identify malingering were provided. The authors reported a 20% reduction in ability to detect coached from uncoached malingerers when given those instructions. Participants were administered the demographic questionnaire, followed by the TOMM and WCT and lastly the manipulation check. The administrative order of the TOMM and WCT was counterbalanced between participants to reduce the potential for order effects. Data Analysis Data were analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) Version 18. Initial analysis was performed using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), which was used to investigate the demographic characteristics of the group, overall group effects, and simple between-group contrasts. The dependent variables were scores on the WCT and on Trial 2 of the TOMM. The independent variable was group. The sensitivity and specificity of each test were examined using the MedCalc software ( RESULTS Participants included 25 males (27%) and 67 females (73%), with ages ranging from 17 to 51 years (M ¼ years, SD ¼ 4.12 years). Reported years of education ranged from 6 years to 20 years (M ¼ years, SD ¼ 1.64 years). Two participants who had less than 12 years of education were admitted to the university via an entry examination due to having a culturally diverse background with limited academic experience. There were 23 participants in the distraction group, 22 in the uncoached group, 24 in the coached group, and 23 in the full-effort group. The four groups did not significantly differ on age, F(3, 88) ¼ 0.83, p ¼.48, or education, F(3, 88) ¼ 0.43, p ¼.73. While the ratio of females to males was not equal (67:25) across the whole sample, of critical import was whether the sex ratio was independent of experimental group. A chi-square analysis revealed Pearson s v 2 (3, N ¼ 92) ¼ 2.135, p ¼.545, showing no evidence of sex ratio being anything other than independent of experimental group membership. There was no significant difference between males and females on the TOMM (F ¼ 2.74, p ¼.10) or the WCT (F ¼ 2.95, p ¼.09). The descriptive information pertaining to age, education, and sex within each group is presented in Table 1. Univariate ANOVA revealed a significant overall effect of group on test scores for Trial 1 of the TOMM, F(3, 88) ¼ 25.13, p <.0005, Trial 2 of the TOMM, F(3, 88) ¼ 33.48, p <.0005, and the WCT, F(3, 88) ¼ 24.21, p < There were also moderate effect sizes found for each of these effects with a partial eta-square of

6 COMPARISON OF THE TOMM AND THE WCT 5 Downloaded by [E. Arthur Shores] at 02:05 16 August 2014 TABLE 1 Descriptive Information on the Demographic Variables Within Each Group Distraction (n ¼ 23) TABLE 2 Means and Standard Deviations for Each Group on the TOMM and WCT Distraction Mean (SD) Uncoached (n ¼ 22) Uncoached Mean (SD) Coached (n ¼ 24) Coached Mean (SD) Full Effort (n ¼ 23) Age (years) Mean (SD) (1.93) (7.38) (3.15) (1.82) Range Education (years) Mean (SD) (1.93) (1.27) (1.96) (1.31) Range Sex Male n (%) 8 (35) 6 (27) 4 (17) 7 (31) Female n (%) 15 (65) 16 (73) 20 (83) 16 (69) SD ¼ standard deviation; n ¼ group sample size; % ¼ percentage. Full Effort Mean (SD) TOMM (3.88) (9.42) (10.49) (3.03) Trial 1 TOMM (2.56) (9.80) (11.79) (0.65) Trial 2 WCT (2.14) (9.46) (12.50) (0.71) TOMM ¼ Test of Memory Malingering; WCT ¼ Word Choice Test; SD ¼ standard deviation..46 on Trial 1 of the TOMM,.53 on Trial 2 of the TOMM, and.45 on the WCT. The means and standard deviations of each group on Trial 2 of the TOMM and the WCT are presented in Table 2. Between-group contrasts were performed using the multivariate analysis of variance procedure in SPSS to investigate all hypotheses. On Trial 2 of the TOMM, a statistically significant difference was found between the full-effort group and the uncoached group, F(1, 88) ¼ 44.59, p <.0005, and between the full-effort group and the coached group, F(1, 88) ¼ 62.91, p < Similarly, on the WCT, there was a statistically significant TABLE 3 Mean and SD of the Combined Groups on the TOMM Trial 2 and the WCT Mean TOMM Combined Full Effort (Full Effort þ Distraction) TOMM Simulators (Uncoached þ Coached) WCT Combined Full Effort (Full Effort þ Distraction) WCT Simulators (Uncoached þ Coached) TOMM ¼ Test of Memory Malingering; WCT ¼ Word Choice Test; SD ¼ standard deviation. SD TABLE 4 Number of Participants in Each Group Who Failed=Passed the TOMM and the WCT Passed TOMM and WCT Failed TOMM and WCT Passed TOMM and Failed WCT Failed TOMM and Passed WCT Full Effort Distraction Coached Uncoached Total 47 (51%) 37 (40%) 2 (2%) 6 (7%) TOMM ¼ Test of Memory Malingering; WCT ¼ Word Choice Test. difference found between the full-effort group and the uncoached group, F(1, 88) ¼ 39.55, p <.0005, and between the full-effort group and the coached group, F(1, 88) ¼ 41.04, p < As expected, there was no statistically significant difference between the full-effort and distraction groups on either the TOMM, F(1, 88) ¼ 0.34, p ¼.56, or the WCT, F(1, 88) ¼ 0.47, p ¼.50, or between the coached and uncoached groups on either the TOMM, F(1, 88) ¼ 1.20, p ¼.28, or the WCT, F(1, 88) ¼ 0.00, p ¼.98. Due to a lack of significant difference, the two groups applying full effort (full-effort and distraction groups) Score Control (cum. %) TABLE 5 Scores on the TOMM Trial 2 by Group Distraction (cum. %) Uncoached (cum. %) Coached (cum. %) Total (cum. %) 0 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 1 (4.5) 1 (4.1) 2 (2.2) 16 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (4.5) 1 (8.3) 1 (3.3) 17 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (4.5) 2 (16.7) 2 (5.4) 24 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (4.5) 2 (25) 2 (7.6) 25 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 1 (9.1) 2 (33.3) 3 (10.9) 28 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 1 (13.6) 0 (33.3) 1 (12) 29 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 3 (27.3) 0 (33.3) 3 (15.2) 31 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (27.3) 2 (41.7) 2 (17.4) 32 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 2 (36.4) 0 (41.7) 2 (19.6) 33 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 1 (40.1) 1 (45.8) 2 (21.7) 34 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 1 (45.5) 1 (50) 2 (23.9) 35 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (45.5) 2 (58) 2 (26.1) 36 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 1 (50) 0 (58) 1 (27.2) 37 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 3 (63.6) 3 (70.8) 6 (33.7) 38 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 1 (68.2) 2 (79.2) 3 (37) 39 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 2 (77.3) 2 (87.5) 4 (41.3) 40 0 (0.0) 1 (4.3) 1 (81.8) 0 (87.5) 2 (43.5) 41 0 (0.0) 0 (4.3) 1 (86.4) 0 (87.5) 1 (44.6) 43 0 (0.0) 1 (8.7) 1 (90.1) 0 (87.5) 2 (46.7) 46 0 (0.0) 2 (17.4) 1 (95.5) 0 (87.5) 3 (50) 47 1 (4.3) 0 (17.4) 0 (95.5) 0 (87.5) 1 (51.1) 48 0 (4.3) 3 (30.4) 0 (95.5) 0 (87.5) 3 (54.3) 49 1 (8.7) 4 (47.8) 0 (95.5) 0 (87.5) 5 (59.8) (100) 12 (100) 1 (100) 3 (100) 37 (100) Total TOMM ¼ Test of Memory Malingering; cum. % ¼ cumulative percentage.

7 6 BARHON ET AL. TABLE 6 Scores on the WCT by Group Score Control Distraction Uncoached Coached Total 0 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 1 (4.5) 1 (4.2) 2 (2.2) 15 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (4.5) 1 (8.3) 1 (3.3) 17 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (4.5) 1 (12.5) 1 (4.4) 25 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (4.5) 1 (16.7) 1 (5.4) 27 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (4.5) 2 (25.0) 2 (7.6) 28 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 1 (9.1) 1 (29.2) 2 (9.8) 29 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 2 (18.2) 1 (33.3) 3 (13.0) 30 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (18.2) 1 (37.5) 1 (14.1) 32 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 3 (31.8) 0 (37.5) 3 (17.4) 33 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 1 (36.4) 1 (41.7) 2 (19.6) 34 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 2 (45.5) 1 (45.8) 3 (22.8) 35 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 1 (50.0) 1 (50.0) 2 (25.0) 36 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 2 (59.1) 1 (54.2) 3 (28.3) 37 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 2 (68.2) 2 (62.5) 4 (32.6) 38 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 3 (81.8) 0 (62.5) 3 (35.9) 40 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 2 (90.1) 1 (66.7) 3 (39.1) 41 0 (0.0) 1 (4.3) 0 (90.1) 1 (70.8) 2 (41.3) 42 0 (0.0) 0 (4.3) 0 (90.1) 1 (75.0) 1 (42.4) 44 0 (0.0) 1 (8.7) 0 (90.1) 0 (75.0) 1 (43.5) 46 0 (0.0) 2 (17.4) 0 (90.1) 0 (75.0) 2 (45.7) 47 0 (0.0) 6 (43.5) 0 (90.1) 2 (83.3) 8 (54.3) 48 3 (13) 2 (52.2) 0 (90.1) 0 (83.3) 5 (59.8) 49 9 (52.2) 7 (92.6) 1 (95.5) 0 (83.3) 17 (78.3) (100.0) 4 (100.0) 1 (100.0) 4 (100.0) 20 (100.0) Total WCT ¼ Word Choice Test; cum. % ¼ cumulative percentage. were combined, and the two groups simulating a TBI (coached and uncoached) were combined for both the TOMM and the WCT (see Table 3). Subsequent analysis revealed there was a statistically significant difference between those applying full effort and those instructed to simulate on the WCT, F(1, 88) ¼ 72.11, p <.0005, as well as on the TOMM, F(1, 88) ¼ 98.14, p < Trial 2 of the TOMM has an established cut score of 45 out of 50 (Rees et al., 1998; Tombaugh, 1996), which was found to produce optimal specificity and sensitivity in the present sample. In participants applying full effort (n ¼ 46), there were no misclassifications from the full-effort group and two misclassifications from the distraction group. In those instructed to simulate cognitive impairment (n ¼ 46), 21 out of the 22 uncoached group members and 20 out of the 24 coached group members were correctly identified as applying suboptimal effort, revealing five false negatives in this sample. The results yielded a sensitivity of 89% (95% confidence interval [CI] [77%, 95%]) and specificity of 96% (95% CI [86%, 99%]). There is currently no defined cut score available for the WCT. Sensitivity and specificity were therefore used to identify an optimal cut point on the WCT to use as a diagnostic indicator of suspected simulation. A cut score of 42 (41 or less indicating a fail) was chosen as it produced the optimal point of sensitivity and specificity in the current sample. Of those who applied full effort (n ¼ 46), no participants from the full-effort group were misclassified as simulating and only 1 participant from the distraction group was misclassified as simulating. Therefore, applying the cut score of 42 resulted in only one false-positive classification. In the simulating conditions (n ¼ 46), 20 out of the 22 uncoached group members and 18 out of the 24 full-effort group participants were correctly classified as simulators, producing a total of eight false negatives. Using a cut score of 42 out of 50 resulted in a sensitivity of 83% (95% CI [69%, 91%]) and a specificity of 98% (95% CI [88%, 99%]). There was a 91% concordance between the two measures using the reported cutoff scores (45 on Trial 2 of the TOMM and 42 on the WCT). The WCT did not detect 7% of those who failed on the TOMM, and the TOMM did not detect 2% of those who failed on the WCT. The number and percentage of those who failed or passed either measure within each group is presented in Table 4. The cumulative percentages of scores within each group on each test are presented in Tables 5 and 6 to demonstrate the spread of performance within the TABLE 7 Sensitivity, Specificity, PPV, NPV, and Classification of Different Cut Scores on the TOMM Trial 2 Cut Score Sensitivity (95% CI) Specificity (95% CI) PPV (95% CI) NPV (95% CI) Classification (79.19, 97.53) (76.42, 96.33) (76.88, 96.41) (78.76, 97.47) (71.19, 97.53) (71.19, 97.53) (71.19, 97.53) (71.19, 97.53) (76.42, 96.33) (85.13, 99.34) (84.16, 99.30) (77.76, 96.56) (76.42, 96.33) (85.13, 99.34) (84.16, 99.30) (77.76, 96.56) (76.42, 96.33) (85.13, 99.34) (84.16, 99.30) (77.76, 96.56) (73.73, 95.03) (88.43, 99.64) (87.10, 99.59) (76.12, 95.53) (73.73, 95.03) (88.43, 99.64) (87.10, 99.59) (76.12, 95.53) (71.12, 93.63) (88.43, 99.64) (86.79, 99.58) (86.54, 94.39) (68.57, 92.16) (92.22, ) (90.66, ) (72.87, 93.36) (58.87, 85.72) (92.22, 100,00) (89.62, ) (66.64, 88.82) (51.98, 80.46) (92.22, ) (88.68, ) (88.68, ) PPV ¼ positive predictive value; NPV ¼ negative predictive value; TOMM ¼ Test of Memory Malingering; CI ¼ confidence interval.

8 COMPARISON OF THE TOMM AND THE WCT 7 TABLE 8 Sensitivity, Specificity, PPV, NPV, and Classification of Different Cut Scores on the WCT Cut Score Sensitivity (95% CI) Specificity (95% CI) PPV (95% CI) NPV (95% CI) Classification (73.73, 95.03) (65.00, 89.51) (66.28, 89.95) (72.64, 94.79) (68.57, 92.16) (79.19, 97.53) (77.36, 97.29) (70.88, 92.81) (68.57, 92.16) (85.13, 99.34) (83.05, 99.24) (71.91, 93.10) (68.57, 92.16) (85.13, 99.34) (83.05, 99.24) (71.91, 93.10) (68.57, 92.16) (88.43, 99.64) (86.47, 99.57) (72.40, 93.23) (68.57, 92.16) (88.43, 99.64) (86.47, 99.57) (72.40, 93.23) (68.57, 92.16) (88.43, 99.64) (86.47, 99.57) (72.40, 93.23) (63.63, 89.03) (92.22, ) (92.22, ) (69.60, 91.08) (56.54, 84.00) (92.22, ) (92.22, ) (65.27, 87.70) (56.54, 84.00) (92.22, ) (92.22, ) (65.27, 87.70) (49.75, 78.64) (92.22, ) (92.22, ) (61.50, 84.47) PPV ¼ positive predictive value; NPV ¼ negative predictive value; WCT ¼ Word Choice Test; CI ¼ confidence interval. Downloaded by [E. Arthur Shores] at 02:05 16 August 2014 current sample. Additionally, the sensitivity, specificity, PPV, and NPV of a range of cut scores on both the TOMM and the WCT are presented in Tables 7 and 8. The results of the current study, including the information on the predictive accuracy of the measures, was based on a nonclinical undergraduate sample and thus cannot be generalized to clinical or forensic samples. Results from the manipulation check revealed that there was at least moderate effort applied toward completing the experiment, F(3, 88) ¼ 1.04, p ¼.38, and that those in the groups asked to simulate a TBI found it more difficult to follow the instructions than did those who were instructed to apply full effort, F(3, 88) ¼ 11.97, p < DISCUSSION Detecting poor effort is often a difficult task and ensuring the implementation of valid and reliable tests is crucial. To ensure the ability of each test to distinguish simulators from those applying full effort, the effects of both distraction and coaching need to be investigated. The aims of the study were to examine the predictive validity of the WCT relative to the TOMM, by investigating the effect of a dual (distraction) task as well as coaching on the diagnostic accuracy of both tests in an attempt to compare the level of effort required to complete each of these measures. The final aim of the research was to establish an optimal cut score for the WCT, this being of potential importance to those using the test for clinical purposes. In keeping with the first hypothesis that the WCT would be as effective as the TOMM in detecting those simulating a TBI, the results indicated that 83% of those instructed to simulate impairment were accurately detected by the WCT compared with 89% detected by the TOMM. Although there were slightly more false negatives on the WCT, the difference between the two tests was not large enough to attain significance. The slightly lower sensitivity of the WCT in detecting those simulating a TBI in the current sample may have reflected factors other than those under investigation, such as sampling error, poor understanding of test instructions, or the fact that unlike the TOMM, the WCT is a single-trial task. Given that some studies using clinical and forensic TBI samples have found the TOMM to have a high rate of false positives, the WCT and other SVTs need to be further examined in these populations (Armistead-Jehle & Gervais, 2011; Gervais et al., 2004; Green, 2011). In line with the second hypothesis, a highly significant overall effect of group was revealed for both the WCT and the TOMM with almost identical scores obtained for the two groups applying full effort and almost identical scores for the two groups instructed to simulate a TBI. As expected, those groups applying full effort scored significantly higher compared with those instructed to simulate a TBI on both tests. Further, the WCT only misclassified one participant applying full effort as a simulator, whereas the TOMM misclassified two. Importantly, these three misclassified participants were from the distraction group, and there were no misclassifications from the full-effort group. The current results were consistent with those of a number of other studies investigating the efficacy of the TOMM (Iverson et al., 2010; Merten et al., 2007; Powell et al., 2004; Rees et al., 1998) and demonstrated that in undergraduate volunteer students, the TOMM is a reliable measure with which to compare performance on the WCT. In keeping with the third hypothesis that performance on the WCT and the TOMM would be unaffected by the dual (distraction) task, no significant difference was found between the distraction and full-effort groups on either measure. In relation to the TOMM, the current results replicated those reported by Batt et al. (2008). More importantly, however, the

9 8 BARHON ET AL. present results supported the contention that the WCT is a measure of effort and has little dependence on cognitive ability. The findings provide evidence for the construct validity of this measure. Although it is possible that the distraction task was not sufficiently difficult to impact participants ability to attend to the test, the dual (distraction) task used in the current research was identical to that used by Batt et al., who demonstrated that performance on the Word Memory Test (Green, Allen, & Astner, 1996) was significantly negatively affected by the task. The fourth hypothesis that both the TOMM and the WCT would be robust to the effects of coaching was strongly supported. There was no significant difference between those participants who were coached and those who were not coached on either the TOMM or the WCT. This indicates that with the coaching instructions given, participants who were coached were not able to better avoid being detected as applying poor effort compared with those who were not given any coaching on either measure. This finding has not been previously demonstrated for the WCT. Although similar findings have been reported for the TOMM (Jelicic et al., 2011; Powell et al., 2004; Weinborn, Woods, Nulsen, & Leighton, 2012), other studies have demonstrated that coaching can affect the test s predictive accuracy (Brennan et al., 2009; DenBoer & Hall, 2007; Sollman & Berry, 2011). The disparity in findings may be attributed to the different methods of coaching employed as well as whether a warning was given to participants regarding the potential for detection of poor effort (Weinborn et al., 2012). The availability of information regarding effort measures such as the TOMM is of significant concern as it facilitates lawyers to coach clients. Accordingly, there is an increasing need for newly developed and validated measures of effort, such as the WCT, to be implemented in clinical practice as this will minimize this risk for lawyers being able to readily identify and inform their clients about the measure. The final hypothesis that the TOMM and the WCT were equally able to detect the coached or uncoached participants simulating a TBI was partially confirmed, with the TOMM correctly classifying slightly more of those who were simulating a TBI compared with the WCT. The TOMM correctly identified 95% of the uncoached group and 83% of the coached group. The WCT, however, correctly classified 91% of the uncoached group and 75% of the coached group. Although the effect of coaching was not statistically significant in the present study, prior research has suggested that coaching may produce more sophisticated simulating of cognitive impairment associated with TBI (DenBoer & Hall, 2007; Jelicic et al., 2011; Powell et al., 2004). The TOMM correctly classified 95.7% of those applying full effort giving it quite a low rate of false positives. Fail rates were slightly different for the WCT and the TOMM, with the TOMM having been slightly more sensitive, although only by approximately 6%. It remains unclear whether this is due to the efficacy of the test or other factors such as the nature of the material comprising the measure. Word processing is automatic as demonstrated in cognitive tasks such as the Stroop task (MacLeod, 1991; Stroop, 1935). However, the superior recognition of pictures over words is also well demonstrated (Paivio, 1991; Shepard, 1967). There is currently no universally accepted cut score for the WCT; however, our findings are consistent with those of Miller et al. (2011), who also found a score of 41 or less to be indicative of an invalid performance. The consistency of our findings with those of Miller and colleagues provides further support for the generalizability of these findings regarding an acceptable cut score for the WCT. The current results indicated that a cut score of 42 out of 50 produced only one false positive. Kim et al. (2010) used a cut score of 42 out of 50 for the RMT, a measure very similar to the WCT. Batt et al. (2008) found the TOMM had a PPV of 73% at a base rate of 30%, whereas at the same base rate in the current sample, the PPV of the test was much higher at 90%. The current findings were slightly lower than those of O Bryant and Lucas (2006), who reported the TOMM to have a PPV of 98% at a base rate of 30%. The PPV obtained for the WCT was even higher than the TOMM, having been 94% at a base rate of 30%. Although the WCT has a high PPV within this sample, further research is needed to demonstrate its effectiveness as a measure of effort in clinical samples. Also, the WCT was only slightly less sensitive than the TOMM, with an NPV of 93%, compared with 95% for the TOMM using a base rate of 30%. Miller et al. (2011) reported a slightly lower sensitivity and specificity for the WCT. Thus, the validity indicators derived from the present sample need to be replicated in further research. The current research utilized an analogue design, with an attempt to simulate real-life situations under controlled conditions (Cochrane, Baker, & Meudell, 1998; Larrabee, 2007) to investigate the predictive accuracy of the WCT as well as its robustness to coaching and distraction. To date, this is the first time that these properties of the WCT have been investigated. The WCT has the advantage of being relatively new and not widely known in the medico-legal setting. Currently, little information about the test is available via the Internet, unlike the detailed information relating to the TOMM, which is readily available on the Internet and in books and is well known by attorneys (Bauer & McCaffrey, 2006; Brennan et al., 2009; Russler et al.,

10 COMPARISON OF THE TOMM AND THE WCT ). Further, Miller and colleagues (2011) demonstrated that the WCT added incremental value to the predictive accuracy of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Fourth Edition. The results of the current study were consistent with all hypotheses and provided preliminary data to support the clinical utility of the WCT. Although the study made an important contribution to establishing the WCT as a reliable measure of simulation, there are a number of potential limitations that need to be addressed. The use of analogue research on simulation may limit the generalizability of the results; however, attempts to control these potential limitations as suggested in the criteria by Rogers (1997) were implemented in the experimental design. Although a simulation design can have very high internal validity, it often is restricted by low external validity. The use of simulators as opposed to true malingerers can often overestimate the discriminatory abilities of the tests, meaning it can inflate the obtained specificity (Bender, 2008; Larrabee, 2007). The use of simulators may also mean participants do not have enough motivation to truly replicate the behavior of a malingerer who may be rewarded with significant financial benefits for effectively malingering and who have the threat of severe consequences if detected (Larrabee, 2007; Powell et al., 2004). It is recognized that the coaching instructions given were simple and brief, and this may have not been enough to produce effective feigning of impairment on these measures. Although external validity may be low, experimenters can be relatively sure that the changes observed in the dependent variables (scores on the WCT and the TOMM) are a result of the experimental manipulations, which is a key advantage of using a simulation design. Thus, in the current research, between-group differences are most likely to be solely explained by the different instructions provided to the groups. Further, as the research utilized a simulation design in which participants were all from a nonclinical setting and were allocated to a specific group, the researcher can be confident in the group membership of each participant. Although it is not known whether those in the control group were truly applying effort, inspection of the results revealed that all members of the control group had a near-perfect score. As identified by Weinborn et al. (2012), it is also possible that the resistance to coaching may have been due to the type of coaching provided to the participants. Future research should therefore investigate the effects of more thorough coaching scenarios as well as investigate any differences on these measures between symptom- and test-based coaching. Future research should consider including a simulation group given a distraction manipulation task to address the effect of distraction on unmotivated individuals. Typically, effort measures are administered in the context of a full neuropsychological test battery. Administration of such tests in isolation or in a relatively small battery provides a less realistic picture of how they contribute to the results of an assessment and does not provide insight into whether the inclusion of such measures affects performance on other tasks. Using a full battery was not practical for the purpose of this assessment, especially given the fact that a nonclinical sample was utilized, and future research should look to further investigate the effects of coaching and distraction on the WCT in the context of a full battery. The use of a student sample in the present study was effective in establishing the predictive accuracy of a newly published measure in a design where the group membership was known. Future research should look to include a clinical sample to compare to the performance of the simulating malingerers. The results of the current research contribute to the expanding literature validating current methods of detecting poor effort. The WCT was found to be as effective as the TOMM in differentiating simulators from participants applying full effort, supporting the results reported by Miller et al. (2011). Moreover, the results of the current study supported the contention that, as with the TOMM, the WCT is primarily a measure of effort rather than cognitive ability. This is important given the frequency with which the WCT will potentially be used in assessing individuals with cognitive impairment secondary to brain injury. REFERENCES An, K., Zakzanis, K. K., & Joordens, S. (2012). Conducting research with non-clinical healthy undergraduates: Does effort play a role in neuropsychological test performance? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 27, Armistead-Jehle, P., & Gervais, R. O. (2011). Sensitivity of the Test of Memory Malingering and the Nonverbal Medical Symptom Validity Test: A replication study. Applied Neuropsychology, 18, Axelrod, B. N., & Schutte, C. (2011). Concurrent validity of three forced-choice measures of symptom validity. Applied Neuropsychology, 18, doi: = Batt, K., Shores, A., & Chekaluk, E. (2008). The effect of distraction on the Word Memory Test and Test of Memory Malingering performance in patients with a severe brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, doi: =s x Bauer, L., & McCaffrey, R. J. (2006). Coverage of the Test of Memory Malingering, Victoria Symptom Validity Test, and Word Memory Test on the Internet: Is test security threatened? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, doi: =j.acn Bender, S. D. (2008). Malingered traumatic brain injury. In R. Rogers (Eds.), Clinical assessment of malingering and deception (3rd ed., pp ). New York, NY: Guilford. Brennan, A. M., Meyer, S., David, E., Pella, R., Hill, B. D., & Gouvier, D. (2009). The vulnerability to coaching across measures

11 10 BARHON ET AL. of effort. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23, doi: = Cochrane, H. J., Baker, G. A., & Meudell, P. R. (1998). Simulating a memory impairment: Can amnesics implicitly outperform simulators? British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37, Craik, F. J. M. (1982). Selective changes in encoding as a function of reduced processing capacity. In F. Klic, J. Hoffman & E. Van der Meers (Eds.), Cognitive research in psychology (pp ). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North-Holland Publishing. DenBoer, J. W., & Hall, S. (2007). Neuropsychological test performance of successful brain injury simulators. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 21, doi: = Erdal, K. (2004). The effects of motivation, coaching, and knowledge of neuropsychology on the simulated malingering of head injury. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, Gervais, R. O., Rohling, M. L., Green, P., & Ford, W. (2004). A comparison of WMT, CARB and TOMM failure rates in non-head injured disability claimants. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, Green, P. (2011). Comparison between the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM) and the Nonverbal Medical Symptom Validity Test (NV- MSVT) in adults with disability claims. Applied Neuropsychology, 18, Green, P., Allen, L. M., & Astner, K. (1996). The Word Memory Test: A user s guide to the oral and computer-administered forms, US Version 1.1. Durham, NC: CogniSyst. Heilbronner, R. L., Sweet, J. J., Morgan, J. E., Larrabee, G. J., Millis, S. R., & Conference Participants. (2009). American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology consensus conference statement on the neuropsychological assessment of effort, response bias, and malingering. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23, doi: = Iverson, G. L., Lange, R. T., Brooks, B. L., & Rennison, V. L. A. (2010). Good old days bias following mild traumatic brain injury. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 24, doi: = Jelicic, M., Ceunen, E., Peters, M. J. V., & Merckelbach, H. (2011). Detecting coached feigning using the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM) and the Structured Inventory of Malingered Symptomatology (SIMS). Journal of Clinical Psychology, 67, doi: =jclp=20805 Kim, M., Boone, K., Victor, T., Marion, S., Amano, S., Cottingham, M.,... Zeller, M. A. (2010). The Warrington Recognition Memory Test for Words as a measure of response bias: Total score and response time cutoffs developed on real world credible and noncredible subjects. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 25(1), Larrabee, G. J. (2007). Introduction: Malingering, research designs, and base rates. In G. J. Larrabee (Ed.), Assessment of malingered neuropsychological deficits (pp. 3 13). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, doi: = McCarter, R. J., Walton, N. H., Brooks, D. N., & Powell, G. E. (2009). Effort testing in contemporary UK neuropsychological practice. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23, doi: = Merten, T., Boosink, L., & Schmand, B. (2007). On the limits of effort testing: Symptom validity tests and severity of neurocognitive symptoms in nonlitigant patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29, doi: = Miller, J. B., Millis, S. R., Rapport, L. J., Bashem, J. R., Hanks, R. A., & Axelrod, B. N. (2011). Detection of insufficient effort using the Advanced Clinical Solutions for the Wechsler Memory Scale, Fourth Edition. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 25, doi: = O Bryant, S. E., & Lucas, J. A. (2006). Estimating the predictive value of the Test of Memory Malingering: An illustrative example of clinicians. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20, doi: = Paivio, A. (1991). Images in mild: The evolution of theory. New York, NY: Harvester Wheatsheaf. Pearson. (2009). Advanced clinical solutions for the WAIS-IV and WMS-IV. Sans Antonio, TX: Pearson Education. Pivovarova, E., Rosenfeld, B., Dole, T., Green, D., & Zapf, P. (2009). Are measures of cognitive effort and motivation useful in differentiating feigned from genuine psychiatric symptoms? International Journal of Forensic Mental Health, 8, doi: = Powell, M. R., Gfeller, J. D., Hendricks, B. L., & Sharland, M. (2004). Detecting symptom- and test-coached simulators with the Test of Memory Malingering. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, doi: =j.acn Rees, L. M., Tombaugh, T. N., Gansler, D. A., & Moczynski, N. P. (1998). Five validation experiments of the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM). Psychological Assessment, 10, Rogers, R. (1997). Clinical assessment of malingering and deception (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford. Russler, J., Brett, A., Klaue, U., Sailer, M., & Munte, T. F. (2008). The effect of coaching on the simulated malingering of memory impairment. BMC Neurology, 8, Sharland, M. J., & Gfeller, J. D. (2007). A survey of neuropsychologists beliefs and practices with respect to the assessment of effort. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, doi: = j.acn Shepard, R. N. (1967). Recognition memory for words, sentences, and pictures. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 61, Sollman, M. J., & Berry, D. T. R. (2011). Detection of inadequate effort on neuropsychological testing: A meta-analytic update and extension. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 26, doi: =arclin=acr066 Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of inference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, Suhr, J. A., & Gunstad, J. (2007). Coaching and malingering: A review. In G. J. Larrabee (Ed.), Assessment of malingered neuropsychological deficits (pp ). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Teicher, G., & Wagner, M. T. (2004). Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM): Normative data from cognitively intact, cognitively impaired, and elderly patients with dementia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, Tombaugh, T. N. (1996). TOMM Test of Memory Malingering. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems. Tombaugh, T. N. (1997). The Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM): Normative data from cognitively intact and cognitively impaired individuals. Psychological Assessment, 9, Vickery, C. D., Berry, D. T. R., Dearth, C. S., Vagnini, V. L., Baser, R. E., Crager, D. E., & Orey, S. A. (2004). Head injury and the ability to feign neuropsychological data. Archives of Neuropsychology, 19, Warrington, E. K. (1984). Recognition Memory Test. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Weinborn, M., Woods, S. P., Nulsen, C., & Leighton, A. (2012). Effects of coaching on the Verbal and Nonverbal Medical Symptom Validity Tests. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 26,

The Albany Consistency Index for the Test of Memory Malingering

The Albany Consistency Index for the Test of Memory Malingering Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27 (2012) 1 9 The Albany Consistency Index for the Test of Memory Malingering Jessica H. Gunner 1, *, Andrea S. Miele 1, Julie K. Lynch 2, Robert J. McCaffrey 1,2 1

More information

Effects of severe depression on TOMM performance among disability-seeking outpatients

Effects of severe depression on TOMM performance among disability-seeking outpatients Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 21 (2006) 161 165 Effects of severe depression on TOMM performance among disability-seeking outpatients Y. Tami Yanez, William Fremouw, Jennifer Tennant, Julia Strunk,

More information

The effect of distraction on the Word Memory Test and Test of Memory Malingering performance in patients with a severe brain injury

The effect of distraction on the Word Memory Test and Test of Memory Malingering performance in patients with a severe brain injury Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society (2008), 14, 1074 1080. Copyright 2008 INS. Published by Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. doi:10.10170s135561770808137x BRIEF COMMUNICATION

More information

Anne A. Lawrence M.D. PhD a a Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada Published online: 11 Jan 2010.

Anne A. Lawrence M.D. PhD a a Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada Published online: 11 Jan 2010. This article was downloaded by: [University of California, San Francisco] On: 05 May 2015, At: 22:37 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered

More information

Published online: 25 Aug 2014.

Published online: 25 Aug 2014. This article was downloaded by: [Dr Robert Denney] On: 27 March 2015, At: 10:31 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

Dimitris Pnevmatikos a a University of Western Macedonia, Greece. Published online: 13 Nov 2014.

Dimitris Pnevmatikos a a University of Western Macedonia, Greece. Published online: 13 Nov 2014. This article was downloaded by: [Dimitrios Pnevmatikos] On: 14 November 2014, At: 22:15 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Costanza Scaffidi Abbate a b, Stefano Ruggieri b & Stefano Boca a a University of Palermo

Costanza Scaffidi Abbate a b, Stefano Ruggieri b & Stefano Boca a a University of Palermo This article was downloaded by: [Costanza Scaffidi Abbate] On: 29 July 2013, At: 06:31 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status Effort Scale

The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status Effort Scale Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27 (2012) 190 195 The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status Effort Scale Julia Novitski 1,2, Shelly Steele 2, Stella Karantzoulis 3, Christopher

More information

Back-Calculation of Fish Length from Scales: Empirical Comparison of Proportional Methods

Back-Calculation of Fish Length from Scales: Empirical Comparison of Proportional Methods Animal Ecology Publications Animal Ecology 1996 Back-Calculation of Fish Length from Scales: Empirical Comparison of Proportional Methods Clay L. Pierce National Biological Service, cpierce@iastate.edu

More information

Your choice of SVTs is fundamental to the Slick et al criteria Paul Green Ph.D. paulgreen@shaw.ca www.wordmemorytest.com Central to the criteria is the presence of cognitive symptom exaggeration or feigning

More information

WPE. WebPsychEmpiricist

WPE. WebPsychEmpiricist McKinzey, R. K., Podd, M., & Kreibehl, M. A. (6/25/04). Concurrent validity of the TOMM and LNNB. WebPsychEmpiricist. Retrieved (date), from http://wpe.info/papers_table.html WPE WebPsychEmpiricist Concurrent

More information

The unexamined lie is a lie worth fibbing Neuropsychological malingering and the Word Memory Test

The unexamined lie is a lie worth fibbing Neuropsychological malingering and the Word Memory Test Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 17 (2002) 709 714 The unexamined lie is a lie worth fibbing Neuropsychological malingering and the Word Memory Test David E. Hartman Private Practice/Chicago Medical

More information

Factors Influencing the Face Validity of Effort Tests: Timing of Warning and Feedback

Factors Influencing the Face Validity of Effort Tests: Timing of Warning and Feedback University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers Graduate School 2 Factors Influencing the Face Validity of Effort Tests: Timing

More information

Cognitive Enhancement Using 19-Electrode Z-Score Neurofeedback

Cognitive Enhancement Using 19-Electrode Z-Score Neurofeedback This article was downloaded by: [Lucas Koberda] On: 22 August 2012, At: 09:31 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

Published online: 12 Dec 2014.

Published online: 12 Dec 2014. This article was downloaded by: [52.1.27.110] On: 26 August 2015, At: 23:28 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: 5 Howick Place,

More information

Performance profiles and cut-off scores on the Memory Assessment Scales

Performance profiles and cut-off scores on the Memory Assessment Scales Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) 489 496 Performance profiles and cut-off scores on the Memory Assessment Scales Sid E. O Bryant a, Kevin Duff b, Jerid Fisher c, Robert J. McCaffrey a,d,

More information

To link to this article:

To link to this article: This article was downloaded by: [University of Kiel] On: 24 October 2014, At: 17:27 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

To link to this article:

To link to this article: This article was downloaded by: [University of Notre Dame] On: 12 February 2015, At: 14:40 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:

More information

Chapter 23. Detection of Children s Malingering on Raven s Standard Progressive Matrices*

Chapter 23. Detection of Children s Malingering on Raven s Standard Progressive Matrices* Chapter 23 Detection of Children s Malingering on Raven s Standard Progressive Matrices* R. Kim McKinzey, Jörg Prieler, and John Raven** Abstract A formula for detecting faked Raven s Standard Progressive

More information

Published online: 17 Feb 2011.

Published online: 17 Feb 2011. This article was downloaded by: [Iowa State University] On: 23 April 2015, At: 08:45 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

The Flynn effect and memory function Sallie Baxendale ab a

The Flynn effect and memory function Sallie Baxendale ab a This article was downloaded by: [University of Minnesota] On: 16 August 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 917397643] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England

More information

NANCY FUGATE WOODS a a University of Washington

NANCY FUGATE WOODS a a University of Washington This article was downloaded by: [ ] On: 30 June 2011, At: 09:44 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer

More information

Laura N. Young a & Sara Cordes a a Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut

Laura N. Young a & Sara Cordes a a Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut This article was downloaded by: [Boston College] On: 08 November 2012, At: 09:04 Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Lora-Jean Collett a & David Lester a a Department of Psychology, Wellesley College and

Lora-Jean Collett a & David Lester a a Department of Psychology, Wellesley College and This article was downloaded by: [122.34.214.87] On: 10 February 2013, At: 16:46 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

Richard Lakeman a a School of Health & Human Sciences, Southern Cross University, Lismore, Australia. Published online: 02 Sep 2013.

Richard Lakeman a a School of Health & Human Sciences, Southern Cross University, Lismore, Australia. Published online: 02 Sep 2013. This article was downloaded by: [UQ Library] On: 09 September 2013, At: 21:23 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

Ecological Validity of the WMS-III Rarely Missed Index in Personal Injury Litigation. Rael T. Lange. Riverview Hospital.

Ecological Validity of the WMS-III Rarely Missed Index in Personal Injury Litigation. Rael T. Lange. Riverview Hospital. This is the authors version of a paper that will be published as: Lange, Rael T. Lange and Sullivan, Karen A. and Anderson, Debbie (2005) Ecological validity of the WMS-III Rarely Missed Index in personal

More information

The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System Tower Test Resilience to Response Bias

The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System Tower Test Resilience to Response Bias Ursidae: The Undergraduate Research Journal at the University of Northern Colorado Volume 1 Number 2 Article 2 January 2012 The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System Tower Test Resilience to Response

More information

Interpretive Report. Client Information

Interpretive Report. Client Information Interpretive Report Developed by Michelle R. Widows, PhD, Glenn P. Smith, PhD, and PAR Staff Client Information Client name: Sample Client Client ID: SIMS Test date: 08/12/2013 Date of birth: 02/03/1975

More information

Characterization of the Medical Symptom Validity Test in evaluation of clinically referred memory disorders clinic patients

Characterization of the Medical Symptom Validity Test in evaluation of clinically referred memory disorders clinic patients Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 753 761 Characterization of the Medical Symptom Validity Test in evaluation of clinically referred memory disorders clinic patients Abstract Laura L.S. Howe

More information

Effects of Coaching on Detecting Feigned Cognitive Impairment with the Category Test

Effects of Coaching on Detecting Feigned Cognitive Impairment with the Category Test Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 15, No. 5, pp. 399 413, 2000 Copyright 2000 National Academy of Neuropsychology Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0887-6177/00 $ see front matter PII S0887-6177(99)00031-1

More information

Head injury and the ability to feign neuropsychological deficits

Head injury and the ability to feign neuropsychological deficits Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) 37 48 Head injury and the ability to feign neuropsychological deficits Chad D. Vickery, David T.R. Berry, Chantel S. Dearth, Victoria L. Vagnini, Raymond

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by:[university of Virginia] On: 26 November 2007 Access Details: [subscription number 785020474] Publisher: Informa Healthcare Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered

More information

Wild Minds What Animals Really Think : A Museum Exhibit at the New York Hall of Science, December 2011

Wild Minds What Animals Really Think : A Museum Exhibit at the New York Hall of Science, December 2011 This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 09 June 2015, At: 10:40 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Advanced Projects R&D, New Zealand b Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Online publication date: 30 March 2011

Advanced Projects R&D, New Zealand b Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Online publication date: 30 March 2011 This article was downloaded by: [University of Canterbury Library] On: 4 April 2011 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 917001820] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in

More information

THE VALIDITY OF THE LETTER MEMORY TEST AS A MEASURE OF MEMORY MALINGERING: ROBUSTNESS TO COACHING. A dissertation presented to.

THE VALIDITY OF THE LETTER MEMORY TEST AS A MEASURE OF MEMORY MALINGERING: ROBUSTNESS TO COACHING. A dissertation presented to. THE VALIDITY OF THE LETTER MEMORY TEST AS A MEASURE OF MEMORY MALINGERING: ROBUSTNESS TO COACHING A dissertation presented to the faculty of the College of Arts and Sciences of Ohio University In partial

More information

Differential diagnosis in the memory clinic: Exploring the value of improved neuropsychological examination Rienstra, A.

Differential diagnosis in the memory clinic: Exploring the value of improved neuropsychological examination Rienstra, A. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Differential diagnosis in the memory clinic: Exploring the value of improved neuropsychological examination Rienstra, A. Link to publication Citation for published

More information

Version of record first published: 25 Apr 2012.

Version of record first published: 25 Apr 2012. This article was downloaded by: [Dr William T. Tsushima] On: 23 October 2012, At: 13:21 Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use: This article was downloaded by: [Chiara, Andrea Di] On: 30 December 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 931692396] Publisher Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales

More information

Background 6/24/2014. Validity Testing in Pediatric Populations. Michael Kirkwood, PhD, ABPP/CN. Conflict of Interest Statement

Background 6/24/2014. Validity Testing in Pediatric Populations. Michael Kirkwood, PhD, ABPP/CN. Conflict of Interest Statement Validity Testing in Pediatric Populations Michael Kirkwood, PhD, ABPP/CN Background Board Certified Clinical Neuropsychologist at Children s Hospital Colorado Exclusively pediatric-focused Patient work

More information

Sandbagging Baseline Test Performance on ImPACT, Without Detection, Is More Difficult than It Appears

Sandbagging Baseline Test Performance on ImPACT, Without Detection, Is More Difficult than It Appears Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 236 244 Sandbagging Baseline Test Performance on ImPACT, Without Detection, Is More Difficult than It Appears Philip Schatz*, Colette Glatts Department of

More information

The vulnerability to coaching across measures of malingering

The vulnerability to coaching across measures of malingering Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School 2007 The vulnerability to coaching across measures of malingering Adrianne M. Brennan Louisiana State University

More information

Comparison of Performance of the Test of Memory Malingering and Word Memory Test in a Criminal Forensic Sample

Comparison of Performance of the Test of Memory Malingering and Word Memory Test in a Criminal Forensic Sample Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology Advance Access published May 5, 2015 Comparison of Performance of the Test of Memory Malingering and Word Memory Test in a Criminal Forensic Sample Rachel L. Fazio

More information

THE WORD READING TEST OF EFFORT IN ADULT LEARNING DISABILITY: A SIMULATION STUDY

THE WORD READING TEST OF EFFORT IN ADULT LEARNING DISABILITY: A SIMULATION STUDY The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20: 315 324, 2006 Copyright # Taylor and Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 1385-4046 print=1744-4144 online DOI: 10.1080/13854040590947434 THE WORD READING TEST OF EFFORT IN ADULT

More information

Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology Advance Access published April 27, John H. Denning*

Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology Advance Access published April 27, John H. Denning* Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology Advance Access published April 27, 2012 The Efficiency and Accuracy of The Test of Memory Malingering Trial 1, Errors on the First 10 Items of The Test of Memory Malingering,

More information

AN ESTABLISHMENT OF EMBEDDED SYMPTOM VALIDITY TESTING WITHIN THE DELIS-KAPLAN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING SYSTEM. A Dissertation by. Emanuel J.

AN ESTABLISHMENT OF EMBEDDED SYMPTOM VALIDITY TESTING WITHIN THE DELIS-KAPLAN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING SYSTEM. A Dissertation by. Emanuel J. AN ESTABLISHMENT OF EMBEDDED SYMPTOM VALIDITY TESTING WITHIN THE DELIS-KAPLAN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING SYSTEM A Dissertation by Emanuel J. VonDran Master of Arts, Wichita State University, 2011 Bachelor of

More information

Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan

Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan Grant L. Iverson, Ph.D, Professor Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation Harvard Medical School & Red Sox

More information

International Journal of Forensic Psychology Copyright Volume 1, No. 3 SEPTEMBER 2006 pp

International Journal of Forensic Psychology Copyright Volume 1, No. 3 SEPTEMBER 2006 pp International Journal of Forensic Psychology Copyright 2006 Volume 1, No. 3 SEPTEMBER 2006 pp. 29-37 The Word Memory Test and the One-in-Five-Test in an Analogue Study with Russian Speaking Participants

More information

E. Miriam Schmitt-Monreal

E. Miriam Schmitt-Monreal RUNNING HEAD: Feigned TBI TOMM, DCT and Coaching 1 Feigning Cognitive Symptoms after TBI: Validation of the TOMM and DCT and the Influence of Coaching E. Miriam Schmitt-Monreal Master Thesis - Master programme

More information

Healthy Children Get Low Scores Too: Prevalence of Low Scores on the NEPSY-II in Preschoolers, Children, and Adolescents

Healthy Children Get Low Scores Too: Prevalence of Low Scores on the NEPSY-II in Preschoolers, Children, and Adolescents Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 25 (2010) 182 190 Healthy Children Get Low Scores Too: Prevalence of Low Scores on the NEPSY-II in Preschoolers, Children, and Adolescents Brian L. Brooks 1, *, Elisabeth

More information

The Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM): normative data from cognitively intact, cognitively impaired, and elderly patients with dementia

The Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM): normative data from cognitively intact, cognitively impaired, and elderly patients with dementia Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) 455 464 The Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM): normative data from cognitively intact, cognitively impaired, and elderly patients with dementia Gordon Teichner,

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use: This article was downloaded by: [University of Cardiff] On: 3 March 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 906511392] Publisher Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales

More information

Influence of poor effort on self-reported symptoms and neurocognitive test performance following mild traumatic brain injury

Influence of poor effort on self-reported symptoms and neurocognitive test performance following mild traumatic brain injury Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology ISSN: 1380-3395 (Print) 1744-411X (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ncen20 Influence of poor effort on self-reported symptoms

More information

NEUROCOGNITIVE VARIABLES UNDERLYING GROUP PERFORMANCE ON A MEASURE OF EFFORT: THE MEDICAL SYMPTOM VALIDITY TEST (MSVT) Julie Hart Covert, M.S.

NEUROCOGNITIVE VARIABLES UNDERLYING GROUP PERFORMANCE ON A MEASURE OF EFFORT: THE MEDICAL SYMPTOM VALIDITY TEST (MSVT) Julie Hart Covert, M.S. NEUROCOGNITIVE VARIABLES UNDERLYING GROUP PERFORMANCE ON A MEASURE OF EFFORT: THE MEDICAL SYMPTOM VALIDITY TEST (MSVT) Julie Hart Covert, M.S. Dissertation Prepared for the Degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

More information

Criterion validity of the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II) after traumatic brain injury

Criterion validity of the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II) after traumatic brain injury Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 143 149 Criterion validity of the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II) after traumatic brain injury Monica L. Jacobs, Jacobus Donders

More information

Award Number: W81XWH

Award Number: W81XWH AD Award Number: W81XWH-08-2-0050 TITLE: PT073853: Mild TBI Following Exposure to Explosive Devices: Device Characteristics, Neuropsychological Functioning, and Symptoms of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder

More information

Assessments in Private Practice

Assessments in Private Practice Mansfield Counseling Presents Assessments in Private Practice Jeffrey S Gallup MA LPC NCC 1 Using Assessments Facts, Theory, Ethics and Practical Applications 2 Objectives Using assessments - Facts, Theory,

More information

Potential for interpretation disparities of Halstead Reitan neuropsychological battery performances in a litigating sample,

Potential for interpretation disparities of Halstead Reitan neuropsychological battery performances in a litigating sample, Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 21 (2006) 809 817 Potential for interpretation disparities of Halstead Reitan neuropsychological battery performances in a litigating sample, Abstract Christine L.

More information

Malingering detection in a Spanish population with a known-groups design

Malingering detection in a Spanish population with a known-groups design Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 23 (2008) 365 377 Malingering detection in a Spanish population with a known-groups design Raquel Vilar-López a,, Manuel Gómez-Río b, Sandra Santiago-Ramajo a, Antonio

More information

Validation of the Symptoms of Post- Concussion Syndrome Questionnaire as a Self-Report Symptom Validity Test: A Simulation Study. Victoria Jayne Reece

Validation of the Symptoms of Post- Concussion Syndrome Questionnaire as a Self-Report Symptom Validity Test: A Simulation Study. Victoria Jayne Reece Validation of the Symptoms of Post- Concussion Syndrome Questionnaire as a Self-Report Symptom Validity Test: A Simulation Study Victoria Jayne Reece Thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements

More information

Malingering Detection among Accommodation- Seeking University Students

Malingering Detection among Accommodation- Seeking University Students Brigham Young University BYU ScholarsArchive All Theses and Dissertations 2010-06-25 Malingering Detection among Accommodation- Seeking University Students Spencer Paul Clayton Brigham Young University

More information

Minimizing Misdiagnosis: Psychometric Criteria for Possible or Probable Memory Impairment

Minimizing Misdiagnosis: Psychometric Criteria for Possible or Probable Memory Impairment Original Research Article DOI: 10.1159/000215390 Accepted: January 30, 2009 Published online: April 28, 2009 Minimizing Misdiagnosis: Psychometric Criteria for Possible or Probable Memory Impairment Brian

More information

Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised*

Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised* The Clinical Neuropsychologist -//-$., Vol., No., pp. - Swets & Zeitlinger Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised* Rodney D. Vanderploeg, John A. Schinka, Tatyana Jones, Brent J. Small,

More information

APPLYING REACTION TIME (RT) AND EVENT-RELATED POTENTIAL (ERPS) MEASURES TO DETECT MALINGERED NEUROCOGNITIVE DEFICIT

APPLYING REACTION TIME (RT) AND EVENT-RELATED POTENTIAL (ERPS) MEASURES TO DETECT MALINGERED NEUROCOGNITIVE DEFICIT University of Kentucky UKnowledge University of Kentucky Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School 2007 APPLYING REACTION TIME (RT) AND EVENT-RELATED POTENTIAL (ERPS) MEASURES TO DETECT MALINGERED NEUROCOGNITIVE

More information

Noncredible Explanations of Noncredible Performance on Symptom Validity Tests

Noncredible Explanations of Noncredible Performance on Symptom Validity Tests Noncredible Explanations of Noncredible Performance on Symptom Validity Tests 5 Paul Green & Thomas Merten WHAT DO NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL AND EFFORT TESTS MEASURE? Neuropsychological Tests Neuropsychologists

More information

Key Knowledge Generation Publication details, including instructions for author and Subscription information:

Key Knowledge Generation Publication details, including instructions for author and Subscription information: This article was downloaded by: Publisher: KKG Publications Registered office: 18, Jalan Kenanga SD 9/7 Bandar Sri Damansara, 52200 Malaysia Key Knowledge Generation Publication details, including instructions

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by:[wayne State University] On: 27 February 2008 Access Details: [subscription number 788872989] Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered

More information

Commentary on Delis and Wetter, Cogniform disorder and cogniform condition: Proposed diagnoses for excessive cognitive symptoms

Commentary on Delis and Wetter, Cogniform disorder and cogniform condition: Proposed diagnoses for excessive cognitive symptoms Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 683 687 Abstract Commentary Commentary on Delis and Wetter, Cogniform disorder and cogniform condition: Proposed diagnoses for excessive cognitive symptoms

More information

Using Neuropsychological Experts. Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD

Using Neuropsychological Experts. Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD Using Neuropsychological Experts Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD Prepared for Advocate. Arizona Association for Justice/Arizona Trial Lawyers Association. September, 2011 Neurocognitive Associates 9813 North

More information

Malingering vs Credibility: A call of caution and robust clinical analysis

Malingering vs Credibility: A call of caution and robust clinical analysis Malingering vs Credibility: A call of caution and robust clinical analysis CAPDA January 2012 Dr. J. Douglas Salmon Jr. CEO, Rehabilitation Research, Education & Evaluation Services (RREES) Slick et al.

More information

Cutoff Scores for MMPI-2 and MMPI-2-RF Cognitive-Somatic Validity Scales for Psychometrically Defined Malingering Groups in a Military Sample

Cutoff Scores for MMPI-2 and MMPI-2-RF Cognitive-Somatic Validity Scales for Psychometrically Defined Malingering Groups in a Military Sample Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 31 (2016) 786 801 Cutoff Scores for MMPI-2 and MMPI-2-RF Cognitive-Somatic Validity Scales for Psychometrically Defined Malingering Groups in a Military Sample Abstract

More information

Improving Accuracy in the. through the use of Technology

Improving Accuracy in the. through the use of Technology Improving Accuracy in the Assessment of Malingering through the use of Technology Lisa Drago Piechowski, PhD, ABPP American School of Professional Psychology, Washington DC Key Points Assessment of malingering

More information

Diagnostic accuracy of the Structured Inventory of Malingered Symptomatology (SIMS) in detecting instructed malingering

Diagnostic accuracy of the Structured Inventory of Malingered Symptomatology (SIMS) in detecting instructed malingering Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 145 152 Diagnostic accuracy of the Structured Inventory of Malingered Symptomatology (SIMS) in detecting instructed malingering Harald Merckelbach a,, Glenn

More information

Simulated subaverage performance on the Block Span task of the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales- Fifth Edition

Simulated subaverage performance on the Block Span task of the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales- Fifth Edition Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Master's Theses Graduate School 2011 Simulated subaverage performance on the Block Span task of the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales- Fifth Edition

More information

Comparison of Predicted-difference, Simple-difference, and Premorbid-estimation methodologies for evaluating IQ and memory score discrepancies

Comparison of Predicted-difference, Simple-difference, and Premorbid-estimation methodologies for evaluating IQ and memory score discrepancies Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) 363 374 Comparison of Predicted-difference, Simple-difference, and Premorbid-estimation methodologies for evaluating IQ and memory score discrepancies Reid

More information

Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores

Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 20 (2005) 1043 1051 Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores Brian L.

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by:[university of Virginia] On: 26 November 2007 Access Details: [subscription number 785020474] Publisher: Informa Healthcare Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered

More information

Marie Stievenart a, Marta Casonato b, Ana Muntean c & Rens van de Schoot d e a Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Universite

Marie Stievenart a, Marta Casonato b, Ana Muntean c & Rens van de Schoot d e a Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Universite This article was downloaded by: [UCL Service Central des Bibliothèques], [Marie Stievenart] On: 19 June 2012, At: 06:10 Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered

More information

Determining causation of traumatic versus preexisting. conditions. David Fisher, Ph.D., ABPP, LP Chairman of the Board PsyBar, LLC

Determining causation of traumatic versus preexisting. conditions. David Fisher, Ph.D., ABPP, LP Chairman of the Board PsyBar, LLC Determining causation of traumatic versus preexisting psychological conditions David Fisher, Ph.D., ABPP, LP Chairman of the Board PsyBar, LLC 952 285 9000 Part 1: First steps to determine causation Information

More information

Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA b University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA

Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA b University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA This article was downloaded by: [Hicks, Joshua A.][Texas A&M University] On: 11 August 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 915031380] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered

More information

Exaggerated MMPI-2 symptom report in personal injury litigants with malingered neurocognitive deficit

Exaggerated MMPI-2 symptom report in personal injury litigants with malingered neurocognitive deficit Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 673 686 Exaggerated MMPI-2 symptom report in personal injury litigants with malingered neurocognitive deficit Glenn J. Larrabee Suite 202, 630 South Orange

More information

Life Events, Social Support, and Depression Among Taiwanese Female Homemakers

Life Events, Social Support, and Depression Among Taiwanese Female Homemakers This article was downloaded by: [National Taiwan University] On: 17 November 2014, At: 18:56 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:

More information

Experimental study examining observational and objective methods of assessing effort in an undergraduate sample

Experimental study examining observational and objective methods of assessing effort in an undergraduate sample University of Northern Colorado Scholarship & Creative Works @ Digital UNC Dissertations Student Research 12-1-2013 Experimental study examining observational and objective methods of assessing effort

More information

Detecting Malingering in Correctional Settings: A Comparison of Several Psychological Tests

Detecting Malingering in Correctional Settings: A Comparison of Several Psychological Tests Pacific University CommonKnowledge School of Graduate Psychology College of Health Professions 12-11-2009 Detecting Malingering in Correctional Settings: A Comparison of Several Psychological Tests David

More information

Published online: 14 Dec 2007.

Published online: 14 Dec 2007. This article was downloaded by: [University of Cambridge] On: 09 October 2014, At: 08:25 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

EHPS 2012 abstracts. To cite this article: (2012): EHPS 2012 abstracts, Psychology & Health, 27:sup1, 1-357

EHPS 2012 abstracts. To cite this article: (2012): EHPS 2012 abstracts, Psychology & Health, 27:sup1, 1-357 This article was downloaded by: [158.197.72.142] On: 30 August 2012, At: 04:44 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

Neuropsychological Performance in Cannabis Users and Non-Users Following Motivation Manipulation

Neuropsychological Performance in Cannabis Users and Non-Users Following Motivation Manipulation University at Albany, State University of New York Scholars Archive Psychology Honors College 5-2010 Neuropsychological Performance in Cannabis Users and Non-Users Following Motivation Manipulation Michelle

More information

The merits of mental age as an additional measure of intellectual ability in the low ability range. Simon Whitaker

The merits of mental age as an additional measure of intellectual ability in the low ability range. Simon Whitaker The merits of mental age as an additional measure of intellectual ability in the low ability range By Simon Whitaker It is argued that mental age may have some merit as a measure of intellectual ability,

More information

Discriminant Function Analysis of Malingerers and Neurological Headache Patients Self- Reports of Neuropsychological Symptoms

Discriminant Function Analysis of Malingerers and Neurological Headache Patients Self- Reports of Neuropsychological Symptoms Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 13, No. 6, pp. 561 567, 1998 Copyright 1998 National Academy of Neuropsychology Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0887-6177/98 $19.00.00 PII S0887-6177(97)00072-3

More information

A Dissertation. Submitted to the Faculty. Xavier University. in Partial Fulfillment of the. Requirements for the Degree of. Doctor of Psychology

A Dissertation. Submitted to the Faculty. Xavier University. in Partial Fulfillment of the. Requirements for the Degree of. Doctor of Psychology A Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of Xavier University in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Psychology by Julianne Gast, M.A. January, 2009 Approved: Christine M.

More information

THE EFFICIACY OF THE MMPI-2 LEES- HALEY FAKE BAD SCALE (FBS) FOR DIFFERENTIATING NEUROCOGNITIVE AND PSYCHIATRIC FEIGNERS

THE EFFICIACY OF THE MMPI-2 LEES- HALEY FAKE BAD SCALE (FBS) FOR DIFFERENTIATING NEUROCOGNITIVE AND PSYCHIATRIC FEIGNERS University of Kentucky UKnowledge University of Kentucky Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School 2003 THE EFFICIACY OF THE MMPI-2 LEES- HALEY FAKE BAD SCALE (FBS) FOR DIFFERENTIATING NEUROCOGNITIVE AND

More information

Kyle Richard Stephenson a & Cindy M. Meston a a The University of Texas at Austin, Psychology, Austin, Texas, USA

Kyle Richard Stephenson a & Cindy M. Meston a a The University of Texas at Austin, Psychology, Austin, Texas, USA This article was downloaded by: [University of Texas at Austin] On: 16 January 2013, At: 11:58 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:

More information

Two-Way Independent ANOVA

Two-Way Independent ANOVA Two-Way Independent ANOVA Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) a common and robust statistical test that you can use to compare the mean scores collected from different conditions or groups in an experiment. There

More information

VALIDATION OF THE MILLER FORENSIC ASSESSMENT OF SYMPTOMS TEST (M- FAST) IN A CIVIL FORENSIC POPULATION

VALIDATION OF THE MILLER FORENSIC ASSESSMENT OF SYMPTOMS TEST (M- FAST) IN A CIVIL FORENSIC POPULATION University of Kentucky UKnowledge University of Kentucky Master's Theses Graduate School 2006 VALIDATION OF THE MILLER FORENSIC ASSESSMENT OF SYMPTOMS TEST (M- FAST) IN A CIVIL FORENSIC POPULATION Jessica

More information

KEVIN J. BIANCHINI, PH.D., ABPN

KEVIN J. BIANCHINI, PH.D., ABPN KEVIN J. BIANCHINI, PH.D., ABPN Slick et al., 1999 Bianchini et al., 2005 4 4 Criterion A: Evidence of significant external incentive Criterion B: Evidence from physical evaluation 1. Probable effort

More information

SYMPTOM VALIDITY TESTING OF FEIGNED DISSOCIATIVE AMNESIA: A SIMULATION STUDY

SYMPTOM VALIDITY TESTING OF FEIGNED DISSOCIATIVE AMNESIA: A SIMULATION STUDY Psychology, Crime & Law, 2002, Vol. 8, pp. 311 318 SYMPTOM VALIDITY TESTING OF FEIGNED DISSOCIATIVE AMNESIA: A SIMULATION STUDY HARALD MERCKELBACH*, BEATRIJS HAUER and ERIC RASSIN Department of Experimental

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [Brown University] On: 3 March 2009 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 784168974] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales

More information

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Zihui Lu (zihui.lu@utoronto.ca) Meredyth Daneman (daneman@psych.utoronto.ca) Eyal M. Reingold (reingold@psych.utoronto.ca)

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition

Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 917 924 Abstract Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition Alison J. Donnell a, Neil Pliskin a, James Holdnack

More information

Donald A. Davidoff, Ph.D., ABPDC Chief, Neuropsychology Department, McLean Hospital Assistant Professor of Psychology, Harvard Medical School

Donald A. Davidoff, Ph.D., ABPDC Chief, Neuropsychology Department, McLean Hospital Assistant Professor of Psychology, Harvard Medical School Donald A. Davidoff, Ph.D., ABPDC Chief, Neuropsychology Department, McLean Hospital Assistant Professor of Psychology, Harvard Medical School Interests: Adult/Geriatric/Forensic Neuropsychology ddavidoff@mclean.harvard.edu

More information