Several studies with significant C1 attention effects survive critical analysis

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Several studies with significant C1 attention effects survive critical analysis"

Transcription

1 COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE, 2018 VOL. 9, NOS. 1 2, AUTHOR RESPONSE Several studies with significant C1 attention effects survive critical analysis Scott D. Slotnick Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut Hill, MA, USA ABSTRACT In a discussion paper (Slotnick, this issue), I conducted a selective review of spatial attention studies to compare experimental parameters and determine whether particular stimulus, task, or analysis conditions were more likely to produce significant attentional modulation of the eventrelated potential (ERP) C1 component. It was concluded that to maximize C1 attention effects, stimuli should be in the upper visual field, there should be distractors, conditions should be high perceptual or attentional load, there should be exogenous cuing, and effects should be measured at midline parietal-occipital electrodes POz, Pz, and CPz. Commentaries were received by Fu (this issue), Qu and Ding (this issue), Zani and Proverbio (this issue), Pitts and Hillyard (this issue), Di Russo (this issue), and Mohr and Kelly (this issue). Comments included additional ideas to amplify C1 attention effects, support for some conclusions, and challenges to some conclusions. The challenges led to a more in depth analysis of many issues pertaining to C1 attention effects including optimal electrode and stimulus locations, null V1 source localization attention effects, whether all significant C1 attention effects can be discounted, and the number of studies with null versus significant C1 attention effects. Analysis of the studies that survived critical analysis, which included several that observed significant C1 attention effects, led to the same conclusions as Slotnick (this issue). Lines of future research include replicating studies that have observed C1 attention effects using identical experimental parameters and systematically manipulating parameters to determine the impact of each parameter on C1 spatial attention effects. ARTICLE HISTORY Recieved 15 October 2017 Revised 19 October 2017 Published online 8 November 2017 KEYWORDS Attention; spatial attention; C1; P1; V1; ERP In Slotnick (this issue), I conducted a selective review of spatial attention studies to compare experimental parameters and determine whether particular stimulus, task, or analysis conditions were more likely to produce significant attentional modulation of the event-related potential (ERP) C1 component. It was concluded that to maximize C1 attention effects, stimuli should be in the upper visual field, there should be distractors, conditions should be high perceptual or attentional load, there should be exogenous cuing, and effects should be measured at midline parietal-occipital electrodes POz, Pz, and CPz. Six commentaries on that paper were received by Fu (this issue), Qu and Ding (this issue), Zani and Proverbio (this issue), Pitts and Hillyard (this issue), Di Russo (this issue), and Mohr and Kelly (this issue). All of the commentaries were excellent and included new ideas, support for some of my conclusions, and criticism of some of my conclusions. All of the comments are considered below, which led to a more critical analysis of many studies in the selective review and the addition of a couple other studies. Although the results of the slightly revised review led to the same conclusions as Slotnick (this issue), the commentaries and my responses to them have clarified many issues on the topic of C1 attentional effects and provided some future research directions. Additional ideas to amplify C1 attention effects Three of the commentaries provided suggestions for additional experimental parameters or analysis techniques not considered in Slotnick (this issue) that may amplify C1 attention effects. Fu proposed that stimulus parameters should be adjusted such that the C1 amplitude is not too low or too high such that attention effects can be observed (i.e., floor and ceiling effects should be avoided). Fu also proposed that certain participants could be excluded (e.g., those with a small magnitude or no C1, i.e., low signal, or strong alpha, i.e., high noise) or included (e.g., athletes) to maximize C1 attention effects. With regard to studies that employ exogenous cuing, Fu highlighted that the optimal stimulus onset CONTACT Scott D. Slotnick sd.slotnick@bc.edu Department of Psychology, Boston College, McGuinn Hall, Chestnut Hill, MA 02467, USA 2017 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group

2 76 S. D. SLOTNICK asynchrony (SOA) to maximize C1 attention effects has yet to be identified. Also with regard to exogenous cuing studies, Qu and Ding suggested subtracting ERP effects associated with upper visual field stimulation and lower visual field stimulation to cancel out cue related activity that might contaminate valid trial activity (exogenous cue effects are discussed in more detail below). This method assumes that cue-related activity is identical for upper visual field stimuli and lower visual field stimuli and thus could be subtracted out; however, this assumption is questionable as the magnitude and topography of ERP components are known to vary for upper versus lower visual field stimuli (see Figure 1 in Slotnick, this issue). Zani and Proverbio correctly pointed out that the conclusion that exogenous cueing should be employed to maximize C1 attention effects was based on only spatial attention studies. They proposed that endogenous cueing may maximize C1 attention effects for non-spatial (e.g., object-based) attention studies. Providing some counterevidence for this proposal, one study that employed exogenous cuing during non-spatial/surface-based attention observed significant C1 attention effects (Khoe, Mitchell, Reynolds, & Hillyard, 2005). All of the suggestions above are topics of future research. Optimal electrode locations to measure C1 attention effects In Slotnick (this issue), studies were only included in the primary analysis that measured C1 attention effects at or near midline parietal-occipital electrodes in an effort to minimize the influence of early P1 attention effects, which are maximal at lateral occipital electrodes. Bases on the electrode locations that showed significant C1 attention effects in these studies, it was concluded that such effects should be measured at midline parietal-occipital electrodes POz, Pz, and CPz. In their commentaries, Pitts and Hillyard along with Di Russo correctly pointed out that upper visual field stimuli typically produce a C1 maximum that is ipsilateral to the stimulus and lower visual field stimuli typically produce a C1 maximum that is contralateral to the stimulus. Of importance, the C1 maximum is only slightly lateral to the midline (see Figure 1 in Slotnick, this issue). As can be seen in Table 1 of Slotnick (this issue), these slightly lateral electrodes (i.e., PO1, PO2, P1, P2, CP1, CP2) were considered in the selective review. For upper visual field stimuli, which produce an ipsilateral C1 maximum, slightly off-midline electrodes may be more optimal to measure C1 attention effects than midline electrodes. For instance, for left upper visual field stimuli, ipsilateral electrodes PO1, P1, and CP1, may be more sensitive than electrodes POz, Pz, and CPz. For lower visual field stimuli, caution must be taken, as measuring C1 attention effects at contralateral electrode locations will increase the likelihood of picking up contralateral early P1 attention effects. In Slotnick (this issue), studies were not included in the primary analysis that measured C1 attention effects at lateral occipital electodes in an effort to avoid early P1 attention effects. Of particular relevance, the studies of Kelly, Gomez-Ramirez, and Foxe (2008) and Baumgartner, Graulty, Hillyard, and Pitts (this issue) were excluded from the primary analysis. In these studies, upper visual field C1 attention effects were measured at ipsilateral occipital electrodes, which I argued may have reflected the ipsilateral voltage sink corresponding to the contralateral early P1 voltage source, and lower visual field C1 attention effects were measured at contralateral occipital electrodes, which I argued may have reflected the contralateral early P1 to some degree. There were a number of arguments in the commentaries against the claim that upper visual field C1 effects measured at ipsilateral occipital electrodes may have reflected the early P1 voltage sink. Critically, the issue is not whether the upper visual field ipsilateral C1 reflects only the early contralateral P1 sink, but rather whether the C1 reflects the P1 sink to some degree, such that significant V1 attention effects may actually reflect extrastriate attention effects. Said another way, the burden of proof is on those who propose to measure C1 attention effects at lateral occipital electrodes to convincingly show that these effects do not reflect any P1 attention effects. Pitts and Hillyard stated that dipole modeling has shown that the upper visual field C1 source is separate from the diffuse far-field sink of the P1 source (citing Clark, Fan, & Hillyard, 1994, and Di Russo, Martínez, & Hillyard, 2003). The dipole modeling results from these studies (see Figure 9 in Clark et al., 1994, and Figures 7 and 8 in Di Russo et al., 2003) show the C1 dipole is in medial cortex (corresponding to V1) and the P1 dipole is in contralateral occipital

3 COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE 77 cortex (corresponding to extrastriate cortex). However, that the C1 and P1 dipoles are spatially separate only shows that the C1 and P1 voltage topographies are not identical, and does not rule out that the C1 is influenced by the P1 to some degree. As the ipsilateral P1 sink is generated by a deeper source (further from the scalp), it would be expected to produce a more diffuse topography than the ipsilateral C1, as proposed. However, the separate C1 and P1 dipole voltage topographies were not shown, which would be necessary to assess the degree to which the ipsilateral C1 and ipsilateral P1 sink overlap. Mohr and Kelly argued that the ipsilateral C1 and P1 sink were separable because Baumgartner et al. found significant P1 effects and non-significant C1 effects. Although this shows the ipsilateral C1 and P1 sink are not identical, it is still possible that the ipsilateral C1 reflects the P1 sink to some degree. Mohr and Kelly also argued that the ipsilateral C1 did not reflect the early contralateral P1 sink because the ipsilateral C1 was diminished when the early contralateral P1 peaked in magnitude (citing Di Russo, Martínez, Sereno, Pitzalis, & Hillyard, 2002; anddi Russo et al., 2005). This is a correct description (see Figures 5 and 6 in Di Russo et al., 2002, andfigure3 in, 2005). However, when the early contralateral P1 peaks in these studies, there is still a robust ipsilateral negativity that appears to reflect the corresponding P1 voltage sink and largely overlaps with the ipsilateral C1. The contralateral P1 onset can be as early as 72 milliseconds after stimulus onset and both Kelly et al. and Baumgartner et al. measured C1 attention effects from milliseconds after stimulus onset, thus it seems possible that the C1 attention effects in these studies may have reflected the early contralateral P1 sink to some degree. In a final argument, Mohr and Kelly highlighted that the C1 attention effects in their study began at 57 milliseconds after stimulus onset, well before the onset of the early contralateral P1. This argument is compelling, as there is no way for a P1 sink to influence the ipsilateral C1 before P1 onset. Therefore, based on this argument, Kelly et al. s rapid C1 attention effects (before 72 milliseconds after stimulus onset) reflect evidence of significant V1 attentional modulationandareconsideredinthereanalysisbelow. Pitts and Hillyard stated that it would be preferable to measure C1 where its amplitude is maximal on an individual participant basis. Although this would seem to be ideal, requiring individual participant mapping would reduce the possibility of replicating the previous studies that observed significant C1 attention effects at or just lateral to electrodes POz, Pz, and CPz (see Table 1 in Slotnick, this issue). Moreover, based on the findings of Kelly et al. and Baumgartner et al., individual participant C1 mapping yields selection of lateral occipital electrodes that may overlap with the early contralateral P1 source or ipsilateral P1 sink to some degree. One solution might be to conduct individual participant C1 mapping and analyze C1 attention effects from milliseconds after stimulus onset (i.e., before P1 onset). However, given that this early time range is before the C1 typically peaks, the combination of individual participant C1 mapping and assessment of C1 attention effects in an early time window (before P1 onset) would be expected to have limited sensitivity. Therefore, measuring C1 attention effects at or just lateral to POz, Pz, and CPz (approximately when the C1 peaks) is still recommended, as this has been shown to be sensitive to C1 attention effects and avoids contamination by P1 attention effects. Optimal stimulus locations to produce C1 attention effects Based on the studies in the primary analysis of Slotnick (this issue), it was concluded that to maximize C1 attention effects stimuli should be in the upper visual field. This conclusion was qualified in Slotnick (this issue) by acknowledging that many studies that observed significant C1 attention effects tested only upper visual field locations and there was some evidence for marginally significant C1 attention effects for lower visual field locations. Of particular relevance, Rauss, Pourtois, Vuilleumier, and Schwartz (2009) employed stimuli in the upper visual field or lower visual field and observed significant C1 attention effects for upper visual field stimuli (F(1,13) = 6.17, p =.027) and null C1 attention effects for lower visual field stimuli (F < 1). Although these are the results of only one study, they provide some evidence that C1 attention effects may be stronger in the upper visual field than the lower visual field. Related to the previous section, another reason to employ upper visual field stimuli is that the C1 topography will have negative polarity that is maximum over the ipsilateral

4 78 S. D. SLOTNICK hemisphere, making it more spatially separable from the early P1 topography that has positive polarity over the contralateral hemisphere (see Figure 1 in Slotnick, this issue). By contrast, the lower visual field C1 topography will have the same polarity and overlap with the early contralateral P1 topography; thus, it is more difficult to disentangle lower visual field contralateral C1 attention effects from early contralateral P1 attention effects. Mohr and Kelly pointed out that C1 topography and polarity vary as a function of stimulus position and questioned the assumption that upper visual field stimuli produce a C1 that is negative in magnitude and lower visual field stimuli produce a C1 that is positive in magnitude. Figure 1 illustrates the group average variation in C1 topography and polarity (adapted from Figure 6 in Clark et al., 1994). Although there can be individual participant variability in C1 topography and polarity (see Figure 7 in Clark et al., 1994), the group average visual field polarity map illustrates the most commonly observed pattern of activity. Mohr and Kelly are correct that it is theoretically possible for the upper visual field-negative polarity assumption to be Figure 1. Voltage topography as a function of stimulus position from milliseconds after stimulus onset. C1 and P1 are labeled in the right upper visual field and the left upper visual field. Stimuli were presented 8 in visual angle from fixation. Voltage topographies as a function of polar angle from the horizontal meridian are labeled in the right visual field and the analogous voltage topographies are shown in the left visual field (voltage magnitude key at the bottom left). Reprinted from Human Brain Mapping, Volume 2, Vincent P. Clark, Silu Fan, and Steven A. Hillyard, Identification of early visual evoked potential generators by retinotopic and topographic analysis, Pages , Copyright (1994), with permission from John Wiley and Sons.

5 COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE 79 violated near the upper vertical meridian. However, this assumption was not violated in the studies included in the primary analysis of Slotnick (this issue) as all of them employed stimuli close to 45 of polar angle from the horizontal meridian. That is, based on the general pattern of activity across participants (Figure 1), the studies included in the primary analysis of Slotnick (this issue) employed stimulus locations that were well within the safe zone, far from where polarity reversals occur. In support of this point, if the topography and polarity reversals were not consistent across participants in the studies included in the primary analysis of Slotnick (this issue), significant C1 attention effects would not have been observed (since several studies reported significant C1 attention effects, this is not of concern). It is notable that the stimuli employed by Rauss et al. (2009) and Rauss, Pourtois, Vuilleumier, and Schwartz (2012) extended from near the horizontal meridian to near the vertical meridian within both upper visual field quadrants. Based on the visual field C1 polarity map (Figure 1), this means that the stimuli in those studies would primarily produce a C1 of negative polarity but a portion of the stimuli near the upper vertical meridian would produce a C1 with positive polarity, thus muting the upper visual field C1 response to some degree. Of importance, in accordance with the upper visual field-negative polarity assumption, a robust upper visual field C1 response with negative polarity was observed and significant C1 attention effects were reported in Rauss et al. (2009, 2012), so this is not of concern. Pitts and Hillyard took issue with the claim in Slotnick (this issue) that the horizontal meridian stimuli in Martínez et al. (1999, 2001) would produce a muted C1 response that would limit sensitivity to detect attention effects. They correctly point out that the C1 reverses in polarity approximately 20 below the horizontal meridian. Note that when the term muted was used in Slotnick (this issue) to describe the C1 response in studies that presented stimuli at the horizontal meridian, this meant largely diminished (like a trombone mute rather than a TV mute). Figure 1 shows that stimuli presented at the horizontal meridian are largely diminished when compared to stimuli presented at or just below 45 of polar angle from the horizontal meridian. As such, studies that employed stimuli along the horizontal meridian were not considered in the primary analysis of Slotnick (this issue), as they would be expected to produce a largely diminished C1 response such that it would be difficult to observe significant C1 attention effects. Regarding optimal stimulus location, based on the above considerations and the visual field C1 polarity map (Figure 1), it is suggested that stimuli in the upper visual field should be presented at or just below the line 45 of polar angle from the horizontal meridian, stimuli in the lower visual field should be presented just below the line 45 of polar angle from the horizontal meridian, and stimuli should not be so large that they extend to visual field regions where the C1 is diminished (i.e., near the upper vertical meridian or near/just below the horizontal meridian). If resources were unlimited, it would be ideal, as suggested by Mohr and Kelly along with Pitts and Hillyard, to map out optimal stimulus locations for each participant, but this does not seem necessary as presenting stimuli at approximately 45 from the horizontal meridian produces a robust C1 response that is sensitive to attention effects. Be wary of null V1 cortical source localization attention effects Qu and Ding questioned whether the C1 attention effects of Rauss et al. (2009) and Rauss et al. (2012) reflected modulation of V1 given that their cortical sources were primarily localized to the prefrontal and frontal/temporal/parietal regions, respectively. To weight these null V1 source localization attention effects, cortical source localization methods must be considered in some detail. Cortical source localization involves converting a scalp voltage topography into a limited number of cortical sources. For example, within the millisecond time window, the voltage magnitudes at 128 electrodes might be converted into one V1 dipole current source that is fixed in location and orientation with a magnitude that varies over time. Source localization typically requires modeling the head (e.g., the brain, skull, cerebrospinal fluid, and scalp; such head models are universally poor), modeling cortical activity, and adjusting the cortical dipole(s) using an iterative model fitting procedure to minimize the difference between the observed scalp voltage topography and the

6 80 S. D. SLOTNICK model scalp voltage topography (for a review, see Slotnick, 2004). Of particular relevance, cortical source localization is a massive data reduction, where activity from a large number of electrodes is typically converted into one or a few cortical sources. Said another way, each cortical source represents the activity from a large number of electrodes. Given that significant C1 attentional modulation typically occurs at one or a few electrodes (see Table 1 in Slotnick, this issue) and cortical source localization depends on activity from a large number of electrodes, cortical source localization techniques would be expected to produce null attention effects in V1. In short, a significant attention effect at one or a few electrodes will be washed out by the non-significant attention effects at the numerous other electrodes distilled into the cortical source (unless the attention effect is very robust). As such, the null C1/V1 source localization attention effects in Rauss et al. (2009) are not informative, while the marginally significant C1/V1 source localization attention effects in Rauss et al. (2012; see their Figure 4B, region 4) do providesomeevidenceforattentionalmodulation within this region. That is, there appears to be some degree of V1 attentional modulation in Rauss et al. (2012) despite the largely null electophyisiological activity included in the source localization analysis. The expectation of null V1 source localization attention effects has potential implications for Hillyard and colleagues findings that dipole source localization C1/V1 attention effects are initially non-significant (< 90 milliseconds after stimulus onset) and then become significant later in time, presumably due to feedback from extrastriate cortex (Di Russo et al., 2003; Martínez et al., 1999, 2001; Noesselt et al., 2002). The initial null V1 attention effects may be due to limited sensitivity of cortical source localization coupled with the small magnitude of C1 attentional modulation. A magnetoencephalography (MEG) spatial attention study by Poghosyan and Ioannides (2008) provided evidence that the dipole source localization techniques employed by Hillyard and colleagues had limited sensitivity to detect attentional modulation in the initial wave of V1 activity. Before employing the dipole modeling procedure of Hillyard and colleagues, Poghosyan and Ioannides observed significant V1 spatial attention effects beginning 55 to 60 milliseconds after stimulus onset and peaking at approximately 70 milliseconds after stimulus onset. After employing thesamedipolemodelingproceduresashillyard and colleages, Poghosyan and Ioannides observed null V1 attention effects 55 to 70 milliseconds after stimulus onset and attentional enhancement later in time. One of my spatial attention ERP source localization studies (Slotnick, Hopfinger, Klein, & Sutter, 2002) provided evidence that the initial V1 attention effects are much smaller in magnitude than subsequent attention effects in this region. This study employed a multi-stimulus array where each of 60 checkerboards were independently modulated (i.e., reversed in luminance) tens of thousands of times according to a binary m-sequence, which produced a very high signal-to-noise ratio. A previous study using the same stimulus and source localization procedures showed that the dipole locations varied systematically as stimulus position moved from the top to the bottom of each hemifield (Slotnick, Klein, Carney, Sutter, & Dastmalchi, 1999). This is diagnostic of a V1 cortical source, where the cortical surface is continuous from the upper vertical meridian to the lower vertical meridian of each hemifield (smoothly traversing the horizontal meridian representation near the base of the calcarine sulcus), and inconsistent with an extrastriate (e.g., V2, V3) cortical source, where the cortical surface is disjoint near the horizontal meridian (see Figure 2 in Slotnick, Thompson, & Kosslyn, 2005, which illustrates detailed functional magnetic resonance imaging, fmri, retinotopic maps of early visual regions). Moreover, another study that used the same stimulus and source localization procedures as Slotnick et al. (2002) showed that the cortical magnification factor computed using dipole locations were similar to V1 cortical magnification estimates from psychophysics, cortical stimulation, and fmri (Slotnick, Klein, Carney, & Sutter, 2001). Slotnick et al. (2002) observed significant attentional modulation of the V1 dipole magnitude corresponding to the stimulus at the attended location that did not change over time from the early epoch (50 80 milliseconds after stimulus onset) to the late epoch ( milliseconds after stimulus onset). By contrast, for V1 dipoles corresponding to stimuli in a broader region

7 COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE 81 extending from the attended location to fixation, there was no significant attentional modulation in the early epoch but significant attentional modulation in the late epoch, which is reminiscent of the findings of Hillyard and colleagues. The overall magnitude of the attention effects within the broader facilitatory region were over five times larger in magnitude than at the attended location. This larger magnitude of V1 attentional modulation surrounding the attended location can be attributed to feedback from extrastriate cortex, given that extrastriate regions have larger receptive fields than V1 and are active within the late epoch. These results suggest that attention produces a small change in V1 dipole magnitudes during the initial V1 response, which will typically be dominated by noise and produce null findings. C1/V1 dipole source localization attention effects are usually highly influenced by noise and therefore difficult to trust. It is important to identify the optimal conditions to maximize C1 attention effects, such that experiments (and dipole modeling) can have a higher signal-to-noise ratio and produce more convincing results. cuing paradigms, a significant C1 attention effect can be considered reliable (i.e., not due to differential cue effects) if the baseline level of activity is close to zero and there is no differential activity in valid versus invalid trials before C1 onset. Figure 2 shows that in both Fu et al. (2009) and Dassanayake, Michie, and Fulham (2016), the baseline magnitudes of activity were close to zero and there was no differential valid versus invalid activity before C1 onset. Thus, the C1 attention effects in these two studies appear to be real as there was no evidence of cue related Discounting significant C1 attention effects Qu and Ding along with Pitts and Hillyard highlighted that in attention studies with exogenous cuing, valid trials are preceded by a cue in the target location while invalid trials are not preceded by a cue in the target location. They correctly pointed out that C1 attention effects, corresponding to a higher magnitude of activity for valid than invalid trials, might reflect an increase in activity due to the preceding cue for valid trials. With regard to the studies considered in the primary analysis of Slotnick (this issue), Pitts and Hillyard noted that in Fu, Fedota, Greenwood, and Parasuraman (2010), valid trials had a higher magnitude of activity than invalid trials that was developed approximately 30 to 40 milliseconds after stimulus onset, which is before C1 onset. Such an early onset does suggest a cue-related effect occurred in this study that may have artifactually produced a significant C1 effect. As such, the study by Fu et al. (2010) did not present convincing evidence of a significant C1 attention effect and is not considered in the reanalysis below. Following the same logic as Pitts and Hillyard, for exogenous Figure 2. Significant C1 attention effects in two exogenous cuing studies. (a) Valid and invalid activation timecourses at electrode Pz are shown in red and green, respectively (key to the right), corresponding to stimulation in the upper visual field. C1, P1m, and N1 components are labeled. Each tick on the horizontal axis represents 50 milliseconds and each tick on the vertical axis represents 1 microvolt (negative is up and the axes cross at 0). Reprinted from NeuroImage, 48/1, Shimin Fu, Yuxia Huang, Yuejia Luo, Yan Wang, John Fedota, Pamela M. Greenwood, and Raja Parasuraman, Perceptual load interacts with involuntary attention at early processing stages: Eventrelated potential studies, , 2009, with permission from Elsevier. (b) Valid and invalid activation timecourses at electrode POz are shown as solid lines and dotted lines, respectively (key to the right). C1 and P1m components are labeled. Each tick on the horizontal axis represents 100 milliseconds and each tick on the vertical axis represents 1 microvolt (negative is up and the axes cross at 0). Activation timecourses corresponding to stimulation in the upper visual field (thicker lines) produced C1 components with negative polarity and were used to test attention effects. Activation timecourses corresponding to stimulation in the lower visual field (thinner lines) produced C1 components with positive polarity and were shown to illustrate polarity reversal. Reprinted from International Journal of Psychophysiology, 105, Tharaka L. Dassanayake, Patricia T. Michie, and Ross Fulham, Effect of temporal predictability on exogenous attentional modulation of feedforward processing in the striate cortex, 9 16, 2016, with permission from Elsevier.

8 82 S. D. SLOTNICK activity before C1 onset. Although a theoretical argument can be made that all exogenous cueing studies should be discounted because the effects might be due to differential valid versus invalid cueing, baseline/pre-c1 activity can simply be evaluated to show that this is not the case. Related to this, Qu and Ding mentioned that Rauss et al. (2012) had a baseline shift; however, this is not of concern as the magnitude of pre-c1 activity was less than one-fourth of the magnitude of the C1 attention effect. Pitts and Hillyard also pointed to Dassanayake et al. s interpretation that their C1 and P1 modulation could be described as a third negative component that spanned C1 and P1. Figure 2(b) provides some evidence against this interpretation as the magnitude of the P1 attention effect is four times larger than the magnitude of the C1 attention effect, while a protracted negativity that spanned both components would be expected to produce effects of approximately the same magnitude. Still, it is possible that a third negative component produced differential C1 and P1 effects, which should be investigated further. Along the same lines, Pitts and Hillyard argued that attended and unattended C1 effects should have the same voltage topography/ source, with attention effects corresponding to amplification of this source. I agree it would be ideal to show the voltage topography in the attended and unattended conditions to illustrate a common neural generator that is amplified by attention. However, this is often not done because it is reasonable to assume that an ERP component at a particular electrode where the attended activity and the unattended activity begin, increase, peak, and decrease with nearly identical timing can be attributed to the same cortical source. In addition, excluding studies that did not provide voltage topographies would remove many studies that have reported both significant C1 attention effects and null C1 attention effects. It is notable that Hopfinger and West (2006) did not show voltage topographies for attended and unattended stimuli, but Pitts and Hillyard considered their null findings as evidence against the hypothesis that exogenous cueing modulates C1. Mohr and Kelly aimed to discount the studies that manipulated attentional load (Ding, Martínez, Qu, & Hillyard, 2014; Rauss et al., 2009, 2012) by claiming that they manipulated load rather than spatial attention. This is not a valid argument as these studies manipulated attentional load at central fixation which, as noted in Slotnick (this issue), changes the allocation of spatial attention at other visual field locations. Thus, although spatial attention was not manipulated directly, it was manipulated indirectly, as indicated by the significant C1 spatial attention effects to peripheral distractors in Rauss et al. (2009, 2012). Null C1 attention effects versus significant C1 attention effects Slotnick (this issue) only considered papers in the primary analysis that employed parameters that were sensitive to C1 attention effects, including measuring these effects at or near midline parietal-occipital electrodes (to avoid P1 contamination) and stimulating within each quadrant (to avoid a muted C1 response). Mohr and Kelly claimed that Table 1 in Slotnick (this issue) left out a number of studies that have shown null C1 attention effects (citing ten studies). In fact, four of these studies (Fu, Greenwood, & Parasuraman, 2005; Fu et al., 2008; Martínez et al., 1999, 2001) were intentionally excluded in the Other spatial attention studies section of Slotnick (this issue) because they employed non-optimal methods. Of the remaining six studies referenced by Mohr and Kelly, four studies fit the exclusion criteria of Slotnick (this issue): one study because they presented stimuli along the horizontal meridian and did not measure C1 attention effects (Curran, Hills, Patterson, & Strauss, 2001), one study because all statistical tests of C1 attention effects included occipital electrodes (Fu, Fan, Chen, & Zhuo, 2001; the C1ʹ component also peaked after 100 milliseconds), one study because they only measured C1ʹ attention effects at lateral electodes (Johannes, Münte, Heinze, & Mangun, 1995), and one study because it was a review paper (Hillyard, Teder-Sälejärvi, & Münte, 1998; the only potentially new empirical paper that was referenced in this review, Gomez Gonzales, Clark, Fan, Luck, & Hillyard, 1994, presented stimuli along the horizontal meridian). The remaining two studies (Clark & Hillyard, 1996; Wijers, Lange, Mulder, & Mulder, 1997), which both employed endogenous cuing with no distractors, did report null C1 attention effects and are considered in the reanalysis below.

9 COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE 83 Di Russo took issue with my characterizing C1 attention effects with p values greater than 0.05 as marginally significant. In Slotnick (this issue), it was stated that the attention effect in the right lower visual field of Di Russo et al. (2003) would likely be significant if tested alone. Di Russo kindly conducted this statistical test in his commentary, which was a non-significant F-test (F(1,29) = 4.06, p =.0563). It is notable that this is equivalent to a significant onetailed t-test (t(29) = 2.015, p =.027); a one-tailed test is justified as it is known that spatial attention produces a greater magnitude of activity in V1 for attended than unattended stimuli (e.g., Slotnick, Schwarzbach, & Yantis, 2003; Tootell et al., 1998). Still, it is reasonable to take a more conservative statistical approach and characterize marginally significant findings as non-significant, as will be done in the reanalysis below. The key point is that such marginally significant results should not be taken to support the null hypothesis. As stated by Wagenmakers, Fisherian p values are not designed to quantify support in favor of the null hypothesis. A p value indicates the evidence against the null hypothesis. It is not possible to observe the data and corroborate the null hypothesis; one can only fail to reject it (Wagenmakers, 2007, p. 795). Regarding C1 attention effects, null findings can be observed for numerous reasons such as an insufficient number of participants, participant variability, non-optimal electrode (or reference) selection, nonoptimal stimulus location, other non-optimal experimental parameters (e.g., low load conditions or no distractors), or insensitive analysis procedures. Unless optimal experimental conditions and analysis techniques are employed, null findings provide little informational value and should never be taken to support the null hypothesis. If a scientist claims their results provide some support for null C1 attention effects, the burden of proof is on them to show that optimal experimental conditions and analysis techniques were employed. Based on the commentaries and my responses to them, Table 1 illustrates an update of the spatial attention studies that employed methods that were sensitive to C1 attention effects and survived critical analysis. The original conclusions of Slotnick (this issue) were that to maximize sensitivity to C1 attention effects, stimuli should be in the upper visual field, there should be distractors, there should be high perceptual or attentional load, there should be exogenous cuing, and effects should be measured at midline parietal-occipital electrodes POz, Pz, and CPz. Table 1 continues to support all of these suggestions. It is notable that three of the nine studies that employed endogenous cuing observed significant C1 attention effects (and only one of these studies directly manipulated spatial attention), while two of the three studies that employed exogenous curing observed significant C1 attention effects. Five of the eleven studies in Table 1 observed significant C1 attention effects. As eloquently stated by William James, Ifyouwishtoupsetthelaw that all crows are black, you mustn t seek to show that no crows are; it is enough if you prove one single crow to be white (James, 1986, p. 121). Here, we have a murder of eleven crows, and five of them are white. One can pretend that the white crows don t existbuttheydo,onecantrytokill the white crows but they survive, and one can add more black crows but there will always be white crows. It often seems as if we are looking through Table 1. Spatial attention study parameters and C1 significance. Study VF Distractors Load Cue Electrode(s) C1 significance Clark & Hillyard (1996) LUVF/RUVF No Low per/att End IPz Not sig Wijers et al. (1997) LUVF/RUVF No Low per/att End POz Not sig Di Russo et al. (2003) LUVF/RUVF/LLVF/RLVF No Low per/att End POz Not sig Hopfinger and West (2006) LUVF/RUVF No Low per/att End/Exo Pz/POz Not sig Kelly et al. (2008) LUVF/RUVF/LLVF/RLVF No Low per/att End Lateral occ Sig starting at 57 ms Fu et al. (2009) LUVF/RUVF Yes Low/Med/High per Exo POz/Pz/CPz High load sig Pz Rauss et al. (2009) UVF/LVF Yes Low/High att End CP1/CPz/CP2/P1/Pz/P2 UVF low vs. high load sig Di Russo et al. (2012) LUVF/RUVF/LLVF/RLVF No Low per/att End PO1/PO2/P1/P2 Not sig Rauss et al. (2012) UVF Yes Low/High att End CP1/CPz/CP2/P1/Pz/P2 High vs. low load sig Ding et al. (2014) UVF/LVF Yes Low/High att End POz Not sig Dassanayake et al. (2016) LUVF/RLVF Yes High per Exo Oz/POz/Pz UVF sig at POz L = left, R = right, UVF = upper visual field, LVF = lower visual field, Per = perceptual, Att = attentional, End = endogenous, Exo = exogenous, Sig = significant. Significant and non-significant effects are shown in gray and white, respectively. Only low/med/high load and low/high load studies manipulated load. Low perceptual/attentional load studies were classified as such because there were no distractors and the tasks were simple target detection.

10 84 S. D. SLOTNICK thick brush and there are disagreements about whether there is a white crow (C1) or a white pigeon (P1), which highlights the importance of replicating studies that have reported significant C1 attention effects. However, there are now several convincing studies that have shown spatial attention can modulate the C1 component. The C1 spatial attention effect is small in magnitude and requires optimal experimental parameters to uncover, but it exists. Future directions The selective review of Slotnick (this issue) and a slight revision of that review in the present paper based on the commentaries and my responses to them (Table 1) aimed to identify the experimental parameters that affect attentional modulation of the ERP C1 component. It must be underscored that the conclusions drawn were based on the limited number of studies that employed techniques that were sensitive to C1 attentional modulation. I agree with Mohr and Kelly that which parameters affect the C1 attention effect and why those parameters affect the C1 attention effect are largely open questions. The present conclusions should serve as a set of hypotheses that should be investigated in future experiments. Where do we go from here? As noted by Pitts and Hillyard, there have been almost no replications of previous significant C1 attention findings. Specifically, Ding et al. (2014) failed to replicate Rauss et al. (2009) and Baumgartner et al. (this issue) failed to replicate Kelly et al. (2008). Although the experimental parameters in Ding et al. and Baumgartner et al. were not identical to the original studies, they were very similar, which suggests C1 attention effects can be easily perturbed. Although Dassanayake et al. (2016) did replicate Fu et al. (2009), more replications are needed particularly those that employ the identical experimental parameters to further illustrate the conditions under which attention can modulation the C1 component. It is critically important to reproduce findings across laboratories, which is one line of future research. As noted by Mohr and Kelly, many of the studies that observed significant C1 attentional modulation employed the same experimental parameters, such as exogenous cuing with distractors. As suggested by Slotnick (this issue), another line of future research will be to systematically manipulate experimental parameters to determine the impact of each parameter on C1 spatial attention effects. Disclosure statement No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author. References Clark, V. P., Fan, S., & Hillyard, S. A. (1994). Identification of early visually evoked potential generators by retinotopic and topographic analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 2, Clark, V. P., & Hillyard, S. A. (1996). Spatial selective attention affects early extrastriate but not striate components of the visual evoked potential. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, Curran, T., Hills, A., Patterson, M. B., & Strauss, M. E. (2001). Effects of aging on visuospatial attention: An ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 39, Dassanayake, T. L., Michie, P. T., & Fulham, R. (2016). Effect of temporal predictability on exogenous attentional modulation of feedforward processing in the striate cortex. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 105, Di Russo, F., Martínez, A., & Hillyard, S. A. (2003). Source analysis of event-related cortical activity during visuo-spatial attention. Cerebral Cortex, 13, Di Russo, F., Martínez, A., Sereno, M. I., Pitzalis, S., & Hillyard, S. A. (2002). Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential. Human Brain Mapping, 15, Di Russo, F., Pitzalis, S., Spitoni, G., Aprile, T., Patria, F., Spinelli, D., & Hillyard, S. A. (2005). Identification of the neural sources of the pattern-reversal VEP. NeuroImage, 24, Di Russo, F., Stella, A., Spitoni, G., Strappini, F., Sdoia, S., Galati, G.,... Pitzalis, S. (2012). Spatiotemporal brain mapping of spatial attention effects on pattern-reversal ERPs. Human Brain Mapping, 33, Ding, Y., Martínez, A., Qu, Z., & Hillyard, S. A. (2014). Earliest stages of visual cortical processing are not modified by attentional load. Human Brain Mapping, 35, Fu, S., Fan, S., Chen, L., & Zhuo, Y. (2001). The attentional effects of peripheral cueing as revealed by two event-related potential studies. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112, Fu, S., Fedota, J., Greenwood, P. M., & Parasuraman, R. (2010). Early interaction between perceptual load and involuntary attention: An event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters, 468, Fu, S., Greenwood, P. M., & Parasuraman, R. (2005). Brain mechanisms of involuntary visuospatial attention: An event-related potential study. Human Brain Mapping, 25, Fu, S., Huang, Y., Luo, Y., Wang, Y., Fedota, J., Greenwood, P. M., & Parasuraman, R. (2009). Perceptual load interacts with

11 COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE 85 involuntary attention at early processing stages: Eventrelated potential studies. NeuroImage, 48, Fu, S., Zinni, M., Squire, P. N., Kumar, R., Caggiano, D. M., & Parasuraman, R. (2008). When and where perceptual load interacts with voluntary visuospatial attention: An eventrelated potential and dipole modeling study. NeuroImage, 39, Gomez Gonzales, C. M., Clark, V. P., Fan, S., Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1994). Sources of attention-sensitive visual event-related potentials. Brain Topography, 7, Hopfinger, J. B., & West, V. M. (2006). Interactions between endogenous and exogenous attention on cortical visual processing. NeuroImage, 31, Hillyard, S. A., Teder-Sälejärvi, W. A., & Münte, T. F. (1998). Temporal dynamics of early perceptual processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8, James, W. (1986). Essays in psychical research (the works of William James). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Johannes, S., Münte, T. F., Heinze, H. J., & Mangun, G. R. (1995). Luminance and spatial attention effects on early visual processing. Cognitive Brain Research, 2, Kelly, S. P., Gomez-Ramirez, M., & Foxe, J. J. (2008). Spatial attention modulates initial afferent activity in human primary visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 18, Khoe, W., Mitchell, J. F., Reynolds, J. H., & Hillyard, S. A. (2005). Exogenous attentional selection of transparent superimposed surfaces modulates early event-related potentials. Vision Research, 45, Martínez, A., Anllo-Vento, L., Sereno, M. I., Frank, L. R., Buxton, R. B., Dubowitz, D. J.,... Hillyard, S. A. (1999). Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention. Nature Neuroscience, 2, Martínez,A.,DiRusso,F.,Anllo-Vento,L.,Sereno,M.I.,Buxton,R.B., & Hillyard, S. A.(2001). Putting spatial attention on the map: Timing and localization of stimulus selection processes in striate and extrastriate visual areas. Vision Research, 41, Noesselt, T., Hillyard, S. A., Woldorff, M. G., Schoenfeld, A., Hagner, T., Jäncke, L.,... Heinze, H. J. (2002). Delayed striate cortical activation during spatial attention. Neuron, 35, Poghosyan, V., & Ioannides, A. A. (2008). Attention modulates earliest responses in the primary auditory and visual cortices. Neuron, 58, Rauss, K. S., Pourtois, G., Vuilleumier, P., & Schwartz, S. (2009). Attentional load modifies early activity in human primary visual cortex. Human Brain Mapping, 30, Rauss, K., Pourtois, G., Vuilleumier, P., & Schwartz, S. (2012). Effects of attentional load on early visual processing depend on stimulus timing. Human Brain Mapping, 33, Slotnick, S. D. (2004). Source localization of ERP generators. In T. C. Handy (Ed.), Event-related potentials: A methods handbook (pp ). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Slotnick, S. D., Hopfinger, J. B., Klein, S. A., & Sutter, E. E. (2002). Darkness beyond the light: Attentional inhibition surrounding the classic spotlight. NeuroReport, 13, Slotnick, S. D., Klein, S. A., Carney, T., & Sutter, E. E. (2001). Electrophysiological estimate of human cortical magnification. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112, Slotnick, S. D., Klein, S. A., Carney, T., Sutter, E., & Dastmalchi, S. (1999). Using multi-stimulus VEP source localization to obtain a retinotopic map of human primary visual cortex. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110, Slotnick, S. D., Schwarzbach, J., & Yantis, S. (2003). Attentional inhibition of visual processing in human striate and extrastriate cortex. NeuroImage, 4, Slotnick, S. D., Thompson, W. L., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2005). Visual mental imagery induces retinotopically organized activation of early visual areas. Cerebral Cortex, 15, Tootell, R. B., Hadjikhani, N., Hall, E. K., Marrett, S., Vanduffel, W., Vaughan, J. T., & Dale, A. M. (1998). The retinotopy of visual spatial attention. Neuron, 21, Wagenmakers, E. J. (2007). A practical solution to the pervasive problems of p values. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, Wijers, A. A., Lange, J. J., Mulder, G., & Mulder, L. J. (1997). An ERP study of visual spatial attention and letter target detection for isoluminant and nonisoluminant stimuli. Psychophysiology, 34,

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping Retinotopy & Phase Mapping Fani Deligianni B. A. Wandell, et al. Visual Field Maps in Human Cortex, Neuron, 56(2):366-383, 2007 Retinotopy Visual Cortex organised in visual field maps: Nearby neurons have

More information

Asymmetry between the upper and lower visual fields: An event-related potential study

Asymmetry between the upper and lower visual fields: An event-related potential study Chinese Science Bulletin 2006 Vol. 51 No. 5 536 541 DOI: 10.1007/s11434-006-0536-3 Asymmetry between the upper and lower visual fields: An event-related potential study QU Zhe 1,2, SONG Yan 3 & DING Yulong

More information

Early Spatial Attentional Modulation of Inputs to the Fovea

Early Spatial Attentional Modulation of Inputs to the Fovea The Journal of Neuroscience, March 31, 2010 30(13):4547 4551 4547 Brief Communications Early Spatial Attentional Modulation of Inputs to the Fovea Hans-Peter Frey, 1,2 Simon P. Kelly, 1,3 Edmund C. Lalor,

More information

Self-construal priming modulates visual activity underlying global/local perception

Self-construal priming modulates visual activity underlying global/local perception Biological Psychology 77 (2008) 93 97 Brief report Self-construal priming modulates visual activity underlying global/local perception Zhicheng Lin a,b, Yan Lin c, Shihui Han a,b, * a Department of Psychology,

More information

Texas at Dallas, Dallas, USA b Department of Psychology, Boston College, Boston, USA. Published online: 15 Jan 2013.

Texas at Dallas, Dallas, USA b Department of Psychology, Boston College, Boston, USA. Published online: 15 Jan 2013. This article was downloaded by: [The University of Texas at Dallas] On: 03 June 2013, At: 06:41 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:

More information

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events Psychophysiology, 40 (2003), 827 831. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2003 Society for Psychophysiological Research BRIEF REPT Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the

More information

ERP Studies of Selective Attention to Nonspatial Features

ERP Studies of Selective Attention to Nonspatial Features CHAPTER 82 ERP Studies of Selective Attention to Nonspatial Features Alice Mado Proverbio and Alberto Zani ABSTRACT This paper concentrates on electrophysiological data concerning selective attention to

More information

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials Chinese Science Bulletin 2007 Science in China Press Springer-Verlag Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials SONG Yan 1, PENG

More information

Evoked alpha and early access to the knowledge system: The P1 inhibition timing hypothesis

Evoked alpha and early access to the knowledge system: The P1 inhibition timing hypothesis available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Evoked alpha and early access to the knowledge system: The P1 inhibition timing hypothesis Wolfgang Klimesch University

More information

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search Clayton Hickey 1,2, Vincent Di Lollo 2, and John J. McDonald 2 Abstract & Attentional selection of a target presented among

More information

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Psychon Bull Rev (2013) 20:296 301 DOI 10.3758/s13423-012-0353-4 BRIEF REPORT Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Risa Sawaki & Steven J. Luck Published online: 20 December 2012 #

More information

The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture

The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture Psychophysiology, 53 (2016), 650 662. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright VC 2016 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12615 The role of top-down spatial attention

More information

Neural basis of auditory-induced shifts in visual time-order perception

Neural basis of auditory-induced shifts in visual time-order perception Neural basis of auditory-induced shifts in visual time-order perception John J McDonald 1, Wolfgang A Teder-Sälejärvi 2, Francesco Di Russo 3,4 & Steven A Hillyard 2 Attended objects are perceived to occur

More information

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: A Comparison of Electrophysiological and Other Neuroimaging Approaches Leun J. Otten Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology University

More information

Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return

Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return David J. Prime and Pierre Jolicoeur Abstract & Here we examined the relationship between inhibition of return (IOR) and response-selection

More information

Follow this and additional works at: Part of the Neurosciences Commons

Follow this and additional works at:   Part of the Neurosciences Commons University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Open Access Dissertations 9-2012 Gradients and Ranges of Visually Selective Attention Based on Location, Objects, Color, and Size: Gradients

More information

Attentional enhancement during multiple-object tracking

Attentional enhancement during multiple-object tracking Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2009, 16 (2), 411-417 doi:10.3758/pbr.16.2.411 Attentional enhancement during multiple-object tracking TRAFTON DREW AND ANDREW W. MCCOLLOUGH University of Oregon, Eugene,

More information

Perceptual Learning Increases the Strength of the Earliest Signals in Visual Cortex

Perceptual Learning Increases the Strength of the Earliest Signals in Visual Cortex 15080 The Journal of Neuroscience, November 10, 2010 30(45):15080 15084 Brief Communications Perceptual Learning Increases the Strength of the Earliest Signals in Visual Cortex Min Bao ( ),1 Lin Yang,

More information

Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex

Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (5), 834-839 Brief Reports Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex Jeffrey S. Johnson, Geoffrey F. Woodman, Elsie Braun, and Steven

More information

Attentional inhibition of visual processing in human striate and extrastriate cortex

Attentional inhibition of visual processing in human striate and extrastriate cortex NeuroImage 19 (2003) 1602 1611 www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Attentional inhibition of visual processing in human striate and extrastriate cortex Scott D. Slotnick, a, * Jens Schwarzbach, b and Steven

More information

Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention

Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention A. Martínez 1, L. Anllo-Vento 2, M. I. Sereno 3, L. R. Frank 4, R. B. Buxton 4, D. J. Dubowitz 5, E. C. Wong 4,6, H. Hinrichs

More information

Putting spatial attention on the map: timing and localization of stimulus selection processes in striate and extrastriate visual areas

Putting spatial attention on the map: timing and localization of stimulus selection processes in striate and extrastriate visual areas Vision Research 41 (2001) 1437 1457 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Putting spatial attention on the map: timing and localization of stimulus selection processes in striate and extrastriate visual areas

More information

Attention Modulates Earliest Responses in the Primary Auditory and Visual Cortices

Attention Modulates Earliest Responses in the Primary Auditory and Visual Cortices Article Attention Modulates Earliest Responses in the Primary Auditory and Visual Cortices Vahe Poghosyan 1,2 and Andreas A. Ioannides 1,2, * 1 Laboratory for Human Brain Dynamics, AAI Scientific Cultural

More information

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Cerebral Cortex March 2006;16:415-424 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi121 Advance Access publication June 15, 2005 Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Mika

More information

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Journal: Psychological Science Manuscript ID: PSCI-0-0.R Manuscript Type: Short report Date Submitted by the Author:

More information

HST 583 fmri DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION

HST 583 fmri DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION HST 583 fmri DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION Neural Signal Processing for Functional Neuroimaging Neuroscience Statistics Research Laboratory Massachusetts General Hospital Harvard Medical School/MIT Division

More information

Profiling Attention s Pure Effect on the Sensory-Evoked P1 and N1 Event-Related Potentials of Human Electroencephalography

Profiling Attention s Pure Effect on the Sensory-Evoked P1 and N1 Event-Related Potentials of Human Electroencephalography Profiling Attention s Pure Effect on the Sensory-Evoked P1 and N1 Event-Related Potentials of Human Electroencephalography by Allison Elisabeth Connell A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction

More information

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 2008, 8 (2), 195-210 doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.2.195 The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies MIKA

More information

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task Psychophysiology, 44 (2007), 913 917. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2007 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00561.x BRIEF REPORT Reward prediction

More information

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Javier R. Movellan Institute for Neural Computation University of California San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093-0515 movellan@inc.ucsd.edu Thomas

More information

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus)

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus) EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus) Introduction 17 subjects were tested to measure effects of a Brain.fm focus session on cognition. With 4 additional subjects, we recorded EEG data during baseline and while

More information

An Electrophysiological Study of Attention Capture by Salience: Does Rarity Enable Capture?

An Electrophysiological Study of Attention Capture by Salience: Does Rarity Enable Capture? An Electrophysiological Study of Attention Capture by Salience: Does Rarity Enable Capture? Noesen, B., Lien, M. C., & Ruthruff, E. (2014). An electrophysiological study of attention capture by salience:

More information

Accepted Manuscript. Title: Sensory and cognitive processes of shifts of spatial attention induced by numbers: an ERPs study

Accepted Manuscript. Title: Sensory and cognitive processes of shifts of spatial attention induced by numbers: an ERPs study Accepted Manuscript Title: Sensory and cognitive processes of shifts of spatial attention induced by numbers: an ERPs study Authors: Elena Salillas, Radouane El Yagoubi, Carlo Semenza PII: S0010-9452(07)00083-4

More information

Temporal attention enhances early visual processing: A review and new evidence from event-related potentials

Temporal attention enhances early visual processing: A review and new evidence from event-related potentials available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Temporal attention enhances early visual processing: A review and new evidence from event-related potentials Ángel Correa,

More information

Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention

Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention Tineke Grent- t-jong 1,2, Marty G. Woldorff 1,3* PLoS BIOLOGY 1 Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University,

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neural Netw. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2007 November 1.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neural Netw. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2007 November 1. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Neural Netw. 2006 November ; 19(9): 1447 1449. Models of human visual attention should consider trial-by-trial variability in preparatory

More information

This article was originally published in a journal published by Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided by Elsevier for the author s benefit and for the benefit of the author s institution, for non-commercial

More information

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms Submitted to Current Biology Mental representation of number in different numerical forms Anna Plodowski, Rachel Swainson, Georgina M. Jackson, Chris Rorden and Stephen R. Jackson School of Psychology

More information

Mental imagery: Against the Nihilistic Hypothesis. Giorgio Ganis a,c William L. Thompson a. Harvard University, Department of Psychology

Mental imagery: Against the Nihilistic Hypothesis. Giorgio Ganis a,c William L. Thompson a. Harvard University, Department of Psychology Kosslyn et al., p. 1 Mental imagery: Against the Nihilistic Hypothesis Stephen M. Kosslyn a,b Giorgio Ganis a,c William L. Thompson a a Harvard University, Department of Psychology b Massachusetts General

More information

Pushing around the Locus of Selection: Evidence for the Flexible-selection Hypothesis

Pushing around the Locus of Selection: Evidence for the Flexible-selection Hypothesis Pushing around the Locus of Selection: Evidence for the Flexible-selection Hypothesis Edward K. Vogel 1, Geoffrey F. Woodman 2, and Steven J. Luck 3 Abstract & Attention operates at an early stage in some

More information

Title change detection system in the visu

Title change detection system in the visu Title Attention switching function of mem change detection system in the visu Author(s) Kimura, Motohiro; Katayama, Jun'ich Citation International Journal of Psychophys Issue Date 2008-02 DOI Doc URLhttp://hdl.handle.net/2115/33891

More information

Report. Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Martin Eimer 1, * and Anna Grubert 1 1

Report. Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Martin Eimer 1, * and Anna Grubert 1 1 Current Biology 24, 193 198, January 20, 2014 ª2014 The Authors http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.001 Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects Report Martin

More information

Neural Networks: Tracing Cellular Pathways. Lauren Berryman Sunfest 2000

Neural Networks: Tracing Cellular Pathways. Lauren Berryman Sunfest 2000 Neural Networks: Tracing Cellular Pathways Lauren Berryman Sunfest 000 Neural Networks: Tracing Cellular Pathways Research Objective Background Methodology and Experimental Approach Results and Conclusions

More information

Anticipatory Biasing of Visuospatial Attention Indexed by Retinotopically Specific -Band Electroencephalography Increases over Occipital Cortex

Anticipatory Biasing of Visuospatial Attention Indexed by Retinotopically Specific -Band Electroencephalography Increases over Occipital Cortex The Journal of Neuroscience, 2000, Vol. 20 RC63 1of6 Anticipatory Biasing of Visuospatial Attention Indexed by Retinotopically Specific -Band Electroencephalography Increases over Occipital Cortex Michael

More information

Neural Strategies for Selective Attention Distinguish Fast-Action Video Game Players

Neural Strategies for Selective Attention Distinguish Fast-Action Video Game Players Neural Strategies for Selective Attention Distinguish Fast-Action Video Game Players - Online First - Springer 7/6/1 1:4 PM Neural Strategies for Selective Attention Distinguish Fast-Action Video Game

More information

Inner Vision: Seeing the Mind s Eye

Inner Vision: Seeing the Mind s Eye William James prize article : Seeing the Mind s Eye Abstract Does imagining a visual pattern involve the same mechanisms as actually seeing that pattern? If so, what are the functional consequences of

More information

Supplementary Note Psychophysics:

Supplementary Note Psychophysics: Supplementary Note More detailed description of MM s subjective experiences can be found on Mike May s Perceptions Home Page, http://www.senderogroup.com/perception.htm Psychophysics: The spatial CSF was

More information

On the role of response conflicts and stimulus position for hemispheric differences in global/local processing: an ERP study

On the role of response conflicts and stimulus position for hemispheric differences in global/local processing: an ERP study On the role of response conflicts and stimulus position for hemispheric differences in global/local processing: an ERP study Gregor Volberg, Ronald Hübner Universität Konstanz, Fachbereich Psychologie,

More information

The Effect of Spatial Attention on Contrast Response Functions in Human Visual Cortex

The Effect of Spatial Attention on Contrast Response Functions in Human Visual Cortex The Journal of Neuroscience, January 3, 2007 27(1):93 97 93 Brief Communications The Effect of Spatial Attention on Contrast Response Functions in Human Visual Cortex Giedrius T. Buracas and Geoffrey M.

More information

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT (1) Foreword (optional) (2) Table of Contents (if report is more than 10 pages) (3) List of Appendixes, Illustrations and Tables (if applicable) (4) Statement of the problem studied FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

More information

The influence of predictive value of cues in the endogenous orienting paradigm examined with event-related lateralizations

The influence of predictive value of cues in the endogenous orienting paradigm examined with event-related lateralizations The influence of predictive value of cues in the endogenous orienting paradigm examined with event-related lateralizations Franka Roorda First supervisor: Rob van der Lubbe Second supervisor: Suzanne Vosslamber

More information

What do you notice? Woodman, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 2010

What do you notice? Woodman, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 2010 What do you notice? Woodman, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 2010 You are trying to determine if a small amplitude signal is a consistent marker of a neural process. How might you design an experiment to

More information

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Cognitive Brain Research 18 (2004) 255 272 Research report Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Audrey Duarte a, *, Charan Ranganath

More information

Rapid Feature-driven Changes in the Attentional Window

Rapid Feature-driven Changes in the Attentional Window Rapid Feature-driven Changes in the Attentional Window Carly J. Leonard, Javier Lopez-Calderon, Johanna Kreither, and Steven J. Luck Abstract Spatial attention must adjust around an object of interest

More information

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention University of New Mexico UNM Digital Repository Psychology ETDs Electronic Theses and Dissertations 2-9-2010 Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention David B.

More information

Experimental Design. Thomas Wolbers Space and Aging Laboratory Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems

Experimental Design. Thomas Wolbers Space and Aging Laboratory Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems Experimental Design Thomas Wolbers Space and Aging Laboratory Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems Overview Design of functional neuroimaging studies Categorical designs Factorial designs Parametric

More information

Effects of Attentional Load on Early Visual Processing Depend on Stimulus Timing

Effects of Attentional Load on Early Visual Processing Depend on Stimulus Timing r Human Brain Mapping 33:63 74 (2012) r Effects of Attentional Load on Early Visual Processing Depend on Stimulus Timing Karsten Rauss, 1,2,3 * Gilles Pourtois, 4 Patrik Vuilleumier, 1,2,5 and Sophie Schwartz

More information

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544 Frank Tong Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544 Office: Room 3-N-2B Telephone: 609-258-2652 Fax: 609-258-1113 Email: ftong@princeton.edu Graduate School Applicants

More information

WHAT HOLDS YOUR ATTENTION? THE NEURAL EFFECTS OF MEMORY ON ATTENTION. Emily Leonard Parks

WHAT HOLDS YOUR ATTENTION? THE NEURAL EFFECTS OF MEMORY ON ATTENTION. Emily Leonard Parks WHAT HOLDS YOUR ATTENTION? THE NEURAL EFFECTS OF MEMORY ON ATTENTION Emily Leonard Parks A thesis submitted to the faculty of the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill in partial fulfillment of the

More information

COMPETITION AND SELECTIVITY IN THE VISUAL SYSTEM: EVIDENCE FROM EVENT-RELATED BRAIN POTENTIALS

COMPETITION AND SELECTIVITY IN THE VISUAL SYSTEM: EVIDENCE FROM EVENT-RELATED BRAIN POTENTIALS COMPETITION AND SELECTIVITY IN THE VISUAL SYSTEM: EVIDENCE FROM EVENT-RELATED BRAIN POTENTIALS A Dissertation Presented to The Academic Faculty by Matthew R. Hilimire In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

More information

LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization

LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization THE VISUAL SYSTEM Higher Visual Processing Martin Paré Assistant Professor Physiology & Psychology Most of the cortex that covers the cerebral hemispheres is

More information

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2 Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2 Hillyard(1973) Dichotic Listening Task N1 component enhanced for attended stimuli Supports early selection Effects of Voluntary

More information

ERP INDICES OF ATTENTIONAL DISENGAGEMENT AND FILTERING FOLLOWING REFLEXIVE ATTENTIONAL CAPTURE. Cassie Barasch Ford. Chapel Hill

ERP INDICES OF ATTENTIONAL DISENGAGEMENT AND FILTERING FOLLOWING REFLEXIVE ATTENTIONAL CAPTURE. Cassie Barasch Ford. Chapel Hill ERP INDICES OF ATTENTIONAL DISENGAGEMENT AND FILTERING FOLLOWING REFLEXIVE ATTENTIONAL CAPTURE Cassie Barasch Ford A thesis submitted to the faculty of the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill in

More information

Systematic biases in early ERP and ERF components as a result of high-pass filtering

Systematic biases in early ERP and ERF components as a result of high-pass filtering Manuscript accepted to the Journal of Neuroscience Methods, June 2012 Systematic biases in early ERP and ERF components as a result of high-pass filtering David J. Acunzo Graham MacKenzie Mark C.W. van

More information

The Journal of Neuroscience For Peer Review Only

The Journal of Neuroscience For Peer Review Only Page 1 of 31 Section: Behavioral System Cognitive Senior Editor: John Maunsell Number of figures and tables: 3 Figures and 4 Supplementary items Number of pages: 25 Title: Different Effects of Voluntary

More information

Competing Streams at the Cocktail Party

Competing Streams at the Cocktail Party Competing Streams at the Cocktail Party A Neural and Behavioral Study of Auditory Attention Jonathan Z. Simon Neuroscience and Cognitive Sciences / Biology / Electrical & Computer Engineering University

More information

Viewing a Map Versus Reading a Description of a Map: Modality-Specific Encoding of Spatial Information

Viewing a Map Versus Reading a Description of a Map: Modality-Specific Encoding of Spatial Information Cognitive Science 29 (2005) 807 818 Copyright 2005 Cognitive Science Society, Inc. All rights reserved. Viewing a Map Versus Reading a Description of a Map: Modality-Specific Encoding of Spatial Information

More information

UC Irvine UC Irvine Previously Published Works

UC Irvine UC Irvine Previously Published Works UC Irvine UC Irvine Previously Published Works Title Fast and localized event-related optical signals (EROS) in the human occipital cortex: Comparisons with the visual evoked potential and fmri Permalink

More information

Delineating the Neural Signatures of Tracking Spatial Position and Working Memory during Attentive Tracking

Delineating the Neural Signatures of Tracking Spatial Position and Working Memory during Attentive Tracking The Journal of Neuroscience, January 12, 2011 31(2):659 668 659 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Delineating the Neural Signatures of Tracking Spatial Position and Working Memory during Attentive Tracking

More information

Processing Stages of Visual Stimuli and Event-Related Potentials

Processing Stages of Visual Stimuli and Event-Related Potentials Processing Stages of Visual Stimuli and Event-Related Potentials Carolina Saavedra, Laurent Bougrain To cite this version: Carolina Saavedra, Laurent Bougrain. Processing Stages of Visual Stimuli and Event-Related

More information

Supplemental Information

Supplemental Information Current Biology, Volume 22 Supplemental Information The Neural Correlates of Crowding-Induced Changes in Appearance Elaine J. Anderson, Steven C. Dakin, D. Samuel Schwarzkopf, Geraint Rees, and John Greenwood

More information

Temporal kinetics of prefrontal modulation of the extrastriate cortex during visual attention

Temporal kinetics of prefrontal modulation of the extrastriate cortex during visual attention Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 2004, 4 (4), 609-617 Temporal kinetics of prefrontal modulation of the extrastriate cortex during visual attention ELENA YAGO, AUDREY DUARTE, and TING WONG

More information

THE EFFECTS OF EXOGENOUS ORIENTING ON SUSTAINED ENDOGENOUS ATTENTION. Prerna Bholah

THE EFFECTS OF EXOGENOUS ORIENTING ON SUSTAINED ENDOGENOUS ATTENTION. Prerna Bholah THE EFFECTS OF EXOGENOUS ORIENTING ON SUSTAINED ENDOGENOUS ATTENTION Prerna Bholah A thesis submitted to the faculty of the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill in partial fulfillment of the requirements

More information

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Intro Examine attention response functions Compare an attention-demanding

More information

Covariations in ERP and PET Measures of Spatial Selective Attention in Human Extrastriate Visual Cortex

Covariations in ERP and PET Measures of Spatial Selective Attention in Human Extrastriate Visual Cortex Human Brain Mapping 5:273 279(1997) Covariations in ERP and PET Measures of Spatial Selective Attention in Human Extrastriate Visual Cortex George R. Mangun, 1 * Joseph B. Hopfinger, 1 Clifton L. Kussmaul,

More information

Attention and Sensory Gain Control: A Peripheral Visual Process?

Attention and Sensory Gain Control: A Peripheral Visual Process? Attention and Sensory Gain Control: A Peripheral Visual Process? Todd C. Handy 1 and Wayne Khoe 2 Abstract & Attention-related sensory gain control in human extrastriate cortex is believed to improve the

More information

Visual Mental Imagery Induces Retinotopically Organized Activation of Early Visual Areas

Visual Mental Imagery Induces Retinotopically Organized Activation of Early Visual Areas Cerebral Cortex October 2005;15:1570--1583 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi035 Advance Access publication February 2, 2005 Visual Mental Imagery Induces Retinotopically Organized Activation of Early Visual Areas

More information

Selective Attention. Modes of Control. Domains of Selection

Selective Attention. Modes of Control. Domains of Selection The New Yorker (2/7/5) Selective Attention Perception and awareness are necessarily selective (cell phone while driving): attention gates access to awareness Selective attention is deployed via two modes

More information

Report. A Soft Handoff of Attention between Cerebral Hemispheres

Report. A Soft Handoff of Attention between Cerebral Hemispheres Current iology 24, 1133 1137, May 19, 2014 ª2014 Elsevier Ltd ll rights reserved http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.03.054 Soft Handoff of ttention between Cerebral Hemispheres Report Trafton Drew, 1,2,4,

More information

Neural correlates of non-familiar face processing

Neural correlates of non-familiar face processing Budapest University of Technology and Economics PhD School in Psychology Cognitive Science Krisztina Nagy PhD thesis booklet Supervisor: Prof. Gyula Kovács Budapest, 2013 Contents Abbreviations 2 1 Introduction

More information

Neurophysiological correlates of memory illusion in both encoding and retrieval phases

Neurophysiological correlates of memory illusion in both encoding and retrieval phases available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Neurophysiological correlates of memory illusion in both encoding and retrieval phases Haiyan Geng a,, Yaqiong Qi a,

More information

Accounts for the N170 face-effect: a reply to Rossion, Curran, & Gauthier

Accounts for the N170 face-effect: a reply to Rossion, Curran, & Gauthier S. Bentin, D. Carmel / Cognition 85 (2002) 197 202 197 COGNITION Cognition 85 (2002) 197 202 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Discussion Accounts for the N170 face-effect: a reply to Rossion, Curran, & Gauthier

More information

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory?

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory? Running Head: Visual Working Memory and the CDA Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory? Nick Berggren and

More information

Visual attention: the where, what, how and why of saliency Stefan Treue

Visual attention: the where, what, how and why of saliency Stefan Treue 428 Visual attention: the where, what, how and why of saliency Stefan Treue Attention influences the processing of visual information even in the earliest areas of primate visual cortex. There is converging

More information

When and where is binocular rivalry resolved in the visual cortex?

When and where is binocular rivalry resolved in the visual cortex? Journal of Vision (2010) 10(14):25, 1 11 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/14/25 1 When and where is binocular rivalry resolved in the visual cortex? Michael A. Pitts Antígona Martínez Steven A.

More information

Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response

Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response Human Brain Mapping 6:373 377(1998) Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response Randy L. Buckner 1,2,3 * 1 Departments of Psychology, Anatomy and Neurobiology, and Radiology, Washington University,

More information

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention Sleep Deprivation and Selective Attention An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention Logan T. Trujillo, PhD 1 ; Steve Kornguth,

More information

1 Decoding attentional orientation from EEG spectra. Ramesh Srinivasan, Samuel Thorpe, Siyi Deng, Tom Lappas & Michael D'Zmura

1 Decoding attentional orientation from EEG spectra. Ramesh Srinivasan, Samuel Thorpe, Siyi Deng, Tom Lappas & Michael D'Zmura 1 Ramesh Srinivasan, Samuel Thorpe, Siyi Deng, Tom Lappas & Michael D'Zmura Department of Cognitive Sciences, UC Irvine, SSPA 3151, Irvine, CA 92697 5100 {r.srinivasan,sthorpe,sdeng,tlappas,mdzmura}@uci.edu

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Forsyth et al. 10.1073/pnas.1509262112 SI Methods Inclusion Criteria. Participants were eligible for the study if they were between 18 and 30 y of age; were comfortable reading in

More information

Disparity- and velocity- based signals for 3D motion perception in human MT+ Bas Rokers, Lawrence K. Cormack, and Alexander C. Huk

Disparity- and velocity- based signals for 3D motion perception in human MT+ Bas Rokers, Lawrence K. Cormack, and Alexander C. Huk Disparity- and velocity- based signals for 3D motion perception in human MT+ Bas Rokers, Lawrence K. Cormack, and Alexander C. Huk Supplementary Materials fmri response (!% BOLD) ).5 CD versus STS 1 wedge

More information

Are there Hemispheric Differences in Visual Processes that Utilize Gestalt Principles?

Are there Hemispheric Differences in Visual Processes that Utilize Gestalt Principles? Carnegie Mellon University Research Showcase @ CMU Dietrich College Honors Theses Dietrich College of Humanities and Social Sciences 2006 Are there Hemispheric Differences in Visual Processes that Utilize

More information

fmri and visual brain function Dr. Tessa Dekker UCL Institute of Ophthalmology

fmri and visual brain function Dr. Tessa Dekker UCL Institute of Ophthalmology fmri and visual brain function Dr. Tessa Dekker UCL Institute of Ophthalmology 6 th February 2018 Brief history of brain imaging 1895 First human X-ray image 1950 First human PET scan - uses traces of

More information

Efficiently representing visual information requires selecting

Efficiently representing visual information requires selecting Cortical systems mediating visual attention to both objects and spatial locations Sarah Shomstein* and Marlene Behrmann Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University and Center for the Neural Basis

More information

Perceptual asynchrony: motion leads color

Perceptual asynchrony: motion leads color VISION, CENTRAL Perceptual asynchrony: motion leads color Peng Wang a,shenghe b,silufan a, Zu Xiang Liu a and Lin Chen a a State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, Institute of Biophysics,Chinese

More information

The Roles of Feature-Specific Task Set and Bottom-Up Salience in Attentional Capture: An ERP Study

The Roles of Feature-Specific Task Set and Bottom-Up Salience in Attentional Capture: An ERP Study Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2009, Vol. 35, No. 5, 1316 1328 2009 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0015872 The Roles of Feature-Specific

More information

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 3, 316-320 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0096-3445/94/S3.00 COMMENT Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention:

More information

Seeing faces in the noise: Stochastic activity in perceptual regions of the brain may influence the perception of ambiguous stimuli

Seeing faces in the noise: Stochastic activity in perceptual regions of the brain may influence the perception of ambiguous stimuli R175B BC JP To printer - need negs for pgs, 3,, and Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,?? (?),???-??? Seeing faces in the noise: Stochastic activity in perceptual regions of the brain may influence the perception

More information

Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology

Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology Rui Cao (caorui.beilia@gmail.com) Thomas A. Busey (busey@indiana.edu) Robert M. Nosofsky (nosofsky@indiana.edu) Richard

More information

Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics. Scott Makeig. sccn.ucsd.edu

Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics. Scott Makeig. sccn.ucsd.edu Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics Scott Makeig Institute for Neural Computation University of California San Diego La Jolla CA sccn.ucsd.edu Talk given at the EEG/MEG course

More information